Need by tomorrow 5pm central time

profilesavcarter
HumanDevelopmentALife-SpanViewbyRobertV.KailJohnC.Cavanaughz-lib.org.pdf

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

8e

Human Development A Life-Span View

Robert V. Kail Purdue University

John C. Cavanaugh Consortium of Universities of the Washington Metropolitan Area

Australia • Brazil • Mexico • Singapore • United Kingdom • United States

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

This is an electronic version of the print textbook. Due to electronic rights restrictions, some third party content may be suppressed. Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. The publisher reserves the right to remove content from this title at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. For valuable information on pricing, previous editions, changes to current editions, and alternate formats, please visit www.cengage.com/highered to search by ISBN#, author, title, or keyword for materials in your areas of interest.

Important Notice: Media content referenced within the product description or the product text may not be available in the eBook version.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Printed in the United States of America Print Number: 01 Print Year: 2017

© 2019, 2016 Cengage Learning, Inc.

Unless otherwise noted, all content is © Cengage.

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. No part of this work covered by the copyright herein may be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means, except as permitted by U.S. copy- right law, without the prior written permission of the copyright owner.

Library of Congress Control Number: 2017950383

Student Edition: ISBN: 978-1-337-55483-1

Loose-leaf Edition: ISBN: 978-1-337-56365-9

Cengage 20 Channel Center Street Boston, MA 02210 USA

Cengage is a leading provider of customized learning solutions with employees residing in nearly 40 different countries and sales in more than 125 countries around the world. Find your local representative at www.cengage.com.

Cengage products are represented in Canada by Nelson Education, Ltd.

To learn more about Cengage platforms and services, visit www.cengage.com.

Human Development: A Life-Span View, Eighth Edition Robert V. Kail and John C. Cavanaugh

Product Director: Marta Lee-Perriard

Product Team Manager: Star Burruto

Product Manager: Andrew Ginsberg

Content Developer: Nedah Rose

Product Assistant: Leah Jenson

Digital Content Specialist: Allison Marion

Content Project Manager: Ruth Sakata Corley

Production and Composition Service: SPi Global

Intellectual Property Analyst: Deanna Ettinger

Intellectual Property Project Manager: Kathryn Kucharek

Art Director: Vernon Boes

Text and Cover Designer: Cheryl Carrington

Cover Image: (top to bottom) FatCamera/ Getty Images, Mike Kemp/Blend Images/ Getty Images, Hero Images/Getty Images, Mireya Acierto/Getty Images, John Lund/ Getty Images, Knauer/Johnston/Getty Images, Maskot/Getty Images, andipantz/iStock/ Getty Images

For product information and technology assistance, contact us at Cengage Customer & Sales Support, 1-800-354-9706.

For permission to use material from this text or product, submit all requests online at www.cengage.com/permissions.

Further permissions questions can be e-mailed to [email protected].

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

To Dea and Chris

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

v

RobeRt V. Kail is Distinguished Professor of Psychological Sciences at Purdue University. His undergraduate degree is from Ohio Wesleyan University and his PhD is from the University of Michigan. Kail is editor of Child Development Perspectives and edi- tor emeritus of Psychological Science. He received the McCandless Young Scientist Award from the American Psychological Association, was named the Distinguished Sesquicen- tennial Alumnus in Psychology by Ohio Wesleyan University, is a fellow of the Association for Psychological Science, and is an honorary professor at the University of Heidelberg, Germany. Kail has also written Children and Their Development and Scientific Writing for Psychology. His research focuses on cognitive development during childhood and adolescence. Away from the office, he enjoys photography and working out.

About the Author

John C. CaVanaugh is President and CEO of the Consortium of Universities of the Washington Metropolitan Area. He received his undergraduate degree from the University of Delaware and his PhD from the University of Notre Dame. Cavanaugh is a fellow of the American Psychological Association, the Association for Psychological Science, and the Gerontological Society of America, and has served as president of the Adult Development and Aging Division (Division 20) of the APA. Cavanaugh has also written (with the late Fredda Blanchard-Fields) Adult Development and Aging. His research interests in gerontology concern family caregiving as well as the role of beliefs in older adults’ cognitive performance. For enjoyment, he backpacks, enjoys photography and cooking, and is an avid traveler.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

vii

1 The Study of Human Development 3 part 1 Prenatal Development, infancy, and early Childhood 39 2 Biological Foundations: Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth 39 3 Tools for Exploring the World: Physical, Perceptual, and Motor Development 79 4 The Emergence of Thought and Language: Cognitive Development in Infancy

and Early Childhood 117 5 Entering the Social World: Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Early

Childhood 155

part 2 School-age Children and adolescents 189 6 Off to School: Cognitive and Physical Development in Middle Childhood 189 7 Expanding Social Horizons: Socioemotional Development in Middle Childhood 227 8 Rites of Passage: Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence 263 9 Moving into the Adult Social World: Socioemotional Development in Adolescence 289

part 3 Young and Middle adulthood 317 10 Becoming an Adult: Physical, Cognitive, and Personality Development in Young

Adulthood 317 11 Being with Others: Relationships in Young and Middle Adulthood 355 12 Working and Relaxing 387 13 Making It in Midlife: The Biopsychosocial Challenges of Middle Adulthood 421

part 4 late adulthood 457 14 The Personal Context of Later Life: Physical, Cognitive, and Mental Health Issues 457 15 Social Aspects of Later Life: Psychosocial, Retirement, Relationship,

and Societal Issues 501 16 Dying and Bereavement 541

Brief Contents

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

ix

About the Authors v Preface xxi To the Student xxxi Neuroscience Index xxxv Diversity Index xxxvii

1 The Study of Human Development 3 What Do YOU thinK? Would You Want to Live to Be 142? 4

1.1 thinking about Development 5 Recurring Issues in Human Development 5 Basic Forces in Human Development: The Biopsychosocial Framework 7 Neuroscience: A Window into Human Development 10

Real People Applying Human Development: Muhammad Ali 11

1.2 Developmental theories 12 Psychodynamic Theory 12 Learning Theory 13 Cognitive-Developmental Theory 14 The Ecological and Systems Approach 17 Life-Span Perspective, Selective Optimization with Compensation, and

Life-Course Perspective 18 The Big Picture 21

1.3 Doing Developmental Research 22 Measurement in Human Development Research 23 General Designs for Research 26 Designs for Studying Development 28

Spotlight on Research The Stability of Intelligence from Age 11 to Age 90 Years 29

Integrating Findings from Different Studies 31 Conducting Research Ethically 32 Communicating Research Results 33 Applying Research Results: Social Policy 33

Summary 34 Key Terms 36

2 Biological Foundations: Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth 39 2.1 in the beginning: 23 Pairs of Chromosomes 40

Mechanisms of Heredity 40

part 1 Prenatal Development, infancy, and early Childhood 38

Contents

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

x

Genetic Disorders 43 Heredity, Environment, and Development 45

Real People Applying Human Development: Ben and Matt Pick Their Niches 50

2.2 From Conception to birth 51 Period of the Zygote (Weeks 1–2) 51

What Do YOU thinK? Conception in the 21st Century 52 Period of the Embryo (Weeks 3–8) 53 Period of the Fetus (Weeks 9–38) 55

2.3 influences on Prenatal Development 57 General Risk Factors 57 Teratogens: Drugs, Diseases, and Environmental Hazards 59 Drugs 59 How Teratogens Influence Prenatal Development 62 Prenatal Diagnosis and Treatment 64

2.4 labor and Delivery 67 Stages of Labor 67 Approaches to Childbirth 68 Adjusting to Parenthood 69

Spotlight on Research Links Between Maternal Depression and Children’s Behavior Problems 70

Birth Complications 71 Infant Mortality 72

Summary 74 Key Terms 76

3 Tools for Exploring the World: Physical, Perceptual, and Motor Development 79 3.1 the newborn 80

The Newborn’s Reflexes 80 Assessing the Newborn 80 The Newborn’s States 82 Temperament 85

3.2 Physical Development 87 Growth of the Body 87 The Emerging Nervous System 91

3.3 Moving and grasping: early Motor Skills 96 Locomotion 96 Fine Motor Skills 100

Spotlight on Research Benefits of Training Babies to Grasp 101

3.4 Coming to Know the World: Perception 102 Smell, Taste, and Touch 103 Hearing 103 Seeing 104 Integrating Sensory Information 108

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xi

3.5 becoming Self-aware 109 Origins of Self-Concept 110 Theory of Mind 111

Real People Applying Human Development: “Seeing Is Believing . . .” for 3-Year-Olds 112

Summary 113 Key Terms 115

4 The Emergence of Thought and Language: Cognitive Development in Infancy and Early Childhood 117

4.1 the onset of thinking: Piaget’s account 118 Basic Principles of Cognitive Development 118

Real People Applying Human Development: Learning About Butterflies: Accommodation and Assimilation in Action 119

Sensorimotor Thinking 120 Preoperational Thinking 122 Evaluating Piaget’s Theory 125 Extending Piaget’s Account: Children’s Naïve Theories 126

Spotlight on Research Have a Heart! Preschoolers’ Essentialist Thinking 129

4.2 information Processing during infancy and early Childhood 130 General Principles of Information Processing 130 Attention 131 Learning 131 Memory 132 Learning Number Skills 134

4.3 Mind and Culture: Vygotsky’s theory 136 The Zone of Proximal Development 137 Scaffolding 137 Private Speech 138

4.4 language 139 The Road to Speech 139 First Words and Many More 142 Speaking in Sentences: Grammatical Development 146 Communicating with Others 149

Summary 151 Key Terms 153

5 Entering the Social World: Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Early Childhood 155 5.1 beginnings: trust and attachment 156

Erikson’s Stages of Early Psychosocial Development 156 The Growth of Attachment 157 What Determines Quality of Attachment? 160

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xii

Attachment, Work, and Alternative Caregiving 161 Real People Applying Human Development: Lois, Bill, and Sarah 162

5.2 emerging emotions 163 The Function of Emotions 163 Experiencing and Expressing Emotions 164 Recognizing and Using Others’ Emotions 166 Regulating Emotions 167

5.3 interacting with others 168 The Joys of Play 169 Helping Others 172

5.4 gender Roles and gender identity 176 Images of Men and Women: Facts and Fantasy 177

Spotlight on Research Reasoning About Gender-Related Properties 178 Gender Typing 180 Evolving Gender Roles 184

Summary 185 Key Terms 186

6 Off to School: Cognitive and Physical Development in Middle Childhood 189 6.1 Cognitive Development 190

More Sophisticated Thinking: Piaget’s Version 190 Real People Applying Human Development: Combinatorial Reasoning

Goes to the Races 191 Information-Processing Strategies for Learning and Remembering 192

6.2 aptitudes for School 194 Theories of Intelligence 195 Binet and the Development of Intelligence Testing 198 Do Tests Work? 199 Hereditary and Environmental Factors 200 The Impact of Ethnicity and Socioeconomic Status 201

6.3 Special Children, Special needs 204 Gifted Children 204 Children with Disability 204

Spotlight on Research Improving Children’s Knowledge of the Structure of Words Enhances Their Reading Comprehension 206

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder 208

6.4 academic Skills 209 Reading 209 Writing 212 Math Skills 213

part 2 School-age Children and adolescents 189

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xiii

Real People Applying Human Development: Shin-Ying Loves School 214 Effective Schools, Effective Teachers 216

6.5 Physical Development 218 Growth 218 Development of Motor Skills 219 Physical Fitness 220 Participating in Sports 220

Summary 222 Key Terms 224

7 Expanding Social Horizons: Socioemotional Development in Middle Childhood 227 7.1 Family Relationships 228

The Family as a System 228 Dimensions and Styles of Parenting 229 Siblings 235 Divorce and Remarriage 238 Parent–Child Relationships Gone Awry: Child Maltreatment 239

7.2 Peers 243 Friendships 243 Groups 245 Popularity and Rejection 246 Aggressive Children and Their Victims 248

7.3 electronic Media 249 Television Programs 250 Video Games 251

Spotlight on Research Playing a Video Game Improves Children’s Visual-Spatial Skill 251

Social Media 252

7.4 understanding others 253 Describing Others 253

Real People Applying Human Development: Tell Me About a Girl You Like a Lot 254

Understanding What Others Think 254 Prejudice 256

Summary 258 Key Terms 260

8 Rites of Passage: Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence 263 8.1 Pubertal Changes 264

Signs of Physical Maturation 264 Mechanisms of Maturation 266 Psychological Impact of Puberty 267

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xiv

8.2 health 270 Nutrition 270

Spotlight on Research Evaluating a Program for Preventing Eating Disorders 273

Physical Fitness 274 Threats to Adolescent Well-Being 275

8.3 information Processing During adolescence 276 Working Memory and Processing Speed 276 Content Knowledge, Strategies, and Metacognitive Skill 277 Problem-Solving and Reasoning 277

8.4 Reasoning about Moral issues 279 Kohlberg’s Theory 280

Real People Applying Human Development: Schindler’s List 282 Evaluating Kohlberg’s Theory 282 Promoting Moral Reasoning 283

Summary 285 Key Terms 286

9 Moving into the Adult Social World: Socioemotional Development in Adolescence 289 9.1 identity and Self-esteem 290

The Search for Identity 290 Ethnic Identity 293 Self-Esteem in Adolescence 294 The Myth of Storm and Stress 296

9.2 Romantic Relationships and Sexuality 297 Romantic Relationships 297 Sexual Behavior 298 Sexual Minority Youth 300 Dating Violence 300

9.3 the World of Work 302 Career Development 302

Real People Applying Human Development: “The Life of Lynne”: A Drama in Three Acts 303

Part-Time Employment 305

9.4 the Dark Side 307 Drug Use 307 Depression 308

Spotlight on Research Does Racial Discrimination Lead to Depression? 309 Delinquency 310

What Do YOU thinK? When Juveniles Commit Serious Crimes, Should They Be Tried as Adults? 313

Summary 313 Key Terms 315

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xv

10 Becoming an Adult: Physical, Cognitive, and Personality Development in Young Adulthood 317 10.1 emerging adulthood 318

Role Transitions Marking Adulthood 319 Neuroscience, Behavior, and Emerging Adulthood 320 Achieving Milestones: Education, Workforce, and Erikson’s Intimacy 321 So When Do People Become Adults? 323

Real People Applying Human Development: Does Being Rich and Famous Mean You’re an Adult? 324

10.2 Physical Development and health 325 Growth, Strength, and Physical Functioning 325 Lifestyle Factors in Health 325

What Do YOU thinK? Healthcare Disparities in the United States 333

10.3 Cognitive Development 334 How Should We View Intelligence in Adults? 334 Primary and Secondary Mental Abilities 335

Spotlight on Research The Seattle Longitudinal Study 336 Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence 337 Neuroscience Research and Intelligence 339 Going Beyond Formal Operations: Thinking in Adulthood 340 Integrating Emotion and Logic in Emerging and Young Adulthood 342

10.4 Who Do You Want to be? Personality in Young adulthood 345

Creating Life Stories 346 Possible Selves 347 Personal Control Beliefs 348

Summary 350 Key Terms 352

11 Being with Others: Relationships in Young and Middle Adulthood 355 11.1 Relationship types and issues 356

Friendships 356 Love Relationships 358

Spotlight on Research Patterns and Universals of Romantic Attachment Around the World 361

Violence in Relationships 363

11.2 lifestyles and Relationships 365 Singlehood 365 Cohabitation 366 LGBTQ Relationships 367

part 3 Young and Middle adulthood 317

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xvi

Marriage 368 Real People James Obergefell and John Arthur Make History 369

11.3 Family Dynamics and the life Course 372 The Parental Role 372

What Do YOU thinK? Paid Family Leave 374 Diverse Family Forms 376

11.4 Divorce and Remarriage 379 Divorce 379 Remarriage 382

Summary 384 Key Terms 385

12 Working and Relaxing 387 12.1 occupational Selection and Development 388

The Meaning of Work 388 Occupational Choice Revisited 389 Occupational Development 392 Job Satisfaction 394

12.2 gender, ethnicity, and Discrimination issues 397 Gender Differences in Occupational Selection 397 Women and Occupational Development 398 Ethnicity and Occupational Development 399 Bias and Discrimination 400 Gender Bias, Glass Ceilings, and Glass Cliffs 400

What Do YOU thinK? Helping Women Lean In 401

12.3 occupational transitions 404 Retraining Workers 404 Occupational Insecurity 405 Coping with Unemployment 405

Real People The Politics of Unemployment 406

12.4 Work and Family 408 The Dependent Care Dilemma 408 Juggling Multiple Roles 410

12.5 taking time to Relax: leisure activities 413 Types of Leisure Activities 413 Developmental Changes in Leisure 414

Spotlight on Research Long-Term Effects of Leisure Activities 415

Consequences of Leisure Activities 415

Summary 417 Key Terms 419

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xvii

13 Making It in Midlife: The Biopsychosocial Challenges of Middle Adulthood 421 13.1 Physical Changes and health 422

Changes in Appearance 422 Changes in Bones and Joints 423 Reproductive Changes 427

What Do YOU thinK? Menopausal Hormone Therapy 429 Stress and Health 430 Exercise 434

13.2 Cognitive Development 436 Practical Intelligence 436 Becoming an Expert 438 Lifelong Learning 439

Spotlight on Research Designing Software for Middle-Aged and Older Nonexperts 440

13.3 Personality 441 The Five-Factor Trait Model 441 Changing Priorities in Midlife 443

13.4 Family Dynamics and Middle age 447 Letting Go: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Children 447 Giving Back: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Aging Parents 448

Real People Applying Human Development: Taking Care of Mom 448 Grandparenthood 450

Summary 453 Key Terms 454

14 The Personal Context of Later Life: Physical, Cognitive, and Mental Health Issues 457 14.1 What are older adults like? 458

The Demographics of Aging 458 Longevity 461 The Third–Fourth Age Distinction 464

14.2 Physical Changes and health 465 Biological Theories of Aging 466 Physiological Changes 468 Chronic Disease and Lifestyle Health Issues 475

Real People The “Angelina Jolie Effect” 477

14.3 Cognitive Processes 479 Information Processing 479

part 4 late adulthood 457

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xviii

Memory 481 Creativity and Wisdom 485

What Do YOU thinK? Does Creativity Exist? 486

14.4 Mental health and intervention 488 Depression 488 Anxiety Disorders 490 Dementia 491

Spotlight on Research Training Persons with Dementia to Be Group Activity Leaders 496

Summary 497 Key Terms 499

15 Social Aspects of Later Life: Psychosocial, Retirement, Relationship, and Societal Issues 501 15.1 theories of Psychosocial aging 502

The Goal of Healthy Aging 502 Competence and Environmental Press 503 Preventive and Corrective Proactivity Model 505

Real People Applying Human Development: Katherine Johnson: Human Computer for NASA 507

15.2 Personality, Social Cognition, and Spirituality 508 Integrity Versus Despair 508 Well-Being and Emotion 509

Spotlight on Research The Aging Emotional Brain 510 Spirituality in Later Life 512

15.3 i used to Work at . . . : living in Retirement 514 What Does Being Retired Mean? 514 Why Do People Retire? 515 Adjustment to Retirement 516 Employment and Volunteering 516

15.4 Friends and Family in late life 519 Friends, Siblings, and Socioemotional Selectivity 519 Marriage and Same-Sex Partnerships 521 Caring for a Partner 523 Widowhood 523 Great-Grandparenthood 524

15.5 Social issues and aging 525 Frail Older Adults 526 Housing Options 528 Elder Abuse and Neglect 532 Social Security and Medicare 533

What Do YOU thinK? What to Do About Social Security and Medicare? 535

Summary 536 Key Terms 538

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xix

16 Dying and Bereavement 541 16.1 Definitions and ethical issues 542

Sociocultural Definitions of Death 543 Legal and Medical Definitions 544 Ethical Issues 544

What Do YOU thinK? The Brittany Maynard Case 547

16.2 thinking about Death: Personal aspects 548 A Life-Course Approach to Dying 549

Real People Randy Pausch’s Last Lecture 549 Dealing with One’s Own Death 550 Death Anxiety 551

16.3 end-of-life issues 554 Creating a Final Scenario 554 The Hospice Option 556 Making Your End-of-Life Intentions Known 558

16.4 Surviving the loss: the grieving Process 560 The Grief Process 561 Typical Grief Reactions 563 Coping with Grief 564 Ambiguous Loss 566 Complicated or Prolonged Grief Disorder 567

Spotlight on Research The Costs of Holding in Grief for the Sake of One’s Partner 568

Disenfranchised Grief 568

16.5 Dying and bereavement experiences across the life Span 569 Childhood 570 Adolescence 571 Adulthood 572 Late Adulthood 574 Conclusion 575

Summary 576 Key Terms 578

glossary 580

References 588

name index 653

Subject index 667

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxi

“ W hat do you want to be when you grow up?” “Where do you see yourself in the next 5 or 10 years?” “What kind of person do you want to become?” These and other questions about “becoming” confront us across our lives. Answering them requires us to understand ourselves in very thorough ways. It requires us to understand how we develop.

Human development is both the most fascinating and the most complex science there is. Human Development: A Life-Span View, Eighth Edition, introduces you to the issues, forces, and outcomes that make us who we are.

Contemporary research and theory on human development consistently emphasize the multidisciplinary approach needed to describe and explain how people change (and how they stay the same) over time. Moreover, the great diversity of people requires an appreciation for individual differences throughout development. Human Development: A Life-Span View, Eighth Edition, incorporates both and aims to address three specific goals:

■■ To provide a comprehensive, yet highly readable, account of human development across the life span.

■■ To provide theoretical and empirical foundations that enable students to become educated and critical interpreters of developmental information.

■■ To provide a blend of basic and applied research, as well as controversial topics and emergent trends, to demonstrate connections between the laboratory and life and the dynamic science of human development.

Preface

OrganizatiOn

a Modified Chronological approach The great debate among authors and instructors in the field of human development is whether to take a chronological approach (focusing on functioning at specific stages of the life span, such as infancy, adolescence, and middle adulthood) or a topical approach (following a specific aspect of development, such as personality, throughout the life span). Both approaches have their merits. We have chosen a modified chronological approach that combines the best aspects of both. The overall organization of the text is chronological: We trace development from conception through late life in sequential order and dedicate sev- eral chapters to topical issues pertaining to particular points in the life span (such as infancy and early childhood, adolescence, young adulthood, middle adulthood, and late life).

Because the developmental continuity of such topics as social and cognitive devel- opment gets lost with narrowly defined, artificial age-stage divisions, we dedicate some chapters to tracing their development over larger segments of the life span. These chap- ters provide a much more coherent description of important developmental changes, emphasize the fact that development is not easily divided into “slices,” and provide stu- dents with understandable explications of developmental theories.

Balanced Coverage of the Entire Life Span A primary difference between Human Development: A Life-Span View, Eighth Edition, and similar texts is that this book provides a much richer and more complete descrip- tion of adult development and aging. Following the introductory chapter, the remaining

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxii

15 chapters of the text are evenly divided between childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and aging. This balanced treatment reflects not only the rapid emergence of adult development and aging as a major emphasis in the science of human development but also recognizes that roughly three-fourths of a person’s life occurs beyond adolescence.

As a reflection of our modified chronological approach, Human Development: A  Life-Span View, Eighth Edition, is divided into four main parts. After an introduction to the science of human development (Chapter 1), Part One includes a discussion of the biological foundations of life (Chapter 2) and development during infancy and early childhood ( Chapters 3–5). Part Two focuses on development during middle childhood and adolescence (Chapters 6–9). Part Three (Chapters 10–13) focuses on young and middle adulthood. Part Four examines late adulthood (Chapters 14 and 15) and concludes with a consideration of dying and bereavement (Chapter 16).

COntEnt and apprOaCh: the Biopsychosocial Emphasis Our text provides comprehensive, up-to-date coverage of research and theory from con- ception to old age and death. We explicitly adopt the biopsychosocial framework as an organizing theme, describing it in depth in Chapter 1, then integrating it throughout the text—often in combination with other developmental theories.

an Engaging personal Style On several occasions, we communicate our personal involvement with the issues being discussed by providing examples from our own experiences as illustrations of how human development plays itself out in people’s lives. Additionally, every major section of a chapter opens with a short vignette, helping to personalize a concept just before it is discussed. Other rich examples are integrated throughout the text narrative and showcased in the Real People features.

Emphasis on inclusiveness In content coverage, in the personalized examples used, and in the photo program, we emphasize diversity—within the United States and around the world—in ethnicity, gender, race, age, ability, and sexual orientation.

ChangES in thE Eighth EditiOn The eighth edition has been updated with new graphics and several hundred new refer- ence citations to work from the past 3 years. Of particular note are these content additions, updates, and revisions:

Chapter 1 ■■ New Real People feature on Muhammad Ali

Chapter 2 ■■ Much revised What Do You Think? feature on conception in the 21st century ■■ Much revised coverage of the period of the fetus ■■ Much revised coverage of nutrition during pregnancy ■■ New material about noninvasive prenatal testing

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxiii

Chapter 3 ■■ Much revised coverage of co-sleeping ■■ Much revised coverage of breastfeeding ■■ New Spotlight on Research feature on infant reaching ■■ Much revised coverage of handedness

Chapter 4 ■■ Much revised description of young children’s naïve theories of biology ■■ New Spotlight on Research on preschool children’s essentialist thinking ■■ Much revised description of memory ■■ Much revised description of infants’ number skills ■■ Much revised coverage of infant-directed speech ■■ New coverage on the benefits of touchscreen devices for children’s word learning

Chapter 5 ■■ Much revised coverage of pretend play and solitary play ■■ Much revised coverage of father–infant relationships ■■ Much revised coverage of the impact of child care ■■ Much revised coverage of emotion regulation

Chapter 6 ■■ New Spotlight on Research feature on impaired reading comprehension ■■ Much revised coverage of ADHD ■■ New material on children’s mastery of conceptual and procedural knowledge of math

Chapter 7 ■■ New coverage of impact of quality of sibling relationships ■■ New coverage of open adoptions ■■ Much revised coverage of divorce ■■ Much revised coverage of maltreatment ■■ Much revised coverage of groups ■■ Much revised coverage of bullying ■■ Much revised coverage of electronic media, including new Spotlight on Research feature

Chapter 8 ■■ Much revised material on evaluating Kohlberg’s theory, including new material on

adolescents’ balancing of fairness with group loyalty ■■ Much revised coverage of analytic and heuristic solutions in problem-solving

Chapter 9 ■■ Revised coverage of adolescent storm and stress ■■ Much revised coverage of dating violence ■■ Much revised coverage of sexual minority youth ■■ New material on social cognitive career theory ■■ Much revised coverage of adolescent depression, including new Spotlight on Research

feature

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxiv

Chapter 10 ■■ Revised Emerging Adulthood section that now includes subsections on Neurosci-

ence, Behavior, and Emerging Adulthood; and Achieving Milestones: Education, Workforce, and Erikson’s Intimacy

■■ Expanded discussion of relation between educational attainment and employment ■■ Expanded discussion of quarter-life crisis, including Robinson’s reframing of

Erikson’s intimacy-isolation to commitment-independence and the addition of an emerging adult transition phase.

■■ Revised discussion on binge drinking, sexual assault, and alcohol use disorder. ■■ Revised nutrition discussion to reflect new dietary guidelines and work with Native

American tribes ■■ New discussions of emotional intelligence and impression formation

Chapter 11 ■■ Discussion of Social Baseline Theory to explain how the brain activity reveals how

people seek social relationships to mitigate risk ■■ New Real People feature on James Obergefell and John Arthur ■■ Inclusion of millennial generation lifestyles, including their likely much lower rates of

marriage and likelihood of being less well off than their parents ■■ Rewritten discussion of LGBTQ adults ■■ New What Do You Think? feature on paid family leave

Chapter 12 ■■ New chapter introduction focusing on the shift to the “gig economy” and its impact

on the meaning of work ■■ Differentiation of mentoring and coaching ■■ Mention of burnout effects on the brain ■■ Reduced redundancy in parenting and work–family conflict sections ■■ New Spotlight on Research feature on the long-term health effects of leisure activities ■■ New Real People feature on the politics of unemployment

Chapter 13 ■■ Revised discussion of treatments for arthritis ■■ Revised discussion of the effects of stress on physical health ■■ Addition of the TESSERA (Triggering situations, Expectancy, States/State Expres-

sions, and Reactions model in the discussion of personality traits

Chapter 14 ■■ Expanded discussion of international demographics of older adults ■■ Reorganized and revised section on biological theories of aging ■■ Revised discussion of the role of beta-amyloid protein in brain aging and as a bio-

marker of Alzheimer’s disease ■■ New Real People feature on the “Angelina Jolie effect” on breast cancer screening ■■ Revised discussion on divided attention ■■ Expanded discussion of neuroimaging research on creativity and aging ■■ New What Do You Think? feature on the question of whether creativity exists ■■ Revised discussions about genetics and dementia, and about the beta-amyloid cas-

cade hypothesis

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxv

Chapter 15 ■■ Revised discussion of healthy aging and connection with selective optimization with

compensation framework ■■ New discussion of the preventive and corrective proactivity model ■■ New Real People feature on Katherine Johnson ■■ Revised discussion of spirituality in later life ■■ Revised discussion on LGBT long-term relationships ■■ Expanded and revised discussion of frailty and disability in late life, especially related

to socioeconomic factors, and global issues ■■ Revised discussion of financial exploitation of older adults and the role of financial

institutions in preventing it

Chapter 16 ■■ Table with most frequent causes of death by age ■■ Discussion of updated brain death criteria and implementation issues ■■ New What Do You Think? featuring the Brittany Maynard case ■■ Discussion of death doulas ■■ New Real People feature with focus on Randy Pausch’s last lecture ■■ Discussion of the model of adaptive grieving dynamics ■■ Discussion of disenfranchised grief ■■ Added discussion of ambiguous grief

SpECiaL FEaturES

Three special features are a significant reason why this textbook is unique. These features are woven seamlessly into the narrative—not boxed off from the flow of the chapter. Each box appears in nearly every chapter. The three features are:

pEdagOgiCaL FEaturES Among the most important aspects of Human Development: A Life-Span View, Eighth Edition, is its exceptional integration of pedagogical features, designed to help students maximize their learning.

■■ Section-by-Section Pedagogy. Each major section of a chapter (every chapter has four or five) has been carefully crafted: It opens with a set of learning objectives, a vignette, typically includes one or more Think About It questions in the margin encouraging critical thinking, and ends with a set of questions called Test Yourself that reinforces key elements of the section. For easy assignment and to help readers visually organize the material, major units within each chapter are numbered.

■■ Chapter-by-Chapter Pedagogy. Each chapter opens with a table of contents and con- cludes with a bulleted, detailed Summary (broken down by learning objective within each major section), followed by a list of Key Terms (with page references).

In sum, we believe that our integrated pedagogical system will give the student all the tools she or he needs to comprehend the material and study for tests.

These features emphasize a fuller understanding of the science and scope of life-span development.

Spotlight on Research

What Do You think? These features ask students to think critically about social and developmental issues.

Real People Applying Human Development

These features illustrate the everyday applications of life-span development issues.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxvi

MindTap® for Human Development: A Life-Span View engages and empowers students to produce their best work—consistently. By seamlessly integrating course material with vid- eos, activities, apps, and much more, MindTap® creates a unique learning path that fosters increased comprehension and efficiency.

For students:

■■ MindTap® delivers real-world relevance with activities and assignments that help stu- dents build critical thinking and analytic skills that will transfer to other courses and their professional lives.

■■ MindTap® helps students stay organized and efficient with a single destination that reflects what’s important to the instructor, along with the tools students need to master the content.

■■ MindTap® empowers and motivates students with information that shows where they stand at all times—both individually and compared to the highest performers in class.

Additionally, for instructors, MindTap® allows you to:

■■ Control what content students see and when they see it with a learning path that can be used as-is or matched to your syllabus exactly.

■■ Create a unique learning path of relevant readings and multimedia and activities that move students up the learning taxonomy from basic knowledge and comprehension to analysis, application, and critical thinking.

■■ Integrate your own content into the MindTap® Reader using your own documents or pulling from sources such as RSS feeds, YouTube videos, websites, Googledocs, and more.

■■ Use powerful analytics and reports that provide a snapshot of class progress, time in course, engagement, and completion.

In addition to the benefits of the platform, MindTap® for Human Development: A Life-Span View includes:

■■ Formative assessments at the conclusion of each chapter. ■■ Interactive activities drawn from the What Do You Think? and Real People text fea-

tures that foster student participation through polls, photo shares, and discussion threads.

■■ Illustrative video embedded in the MindTap® Reader to highlight key concepts for the students.

■■ Investigate Development enables students to observe, evaluate, and make decisions about human development so they see the implications of research on a personal level. Students interact with simulated case studies of milestones in a person’s development, observing and analyzing audiovisual cues, consulting research, and making decisions. Instead of rote memorization of isolated concepts, Investigate Development compels students to think critically about research and brings human development to life.

Mindtap®

SuppLEMEntS FOr thE inStruCtOr

Online powerpoint® Slides These vibrant Microsoft® PowerPoint® lecture slides for each chapter assist you with your lecture by providing concept coverage using images, figures, and tables directly from the textbook.

Online instructor's Manual This detailed manual provides sample syllabi, course guidelines, in-class exercises, and chapter objectives to assist instructors in teaching the course.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxvii

Cengage Learning testing, powered by Cognero® instant access Cengage Learning Testing Powered by Cognero® is a flexible, online system that allows you to: import, edit, and manipulate content from the text's test bank or elsewhere, including your own favorite test questions; create multiple test versions in an instant; and deliver tests from your LMS, your classroom, or wherever you want.

aCknOwLEdgMEntS Textbook authors do not produce books on their own. We owe a debt of thanks to many people who helped take this project from a first draft to a bound book. Thanks to Jim Brace-Thompson, for his enthusiasm, good humor, and sage advice at the begin- ning of this project; to Nedah Rose for taking the reins and guiding the eighth edition; and to Andrew Ginsberg, Product Manager; Ruth Sakata-Corley, Content Production Manager; and Vernon Boes, Art Director, for their work in bringing this edition to life.

We would also like to thank the many reviewers who generously gave their time and effort to help us sharpen our thinking about human development and, in so doing, shape the development of this text.

past reviewers of Human Development: A Life-Span View

GARY L. ALLEN University of South Carolina POLLY APPLEFIELD University of North Carolina at Wilmington ANN M. B. AUSTIN Utah State University KENNETH E. BELL University of New Hampshire DANIEL R. BELLACK Trident Technical College MAIDA BERENBLATT Suffolk County Community College L. RENÉ BERGERON University of New Hampshire BELINDA BEVINS-KNABE University of Arkansas at Little Rock DAVID BISHOP Luther College ELIZABETH M. BLUNK Southwest Texas State University JOSETTE BONEWITZ Vincennes University JANINE P. BUCKNER Seton Hall University CYNTHIA B. CALHOUN Southwest Tennessee Community College LANTHAN D. CAMBLIN, JR. University of Cincinnati

PAUL ANDERER CASTILLO State University of New York, Canton LISA DAVIDSON Northern Illinois University CATHERINE DEERING Clayton College and State University CHARLES TIMOTHY DICKEL Creighton University JUDITH DIETERLE Daytona Beach Community College SHELLEY M. DRAZEN SUNY, Binghamton SANDY EGGERS University of Memphis KENNETH ELLIOTT University of Maine, Augusta MARTHA ELLIS Collin County Community College NOLEN EMBRY Lexington Community College WILLIAM FABRICIUS Arizona State University STEVE FINKS University of Tennessee LINDA FLICKINGER St. Clair County Community College DOUGLAS FRIEDRICH University of West Florida REBECCA GLOVER University of North Texas

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxviii

J. A. GREAVES Jefferson State Community College TRESMAINE R. GRIMES Iona College PATRICIA GUTH Westmoreland County Community College LANA-LEE HARDACRE Conestoga College JULIE A. HASELEU Kirkwood Community College PHYLLIS HEATH Central Michigan University MYRA HEINRICH Mesa State College BRETT HEINTZ Delgado Community College SANDRA HELLYER Indiana University–Purdue University at Indianapolis SHIRLEY-ANNE HENSCH University of Wisconsin Center THOMAS HESS North Carolina State University HEATHER M. HILL University of Texas, San Antonio SUSAN HORTON Mesa Community College ALYCIA M. HUND Illinois State University KATHLEEN HURLBURT University of Massachusetts–Lowell JENEFER HUSMAN University of Alabama KAREN IHNEN St. Cloud Technical and Community College HEIDI INDERBITZEN University of Nebraska at Lincoln ERWIN J. JANEK Henderson State University WAYNE JOOSE Calvin College RICHARD KANDUS Mt. San Jacinto College MARGARET D. KASIMATIS Carroll College MICHELLE L. KELLEY Old Dominion University JOHN KLEIN Castleton State College WENDY KLIEWER Virginia Commonwealth University

AMY LANDERS Old Dominion University KIRSTEN D. LINNEY University of Northern Iowa BLAKE TE-NEIL LLOYD University of South Carolina SANFORD LOPATER Christopher Newport University NANCY MACDONALD University of South Carolina, Sumter SUSAN MAGUN-JACKSON University of Memphis MARION G. MASON Bloomsburg University of Pennsylvania MICHAEL JASON MCCOY Cape Fear Community College LISA MCGUIRE Allegheny College JULIE ANN MCINTYRE Russell Sage College BILL MEREDITH University of Nebraska at Omaha EDWARD J. MORRIS Owensboro Community College MARTIN D. MURPHY University of Akron JANET D. MURRAY University of Central Florida MARY ANNE O’NEILL Rollins College Hamilton Holt School JOHN W. OTEY Southern Arkansas University SHANA PACK Western Kentucky University MARIBETH PALMER-KING Broome Community College ELLEN E. PASTORINO Valencia Community College IAN PAYTON Bethune-Cookman College JOHN PFISTER Dartmouth College BRADFORD PILLOW Northern Illinois University GARY POPOLI Hartford Community College ROBERT PORESKY Kansas State University JOSEPH M. PRICE San Diego State University HARVE RAWSON Franklin College

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxix

CATHERINE HACKETT RENNER West Chester University ROSEMARY ROSSER University of Arizona LISA ROUTH Pikes Peak Community College ROBERT F. RYCEK University of Nebraska at Kearney JEFF SANDOZ University of Louisiana at Lafayette BRIAN SCHRADER Emporia State University CAROLYN A. SHANTZ Wayne State University STACIE SHAW Presentation College TIMOTHY O. SHEARON Albertson College of Idaho CYNTHIA K. SHINABARGER REED Tarrant County College MARCIA SOMER University of Hawaii-Kapiolani Community College LINDA SPERRY Indiana State University TRACY L. SPINRAD Arizona State University CARRIE SWITZER University of Illinois, Springfield SUSAN D. TALLEY Utah State University

KELLI W. TAYLOR Virginia Commonwealth University LORRAINE C. TAYLOR University of South Carolina VIRGINIA TOMPKINS Ohio State University BARBARA TURNAGE University of Central Florida YOLANDA VAN ECKE Mission College ANNE WATSON West Virginia University CAROL G. WEATHERFORD Clemson University FRED A. WILSON Appalachian State University CAITLIN WILLIAMS San Jose State University NANCI STEWART WOODS Austin Peay State University SANDY WURTELE University of Colorado at Colorado Springs VIRGINIA WYLY State University of New York College at Buffalo KAREN YANOWITZ Arkansas State University CHRISTINE ZIEGLER Kennesaw State University

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxi

Human Development: A Life-Span View is written with you, the student, in mind. In the next few pages, we describe several features of the book that will make it easier for you to learn. Please don’t skip this material; it will save you time in the long run.

Learning and Study aids Each chapter includes several distinctive features to help you learn the material and orga- nize your studying.

■■ Each chapter opens with an overview of the main topics and a detailed outline. ■■ Each major section within a chapter begins with a set of learning objectives. There

is also a brief vignette introducing one of the topics to be covered in that section and providing an example of the developmental issues people face.

■■ When key terms are introduced in the text, they appear in bold, orange type and are defined in the margin. This should make key terms easy to find and learn.

■■ Key developmental theories are introduced in Chapter 1 and are referred to through- out the text.

■■ Critical thinking questions appear in the margins. These Think About It questions are designed to help you make connections across sections within a chapter or across chapters.

■■ The end of each section includes a feature called Test Yourself, which will help you check your knowledge of major ideas you just read about. The Test Yourself ques- tions serve two purposes. First, they give you a chance to spot-check your under- standing of the material. Second, the questions will relate the material you have just read to other facts, theories, or the biopsychosocial framework you read about earlier.

■■ Text features expand or highlight a specific topic. This book includes the following three features:

■■ Spotlight on Research elaborates a specific research study discussed in the text and provides more details on the design and methods used.

■■ What Do You Think? offers thought-provoking discussions about current issues affecting development.

■■ Real People: Applying Human Development is a case study that illustrates how an issue in human development discussed in the chapter is manifested in the life of a real person.

■■ The end of each chapter includes several special study tools. A Summary organized by learning objective within major section headings provides a review of the key ideas in the chapter. Next is a list of Key Terms that appear in the chapter.

We strongly encourage you to take advantage of these learning and study aids as you read the book. We have also left room in the margins for you to make notes to yourself on the material, so you can more easily integrate the text with your class and lecture material.

Your instructor will probably assign about one chapter per week. Don’t try to read an entire chapter in one sitting. Instead, on the first day, preview the chapter. Read the intro- duction and notice how the chapter fits into the entire book; then page through the chapter, reading the learning objectives, vignettes, and major headings. Also read the italicized sentences and the boldfaced terms. Your goal is to get a general overview of the entire chapter—a sense of what it’s all about.

To the Student

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxii

Now you’re ready to begin reading. Go to the first major section and preview it again, reminding yourself of the topics covered. Then start to read. As you read, think about what you’re reading. Every few paragraphs, stop briefly. Try to summarize the main ideas in your own words; ask yourself if the ideas describe your own experience or that of others you know; tell a friend about something interesting in the material. In other words, read actively—get involved in what you’re reading. Don’t just stare glassy-eyed at the page!

Continue this pattern—reading, summarizing, thinking—until you finish the section. Then answer the Test Yourself questions to determine how well you’ve learned what you’ve read. If you’ve followed the read-summarize-think cycle as you worked your way through the section, you should be able to answer most of the questions.

The next time you sit down to read (preferably the next day), start by reviewing the second major section. Then complete it with the read-summarize-think cycle. Repeat this procedure for all the major sections.

When you’ve finished the last major section, wait a day or two and then review each major section. Pay careful attention to the italicized sentences, the boldfaced terms, and the Test Yourself questions. Also, use the study aids at the end of the chapter to help you integrate the ideas in the chapters.

With this approach, it should take several 30- to 45-minute study sessions to com- plete each chapter. Don’t be tempted to rush through an entire chapter in a single session. Research consistently shows that you learn more effectively by having daily (or nearly daily) study sessions devoted to both reviewing familiar material and taking on a relatively small amount of new material.

terminology A few words about terminology before we embark. We use certain terms to refer to dif- ferent periods of the life span. Although you may already be familiar with the terms, we want to clarify how they will appear in this text. The following terms will refer to a specific range of ages:

Newborn: birth to 1 month Infant: 1 month to 1 year Toddler: 1 year to 2 years Preschooler: 2 years to 6 years School-age child: 6 years to 12 years Adolescent: 12 years to 20 years Young adult: 20 years to 40 years Middle-age adult: 40 years to 60 years Young-old adult: 60 years to 80 years Old-old adult: 80 years and beyond

Sometimes, for the sake of variety, we will use other terms that are less tied to specific ages, such as babies, youngsters, and older adults. However, you will be able to determine the specific ages from the context.

Organization Authors of textbooks on human development always face the problem of deciding how to organize the material into meaningful segments across the life span. This book is organized into four parts: Prenatal Development, Infancy, and Early Childhood; School-Age Children and Adolescents; Young and Middle Adulthood; and Late Adulthood. We believe this orga- nization achieves two major goals. First, it divides the life span in ways that relate to the divisions encountered in everyday life. Second, it enables us to provide a more complete account of adulthood than other books do.

Because some developmental issues pertain only to a specific point in the life span, some chapters are organized around specific ages. Overall, the text begins with conception

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxiii

and proceeds through childhood, adolescence, adulthood, and old age to death. But because some developmental processes unfold over longer periods of time, some of the chapters are organized around specific topics.

Part One covers prenatal development, infancy, and early childhood. Here we will see how genetic inheritance operates and how the prenatal environment affects a person’s future development. During the first two years of life, the rate of change in both motor and perceptual arenas is amazing. How young children acquire language and begin to think about their world is as intriguing as it is rapid. Early childhood also marks the emergence of social relationships, as well as an understanding of gender roles and identity. By the end of this period, a child is reasonably proficient as a thinker, uses language in sophisticated ways, and is ready for the major transition into formal education.

Part Two covers the years from elementary school through high school. In middle childhood and adolescence, the cognitive skills formed earlier in life evolve to adult-like levels in many areas. Family and peer relationships expand. During adolescence, there is increased attention to work, and sexuality emerges. The young person begins to learn how to face difficult issues in life. By the end of this period, a person is on the verge of legal adulthood. The typical individual uses logic and has been introduced to most of the issues that adults face.

Part Three covers young adulthood and middle age. During this period, most people achieve their most advanced modes of thinking, achieve peak physical performance, form intimate relationships, start families of their own, begin and advance within their occupa- tions, manage to balance many conflicting roles, and begin to confront aging. Over these years, many people go from breaking away from their families to having their children break away from them. Relationships with parents are redefined, and the pressures of being caught between the younger and older generations are felt. By the end of this period, most people have shifted focus from time since birth to time until death.

Part Four covers the last decades of life. The biological, physical, cognitive, and social changes associated with aging become apparent. Although many changes reflect decline, many other aspects of old age represent positive elements: wisdom, retirement, friend- ships, and family relationships. We conclude this section, and the text, with a discussion of the end of life. Through our consideration of death, we will gain additional insights into the meaning of life and human development.

We hope the organization and learning features of the text are helpful to you—making it easier for you to learn about human development. After all, this book tells the story of people’s lives. Understanding the story is what it’s all about.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxv

Neuroscience Index

Note: Page numbers in italics indicate figures, tables, or illustrations.

adolescent brain, growth of, 265, 266 aging

brain efficiency and, 437 cellular theories of, 466–467 of emotional brain, 510–511, 511 metabolic theories of, 466 physiological changes of, 468–475 programming theories of, 467

alcohol use, effect on brain, 329 Alzheimer’s disease, 492–495 attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD), 208–209 autism spectrum disorders (ASD),

112–113 autosomes

defined, 40 Down syndrome and, 44

beta-amyloid, 468–469, 469 beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis, 495 beta-blockers, 491 biological forces

in biopsychosocial framework, 7, 8 death and, 575 defined, 7 human development created by,

summary of, 21 low birth weight and, 72 mechanics of intelligence and, 437 neuroscience and, 11

biological theories of aging, 466–467 cellular, 466–467 genetic programming, 467 metabolic, 466

brain activity amygdala and, 395, 509–510, 511,

543, 552 death anxiety and, 551–554 imaging techniques and, 10, 25 injuries and, 11 left rostral anterior cingulate cortex

and, 552 meditation and, 513 physiological measures of, 24–25 right caudate nucleus and, 552 spiritual practices and, 513 study of (See neuroscience) temperament, 85–86

brain–behavior relationships, 10, 25, 469–470

brain circuitry experience-dependent growth

and, 95 face-recognition skills and,

107–108 love and, 362

brain development in adolescence, 265, 266 malnourishment and, 90–91, 91 in period of the fetus, 55 in prenatal development, 92–93 risky behavior and, 275, 320, 328 of specialized brain, 93–95

brain efficiency, aging and loss of, 437 brain functions

autism and, 112 close friendships and, 357 exercise and, 435 mapping, cognitive impairment

and, 511 brain imaging research, 339,

339–340, 437 brain maturation

in adolescence, 94, 265, 266 in adulthood, 94 in infants, 8, 91–95 innovative thinking during adulthood

and, 486 prenatal, 8, 91–93 See also brain specialization

brain specialization experience-dependent growth

and, 95 experience-expectant growth and,

94–95 general principles of, 93–94 growth of, in infants, 93–95 in period of embryo, 53–54 in period of fetus, 55

cerebral cortex, 55, 91 cerebral vascular accidents (CVAs), 470 cognitive decline, 469, 496 cognitive development

academic skills and, 209–217 accommodation and, 119 in adolescence, 276–284 aptitudes for school and, 194–203

(See also intelligence) assimilation and, 119 concrete operational period and, 120 content knowledge and, 277 equilibration and, 119, 120 formal operational period and, 120 guided participation and, 136–137 in infancy and early childhood,

118–130 intelligence and, 334–340, 436–439 intersubjectivity and, 136 lifelong learning and, 439–440 memory strategies and, 277 mental abilities and, 335–337,

337, 338 metacognitive skill and, 277 in middle adulthood, 334–345,

436–440 in middle childhood, 190–217 moral issues and, 279–284 Piaget’s theory of, 118–130, 190–192 preoperational period and, 120,

122–125 principles of, 118–120 problem-solving and, 277–284 processing speed and, 276–277 reasoning and, 277–284 sensorimotor period and, 120,

120–122 special needs of children and,

204–209

Vygotsky’s theory of social development and, 136–138

working memory and, 276–277 in young and middle adulthood,

334–345 See also cognitive-developmental

theory; information processing; memory

cognitive-developmental theory, 14–17 information-processing theory, 15, 16 Piaget’s theory of, 15, 15–16 (See

also Piaget’s theory of cognitive development)

summarized, 21 Vygotsky’s theory of, 16–17 (See

also Vygotsky’s theory of social development)

cognitive processes, in late adulthood, 479–487

creativity, 485, 485–486 information processing, 479–481 memory, 481–484 wisdom, 486, 487

cognitive reappraisal task, 510, 510 cognitive self-regulation, 193 corpus callosum, 92

death anxiety, 551–554 dealing with, 553–554 defined, 551 terror management theory and, 552,

552–553 dementia, 491–497 dendrite, 91

electroencephalography, 93 emotion-focused coping, 433 emotion-focused research, 509–510

fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD), 60

fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), 60, 60 frontal cortex, 92 functional magnetic resonance imaging

(fMRI) of aging emotional brain, 510–511, 511 defined, 93

functional neuroimaging, 470

hemispheres defined, 92, 92 left, 92, 92, 94, 95, 139, 141, 148, 362 right, 92, 92, 94, 196

heterocyclic antidepressants (HCAs), 490 hippocampus, 95, 132, 433, 483, 486 hormones

after pregnancy, 69 breast-feeding and, 70 diethylstilbestrol and, 64 in fetal medicine, 66 implantation and, 53 in period of the fetus, 55 postpartum depression and, 69, 70 during pregnancy, 48, 58 stress and, 48, 58

hypoxia, 71

information processing in adolescence, 276–279 attention and, 131 content knowledge and, 277 dividing attention among multiple

tasks and, 481 general principles of, 130–131 in infancy and early childhood,

130–136 in late adulthood, 479–481 learning and, 131–132 memory strategies and, 132–134,

192, 277 metacognition and, 193–194,

194, 277 number skills and, learning,

134–136, 135 problem-solving and, 277–278 reasoning and, 277–278 in school-aged children, 192–194 speed of, 276, 276–277, 480 summarized, 279 working memory and, 276–277

information-processing theory cognitive-developmental theory and,

15, 16 defined, 16 summarized, 21

intelligence auditory organization and, 338 bodily-kinesthetic, 196, 196 categories of intellectual skill, 195,

195–196 crystallized, 195, 196, 337, 338,

339, 339 environmental factors and, 200 ethnicity and, 201–203 expertise and, 438–439 fluid, 195, 196, 337, 338, 339, 339 general, 195, 195–196 genetic factors and, 201 heredity and, 45, 48, 200 interpersonal, 196, 196, 197 intrapersonal, 196, 196 linguistic, 196, 196, 197 logical-mathematical, 196, 196, 197 long-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 mechanics of, 437–438, 438 mental abilities and, 335–337, 337 in middle adulthood, 334–340,

436–439 in middle childhood, 194–203 musical, 196, 196, 197 naturalistic, 196, 196 neuroscience research and, 339–340 P-FIT and, 339–340 practical, 436–437 pragmatics of, 437–438, 438 short-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 socioeconomic factors and, 201–203 spatial, 196, 196, 197 stability of, from 11 to age 90

years, 29 theories of (See intelligence theories)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxvi

intelligence (continued) views of, 334–336 visual organization and, 338 in young and middle adulthood,

334–340 intelligence tests, 198–200 intelligence theories, 195–198

Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences, 196, 196–197

hierarchical, 195, 195–196 psychometricians and, 195 Sternberg’s theory of successful

intelligence, 197–198

language exposure, impact of, 139–140 growth, encouraging, 146

language development, 139–151 communicating with others, 149–150 first words and, 142–146 milestones of, 151 speaking in sentences, 146–148 speech and, 139–142

left rostral anterior cingulate cortex, 552 limbic system, 266

magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 470

magnetoencephalography, 470 malnourished

defined, 90 growth and, 90–91, 91

memory abnormal changes in, 483 of adolescence, 276–277 aids, 484 autobiographical, 133, 347, 483 in early childhood, 132–134 E-I-E-I-O framework, 484, 484 explicit, 482 gender-related differences in, 179 implicit, 482 in late adulthood, 481–484 long-term acquisition and

retrieval, 338 phonological, 145

preschoolers as eyewitnesses and, 133–134

problems, remediating, 484 short-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 working, 481–482 in young and middle adulthood,

338, 347 midbrain, 495 monoamine oxidase (MAO)

inhibitors, 490 motor skills

assessing, 81 defined, 96 fine, 100–102 gender differences in, 220 in infants, 96–102 locomotion, 96–99 in middle childhood, 219, 219–221 physical fitness and, 220 sports participation and, 220–221

multichannel encephalography, 470 myelin, 92

near infrared spectroscopic imaging (NIRSI), 470

nervous system, of infants, 91–95 brain structures, 92, 92–93 specialized brain, growth of, 93–95

neural efficiency hypothesis, 340 neural network connections, 483 neural plate, 92 neuritic plaques, 468, 493, 495 neurofibrillary tangles, 468 neuroimaging

brain–behavior relations and, 25, 469–470

functional, 470 neuroscience and intelligence and,

339, 339–340 response to threat and, 357 structural, 470

neurons beta-amyloids, 468–469, 469 changes in, age-related, 468–470 defined, 91

neuroimaging, 469–470 neurotransmitters, 469 structure of, 468

neuropsychological tests, 471–472, 483

neuroscience defined, 10–11 love relationships and, 362–363

neuroscience research, 339–340 on binge drinking, 329 emotion-focused, 509–510 neural efficiency hypothesis, 340 P-FIT, 339–340

neurostimulator, 496 neurotransmitters, 91, 469

occipital cortex, 92

parietal cortex, 92, 94, 339 parieto-frontal integration theory

(P-FIT), 339–340 Parkinson’s disease, 495, 497 perception, 102–109

of color, 104, 105 defined, 103 of depth, 105–106 of faces, 107 hearing, 103–104 integrating sensory information,

108–109 of objects, 106–107, 107, 108 seeing, 104–108 smell, 103 of speech, 139–141 taste, 103, 103 touch, 103, 103

persistent vegetative state, 544 Piaget’s theory of cognitive

development, 15, 15–16, 118–130, 190–192

criticisms of, 125–126, 192 evaluating, 125–126 guidelines for fostering cognitive

development, 125 naïve theories of children and,

126–130

stages of, 15, 15, 190–191, 192 summarized, 21 See also cognitive development

positron emission tomography (PET), 470

prefrontal cortex, 132, 320, 339, 343, 395, 483, 509–510, 511

psychomotor speed, 510

reflexes defined, 80 evaluation of, 81–82 major, overview of, 81 of newborns, 80–82, 81

right caudate nucleus, 552

selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), 490

sensory information, integrating, 108–109

single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT), 470

stem cell research, 33, 468 strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents

(CVAs), 470 structural neuroimaging, 470 synaptic pruning, 93

transient ischemic attacks (TIAs), 471

traumatic brain injury (TBI), 496

vascular dementia, 471 Vygotsky’s theory of social

development, 16–17, 136–138

private speech and, 138 scaffolding and, 137–138 summarized, 21 zone of proximal development

and, 137

whole-brain death, 544 working memory, 192, 482

X-rays, 44, 62, 470

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxvii

Diversity Index

Note: Page numbers in italics indicate figures, tables, or illustrations.

abusive relationships, 300–301, 363–364, 364

academic skills, 209–217 active euthanasia, 545 adopted children, 46–47, 236–237 adoptive parenting, 377–378 Afghanistan, 72–73, 73 Africa

AIDS in, 563 breastfeeding in, 89 co-sleeping in, 83 divorce in, 379, 380 ethnic identity in, 293 grief reactions in, 563 longevity in, 463 malnutrition in, 90 romantic attachment in, 361 women’s careers in, 400

African Americans ADHD and, 208–209 altruism and, 175 Alzheimer’s disease and, 495 cardiovascular diseases and, 470 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 366–367 depression and, 489 division of household labor and, 410 divorce and, 379 educational attainment and, 321 ethnic identity and, 293, 294 face-recognition skills and, 107–108 five-factor trait model and, 442 glass ceiling and, 400 grandparenthood and, 451 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 hospice and, 556 intelligence tests and, 201 job loss and, effects of, 407 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 leading cause of death among young

adults in, 323 longevity and, 463 marriage and, in late life, 521 menopause and, 428 obesity rates and, 332 occupational choice and, 397 occupational development and, 399 parenting and, 375, 376 prejudice and, 257 racial discrimination leading to

depression in, 309–310, 310 remarriage and, 383 reproductive changes and, age-

related, 428 retirement and, 515, 516 self-esteem and, 295 sibling relations and, 236 singlehood and, 366 as single parents, 376 social support and, in late life, 519

spirituality and, 512 stereotype threat and, 203 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 teenage pregnancy and, 299 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 violence in relationships and, 363 widowhood and, 523 women in labor force, 399

age discrimination, 403 age of viability, 55, 56, 56 aging

brain efficiency and, 437 cellular theories of, 466–467 demographics of, 458–461, 460 of emotional brain, 510–511, 511 goal of healthy, 502–503 job loss and, effects of, 406 metabolic theories of, 466 parents, 448–450, 450 physiological changes of, 468–475 programming theories of, 467

American Indians. See Native Americans

Angola, 73 Apache menarche celebration, 267, 268 Arab/Islamic/Muslim cultures

anxiety disorders in, 491 brain death and, 544 five-factor trait model and, 442 honor killings in, 364 Iran, 111, 112, 360, 369 longevity and, 4 marital success in, 369 matchmaking and, 362 romantic attachment and, 361 social welfare services and, 545 sociocultural forces and, 8 storm and stress in, 296 women who remain single in, 366

Argentina, 367 Asia

adoption in, 378 breastfeeding in, 89 career plateauing in, 404 children’s temperament in, 86 co-sleeping in, 83 death anxiety in, 553 death in, reactions to, 543 dependent care in, work and, 410 diversity of older adults in, 460, 460 divorce in, 379, 380 East Asia, 361, 361 emotional expression in, 166 face-recognition skills in, 108 Hong Kong, 403 India, 90, 169, 360, 361–362 infant mortality in, 73 job satisfaction in, 394 Korea, 378, 410, 460, 464 language in, 142 longevity in, 463, 464 make-believe in, 169

Malaysia, 366, 440 malnutrition in, 90 mate selection in, 360, 361–362 math skills in, 214, 215, 215 Papua New Guinea, 543 physical punishment in, 232, 240 popular children in, 247 role transitions in, 319 romantic attachment in, 361, 361 scaffolding in, 137 sexual harassment in, 402 singlehood in, 366 Sri Lanka, 378 students in, compared to US

students, 166 Taiwan, 214, 394, 553 See also China; Japan

Asian Americans autobiographical memory and, 133 career preferences of, 304 depression and, 489 divorce and, 379 educational attainment and, 321 emotions expressed by, 166 ethnic identity and, 293, 294, 296 face-recognition skills and, 107–108 filial obligation and, 448 generativity and, 445 glass ceiling and, 400 grandparenthood and, 452 intelligence tests and, 201 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 make-believe and, 169 remarriage and, 383 romantic relationships and, 297 speech and, 142 stress and parenthood and, 375 terms for describing, 9

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 208–209

Australia career plateauing in, 404 child-free couples in, 373 cohabitation in, 366–367 divorce in, 380 infant mortality in, 73 infant mortality rate in, 73 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 online dating in, 359 online friendships in, 357 well-being in, 509

Austria, 215, 240, 373, 464 autobiographical memory, 133, 347,

483, 491

Bangladesh, 90 Belgium

divorce in, 380 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 physician-assisted suicide in, 546 storm and stress in, 296

Bhutan, 73 bilingualism, 145 biopsychosocial framework, 7–10, 8 biracial children, 294, 375–376 blended family, 239 body image, 267 Brazil, 7, 198, 375 Buddhism/Buddhists, 4, 166, 512 Burundi, 73

Cambodia, 73 Cameroon, 445 Canada

burnout in, 396 career plateauing in, 404 cohabitation in, 366–367 division of household labor in, 411 infant mortality rate in, 73 job satisfaction in, 394 math skills in, 215 popular children in, 247

caregivers/caregiving of aging parents, 448–450 alternative, 161–162 employed, 408–409 grandparents caring for

grandchildren, 452 for partner, in late adulthood, 523

Caribbean countries, 366 Catholics, 4, 319, 544 Central America, 428, 563 childbirth, 68–73, 73 child custody after divorce, 381–382 child-free couples, 371, 373 Chile, 215 China

adoption in, 236, 377–378 adults caring for aging parents in, 449 couple-forming behaviors in, 360 depression among older adults in, 488 diversity of older adults in, 460 divorce rates in, 380 intimate partner violence in, 364 language in, 142 leisure activities in, 415 only children in, 238 parenting styles in, 230 popular children in, 247 quality-of-life outcomes in oldest-

old, 506 theory of mind in, 111, 112 work-family conflict in, 412

Chinese Americans. See Asian Americans

cohabitation, 366–367 college enrollment, by income, 321, 321 Colombia, 360, 546 Costa Rica, 232, 380, 445 counting skills, learning, 135 Croatia, 240 cross-cultural comparisons

adults caring for aging parents, 449–450

appearance changes, age-related, 423

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxviii

cross-cultural comparisons (continued) attachment to place, 528 cohabitation, 366–367 coordinating skills, 98–99 couple-forming behaviors,

360–361, 361 death and dying, 543–544 death anxiety, 553 dependent care and work, 410 elder abuse, 532–533 euthanasia, 545 experience-dependent growth, 95 five-factor trait model, 442 frail older adults, 31, 527, 32526–527 friendships in adulthood, 358 gender differences in occupational

selection, 397, 398 generativity, 454–456 grandparents raising

grandchildren, 452 grief reactions, 563 job satisfaction, 394–395 labor force participation rate,

397, 398 leisure activities, 416 marital success, 368, 368–369 of marital success, 368–369 math skills, 214, 215, 215 meta-analysis in, 31, 32 motor skills development, 99 parental styles, 230–231 personal control beliefs, 349 primary mental abilities, 336,

336–337, 337 reflective judgment, 341 role transitions, 319–320 sequential design in, 30 on stability of intelligence from 11 to

age 90 years, 29, 29 of violence in relationships, 364 well-being, 509

Cuba, 380 cultural context

bioethics and, 545 child abuse and, 240 intelligence and, 7, 198, 203 life story and, 347 in TESSERA framework of adult

personality development, 444 thinking and, 16

culture of aging in America, 531 anger and, expression of, 166 attachment to place and, 528 bioethics and, 545 burnout and, 412 chastity and, 360 child maltreatment and, 240, 241 cognitive development and, 117, 126 co-sleeping and, 83 couple-forming behaviors and, 360 crying and, 82 death and, children’s understanding

of, 570 death and, definition of, 543 death and, feelings about, 549 death anxiety and, 553 dependent care and, 410 desirable mates and, characteristics

of, 360 division of household labor and, 410 of drinking in college, 328 dying process and, 555 eating disorders and, 272 elder abuse and, 532–533 electronic media and, 249

emotional expression and, 166 ethnic identity and, 293–294 euthanasia and, 545 experience-dependent growth

and, 95 five-factor trait model and, 442 generativity and, 445 grief and, 543, 561, 563, 564, 567 hook-ups and, 360 housing arrangements and, 531 IADLs and, 526 identity and, 347 integrity versus despair and, 509 intelligence and, 337, 337, 338 job satisfaction and, 394 labels and, 9 language and, 141, 142, 145 left-handedness and, 102 leisure and, 416 low birth weight and, 72 make-believe and, 169 marriage and, 319, 369 menarche and, 267–268, 268 mentoring and coaching model

and, 394 moral reasoning and, 282, 283 motor development and, 98–99 mourning rituals and, 543 music and, early sensitivity to, 104 occupational development and, 392 occupational selection and, 391, 397 parenting styles and, 230–231 personal control beliefs and, 348, 349 physician-assisted suicide and, 546 Piaget’s theory and, 126 popular children and, 247 remarriage and, 383 rites of passage and, 324 role transitions and, 319–320 romantic relationships and, 297,

360–362, 361 same-sex friends and, 358 self-concept and, 110 sibling relations and, 235 socialization and, 227, 228 sociocultural forces and, 7, 8–9 stress of parenthood and, 375 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 in systems view of family, 229 temperament and, 86 theory of mind and, 111–112 third-person perspective and, 255 violence in relationships and, 364 Vygotsky’s theory and, 117, 136–138 well-being in late adulthood and, 509

Czech Republic, 73, 73, 215, 380, 464

death and dying adolescent’s experience with,

571–572 of child in young adulthood, 573 of child or grandchild in late

adulthood, 574 children’s understanding of, 570–571 contextual theory of, 551 dealing with one’s own, 550–551 death anxiety and, 551–554 end-of-life issues, 554–560 ethical issues in, 544–548 euthanasia and, 545–546 Kübler-Ross’s stages of, 551 legal and medical definitions of, 544 life-course approach to, 549–550 life-sustaining care and, price of,

547–548

older adult’s feelings toward, 574–575 of parent, 573 of partner in late adulthood, 574–575 of partner in young adulthood,

572–573 physician-assisted suicide and,

546–547 sociocultural definitions of, 543–544 thinking about, 548–554 trajectories of, 550, 550–551 young adult’s feelings toward,

572–573 See also grief

Denmark, 215, 464 disabilities

children with, 204–207 chronic disease and, 527 global differences in, 528 intellectual, 205 learning, 205–206 in middle childhood, 204–207 over time, in frail older adults,

526–527, 527 discrimination

age, 403 gender, 400–401 racial, leading to depression,

309–310, 310 in workplace, 400–402

diversity of older adults, 460, 460 divorce, 238–239

blended families and, 239 children and, effects on, 238–239,

381–382 couples and, effects on, 381 divorce hangover, 381 rates, 379, 380 reasons for, 379–381 relationships with adult children and,

effects on, 382 remarriage and, 239 stress related to, 238, 382 in young and middle adulthood,

379–382 Dutch, 210, 378, 382 Dutch Asian American, 293, 296

East Asia, 142, 166, 361, 361 Eastern Europe, 361, 545 Egypt, 360, 380, 544 end-of-life issues, 554–560 Estonia, 215, 464 ethnic identity, 293–294 ethnicity

depression and, 489 diabetes mellitus and, 475 educational attainment and, 321 equal pay for equal work and, 401, 401 face-recognition skills and, 108 generativity and, 445 grandparenthood and, 451–452 grief and, 563 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 health issues and, 332–333 health-related quality of life and, 528 intelligence and, 201–203 job loss and, effects of, 407 labor force participation and,

399–400 longevity and, 463 multiethnic children and, raising, 375 nursing home placement and, 531 nutrition and, 330 occupational selection and

development and, 399–400 parenting and, 375–376

retirement and, 514, 515–516 romantic relationships and, 297 sexual minority youth and, 300 sociocultural forces and, 7, 8–9 socioeconomic status and, 528

Europe anti-discrimination legislation in, 403 autobiographical memory in, 133 bullying in, 248 children’s temperament in, 86 cohabitation in, 366–367 co-sleeping in, 83 divorce in, 73, 73 effortful control in, 86 emotional expression in, 166 euthanasia in, 545 face-recognition skills in, 108 generativity in, 445 grandparents in, 451 make-believe in, 170 motor development in, 98 physical punishment in, 240 popular children in, 247 romantic attachment in, 361 scaffolding in, 137 temperament in, 86

European Americans ADHD and, 208–209 age-related macular degeneration

and, 473 altruism and, 175 autobiographical memory and, 133 average longevity and, 463 binge drinking and, 328 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 366–367 in developmental research, 49 diversity of older adults and, 460 division of household labor and, 410 educational attainment and, 321 emotions expressed by, 166 face-recognition skills and, 107 filial obligation and, 448 gender-related properties and,

reasoning about, 178 generativity and, 445 grandparenthood and, 451 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 intelligence tests and, 201 job loss and, effects of, 407 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 leading cause of death among young

adults in, 323 make-believe and, 169 marriage and, in late life, 521 menopause and, 428 parenting and, 375, 376 part-time employment and, 305 prejudice and, 257 prosocial behavior of, 175 remarriage and, 382–383 reproductive changes and,

age-related, 428 self-esteem and, 295 singlehood and, 366 as single parents, 376 spousal caregivers and, 523 stress of parenthood and, 375 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 widowhood and, 523, 575

face-recognition skills, 107–108 false beliefs, growing understanding

of, 112

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xxxix

families aging parents and, 448–450 blended, 239 child maltreatment and, 239–242 dependent care and, 409–410 diverse forms of, 376–378 divorce and, 238–239, 379–382 employed caregivers and, 408–409 empty nest and, 447–448 grandparenthood and, 450–452 great-grandparenthood and, 524–525 in late adulthood, 520–525 in middle adulthood, 372–383,

447–452 in middle childhood, 228–242 multiple roles in, 410–413, 411 parental roles in, 372–376 parenting styles and, 229–235 remarriage and, 382–383, 383 siblings and, 235–238, 520 step-, foster-, adoptive, and same-sex

couple parenting and, 377–378 systems view of, 228–229 work and, 408–413 in young and middle adulthood,

372–383 familism, 375 filial obligation, 448 Filipinos, 394, 519 Finland

child-free couples in, 373 couple-forming behaviors in, 360 division of household labor in, 411 infant mortality in, 73 leisure in, 414, 415 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 reading skills in, 210

foster-parenting, 377–378 France

burnout in, 396 diversity of older adults in, 460 division of household labor in, 411 divorce in, 380 infant mortality rate in, 73 intelligence testing developed in, 198 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215

friendships ABCDE model of, 356 benefits of, 244–245, 357 consequences of, 245 defined, 243 friends in, 244 in late adulthood, 519–520 men’s, women’s, and cross-sex, 358 in middle childhood, 243–245 online, 357 sibling, 357–358 themes characterizing, 357 in young and middle adulthood,

356–358

gay and lesbian partnerships. See same-sex partnerships

gender-related differences, 179–180 in aggression, 179 in anger, 180 eating disorders and, 272 in effortful control, 180 in emotions, 180 gender stereotypes and, 179–180 grief and, expression of, 563 grief and, risk factors in, 562 in guilt, 180 in happiness, 180

in labor force participation rate, 397, 398

in longevity, 463 in mathematics, 179 in memory, 179 in motor skills of middle

childhood, 220 in occupational selection and

development, 399–400 research on, 179–180 in retirement, 515–516 in social influences, 179 in spatial ability, 179, 180 in verbal ability, 179 in workplace, 400–402

gender roles, 176, 184 biological influences on, 183 evolving, 184 social, 176

gender-schema theory, 182, 182–183

gender stereotypes, 177–180 defined, 177 gender-related differences and,

179–180 learning, 177–178 research on, 178, 178

gender typing, 180–183 generativity, 444–445, 445 Germany

adults without children in, 373 couples without children in, 373 divorce in, 380 generativity in, 445 infant mortality rate in, 73 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 parental leave from work in, 409 physical punishment in, 240 reading skills in, 210 thalidomide and, 59 well-being in, 509

Ghana, 543 glass ceiling, 400 glass cliff, 400 grandchildren, 450–452, 574 grandparenthood, 450–452 Great Britain, 360, 373, 414, 415 great-grandparenthood, 524–525 Greece

couple-forming behaviors in, 360 divorce in, 379, 380 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 nutrition in, 330, 330 reading skills in, 210

grief adolescent’s expression of, 572 ambiguous loss and, 566–567 anticipatory, 563 children’s expression of,

570–571, 571 complicated or prolonged, 567 coping with, 564–567 culture and, 543 defined, 561 disenfranchised, 568–569 dual process model and, 565,

565–566 four-component model and,

564–565 holding in, costs of, 568 model of adaptive grieving dynamics

and, 566, 566 older adult’s expression of, 574–575 process, 561–563

reactions, 563–564 risk factors in, 562–563 themes, 563 young adult’s expression of, 572–573

Haiti, 543 Hawaiian island of Kauai, 72 health

in adolescence, 270–275 biological forces and, 7, 8 chronic disease and, 475–476 education issues in, 332–333 ethnic issues in, 332–333 exercise and, 434–436, 435 healthcare disparities in US, 333, 333 in late adulthood, 475–478 lifestyle factors in, 325–333, 476, 477 in middle adulthood, 325–333,

430–436 physical fitness and, 274–275, 275 socioeconomic issues in, 332–333 stress and, 430–434 in young and middle adulthood,

325–333 See also well-being

Hindus, 4, 282 Hong Kong, 403 household chores, 410, 411 Hungary, 215, 464

Iceland, 73, 215, 464 identity

in adolescence, 290–296 ethnic, 293–294 gender, 182, 183 peers and, 293 phases or statuses of, 291, 291 self-esteem and, 294–295 sexual minority youth, 300 thinking and, characteristic of,

291–292, 292 India

couple-forming behaviors in, 360 diversity of older adults in, 460 family loyalty in, 361–362 make-believe in, 169 malnutrition in, 90 role transitions in, 319

Indonesia, 360 infants

brain structures of, 92, 92–93 growth of, 87–91, 88, 89 mortality rate, 72–73, 73 motor skills of, 96–102 nervous system of, 91–95 nutrition in, 89–90 specialized brain growth in, 93–95 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 intelligence

auditory organization and, 338 bodily-kinesthetic, 196, 196 categories of intellectual skill, 195,

195–196 crystallized, 195, 196, 337, 338,

339, 339 environmental factors and, 200 ethnicity and, 201–203 expertise and, 438–439 fluid, 195, 196, 337, 338, 339, 339 general, 195, 195–196 genetic factors and, 201 heredity and, 45, 48, 200 intelligence tests, 198–200 intelligence theories, 195–198 interpersonal, 196, 196, 197

intrapersonal, 196, 196 linguistic, 196, 196, 197 logical-mathematical, 196, 196, 197 long-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 mechanics of, 437–438, 438 mental abilities and, 335–337, 337 in middle adulthood, 334–340,

436–439 in middle childhood, 194–203 musical, 196, 196, 197 naturalistic, 196, 196 neuroscience research and, 339–340 P-FIT and, 339–340 practical, 436–437 pragmatics of, 437–438, 438 short-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 socioeconomic factors and,

201–203 spatial, 196, 196, 197 stability of, from 11 to age 90 years,

29, 29 views of, 334–336 visual organization and, 338 in young and middle adulthood,

334–340 international differences

in abuse rates, 364 in average longevity, 463 in binge drinking, 327 in burnout, 412 in child custody after divorce,

381–382 in child-free couples, 373–374 in cohabitation, 366–367 in couple-forming behaviors,

360–361, 361 in diversity of older adults, 460,

460–461 in infant mortality rates, 73, 73 in marital success, 368–369 in math skills, 214–215, 215 in parental leave from work, 410 in romantic attachment,

360–361, 361 in sexual harassment, 402

Iran, 111, 112, 360, 369 Ireland, 73, 215, 373, 464 Israel, 73, 240, 247, 379, 380, 464 Italy, 73, 210, 215, 380, 411, 464

Jamaica, 99 Japan

ambiguous loss in, after tsunami, 566

child-free couples in, 373 couples without children in, 373 diversity of older adults in, 460 division of household labor in, 411 divorce in, 379, 380 final scenario in, 555 infant mortality in, 73, 73 infant mortality rate in, 73 leisure in, 414, 415 longevity in, 463 math skills in, 215, 215 middle-aged adults caring for aging

parents in, 448 physician-assisted suicide in, 546 work–family conflict in, 412

Jews, 4, 282, 543 Judaism, 319

Kauai, 72 Kenya, 99, 452

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xl

Korea adoption in, 377–378 dependent care in, work and, 410 depression in, 294 diversity of older adults in, 460 ethnic identity in, 294, 296 job satisfaction in, 394 language in, 142 make-believe in, 169 math skills in, 215

Korean Americans. See Asian Americans

labels, cultural, 9 language

development, 139–151 exposure, impact of, 139–140 growth, encouraging, 146

Latin America death of grandchild in, 574 divorce in, 380 familism in, 375 gender discrimination in, 400 grief reactions, 563 intelligence in, 198 math skills in, 7 nutrition in, 89 role transitions in, 319 scaffolding in, 137

Latino Americans ADHD and, 208–209 altruism and, 175 average longevity in, 463 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 366–367 depression and, 489 division of household labor and, 410 educational attainment and, 321 ethnic identity and, 293 grandparenthood and, 451 grief and, 563 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 intelligence tests and, 201 job loss and, effects of, 407 labor force participation and, 398 leading cause of death among young

adults in, 323 marriage and, in late life, 521 menopause and, 428 obesity rates and, 332 parenting and, 375 prejudice and, 257 prosocial behavior of, 175 reproductive changes and, age-

related, 428 retirement and, 516 romantic relationships and, 297 self-esteem and, 295 singlehood and, 366 social support and, in late life, 519 spirituality and, 513 teenage pregnancy and, 299 teen suicide and, 310 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 violence in relationships and, 363

learning disability, 205–207 lesbian and gay couples. See same-sex

partnerships LGB partnerships, 522 LGBTQ relationships, 367–368 life expectancy. See longevity life story, 346–347 lifestyle, 325–333, 365–372, 476, 477 longevity, 461–464

active life expectancy and, 462

average, 461–463, 462, 464 dependent life expectancy and, 462 environmental factors in, 462–463 ethnic differences in, 463 gender differences in, 463 genetic factors in, 462 international differences in,

463, 464 maximum, 461 in US, 1900-2014, 462

longitudinal studies on cohabitation, 367 defined, 28 on disability in older adults, causes

of, 527 five-factor trait model and, 441,

442–443 on frail older adults, 31 on friendships, 357 on gifted children, 204 on grief reactions, 564, 568 on intellectual change, 336–337, 337 Kohlberg’s theory of moral

development and, 282 on leisure activities, changes in,

414–415 on link between maternal depression

and children’s behavior problems, 70

on marital dissatisfaction, 371 meta-analysis in, 31, 32 on occupational attainment, 391 on personal control beliefs, 349 on primary mental abilities, 336,

336–337, 337 on quality-of-life outcomes in the

oldest-old, 506 on racial discrimination leading to

depression, 309–310, 310 on reflective judgment, 341 in research design, 28–30 Seattle Longitudinal Study,

336–337, 337 sequential design in, 30 on software designed for middle-aged

and older nonexperts, 440 on stability of intelligence from 11 to

age 90 years, 29, 29 summarized, 31 on supportive environment for low

birth weight babies, 72 on temperament, 85 on training babies to grasp, 101, 101 on training persons with dementia to

be group activity leaders, 496 on word structure and reading

comprehension, 206–207, 207 Luxembourg, 215, 464

Madagascar, 73 Malaysia, 366, 440 Mali, 16 marriage, 368–372

early years of, 370–371 first, median age at, 368 happiness in, 371–372 in late adulthood, 521–522 life course–stress process framework,

522, 522 marital adjustment and, 368 marital quality and, 368 marital satisfaction and, 368 marital success and, 368, 369, 380 military deployments and, 371, 371 rites of passage and, 319 same-sex, 367, 369, 378

vulnerability-stress-adaptation model and, 370, 370

work-related stress and, 234 math skills, 213–215

concepts in, 213–214 gender-related differences in, 179 international comparison,

214–215, 215 learning disability in, 205, 207 techniques for mastering, 214

Mauritania, 73 Melanesians, 543 menarche, 266–268 menopause, 428–429 Mexican Americans. See Latino

Americans Mexico

division of household labor in, 410, 411

divorce in, 380 ethnic identity in, 293 familism in, 375 grief reactions, 563 longevity in, 463, 464 math skills in, 215 menopause and, 428

Middle East. See Arab/Islamic/Muslim cultures

military deployments, marriage and, 371, 371

Mongolia, 166 moral reasoning, 279–284 motor skills, 81, 96–102, 219, 219–221 Mozambique, 73 multiethnic families, 375, 375–376 Muslims, 4, 8

See also Arab/Islamic/Muslim cultures

Native Americans Apache menarche celebration,

267, 268 binge drinking and, 328 cardiovascular disease and, 470 career preferences of, 304 ethnic identity and, 293, 294 grandparenthood and, 451 nutrition and, 330 parenting and, 375 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 swaddling and, 82 teen suicide and, 310 terms for describing, 9

Native Hawaiians, 470, 489 Nepal, 166 Netherlands

adoption in, 377 aging-in-place in, 532 child-free couples in, 373 cohabitation in, 366 couple-forming behaviors in, 360 infant mortality rate in, 73 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 physical punishment in, 232 physician-assisted suicide in, 546

New Guinea, 543 New York Longitudinal Study, 85 New Zealand, 73, 215 Nigeria, 360 North America

autobiographical memory in, 133 co-sleeping in, 83 divorce in, 238 effortful control in, 86

emotional expression in, 166 gender roles in, 177 intelligence in, 198 motor development in, 98–99 romantic attachment in, 361, 361 scaffolding in, 137 step-, foster-, adoptive, and same-sex

couple parenting in, 377 television’s impact on children in, 250 women’s liberation movement in, 181

Norway, 215, 366, 394, 464

Oceania, 361 older adults, diversity of, 460, 460 online dating, 359–360 online friendships, 357

Pacific Islanders, 325, 489 Pakistan, 90 Palestinian refugees in Jordan, 296 Papua New Guinea, 543 Paraguay, 99 parent-child relationships, 234–235

attachment during infancy and, 159 characteristics of children

contributing to, 234–235 child maltreatment and, 239–242 conflict between parents and, 233 divorce and, 238 ethnic diversity and, 375 grief and, 573 in middle childhood, 228–242 play and, 172 storm and stress, myth of, 296 teenage smoking and, 308

parents/parenting adjusting to parenthood, 69–70 aging parents, 448–450 behavior of, 231–233 child maltreatment and, 239–242 as coaches, 172 cost of raising a child from birth to

18, 373 deciding whether to have children,

373–375 ethnic diversity and, 375–376 FMLA and, 374, 409 marital system’s influences on,

233–234 as mediators, 172 obesity and, 271 play influenced by, 172 as playmates, 172 postpartum depression and, 70 single parents, 376, 376–377 as social directors, 172 step-, foster-, adoptive, and same-sex

couples, 377–378 styles of, 229–235 teenage mothers and, 59 in young and middle adulthood,

372–376 See also parent-child relationships

Peru, 82, 169 physical punishment, 232, 240 play, 169–172 Poland, 215, 282, 464 popular children, 246–247 Portugal, 215, 380, 464 prenatal development, 51–66 Protestants, 4 Puerto Rico, 563

race, 7, 8–9 See also ethnicity

remarriage, 239

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

xli

representative sampling, 25, 26 retirement, 514–518 role transitions, 319–320 Roman Catholics, 544 romantic attachment, 360–361, 361 romantic relationships, 297–298,

358–363 in adulthood, 358–359 couple-forming behaviors in, 360 culture and, 360–362, 361 desirable mates in, characteristics

of, 360 developmental forces in, 362–363 falling in love, 359–362 infatuation in, 358–359

Russia, 380 Rwanda, 73

same-sex partnerships in late adulthood, 522 LGB partnerships, 522 LGBTQ relationships, 367–368 marriage, 367, 369, 378 parenting in, 377–378

scaffolding, 137–138 Scotland, 29, 523–524 Senegal, 73 sexual orientation, 300 Sierra Leone, 73

longevity in, 463 Singapore, 380 singlehood, 365–366 single parents, 376, 376–377 Slovak Republic, 215, 464 Slovenia, 215, 464 sociocultural definitions of death,

543–544 sociocultural forces, 7, 8–9 socioeconomic status

birth complications and, 71

delinquency and, 312 health and, 330–331 intelligence and, 201–203 parenting styles and, 230–231 prenatal risk and, 64 puberty and, 267

Somalia, 73 South Africa, 380, 400, 563, 574 South America, 361, 366–367, 428 Southeast Asia, 361, 366 Spain

aging-in-place in, 532 couple-forming behaviors

in, 360 divorce in, 380, 381 infant mortality rate in, 73 job loss in, effects of, 406 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 online dating in, 359 physical punishment in, 232 reading skills in, 210

Sri Lanka, 377 step-parenting, 377–378 stereotype threat, 203 stress, 423–433 Sudan, 73 suicide

depression and, 310 in middle childhood, 275 physician-assisted, 546, 547

Sweden divorce in, 380 infant mortality rate in, 73 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 parental leave from work

in, 409 physical punishment in, 240

Switzerland, 215, 380, 464, 546

Taiwan, 215, 215, 394, 449, 509 Tanzania, 509 teenage pregnancy, 59, 299 Thailand, 166, 380 Turkey, 73, 82, 215, 296, 491

Uganda, 73 United Kingdom

adults without children in, 373 anti-discrimination legislation

in, 403 child-free couples in, 373 couples without children in, 373 divorce in, 380 educational attainment in, 391 genetic screening for breast cancer

in, 477 infant mortality rate in, 73 longevity in, 464 math skills in, 215 Million Women Study in, on

MHT, 429 occupational attainment in, 391 online dating in, 359

United States, 214, 215, 215 anti-discrimination legislation in, 403 average longevity in, 463 binge drinking in, 327, 328 bullying in, 248 child-free couples in, 373–374 cohabitation in, 366–367 couples without children in, 373 death in, reactions to, 543 division of household labor in, 411 divorce in, 379–381, 380 infant mortality rate in, 73 job loss in, effects of, 406 job satisfaction in, 394 labor force participation of older

adults in, 516

leading cause of death among young adults in, 323

leisure in, 414 longevity in, 463, 464 make-believe in, 170 marital success in, 369 menopause in, 428 online dating in, 359 parental leave from work in, 409 physical punishment in, 232, 240 physician-assisted suicide

in, 546 romantic relationships in, 297 television’s impact on children

in, 250 violence in relationships in, 363 violence in relationships in, 297 Women’s Health Initiative in, on

MHT, 429 work–family conflict in, 412

universal and context-specific- development issue, 6–7

Vietnam, 293, 380 violence

continuum of progressive behaviors and, 364

dating, 300–301 on television, 250 in young and middle adulthood

relationships, 363–364

well-being in adolescence, 275 in late adulthood, 509 in newborns, 81 unemployment and, 406, 407

Western Europe, 361, 545

Zambia, 73

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Ra w

pi xe

l/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

3

1The Study of Human Development Jeanne Calment was one of the most important people to have ever lived. Her amazing achieve- ment was not made in sports, government, or any

other profession. When she died in 1996 at age

122 years and 164 days, she set the world record

that still stands for the longest verified human life

span. Jeanne lived her whole life in Arles, France. She

met Vincent Van Gogh and experienced the inven-

tions of the light bulb, automobiles, airplanes, space

travel, computers, and all sorts of everyday conve-

niences. She survived two world wars. Longevity ran

in her family: her older brother François lived to 97,

her father to 93, and her mother to 86. Jeanne was

extraordinarily healthy her whole life, rarely being ill.

She was also active; she learned fencing when she

was 85, and she was still riding a bicycle at age 100.

She lived on her own until she was 110, when she

moved to a nursing home. Her life was documented

in the 1995 film Beyond 120 Years with Jeanne Calment.

Shortly before her 121st birthday, Musicdisc released Time’s Mistress, a CD of

Jeanne speaking over a background of rap and hip-hop music.

Did you ever wonder how long you will live? The people you will meet and

the experiences you will have? Did you ever think about how you managed to

go from being a young child to the more experienced person you are now? Or

what might lie ahead over the next few years or decades? Would you like to

WHAT DO YOU THINK? Would You Want to Live to Be 142? 4

1.1 Thinking About Development 5 Recurring Issues in Human Development 5 Basic Forces in Human Development:

The Biopsychosocial Framework 7

REAL PEOPLE APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT Muhammad Ali 11

Neuroscience: A Window into Human Development 10

1.2 Developmental Theories 12 Psychodynamic Theory 12 Learning Theory 13 Cognitive-Developmental Theory 14 The Ecological and Systems Approach 17 Life-Span Perspective, Selective Optimization with

Compensation, and Life-Course Perspective 18 The Big Picture 21

1.3 Doing Developmental Research 22 Measurement in Human Development Research 23 General Designs for Research 26 Designs for Studying Development 28

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH The Stability of Intelligence from Age 11 to Age 90 Years 29

Integrating Findings from Different Studies 31 Conducting Research Ethically 32 Communicating Research Results 33 Applying Research Results: Social Policy 33

Summary 34

Key Terms 36

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

4 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

break Jeanne Calment’s longevity record? What do you

think other people would want? The What Do You Think?

feature provides the results of a poll of Americans, as well

as provocative questions about extending life radically.

Consider your life to this point. Make a note to yourself about—or share with someone else—your fondest memories from childhood or the events and people who have most influenced you. Also make a note about what you think you might experience during the rest of your life. Put your notes in a safe place (if it’s stored online, don’t forget your password). Then many years from now, retrieve it and see if you were right.

Thinking about your past and future experiences is the beginning of an exciting personal journey. Remember major moments or experiences

you’ve had. What happened? Why do you think things happened the way they did? What major forces have shaped your life?

Likewise, look ahead. What future story do you want to write about yourself? Think about the forces that may shape the course of your life years from now—about those forces that you can influence and those that you cannot. Think about how the changes you expe- rience will affect your future.

In this course, you will have the opportunity to ask some of life’s most basic questions: How did your life begin? How did you go from a single cell—about the size of the period at the end of a sentence in this text—to the fully grown, complex adult person you are today?

Jeanne Calment experienced many changes in society during her 122-year life span.

G eo

rg es

G O

BE T/

A ge

nc e

Fr an

ce P

re ss

e/ N

ew sc

om

Humans may be on the brink of funda-mentally redefining the typical life span. The May 2013 issue of National Geographic magazine showed a baby with the caption “This baby will live to be 120.” Topping that, the February 23/March 2, 2015, double issue of Time magazine devoted its main feature to the possibility the baby on their front cover could live to 142 years (or longer). As you will learn in this book, our knowledge of the fac- tors determining the length of the human life span is extensive, and we have in our grasp the ability to dramatically lengthen the number of years people live. But just because science enables us to think about extending life con- siderably, the key question is whether people will want to live to be 142 (or even 120).

The Pew Research Center (2013a) asked a representative sample of 2,012 U.S. adults whether they would want to live decades longer, to at least 120 years. Interestingly, when people answered from their own per- spective, 56% said they would not want to live that long. But when asked what they thought other people would do, 68% said they thought other people would choose to live to at least 120. We will take a closer look at other aspects of this poll in later chapters,

but in general, the results showed that people are optimistic about their own aging and the scientific advances that will enable them to enjoy a better quality of life in old age.

A dramatic extension of the human life span to 120 years or more would likely raise ethical and moral questions, such as how to define a full and purpose-driven life, especially with respect to how we should handle the end of life. For questions such as these, many people turn to religious leaders for guidance. As part of their survey research project, the Pew Research Center (2013b) also looked at how 18 major American religious groups might approach radical life extension. Because no major religious group in America has taken a formal position on this issue, Pew researchers looked at what bioethicists (people who focus on ethics within health areas, for instance), clergy, and other scholars have said about how their respective traditions might approach the matter.

The Pew report contains links to related writings in the various religious traditions that, as you might imagine, vary across denomina- tion. Buddhists may see longer life as provid- ing more opportunities to learn wisdom and compassion and to achieve nirvana. Catholics may see longer lives as diminishing the search

for the transcendent. Hindus may welcome longer life, as their normal blessing is “Live long.” Muslims and Jews may view longer life as a reflection of God’s plan for humanity. For many Protestants, the key factor would be whether longer life spans are seen as a way to avoid death, which would likely lead them to oppose it. These views reflect different per- spectives that result from interpreting both individual and collective experiences that are influenced in turn by various biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces (explored later in this chapter).

As we move along our journey through the human life span, questions that take us to the intersection of science and personal belief will occur frequently. Later in this chap- ter, we will encounter the rules by which scien- tific research is conducted, so you will better understand what the Pew Research Center did in conducting their poll. In Chapter 16, when we encounter the complex personal issues relating to the end of life, you will have a thorough grounding in how people use (or ignore) research findings in their own lives.

Back to the question posed here—would you like to live to 120? 142? Longer? What do you think?

Would You Want to Live to Be 142?What Do You Think?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 5

Will you be the same or different later in life? How do you influence other people’s lives? How do they influence yours? How do the various roles you play throughout life—child, teenager, partner, spouse, parent, worker, grandparent—shape your development? How do you deal with the thought of your own death and the death of others?

These are examples of the questions that create the scientific foundation of human development, the multidisciplinary study of how people change and how they remain the same over time. Answering these questions requires us to draw on theories and research in the physical and social sciences, including biology, genetics, neuroscience, chemistry, allied health and medicine, psychology, sociology, demography, ethnography, economics, and anthropology. The science of human development reflects the complexity and unique- ness of each person and each person’s experiences as well as commonalities and pat- terns among people. As a science, human development is firmly grounded in theory and research as it seeks to understand human behavior.

Before our journey begins, we need to collect some things to make the trip more rewarding. In this chapter, we pick up the necessary GPS coordinates that point us in the proper direction: a framework to organize theories and research, common issues and influences on development, and the methods developmental scientists use to make dis- coveries. Throughout the book, we will point out how the various theories and research connect to your own experience. Pack well and bon voyage.

human development The multidisciplinary study of how people change and how they remain the same over time.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What fundamental issues of development have scholars

addressed throughout history?

●● What are the basic forces in the biopsychosocial framework? How does the timing of these forces affect their impact?

●● How does neuroscience enhance our understanding of human development?

1.1 Thinking About Development

Hassan Qabbani smiled broadly as he held his newborn grandson for the first time. So many thoughts rushed into his mind: What would Mohammad experience growing up? Would the poor neighborhood they lived in prevent him from reaching his potential? Would he inherit the family genes for good health? How would his life growing up as an Arab American in the United States be different from Hassan’s experiences in Syria?

Like many grandparents, Hassan wonders what the future holds for his grandson. The questions he asks are interesting in their own right, but they are important for another reason: They bear on general issues of human development that have intrigued philoso- phers and scientists for centuries. In the next few pages, we introduce some of these issues, which surface when any aspect of development is being investigated.

Recurring Issues in Human Development What factors shaped the you that you are right now? You might suspect such things as your genetic heritage, your family or neighborhood, the suddenness of some changes in your life and the gradualness of others, and the culture(s) in which you grew up or now live. You also might have noticed that you are like some people you know—and very much unlike others. So you might suspect that everyone’s life is shaped by a complex set of factors.

Your speculations capture three fundamental characteristics of human development: nature and nurture, continuity and discontinuity, and universal and context-specific develop- ment. A person’s development is a blend of these characteristics; for example, some of your characteristics remain the same through life (continuity) and others change (discontinuity). Because these characteristics apply to all the topics in this book, we’ll examine each one.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

6 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Nature and Nurture Think about a particular feature that you and several people in your family have, such as intelligence, good looks, or a friendly and outgoing personality. Why is this feature so prevalent? Did you inherit it from your parents and they from your grandparents? Or is it mainly because of where and how you and your parents were brought up? Answers to these questions illustrate different positions on the nature–nurture issue, which involves the degree to which genetic or hereditary influences (nature) and experiential or environmental influences (nurture) determine the kind of person you are. The key point is that develop- ment is always shaped by both: Nature and nurture are mutually interactive influences.

For example, in Chapter 2, you will see that some individuals inherit a disease that leads to intellectual disability if they eat dairy products. However, if their environment contains no dairy products, they develop normal intelligence. Similarly, in Chapter 10, you will learn that one risk factor for cardiovascular disease is heredity but that lifestyle factors such as diet and smoking play important roles in determining who has heart attacks.

As these examples illustrate, a major aim of human development research is to under- stand how heredity and environment jointly determine development. For Hassan, it means his grandson’s development will surely be shaped both by the genes he inherited and by the experiences he will have.

Continuity and Discontinuity Think of some ways in which you are still the same as you were as a 5-year-old. Maybe you were outgoing and friendly at that age and remain outgoing and friendly today. Such examples suggest a great deal of continuity in development. From this perspective, once a person heads down a particular developmental path—for example, toward friendliness or intelligence—he or she tends to stay on that path throughout life, other things being equal. From a continuity perspective, if Mohammad is a friendly and smart 5-year-old, then he should be friendly and smart as a 25-year-old and a 75-year-old.

The other view is that development is not always continuous. In this view, people can change from one developmental path to another and perhaps several times in their lives. Consequently, Mohammad might be smart and friendly at age 5, smart but obnoxious at 25, and wise but aloof at 75.

The continuity–discontinuity issue concerns whether a particular developmental phenomenon represents a smooth progression throughout the life span (continuity) or a series of abrupt shifts (discontinuity). Of course, on a day-to-day basis, behaviors often look nearly identical, or continuous. But when viewed over the course of many months or years, the same behaviors may have changed dramatically, reflecting discontinuous change. For example, your face may look nearly identical in “selfies” taken on successive days (continu- ity) but change dramatically in photos taken years apart (discontinuity).

Throughout this book, you will find examples of developmental changes that repre- sent continuities and others that are discontinuities. For example, in Chapter 5, you will see evidence of continuity: Infants who have satisfying emotional relationships with their parents typically become children with satisfying peer relationships. But in Chapter  15, you will see an instance of discontinuity: After spending most of adulthood trying to ensure the success of the next generation and to leave a legacy, older adults turn to evalu- ating their own lives in search of closure and a sense that what they have done has been worthwhile.

Universal and Context-Specific Development In many native and indigenous cultures, mathematical concepts are mastered by young children not through formal education about numbers but through everyday tasks such as picking berries and selling goods in street markets (Kisker et al., 2012; Sleeter, 2016). In contrast, children in the United States are formally taught at home or school to identify numbers and to perform the abstract arithmetic operations needed to handle these tasks.

nature–nurture issue The degree to which genetic or hereditary influences (nature) and experiential or environmental influences (nurture) determine the kind of person you are.

Think About It Think of some common, everyday behav- iors such as dancing and playing basket- ball with your friends. How do nature and nurture influence these behaviors?

continuity–discontinuity issue Concerns whether a particular develop- mental phenomenon represents a smooth progression throughout the life span (continuity) or a series of abrupt shifts (discontinuity).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 7

Can one theory explain development in both groups of children? The universal and context-specific development issue concerns whether there is one path of development or several. Some theorists argue that despite what look like differences in development, there is only one fun- damental developmental process for everyone. According to this view, differences in development are simply variations on the same funda- mental process in much the same way cars as different as a Chevrolet, a Honda, and a Lexus are all products of fundamentally the same manufac- turing process.

The alternative view is that differences among people are not simply variations on a theme. Advocates of this view argue that human develop- ment is inextricably intertwined with the context within which it occurs. A person’s development is a product of complex interaction with the environment, and that interaction is not fundamentally the same in all environments. Rather, each environment has its own set of unique pro- cedures that shape development, just as the “recipes” for different cars yield vehicles as different as a Smart car and a stretch limousine.

As is the case for the nature–nurture and continuity– discontinuity issues, the result is a blend; individual development reflects both univer- sal and context-specific influences. For example, the order of develop- ment of physical skills in infancy is essentially the same in all cultures. But how those skills are focused or encouraged in daily life differs.

Putting all three issues together and using personality to illustrate, we can ask how the development of personality is shaped by interactions between heredity and environment, is continuous or discontinuous, and develops in much the same way around the world. To answer these kinds of questions, we need to look at the forces that combine to shape human development.

Basic Forces in Human Development: The Biopsychosocial Framework When trying to explain why people develop as they do, scientists usually consider four interactive forces:

●● Biological forces that include all genetic and health-related factors that affect development.

●● psychological forces that include all internal perceptual, cognitive, emotional, and personality factors that affect development.

●● Sociocultural forces that include interpersonal, societal, cultural, and ethnic factors that affect development.

●● Life-cycle forces that reflect differences in how the same event affects people of different ages.

Each person is a unique combination of these forces. To see why each force is impor- tant, think about whether a mother decides to breast-feed her infant. Her decision will be based on biological variables (e.g., the quality and amount of milk she produces), her attitudes about the virtues of breast-feeding, the influences of other people (e.g., the father, her own mother), and her cultural traditions and societal norms about appropriate ways to feed infants. In addition, her decision will reflect her age and stage of life. Only by focusing on all of these forces can we have a complete view of the mother’s decision.

One useful way to organize the biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces on human development is with the biopsychosocial framework. As you can see in ◗ ●Figure 1.1, the biopsychosocial framework emphasizes that each of the forces interacts with the oth- ers to make up development. Let’s look at the different elements of the biopsychosocial model in more detail.

universal and context-specific development issue Concerns whether there is one path of development or several.

Even with little formal education, this Brazil- ian boy has well-developed mathematical skills. This is an example of cultural contex- tual forces shaping development.

M at

hi as

T . O

pp er

sd or

ff /S

ci en

ce S

ou rc

e

biological forces All genetic and health-related factors that affect development.

psychological forces All internal perceptual, cognitive, emo- tional, and personality factors that affect development.

sociocultural forces Interpersonal, societal, cultural, and ethnic factors that affect development.

life-cycle forces Differences in how the same event affects people of different ages.

biopsychosocial framework Useful way to organize the biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces on human development.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

8 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Biological Forces: Genetics and Health Prenatal development, brain maturation, puberty, and physical aging may occur to you as outcomes of biological forces. Indeed, major aspects of each process are determined by our genetic code. For example, many children resemble their parents, which shows biological influences on development. But biological forces are not only genetic; they also include the effects of such things as diet and exercise. Collectively, biological forces can be viewed as providing the raw material necessary and as setting the boundary conditions (in the case of genetics) for development.

Psychological Forces: Known by Our Behavior Psychological forces seem familiar because they are the ones used most often to describe the characteristics of a person. For example, think about how you describe yourself to others. Most of us say that we have a nice personality and are intelligent, honest, self- confident, or something similar. Concepts such as these reflect psychological forces.

In general, psychological forces are all the internal cognitive, emotional, personality, perceptual, and related factors that help define us as individuals and that influence behav- ior. Psychological forces have received the most attention of the three main developmental forces, and their impact is evident throughout this text. For example, we will see how the development of intelligence enables individuals to experience and think about their world in different ways. We’ll also see how the emergence of self-esteem is related to the beliefs people have about their abilities, which in turn influence what they do.

Sociocultural Forces: Race, Ethnicity, and Culture People develop in the world, not in a vacuum. To understand human development, we need to know how people and their environments interact and mutually influence each other. That is, we need to view an individual’s development as part of a much larger system in which any individual part influences all other aspects of the system. This larger system includes one’s parents, children, siblings, extended family, as well as important individu- als outside the family, such as friends, teachers, and coworkers. The system also includes institutions that influence development, such as schools, media, and the workplace. At a broader level, the society in which a person grows up plays a key role.

All of these people and institutions fit together to form a person’s culture: the knowl- edge, attitudes, and behavior associated with a group of people. Culture can be linked to a particular country or people (e.g., French culture); to a specific point in time (e.g., popular culture of the 2010s); or to groups of individuals who maintain specific, identifi- able cultural traditions (e.g., Native American tribes, Muslims). Knowing the culture from which a person comes provides some general information about important influences that become manifest throughout the life span.

●◗ Figure 1.1 The biopsychosocial framework shows that human development results from interacting forces.

Life- cycle forces

Biological forces

Sociocultural forces

Psychological forces

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 9

Understanding the impact of culture is particularly important in the United States, one of the most culturally diverse countries in the world. Hundreds of different languages are spoken, and in many states, no single racial or ethnic group constitutes a majority. The many customs of people from different cultures offer insights into the broad spectrum of human experience and attest to the diversity of the U.S. population.

Although the U.S. population is changing rapidly, much of the research we describe in this text was conducted on middle-class European Americans. Accordingly, we must be care- ful not to assume that findings from this group necessarily apply to people in other groups. Indeed, there is a great need for research on different cultural groups. Perhaps as a result of taking this course, you will help fill this need by becoming a developmental researcher yourself.

Another practical problem that we face is how to describe racial and ethnic groups. Terminology changes over time. For example, the terms colored people, Negroes, black Americans, and African Americans have all been used to describe Americans of African ancestry. In this book, we use the term African American because it emphasizes their unique cultural heritage. Following the same reasoning, we use European American (instead of Caucasian or white), Native American (instead of Indian or American Indian), Asian American, and Latino American (rather than Hispanic).

These labels are not perfect. In some cases, they blur distinctions among eth- nic groups. For example, people from both Puerto Rico and Mexico may be described as Latinos. However, their cultural backgrounds vary on several important dimensions, so we should not view them as a homogeneous group. Similarly, the term Asian Ameri- can blurs variations among people whose heritage is, for example, Japanese, Chinese, or Korean. Throughout this text, whenever researchers have identified the subgroups in their research sample, we will use the more specific terms in describing results. When we use the more general terms, remember that conclusions may not apply to all subgroups within the group described by the more general term.

Life-Cycle Forces: Timing Is Everything Consider the following two females. Jacqui, a 32-year-old, has been happily married for six years. She and her husband have a steady income. They decide to start a family, and a month later Jacqui learns that she is pregnant. Jenny, a 17-year-old, lives in the same neighborhood as Jacqui. She has been sexually active for about six months but is not in a stable relationship. After missing her period, Jenny takes a pregnancy test and discovers that she is pregnant.

The culture in which you grow up influences how you experience life.

73 82

48 95

61 /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

10 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Although both Jacqui and Jenny became pregnant, the outcome of each pregnancy will certainly be affected by factors in each woman’s situation, such as her age, her finan- cial situation, and the extent of her social support systems. The example illustrates life- cycle forces: The same event can have different effects depending on when it happens in a person’s life. In the scenarios with Jacqui and Jenny, the same event—pregnancy—pro- duces happiness and eager anticipation for one woman but anxiety and concern for the other.

The Forces Interact So far, we’ve described the four forces in the biopsychosocial framework as if they were independent. But as we pointed out, each force mutually shapes the others. Consider eat- ing habits. When the authors of this text were growing up, a “red meat and potatoes” diet was common and thought to be healthy. Scientists later discovered that high-fat diets may lead to cardiovascular disease and some forms of cancer. Consequently, research changed dietary recommendations, and social pressures changed what people eat, the advertise- ments they saw, and the different foods restaurants began to serve. Thus, the biological forces of the consequences of fat in the diet were influenced by the social forces of the times, whether in support of or in opposition to eating beef every evening. As your authors became more educated about diets and their effects on health, the psychological forces of thinking and reasoning also influenced their choice of diets. (We confess, however, that dark chocolate remains a passion for one of us.) However, research on the effects of fat sometimes collided with centuries of cultural food traditions and with the ability to afford healthier alternatives, meaning that some folks were able to change their diets more easily than were others. Finally, the age of the person when this research became widely known mattered, too. Young children eat what their parents provide, whereas adults may have the ability to make choices.

This example illustrates that no aspect of human development can be fully under- stood by examining the forces in isolation. All four must be considered in interaction. In fact, we’ll see later in this chapter that integration across the major forces of the biopsy- chosocial framework is one criterion by which the adequacy of a developmental theory can be judged.

Combining the four developmental forces gives a view of human development that encompasses the life span yet appreciates the unique aspects of each phase of life. From this perspective we can view each life story as a complex interplay among the four forces. Try this for yourself. Read the short biography of Muhammad Ali in the Real People: Applying Human Development feature; then think about how each of the developmental forces would explain how he accomplished as much as he did.

Neuroscience: A Window into Human Development Understanding that the four developmental forces interact is one thing. But what if, like the comic book hero Superman, you could use your X-ray vision and actually see these forces interact? That’s what is possible in the field of neuroscience. Applied to human development, neuroscience is the study of the brain and the nervous system, especially in terms of brain–behavior relationships. Neuroscientists use several methods to do this, from molecular analyses of individual brain cells to sophisticated techniques that yield images of brain activity.

Neuroscientific approaches are being applied to a wide range of issues in human development, especially those involving memory, reasoning, and emotion (e.g., Linden, 2016; Sugiura, in press). For example, neuroscientists are unlocking relations between developmental changes in specific regions of the brain to explain well-known develop- mental phenomena such as adolescents’ tendency to engage in risky behavior and older adults’ short-term memory problems.

neuroscience The study of the brain and the nervous system, especially in terms of brain– behavior relationships.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 11

Recall 1. The nature–nurture issue involves the degree to which

_______________ and the environment influence human development.

2. Azar remarked that her 14-year-old son has been incred- ibly shy ever since he was a baby. This illustrates the _______________ of development.

3. _______________ forces include genetic and health factors.

4. Neuroscience examines _______________ relations.

Interpret ●● How does the biopsychosocial framework provide insight

into the recurring issues of development (nature–nurture, continuity–discontinuity, universal–context-specific)?

Apply ●● How does your life experience reflect the four developmental

forces?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 1.1

Muhammad Ali never left any doubt about how he perceived himself: “I am the greatest.” And he backed up his claim, becoming one of the most impor- tant, controversial, inspiring, and respected figures of the 20th century. Born Cassius Marcellus Clay, Jr. in 1942, he changed his name from what he referred to as his “slave name” to Muhammad Ali to reflect both his religious conversion to Sunni Islam and his support of the civil rights movement in the 1960s.

Ali was the dominant boxer of his genera- tion and remains the only fighter to win the heavyweight title three separate times. His epic bouts against Sonny Liston, Joe Frazier, and George Foreman have become legend- ary. He aimed to “float like a butterfly and sting like a bee” in the ring, and his “rope- a-dope” tactic gave him a way to tire his opponents.

Ali craved the limelight and media. His verbal skills, in particular his poetry, made him a star, and his recordings earned him two Grammy Award nominations. At a time when most boxers, and most sports figures, shied from politics, he made a point to be provocative and even outlandish.

In 1966, two years after winning his first heavyweight title, Ali refused to be drafted into the U.S. military, stating his religious

beliefs and opposition to the Vietnam War as his reasons for being a conscientious objector. He was arrested, convicted of draft evasion, and stripped of his title. Although the U.S. Supreme Court overturned his con- viction in 1971, Ali had lost four years of his prime fighting career.

Ali retired from boxing in 1981 and devoted the remainder of his life to chari- table and religious work. He was one of the most recognized figures globally, and was recognized by U.S. Presidents Bill Clinton and George W. Bush, as well as the United Nations and numerous other organizations.

Diagnosed with Parkinson’s disease in 1984 as a consequence of boxing-related brain injuries, Ali made fewer public appear- ances in the last decades of his life. Most notably, he lit the Olympic flame at the 1996 Atlanta Games. His family provided care for him until his death on June 3, 2016, at the age of 74.

Muhammad Ali’s life demonstrates all of the developmental forces. His phenomenal boxing prowess and his later succumbing to Parkinson’s disease were the result of biological forces. His high self-confidence and quick verbal skills reflect psychological forces. His choice not to serve in the Viet- nam War and his very strong support for civil rights reflected the sociocultural forces of the

times. The arc of his boxing success and his later move to religious and charitable work indicate the action of life-cycle forces. Ali was both a man of his times, and a figure for all times.

Real People Applying Human Development

Muhammad Ali

Fe at

ur ef

la sh

P ho

to A

ge nc

y/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Muhammad Ali

Neuroscience brings an important perspective to human development. Identifying patterns of brain activity helps to reveal interactions between biological, psychological, sociocultural, and life-cycle forces, which allows a better understanding of how each person is a unique expression of these forces.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

12 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Marcus has just graduated from high school, first in his class. For his proud mother, Betty, this is a time to reflect on her son’s past and to ponder his future. Marcus has always been a happy, easygoing child—a joy to rear. And he’s been interested in learning for as long as she can remember. Betty wonders why he is so perpetually good-natured and so curious. If she knew the secret, she laughed, she could write a best-selling book and be a guest on The Late Show with Stephen Colbert.

To answer Betty’s questions about her son’s growth, developmental researchers need to pro- vide a theory of his development. Theories are essential because they provide the “why’s” for development. What is a theory? In human development, a theory is an organized set of ideas that explains development. For example, suppose friends of yours have a baby who cries often. You could imagine several explanations for her crying. Maybe the baby cries because she’s hungry; maybe she cries to get her parents to hold her; maybe she cries because she’s a cranky, unhappy baby. Each of these explanations is a very simple theory: It tries to explain why the baby cries so much. Of course, actual theories in human development are more complicated, but the purpose is the same—to explain behavior and development.

There are no truly comprehensive theories of human development to guide research (Newman & Newman, 2015). Instead, five general perspectives influence current research: psychodynamic theory; learning theory; cognitive theory; ecological and systems theory; and theories involving the life-span perspective, selective optimization with compensa- tion, and the life-course perspective. Let’s consider each approach briefly.

Psychodynamic Theory psychodynamic theories hold that development is largely determined by how well people resolve conflicts they face at different ages. This perspective traces its roots to Sigmund Freud’s theory that personality emerges from conflicts that children experience between what they want to do and what society wants them to do.

Building on Sigmund Freud’s ideas, Erik Erikson (1902–1994) proposed his psychoso- cial theory, the first comprehensive life-span view, which still remains an important theo- retical framework.

Erikson’s Theory In his psychosocial theory, Erikson proposed that personality development is determined by the interaction of an internal maturational plan and external societal demands. He proposed that the life cycle is composed of eight stages and that the order of the stages is biologically fixed (the eight stages shown in ◗ Table 1.1). The name of each stage reflects the challenge people face at a particular age. For example, the challenge for young adults is to become involved in a loving relationship. Challenges are met through a combination of inner psychological influences and outer social influences. When challenges are met suc- cessfully, people are well prepared to meet the challenge of the next stage.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is a developmental theory?

●● How do psychodynamic theories account for development?

●● What is the focus of learning theories of development?

●● How do cognitive-developmental theories explain changes in thinking?

●● What are the main points in the ecological and systems approach?

●● What are the major tenets of life-span and life-course theories?

1.2 Developmental Theories

theory An organized set of ideas that explains development.

psychodynamic theories Hold that development is largely determined by how well people resolve conflicts they face at different ages.

psychosocial theory Erikson theory that personality develop- ment is determined by the interaction of an internal maturational plan and external societal demands.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 13

The sequence of stages in Erikson’s theory is based on the epigenetic principle, which means that each psychosocial strength has its own special period of particular importance. The eight stages represent the order of this ascendancy, and it takes a lifetime to acquire all of the psychosocial strengths. Moreover, Erikson realizes that present and future behav- ior must have its roots in the past because later stages are built on the foundation laid in previous stages.

The psychodynamic perspective emphasizes that the trek to adulthood is difficult because the path is strewn with challenges. Outcomes of development reflect the manner and ease (or difficulty) with which people surmount (or fail to overcome) life’s barriers. When children easily overcome early obstacles, for example, they are better able to handle the later ones. A psychodynamic theorist would tell Betty that her son’s cheerful disposi- tion and his academic record suggest that he has handled life’s early obstacles well, which is a good sign for his future development.

Learning Theory In contrast to psychodynamic theory, learning theory concentrates on how learning influ- ences a person’s behavior. This perspective emphasizes the role of experience, examining whether a person’s behavior is rewarded or punished. This perspective also emphasizes that people learn from watching others around them. Two influential theories in this perspective are behaviorism and social learning theory.

Behaviorism Early in the 20th century, John Watson (1878–1958) believed that infants’ minds were essentially “blank slates” and argued that learning alone determines what people will become. He assumed that with the correct techniques, almost anyone could learn any- thing. In Watson’s view then, experience was about all that mattered in determining the course of development.

Watson did little research to support his claims, but B. F. Skinner (1904–1990) filled this gap. Skinner studied operant conditioning, in which the consequences of a behavior determine whether a behavior is repeated in the future. Skinner showed that two kinds of consequences were especially influential. A reinforcement is a consequence that increases the likelihood of the behavior that it follows. Positive reinforcement consists of giving a reward such as chocolate, gold stars, or paychecks to increase the likelihood of a behav- ior. A father who wants to encourage his daughter to help with chores may reinforce her

Erik Erikson

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Table 1.1

The Eight Stages of Psychosocial Development in Erikson’s Theory

PSYCHOSOCIAL STAGE AGE CHALLENGE

Basic trust vs. mistrust Birth to 1 year To develop a sense that the world is safe, a “good place”

Autonomy vs. shame 1 to 3 years To realize that one is an independent person who can make decisions and doubt

Initiative vs. guilt 3 to 6 years To develop the ability to try new things and to handle failure

Industry vs. inferiority 6 years to adolescence To learn basic skills and to work with others

Identity vs. identity confusion Adolescence To develop a lasting, integrated sense of self

Intimacy vs. isolation Young adulthood To commit to another in a loving relationship

Generativity vs. stagnation Middle adulthood To contribute to younger people through child rearing, child care, or other pro- ductive work

Integrity vs. despair Late life To view one’s life as satisfactory and worth living

epigenetic principle Means by which each psychosocial strength has its own special period of particular importance.

operant conditioning Technique in which the consequences of a behavior determine whether a behavior is repeated in the future.

reinforcement A consequence that increases the likelihood of the behavior that it follows.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

14 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

with praise, food treats, or money whenever she cleans her room. Negative reinforcement consists of rewarding people by taking away unpleasant things. The same father could use negative reinforcement by saying that whenever his daughter cleans her room, she doesn’t have to wash the dishes or weed the garden.

A punishment is a consequence that decreases the likelihood of the behavior that it follows. Punishment suppresses a behavior either by adding something aversive or by with- holding a pleasant event. Should the daughter fail to clean her room, the father may punish her by nagging (adding something aversive) or by not allowing her to FaceTime her friends (withholding a pleasant event).

Skinner’s research was done primarily with animals, but human development researchers showed that the principles of operant conditioning could be extended readily to people, too (Baer & Wolf, 1968). Applied properly, reinforcement and punishment are powerful influences on children, adolescents, and adults; however, compared with punish- ment, reinforcement tends to result in quicker and longer-lasting learning.

Social Learning Theory People sometimes learn without reinforcement or punishment. People learn much by simply watching those around them, which is known as imitation or observational learning. Imita- tion is the reason one toddler throws a toy after seeing a peer do so or when a school-age child offers to help an older adult carry groceries because she’s seen her parents do the same.

Perhaps imitation makes you think of “monkey see, monkey do,” in which people sim- ply mimic what they see. Early investigators had this view, too, but subsequent research showed that this was wrong. People do not always imitate what they see around them. People are more likely to imitate if the person they see is important, popular, smart, or talented. They’re also more likely to imitate when the behavior they see is rewarded than when it is punished. Findings like these imply that imitation is more complex than sheer mimicry. People are not mechanically copying what they see and hear; instead, they look to others for information about appropriate behavior. When peers are reinforced for behav- ing in a particular way, this encourages imitation. This is one explanation for why groups of friends tend to behave, talk, and think similarly.

Albert Bandura (1925–) based his social cognitive theory on this more complex view of reward, punishment, and imitation. Bandura’s theory is “cognitive” because he believes people actively try to understand what goes on in their world; the theory is “social” because, along with reinforcement and punishment, what other people do is an important source of information about the world.

Bandura also argues that experience gives people a sense of self-efficacy, which refers to people’s beliefs about their own abilities and talents. Self-efficacy beliefs help to deter- mine when people will imitate others. A child who sees himself as not athletically talented, for example, will not try to imitate LeBron James dunking a basketball despite the fact that LeBron is obviously talented and popular. Thus, whether an individual imitates others depends on who the other person is, on whether that person’s behavior is rewarded, and on the individual’s beliefs about his or her own abilities.

Bandura’s social cognitive theory is a far cry from Skinner’s operant conditioning. The operant conditioned person who responds mechanically to reinforcement and punish- ment has been replaced by the social cognitive person who actively interprets these and other events. Nevertheless, Skinner, Bandura, and all learning theorists share the view that experience propels people along their developmental journeys. These theorists would tell Betty that she can thank experience for making Marcus both happy and successful academically.

Cognitive-Developmental Theory Another way to approach development is to focus on thought processes and a person con- structing knowledge actively. In cognitive-developmental theory, the key is how people think and how thinking changes over time. Three distinct approaches have developed.

punishment A consequence that decreases the likelihood of the behavior that it follows.

B. F. Skinner

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Think About It How could you use the basic ideas of operant conditioning to explain how chil- dren create theories of the physical and social world?

imitation or observational learning Learning by simply watching those around them.

self-efficacy People’s beliefs about their own abilities and talents.

Albert Bandura

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 15

One approach postulates that thinking develops in a universal sequence of stages; Piaget’s theory of cognitive development (and its extensions) is the best-known example. The second approach proposes that people process information as computers do, becom- ing more efficient over much of the life span; information-processing theory is an example of this view. The third approach emphasizes the contributions of culture on thinking and cognitive growth.

Piaget’s Theory The cognitive-developmental perspective began with a focus on how children construct knowledge and how their constructions change over time. Jean Piaget (1896–1980), the most influential child and adolescent developmental psychologist of the 20th century, pro- posed the best known of these theories. Piaget believed that children naturally try to make sense of their world. Throughout infancy, childhood, and adolescence, individuals want to understand the workings of both the physical and social world. For example, infants want to know about objects: “What happens when I push this toy off the table?” And they want to know about people: “Who is this person who feeds and cares for me?”

As children try to comprehend their world, Piaget believed that they act like scien- tists, creating theories about the physical and social worlds. Children try to weave all that they know about objects and people into a complete theory, which is tested daily by expe- rience because their theories lead children to expect certain things to happen. As with real scientific theories, when the predicted events do occur, a child’s belief in her theory grows stronger. When the predicted events do not occur, the child must revise her theory.

Imagine an infant whose theory of objects includes the idea that “Toys pushed off the table fall to the floor.” If the infant pushes some other object—a plate or an article of clothing—she finds that it, too, falls to the floor, and she can then make the theory more general: “Objects pushed off the table fall to the floor.”

Piaget also believed that children begin to construct knowledge in new ways at a few critical points in development. When this happens, they revise their theories radically. These changes are so fundamental that the revised theory is, in many respects, brand-new. Piaget claimed that these changes occur first at about age 2 years, again at about age 7, and a third time just before adolescence. These changes mean that children go through four distinct stages in cognitive development. Each stage represents a fundamental change in how children understand and organize their environment, and each stage is character- ized by more sophisticated types of reasoning. For example, the first or sensorimotor stage begins at birth and lasts until about 2 years of age. As the name implies, sensorimotor thinking refers to an infant’s constructing knowledge through sensory and motor skills. This stage and the three later stages are shown in ◗ Table 1.2.

Jean Piaget

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Table 1.2

Piaget’s Four Stages of Cognitive Development

STAGE APPROXIMATE AGE CHARACTERISTICS

Sensorimotor Birth to 2 years Infant’s knowledge of the world is based on senses and motor skills; by the end of the period, uses mental representation

Preoperational thought 2 to 6 years Child learns how to use symbols such as words and numbers to repre- sent aspects of the world but relates to the world only through his or her perspective

Concrete operational thought 7 years to early adolescence Child understands and applies logical operations to experiences provided they are focused on the here and now

Formal operational thought Adolescence and beyond Adolescent or adult thinks abstractly, deals with hypothetical situations, and speculates about what may be possible

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

16 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Piaget’s theory has had an enormous influence on how developmentalists and practitioners think about cognitive development during childhood and adolescence. The theory has been applied in many ways—from the creation of discovery learning toys to the ways teachers plan lessons. In Chapter 4, we’ll see how Piaget explained thinking during infancy and the preschool years; in Chapters 6 and 8, we’ll learn about his description of thinking in school-age children and adolescents.

Information-Processing Theory Information-processing theorists draw heavily on how computers work to explain how thinking develops through childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Just as computers consist of both hardware (disk drives, random-access mem- ory, and the central processing unit) and software (the programs the computer runs), information-processing theory proposes that human cognition consists of mental hardware and mental software. Mental hardware refers to cognitive structures, including different memories where information is stored. Mental software includes cognitive processes that enable people to complete specific tasks such as reading a sentence, playing a video game, or hitting a baseball. For example, an information-processing psychologist would say that for students to do well on an exam, they must encode the information as they study, store it in memory, and then retrieve it during the test.

To explain developmental changes in thinking, information-processing psychologists believe that like computers, adolescents and adults have better hardware and better software than do younger children, who have less pow-

erful and sophisticated processors. For example, adolescents typically solve math word problems better than younger children do because adolescents have greater memory capacity to store the facts in the problem and because their methods for performing arithmetic operations are more efficient.

Some researchers also point to deterioration of the mental hardware—along with declines in the mental software—as explanations of cognitive aging. In Chapter 14, we will see, for example, that normative aging brings with it significant changes in people’s ability to process information.

Vygotsky’s Theory Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) was one of the first theorists to emphasize that children’s think- ing is influenced by the sociocultural context in which they grow up. A Russian psycholo- gist, Vygotsky believed that because all societies aim to help children acquire essential cultural values and skills, every aspect of a child’s development must be considered against this backdrop. For example, most parents in the United States want their children to work hard in school and go to college because earning a degree is a key to finding a good job. However, in Mali (an African country), Bambara parents want their children to learn to farm, herd animals such as cattle and goats, gather food such as honey, and hunt because these skills are key to survival in their environment. Vygotsky’s key insight was to view development as an apprenticeship in which children develop as they work with skilled adults, including teachers and parents, to learn what is valued in their culture.

For Piaget, information-processing theorists, and Vygotsky, children’s thinking becomes more sophisticated as they develop. Piaget explained this change as resulting from the more sophisticated knowledge that children actively construct; information- processing psychologists attribute it to improved mental hardware and mental software; Vygotsky claimed learning and thinking developed as a result of and as part of cultural context. What would these theorists say to Betty about Marcus’s good nature? Vygotsky would point out that Betty communicated key aspects of the culture to Marcus, which influenced his good nature. Neither Piaget nor information-processing theorists would have much to say because their theories do not handle personality issues very well. What about Marcus’s academic success? That’s a different story. Piaget would explain that all

information-processing theory Proposes that human cognition consists of mental hardware and mental software.

Information-processing theory helps explain how this girl learns, stores, and retrieves information as she is studying for an exam.

Ro b

M ar

m io

n/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Lev Vygotsky

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 17

children naturally want to understand their world; Marcus is simply unusually skilled in this regard. An information-processing psychologist would point to superior hardware and superior software as the keys to his academic success. Vygotsky would again empha- size Betty’s influence in cultural transmission.

The Ecological and Systems Approach Most developmentalists agree that the environment is an important force in many aspects of development. However, only ecological theories (which get their name from the branch of biology dealing with the relation of living things to their environment and to each other) have focused on the complexities of environments and their links to development. In ecological theory, human development is inseparable from the environmental contexts in which a person develops. The ecological approach proposes that all aspects of devel- opment are interconnected, much like the threads of a spider’s web, so that no aspect of development can be isolated from others and understood independently. An ecologi- cal theorist would emphasize that to understand why adolescents behave as they do, we need to consider the many different systems that influence them, including parents, peers, teachers, media, the neighborhood, and social policy.

We will consider two examples of the ecological and systems approach: Bronfen- brenner’s theory and the competence–environmental press framework.

Bronfenbrenner’s Theory The best-known advocate of the ecological approach was Urie Bronfenbrenner (1917– 2005), who proposed that the developing person is embedded in a series of complex and interactive systems. Bronfenbrenner (1995) divided the environment into the four levels shown in ◗ Figure 1.2: the microsystem, the mesosystem, the exosystem, and the macrosystem.

ecological theory Theory that views human development as inseparable from the environmental con- texts in which a person develops.

●◗ Figure 1.2 Bronfenbrenner’s ecological approach emphasizes the interaction across different systems in which people operate.

M esosystem

F ri

en d

s Sch o

o l

G ov

er n

m en

t an

d

s oc

ia l

p o

li cy

Parents’ social

network

Exosystem P

aren ts’ p

laces of em

p lo

y m

en t

Mac rosystem

H isto

rical

E

th n

ic g

ro u

p

Culture

even ts

Microsystem

Parents

Child

SOURCE: Kopp, C. B., Krakow, J. B. (Eds.). (1982). The Child: Development in a Social Context (p. 648). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Reprinted by permission of Pearson Education.

Urie Bronfenbrenner

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

18 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

The microsystem consists of the people and objects in an individual’s immediate environment. These are the people closest to a person, such as parents or siblings. Some people may have more than one microsystem. For example, a young child might have the microsystems of the family and of the day-care setting. A retired adult might have the microsystems of a spouse and close friends. As you can imagine, microsystems strongly influence development.

Microsystems themselves are connected to create the mesosystem. The mesosystem provides connections across microsystems because what happens in one microsystem is likely to influence others. Perhaps you’ve found that if you have a stressful day at work or school, you’re often grouchy at home. This indicates that your mesosystem is alive and well; your microsystems of home and work are interconnected emotionally for you.

The exosystem refers to social settings that a person may not experience firsthand but that still influence development. For example, changes in government policy regarding welfare may mean that economically disadvantaged children have fewer opportunities for enriched preschool experiences. Although the influence of the exosystem is indirect, its effects on human development can be quite strong.

The broadest environmental context is the macrosystem, the cultures and subcultures in which the microsystem, mesosystem, and exosystem are embedded. A mother, her workplace, her child, and the child’s school are part of a larger cultural setting, such as Chinese Americans living in San Francisco or Italian Americans living in Brooklyn. Members of these cultural groups share a common identity, a common heritage, and common values. The macrosystem evolves over time; each successive generation may develop in a unique macrosystem.

Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory emphasizes the many levels of influence on human development. People are affected directly by family members and friends and indi- rectly by social systems such as neighborhoods and religious institutions—which, in turn, are affected by the beliefs and heritage of one’s culture.

Competence–Environmental Press Theory Another view of the influence of environments on human development comes from Lawton and Nahemow’s (1973) competence–environmental press theory. According to this theory, people adapt most effectively when their competence, or abilities, match the environmental press, or the demands put on them by the environment. This theory was originally pro- posed to account for the ways in which older adults function in their environment, but it applies throughout the life span. For example, the match between an adult’s social skills and her work group’s demands can determine whether she is accepted by the group. As with Bronfenbrenner’s theory, competence–environmental press theory emphasizes that to understand people’s functioning, it is essential to understand the systems in which they live.

Ecological theorists would agree with learning theorists in telling Betty that the envi- ronment has been pivotal in her son’s amiable disposition and his academic achievements. However, the ecological theorist would insist that environment means much more than the reinforcements, punishments, and observations that are central to learning theory; such a theorist would emphasize the different levels of environmental influence on Mar- cus. Betty’s ability to balance home (microsystem) and work (mesosystem) so skillfully (which meant that she was usually in a good mood herself ) contributed positively to Mar- cus’s development, as did Betty’s membership in a cultural group (exosystem) that empha- sized the value of doing well in school. Marcus was also competent enough to handle the external demands (environmental press) put on him.

Life-Span Perspective, Selective Optimization with Compensation, and Life-Course Perspective Most of the theories of human development that we have considered so far pay little atten- tion to the adult years of the life span (Erikson’s theory is the main exception). Histori- cally, adulthood was downplayed, owing to the belief that it was a time when abilities had

competence People’s abilities.

environmental press The demands put on people by the environment.

exosystem Social settings that a person may not expe- rience firsthand but that still influence development.

macrosystem The cultures and subcultures in which the microsystem, mesosystem, and exosystem are embedded.

microsystem The people and objects in an individual’s immediate environment.

mesosystem Provides connections across microsystems because what happens in one microsystem is likely to influence others.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 19

reached a plateau (rather than continuing to develop) and that adulthood was followed by inevitable decline in old age. However, modern perspectives emphasize the importance of viewing human development as a lifelong process. These perspectives view development in terms of where a person has been and where he or she is heading.

Life-Span Perspective and Selective Optimization with Compensation We can only understand adults’ experiences by understanding their childhood and adoles- cence. Placing adults’ lives in this broader context is what the life-span perspective does.

According to the life-span perspective, human development is multiply determined and cannot be understood within the scope of a single framework. The basic premise of the life-span perspective is that aging is a lifelong process of growing up and growing old, beginning with conception and ending with death. No single period of a person’s life (such as childhood, adolescence, or middle age) can be understood apart from its origins and its consequences. To understand a specific period, we must know what came before and what is likely to come afterward (Riley, 1979). In addition, how one’s life is played out is affected by social, environmental, and historical change. Thus, the experiences of one generation may not be the same as those of another.

Paul Baltes (1939–2006) and colleagues provide many of the main approaches to human development from a life-span perspective (Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 2006) in a model that has influenced a wide range of research, especially on adult devel- opment and aging. They identify four key features of the life-span perspective as follows.

●● Multidirectionality Development involves both growth and decline; as people grow in one area, they may lose in another. For example, people’s vocabulary tends to increase throughout life, but their memory skills weaken.

●● Plasticity One’s capacity is not predetermined or carved in stone. Many skills can be learned or improved with practice, even in late life. For example, people can learn bet- ter ways to remember information, which may help them deal with declining memory. There are limits to the degree of potential improvement, though, as described in later chapters.

●● Historical context Each of us develops within a particular set of circumstances deter- mined by the historical time in which we are born and the culture in which we grow up. For example, living in a middle-class suburb in 1980s Atlanta has little in common with living in a poor Latino neighborhood in 2000s Texas.

●● Multiple causation Development reflects the biological, psychological, sociocultural, and life-cycle forces that we mentioned previously. For example, children’s success in school will depend on their heredity, their cognitive skills, their culture’s emphasis on achievement, and whether their parents are teenagers, young adults, or middle aged.

Taken together, the principles of the life-span perspective describe and explain the successful adaptation of people to the changes that occur with aging by proposing an interaction between three processes: selection, compensation, and optimization (Baltes, 1997; Baltes et al., 2006; Baltes & Carstensen, 1999; Baltes & Heydens-Gahir, 2003). Selec- tion processes serve to choose goals, life domains, and life tasks, whereas optimization and compensation concern maintaining or enhancing chosen goals. The basic assumption of the selective optimization with compensation (SOC) model is that the three processes form a system of behavioral action that generates and regulates development and aging.

As people mature and grow old, they select from a range of possibilities or opportu- nities. This selection occurs for two main reasons. Elective selection occurs when people reduce their involvement to fewer domains as a result of new demands or tasks, such as when a college student drops out of some organizations because of the amount of work required in the courses he is taking that term. Loss-based selection occurs when real or anticipated losses in personal or environmental resources cause people to reduce their involvement, such as when an older person stops going to church because he can no lon- ger drive. In either case, selection sometimes means continuing previous goals on a lesser scale or substituting with new goals.

life-span perspective View that human development is multiply determined and cannot be understood within the scope of a single framework.

Paul Baltes

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

selective optimization with compensation (SOC) model View that selection, optimization, and compensation form a system of behavioral action that generates and regulates develop- ment and aging.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

20 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Compensation occurs when people’s skills have decreased so they no longer function well in a particular domain. When people compensate, they search for an alternate way to accomplish the goal. For example, if an older adult can no longer drive because she has a broken leg, she might compensate by taking the bus. Sometimes, compensation requires learning a new skill; for example, an older adult experiencing short-term memory problems might compensate by learning to use a smartphone app for lists. Thus, compensation dif- fers from selection in that the task or goal is maintained but achieved through other means.

Optimization involves minimizing losses and maximizing gains. The main idea is to find the best match possible between one’s resources (biological, psychological, and sociocultural) and one’s desired goals. Because people cannot achieve optimal outcomes in everything, development becomes a dynamic process of selecting the right goals and compensating when possible to help maximize the odds of achieving them.

One can see the SOC model at work in many situations. For example, aging musicians who want to continue to play concerts may reduce the number of pieces they play ( selection), rehearse them more often (optimization), and sing them in a lower key (com- pensation). A college athlete who excels at ice hockey and baseball may decide to concen- trate on hockey (selection), work on training all year (optimization), and develop a wicked wrist shot to make up for a mediocre slap shot (compensation).

The life-span perspective and the SOC model have provided important approaches to the contemporary study of human development. The emphasis on the need for a using multidisciplinary approach and for recognizing many interactive forces will be developed throughout this text.

Life-Course Perspective Adults often describe their lives as a story that includes several key life events and transi- tions (e.g., going to school, getting a first job, getting married, having children). Such sto- ries show how people move through their lives and experience unique interactions of the four forces of development.

The life-course perspective describes the ways in which various generations experi- ence the biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces of development in their respec- tive historical contexts. Specifically, it lets researchers examine the effects of historical time on how people create their lives (Dannefer & Miklowski, 2006; Hagestad & Dannefer, 2001; Hareven, 1995, 2001; Mayer, 2009). A key feature of the life-course perspective is the dynamic interplay between the individual and society. This interplay creates three major dimensions, all involving timing and underlying the life-course perspective:

●● The individual timing of life events in relation to external historical events This dimen- sion addresses the question: How do people time and sequence their lives (e.g., getting a first job, having their first child, entering retirement) in the context of changing his- torical conditions (e.g., economic good times or recession)?

●● The synchronization of individual transitions with collective familial ones This dimen- sion addresses the question: How do people balance their own lives (e.g., work obliga- tions) with those of their family (e.g., children’s soccer games)?

●● The impact of earlier life events, as shaped by historical events, on subsequent ones This dimension addresses the question: How does experiencing an event earlier in life (e.g., a male turning 18 years old) at a particular point in history (e.g., when there is a military draft) affect one’s subsequent life (e.g., choosing a particular career)?

Research from the life-course perspective has shown that major life transitions such as marriage, childbearing, starting and ending a career, and completing one’s education occur at many different ages across people and generations. These differences first appear after adolescence, when people begin to have more control over the course of their lives. Research has also shown that life transitions are more continuous and multidirectional than previously thought. For example, completing an education was relegated to early adulthood in traditional models, yet current realities of returning adult students and life- long learning make this view obsolete. Finally, research shows that the various domains of

life-course perspective Describes the ways in which various generations experience the biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces of development in their respective historical contexts.

At age 93, cellist Pablo Casals said he still practiced three hours a day because he was “beginning to notice some improvement.”

G jo

n M

ili /T

he L

IF E

Pi ct

ur e

Co lle

ct io

n/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 21

people’s lives are highly interdependent; for example, the decision to have a child is often made in the context of where one is in one’s career and education.

The emphasis in the life-course perspective on interrelations between the individual and society with reference to historical time has made it a dominant view in the social sci- ences. In particular, this approach is useful in helping researchers understand how the vari- ous aspects of people’s experiences (work, family, education) interact to create unique lives.

Overall, life-span and life-cycle theories have greatly enhanced developmental theory by drawing attention to the role of aging in the broader context of human development (Gilleard & Higgs, 2016). These theories have played a major role in conceptualizing adult- hood and have greatly influenced the research we consider in Chapters 10 through 15. Theorists espousing a life-span or life-course perspective would tell Betty that Marcus will continue to develop throughout his adult years and that this developmental journey will be influenced by biopsychosocial forces, including his own family.

The Big Picture As summarized in ◗ Table 1.3, each of the theories provides ways of explaining how the biological, psychological, sociocultural, and life-cycle forces create human development. But because no single theory provides a complete explanation of all aspects of develop- ment, we must rely on the biopsychosocial framework to piece together an account based on many different theories. Throughout the remainder of this text, you will read about many theories that differ in focus and in scope. To help you understand them better, each theory will be introduced in the context of the issues that it addresses.

One criterion for a theory is that it be testable. The next section describes the meth- ods by which developmentalists conduct research and test their theories.

Table 1.3

Theoretical Perspectives on Human Development

PERSPECTIVE EXAMPLES MAIN IDEA

EMPHASES IN BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL FRAMEWORK

POSITIONS ON DEVELOPMENTAL ISSUES

Psychodynamic Erikson’s psychosocial theory

Personality develops through sequence of stages

Psychological, social, and life-cycle forces crucial; less emphasis on biological

Nature–nurture interac- tion, discontinuity, universal sequence but individual dif- ferences in rate

Learning Behaviorism (Watson, Skinner)

Environment controls behavior

In all theories, some empha- sis on biological and psycho- logical, major focus on social, little recognition of life cycle

In all theories, strongly nur- ture, continuity, and univer- sal principles of learning

Social learning theory (Bandura)

People learn through modeling and observing

Cognitive Piaget’s theory (and extensions)

Thinking develops in a sequence of stages

Main emphasis on biologi- cal and social forces, less on psychological, little on life cycle

Strongly nature, discontinu- ity, and universal sequence of stages

Information-processing theory

Thought develops by increases in efficiency at handling information

Emphasis on biological and psychological, less on social and life cycle

Nature–nurture interaction, continuity, individual differ- ences in universal structures

Vygotsky’s theory Development influenced by culture

Emphasis on psychological and social forces

Nature–nurture interac- tion, continuity, individual differences

[Continued on next page]

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

22 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

PERSPECTIVE EXAMPLES MAIN IDEA

EMPHASES IN BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL FRAMEWORK

POSITIONS ON DEVELOPMENTAL ISSUES

Ecological and Systems

Bronfenbrenner’s theory Developing person embedded in a series of interacting systems

Low emphasis on biological, moderate on psychologi- cal and life cycle, heavy on social

Nature–nurture interaction, continuity, context-specific

Competence– environmental press (Lawton and Nahemow)

Adaptation is opti- mal when ability and demands are in balance

Strong emphasis on bio- logical, psychological, and social, moderate on life cycle

Nature–nurture interaction, continuity, context-specific

Life-Span Perspective/SOC

Baltes’s life-span per- spective and selective optimization with com- pensation (SOC)

Development is multiply determined; optimiza- tion of goals

Strong emphasis on the interactions of all four forces; cannot consider any in isolation

Nature–nurture interaction, continuity and discontinu- ity, context-specific

Life-Course Perspective

Life-course theory Life-course transitions decreasingly tied to age; increased continuity over time; specific life paths across domains are interdependent

Strong emphasis on psychological, sociocultural, life cycle; less on biological

Nature–nurture interaction, continuity and discontinuity, context-specific

Theoretical Perspectives on Human Development (Continued)

Recall 1. _______________ organize knowledge to provide testable

explanations of human behaviors and the ways in which they change over time.

2. The _______________ perspective proposes that development is determined by the interaction of an internal maturational plan and external societal demands.

3. According to social cognitive theory, people learn from rein- forcements, from punishments, and through _______________.

4. Piaget’s theory and Vygotsky’s theory are examples of the _______________ perspective.

5. According to Bronfenbrenner, development occurs in the context of the _______________, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem.

6. A belief that human development is characterized by multidirectionality and plasticity is fundamental to the _______________ perspective.

Interpret ●● How are the information-processing perspective and Piaget’s

theory similar? How do they differ?

Apply ●● Using three different developmental theories, explain how

LeBron James or Lady Gaga achieved their success.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 1.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do scientists measure topics of interest in studying

human development?

●● What research designs are used to study human development?

●● How do researchers integrate results from multiple studies?

●● What ethical procedures must researchers follow?

●● How do investigators communicate results from research studies?

●● How does research affect public policy?

1.3 Doing Developmental Research

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 23

Leah and Joan are mothers of 10-year-old boys. Their sons have many friends, but the basis for the friendships is not obvious to the mothers. Leah believes “opposites attract”—children form friendships with peers who have complementary interests and abilities. Joan doubts this; her son seems to seek out other boys who are near clones of himself in their interests and abilities.

Suppose Leah and Joan know that you’re taking a course in human development, so they ask you to settle their argument. Leah believes complementary children are more often friends, whereas Joan believes similar children are more often friends. You know that research could show whose ideas are supported under which circumstances, but how?

Human development researchers start their search for knowledge by asking good questions such as these. Once the questions are framed, they make several important deci- sions as they prepare to study a topic. They need to decide how to measure the topic of interest, they design their study, they choose a method for studying development, and they decide whether their plan respects the rights of individuals participating in the research. In practice, all of these steps occur simultaneously; for example, the selection of a measure or method is done in the context of the rights of research participants. But for simplicity, we’ll describe each of these steps in sequence.

Measurement in Human Development Research The first step in doing developmental research is deciding how to measure the topic or behavior of interest. So the first step toward answering Leah and Joan’s question would be to decide how to measure friendships.

Human development researchers use one of four approaches: observing systemati- cally, using tasks to sample behavior, asking people for self-reports, and taking physiologi- cal measures.

Systematic Observation As the name implies, systematic observation involves watching people and carefully recording what they do or say. Two forms of systematic observation are common.

In naturalistic observation, people are observed as they behave spontaneously in a real-life situation. There’s a catch with observation, though. Researchers can’t keep track of everything that someone does, so beforehand they must decide what specific variables to record. For example, researchers studying friendship might decide to observe children as they start their first year in middle school (chosen because this is a time when many children will be making new friends). They could decide to record where children sit in the lunchroom and who talks to whom.

Structured observations differ from naturalistic observations in that the researcher creates a setting that is likely to bring out the behavior of interest. Structured observations are particularly useful for studying behaviors that are difficult to observe naturally, such as how people respond to emergencies. An investigator relying on natural observations to study people’s responses to emergencies wouldn’t make much progress with naturalistic observation because emergencies don’t occur at predetermined times and locations. How- ever, using a structured observation, an investigator might stage an emergency—perhaps cooperating with authorities to simulate an accident—to observe people’s responses.

Some behaviors are difficult for researchers to observe because they occur in private, not public, settings. For example, sexual activity tends to occur in private, where it is dif- ficult for investigators to observe unobtrusively. However, researchers could ask couples to come to the researcher’s laboratory, which might be furnished to resemble a typical bed- room. The researchers would then observe the friends’ activity by watching from another room, watching through a one-way mirror, or videotaping them. Later in this book, we will consider findings from research such as this that has greatly helped us understand human behavior, such as providing insights into how people provide consent for engaging in sexual activity.

systematic observation Watching people and carefully recording what they do or say.

naturalistic observation Observing people as they behave spontane- ously in a real-life situation.

structured observations Method in which the researcher creates a setting that is likely to bring out the behav- ior of interest.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

24 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Structured observations are valuable in enabling researchers to observe behavior(s) that would otherwise be difficult to study. But there are limits. For example, observing couples engaging in sexual activity in a mock bedroom has many artificial aspects to it: The couples are not in their rooms, they were told in general terms what to do, and they know they’re being observed. Any of these factors may cause couples to behave differently than they would in the real world. Researchers must be careful that their method does not make the behavior they are observing unnatural or unrealistic.

Sampling Behavior with Tasks When investigators can’t observe a behavior directly, an alternative is to create tasks that are thought to sample the behavior of interest. One task often used to measure older adults’ memory is “digit span”: Adults listen as a sequence of digits is presented aloud. After the last digit is presented, they try to repeat the digits in order.

Another example is shown in ◗ Figure 1.3. To study the ability to recognize emotions, the child has been asked to look at the photographs and point to the face that looks happy. A child’s answers on this sort of task are useful in determining his or her ability to recog- nize emotions. This approach is popular and convenient; however, a potential problem is that the task may not provide a realistic sample of the behavior of interest. For example, asking children to judge emotions from photographs may not be valid because it underes- timates what they do in real life. Why might this be the case? We mention several reasons on page 34, just before Test Yourself.

Self-Reports Self-reports represent a special case of using tasks to measure people’s behavior. Self-reports are people’s answers to questions about the topic of interest.

When questions are posed in written form, the self-report is a questionnaire; when questions are posed orally, the self-report is an interview. Either way, questions are created that probe different aspects of the topic of interest. For example, if you believe that adults are more often friends when they have interests in common, then you might tell your research participants the following:

Tom and Dave just met each other at work. Tom likes to read, plays the clarinet, and is not interested in sports; Dave likes to tweet his friends, enjoys tinkering with his car, and watches sports all weekend. Do you think Tom and Dave will become friends?

The participants would then decide, perhaps using a rating scale, whether the men are likely to become friends.

Self-reports are useful because they can lead directly to infor- mation on the topic of interest. They are also relatively conve- nient, particularly when they can be administered to groups of participants or administered online.

However, self-reports are not always a good measure of people’s behavior because not all answers are accurate. Why? When asked about past events, for example, people may not remember them accurately. For example, an older adult asked about adolescent friends may not remember those friendships well. Sometimes peo- ple answer incorrectly as a result of “response bias”: For many ques- tions, they are more likely to respond in a socially acceptable manner (e.g., they may be reluctant to admit they had no friends). As long as investigators remain aware of these potential biases, though, self- reports can be a valuable tool for research in human development.

Physiological Measures One less common but potentially powerful form of measurement is measuring people’s physiological responses. Earlier we saw that brain activity is used in neuroscience research to track certain behaviors, such as memory.

self-reports People’s answers to questions about the topic of interest.

●◗ Figure 1.3 In this example of sampling behavior with tasks, the child is to select the face that looks happy.

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 25

◗ Figure 1.4 shows an image that researchers might use in such research. Another measure is heart rate, which often slows down when people are paying close attention to something interesting. Consequently, researchers often measure heart rate to determine a person’s degree of attention. As another example, the hormone cortisol is often secreted in response to stress. By measuring cortisol levels in saliva, scientists can determine when people are experiencing stress.

As these examples suggest, physiological measures are usually specialized—they focus on a particular aspect of a person’s behavior (in these examples, memory, attention, and stress). What’s more, they’re often used with behaviorally oriented methods. A researcher studying stress might observe several people for overt signs of stress, ask parents/part- ners/friends to rate the target person’s stress, and measure cortisol in the target person’s saliva. If all three measures lead to the same conclusions about stress, then the researcher can be more confident about those conclusions.

As we have discussed, there are strengths and weaknesses in each of the four approaches to measurement. These are summarized in ◗ Table 1.4. Which approach a researcher takes will depend on which one best matches the research questions as well as the feasibility and appropriateness of use with particular research participants.

Reliability and Validity After researchers choose a method, they must show that it is both reliable and valid. The reliability of a measure is the extent to which it provides a consistent index of a character- istic. A measure of friendship, for example, is reliable if it consistently estimates a person’s friendship network each time you administer it. Reliability of a measure is essential, but it isn’t enough. A measure must also be valid to be useful.

The validity of a measure refers to whether it actually measures what researchers think it measures. For example, a measure of friendship is valid if it actually measures friendship and not, for example, popularity. Validity is often established by showing that the measure in question is closely related to another measure known to be valid.

Representative Sampling Valid measures also depend on the people who are tested. Researchers are usually interested in broad groups of people called populations. Examples of populations are all American 7-year-olds or all African American grandparents. Virtually all studies include only a sample of people, which is a subset of the population. Researchers must take care that their sample represents the population of interest because an unrepresentative sample

Think About It If you were studying middle-aged adults caring for their aging parents, what would be the relative advantages of systematic observation, sampling behavior with tasks, and self-reports?

●◗ Figure 1.4 Brain imaging techniques provide a physiological measure that helps researchers understand brain–behavior relations.

St eg

er ph

ot o/

Ph ot

ol ib

ra ry

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Table 1.4

Measuring Behaviors of Interest in Human Development Research

METHOD STRENGTH WEAKNESS

Systematic Observation

Naturalistic observation Captures people’s behavior in its natural setting Difficult to use with behaviors that are rare or that typically occur in private settings

Structured observation Can be used to study behaviors that are rare or that typically occur in private settings

May be invalid if structured setting distorts the behavior

Sampling Behavior with Tasks Convenient—can be used to study most behaviors

May be invalid if the task does not sample behavior as it occurs naturally

Self-Reports Convenient—can be used to study most behaviors

May be invalid because people answer incor- rectly (due to either forgetting or response bias)

Physiological Measures Provide a more direct measure of underlying behavior

Highly specific in what they measure and thus cannot be applied broadly

reliability The extent to which a measure provides a consistent index of a characteristic.

validity Whether a measure actually measures what researchers think it measures.

populations Broad groups of people that are of interest to researchers.

sample A subset of the population.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

26 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

can lead to invalid conclusions. For example, if a study of friendship in older adults tested only people who had no siblings, you would probably decide that this sample is not repre- sentative of the population of older adults and question its validity.

As you read on, you’ll soon discover that much of the research we describe was con- ducted with samples that consist mostly of middle-class European American people. Are these samples representative of all people in the United States? Of all people in the world? No. To make samples more representative, some U.S. federal agencies now require researchers to include certain groups (e.g., ethnic minorities, women, children) or explain in detail why they are not, a policy that has resulted in a broader view of developmental processes. But until we have representative samples in all developmental research, we can- not know whether a particular phenomenon applies only to the group studied or to people more generally.

General Designs for Research Having selected a way to measure the topic or behavior of interest, researchers embed this measure in a research design that yields useful, relevant results. Human development researchers rely on two primary designs in their work: correlational studies and experi- mental studies.

Correlational Studies In a correlational study, investigators look at relations between variables as they exist naturally in the world. Imagine a researcher who wants to test the idea that smart people have more friends. To find out, the researcher would measure two variables for each person—the number of friends the person has and the person’s intelligence—and then see whether the two variables are related.

The results of a correlational study are usually measured by calculating a correlation coefficient, which expresses the strength and direction of a relation between two variables. Correlations can range from −1.0 to 1.0. The correlation coefficient reflects one of three possible relations between intelligence and the number of friends:

●● People’s intelligence is unrelated to the number of friends they have, reflected in a cor- relation of 0.

●● People who are smart tend to have more friends than people who are not as smart. That is, more intelligence is associated with having more friends. In this case, the variables are positively related and the correlation is between 0 and 1.

●● People who are smart tend to have fewer friends than people who are not as smart. That is, more intelligence is associated with having fewer friends. In this case, the variables are negatively related and the correlation is between −1 and 0.

In interpreting a correlation coefficient, you need to consider both the sign and size of the correlation. The sign indicates the direction of the relation between variables: A positive sign means that larger values of one variable are associated with larger values of the second variable, whereas a negative sign means that larger values of one variable are associated with smaller values of the second variable.

The size or strength of a relation is measured by how much the correlation differs from 0, either positively or negatively. A correlation of 0.9 between intelligence and number of friends would indicate a very strong relation: Knowing a person’s intelligence, you could accurately predict how many friends the person has. If instead the correlation was only 0.3, then the link between intelligence and number of friends would be weaker: Although more intelligent people would have more friends on average, there would be many exceptions. Similarly, a correlation of −0.9 would indicate a strong negative relation between intelligence and num- ber of friends, whereas a correlation of −0.3 would indicate a weak negative relation.

Although a correlational study can determine whether variables are related, it doesn’t address the question of cause and effect between the variables. For example, a correlation of 0.7 between intelligence and number of friends indicates that people who are smarter

correlational study Investigation looking at relations between variables as they exist naturally in the world.

correlation coefficient An expression of the strength and direction of a relation between two variables.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 27

have more friends than people who are not as smart. This correlation has three possi- ble interpretations, shown in ◗ Figure 1.5: (1) Being smart causes people to have more friends; (2) having more friends causes people to be smarter; or (3) neither variable causes the other; instead, both intelligence and number of friends are caused by a third variable (such as parents who are supportive) that was not measured in the study. Any of these interpretations could be true, but they cannot be distinguished in a correlational study. Investigators who want to track down causes must resort to a different design, called an experimental study.

Experimental Studies In an experiment, investigators systematically manipulate the factor(s) that they think causes a particular behavior. The factor being manipulated is called the independent variable; the behavior being observed is called the dependent variable. In human develop- ment, an experiment requires that the investigator begin with one or more independent variables (usually treatments, interventions, experiences, or events) that are thought to affect the behavior of interest. People are then assigned randomly to conditions that differ in the amount of the independent variable. Finally, an appropriate measure (the depen- dent variable) is taken of all participants to see whether the treatment or treatments had the expected effect. Because each person has an equal chance of being assigned to each treatment condition (the definition of random assignment), any differences between the groups can be attributed to the differential treatment people received in the experiment rather than to other factors.

Suppose, for example, that an investigator believes that older adults perform better in a driving simulator when they are not talking on a mobile phone. The photo on page 28 shows how we might test this hypothesis. Older adults come to the laboratory equipped with a driving simulator. Some are randomly assigned to a condition in which they drive while talking on a mobile phone, but others are assigned to a condition in which they drive but do not talk on a mobile phone.

All participants drive under circumstances that are held as constant as possible— except for the presence or absence of talking on the mobile phone. If on average older adults drive better in the “no phone” condition than in the “mobile phone” condition, then the investigator could say with confidence that the mobile phone has a harmful effect on driving skill. Conclusions about cause and effect are possible because the direct manipula- tion occurred under controlled conditions.

Human development researchers usually conduct experiments in laboratory-like settings because this allows better control over the variables that may influence the out- come of the research. Thus, a shortcoming of laboratory experiments is that the behavior of interest is not studied in its natural setting. For example, driving in a simulator may

●◗ Figure 1.5 There are three basic interpretations of a correlation coefficient because there is no direct way to assess cause and effect.

The first variable causes the second variable. Being smart

Being smart

Having more friends

Having more friends

Parents who are warm and supportive

Children who have more friends

Children who are smart

The second variable causes the first variable.

Neither variable is caused by the other; both are caused by a third variable that was not measured in the study.

1

2

3

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

experiment A systematic way of manipulating the key factor(s) that the investigator thinks causes a particular behavior.

independent variable The factor manipulated in an experiment.

dependent variable The behavior being observed in an experi- ment, used to evaluate the impact of the independent variable.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

28 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

not be the same as driving on a highway. Consequently, there is always  the potential problem that the results may be invalid because they are artificial—specific to the laboratory setting and not representative of the behavior outside of the laboratory.

Each research design used by developmentalists has both strengths and weaknesses. There is no one best method. Conse- quently, no single investigation can definitively answer a ques- tion. Researchers rarely rely on one study or even one method to reach conclusions. Instead, they prefer to find converging evi- dence from as many different kinds of studies as possible.

Qualitative Studies Suppose you live near a children’s playground. Each day, you watch the children play various games with each other and on the swings and sliding board. Because you are interested in learning more about how children go about playing, you decide to watch more carefully. With the parents’ permission, you

video the children’s play each day for several weeks. You then watch the videos and notice whether there are specific patterns that emerge.

What you have done is to conduct one type of qualitative research, which involves gaining in-depth understanding of human behavior and what governs it. Because qualita- tive research typically involves intensive observation of behavior over extended periods of time, the need is for smaller but focused samples rather than large random samples. Frequently used techniques include video recording and detailed interviews, from which qualitative researchers categorize the data into patterns as the primary basis for organiz- ing and reporting results. In contrast, quantitative research relies on numerical data and statistical tests as the bases for reporting results.

Qualitative research can be conducted for its own sake, as a preliminary step, or as a complement to quantitative research. Research reports of qualitative research are usually richer and provide more details about the behavior being observed.

Designs for Studying Development Research in human development usually concerns differences or changes that occur over time. In these cases—in addition to deciding how to measure the behavior of inter- est and whether the study will be correlational or experimental—investigators must also choose one of three designs that allow them to examine development: longitudinal, cross- sectional, or sequential.

Longitudinal Studies In a longitudinal study, the same individuals are observed or tested repeatedly at different points in their lives. As the name implies, the longitudinal approach examines develop- ment over time. It is the most direct way to identify change. More important, it is the only way to determine the stability of behavior: Will characteristics such as aggression, dependency, or mistrust observed in infancy or early childhood persist into adulthood? Will a regular exercise program begun in middle age have benefits in later life? Does people’s intelligence remain the same or change across adulthood? Such questions can be explored only by testing people at one point in their development and then retesting them later.

The Spotlight on Research feature illustrates a longitudinal study that focuses on the stability of intelligence across the life span. As you read, pay close attention to the questions and how the researchers approached them. Doing so provides insight into the creative process of research and into the potential strengths and weaknesses of the study.

An experiment using a driving simulator to examine skills of older drivers. In this “no phone” condition, the driver is not talking on a mobile phone.

Pa ul

D oy

le /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

qualitative research Method that involves gaining in-depth understanding of human behavior and what governs it.

longitudinal study A research design in which the same individuals are observed or tested repeat- edly at different points in their lives.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 29

On ResearchSpotlight

The Stability of Intelligence from Age 11 to Age 90 Years

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? A long- standing issue in human development is the degree to which personal character- istics, such as intelligence, remain stable or change over one’s life. Ian Deary, Alison Pattie, and John Starr (2013) addressed this issue by examining the stability and validity of individual differences based on the same intelligence test (the Moray House Test) given to the same people at ages 11 and 90. This is one of the few studies of a personal characteristic over such a long period of time in the same people.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? The primary assessment of intelligence was the Moray House Test, which mainly tests verbal reasoning, along with some numerical and other items. Participants were also given the National Adult Reading Test, Mini-Mental State Examination (a screening test for dementia), Raven’s Progressive Matrices (a measure of nonverbal reasoning), and the Wechsler Logical Memory Test (a test of verbal recall of stories).

Who were the participants in the study? The participants were members of the 1921 Lothian (Scotland) Birth Cohort. In 1932, at roughly age 11, almost every member of the cohort was given the Moray House Test in school. Between 1999 and 2001, 550 survi- vors were tested between ages 78 and 80. In 2011 and 2012, 106 survivors were tested as close to their 90th birthday as possible. Because of the long period that had elapsed since initial testing, many of the original participants were unable to be retested due to death, mental or physical incapacity, or other reasons.

◗ Figure 1.6 depicts the various reasons why participants were not retested.

What was the design of the study? The study used a longitudinal design.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? All participants were provided infor- mation about the purpose of the study and the tests they would take. Parents provided informed consent for their children; each adult participant provided informed consent. Participants were screened for signs of mental impairment that may have interfered with either their ability to understand the

purpose of the study and provide consent or their ability to understand and complete the tasks.

What were the results? The correlation between raw scores on the Moray House Test at age 11 and age 90 was 0.67. Scores correlated 0.73 between ages 79 and 90. In other words, 11-year-olds with high scores tended to have high scores at 79 and 90. Importantly, scores on the Moray House Test correlated with the other cognitive measures, evidence of the Moray House Test’s validity. Although the sample showed evidence of several physical health problems, there was no evidence that mental ability in childhood predicts individual differences in norma- tive cognitive aging or with the likelihood of dementia.

What did the investigators conclude? Deary, Pattie, and Starr concluded that individual differences in intelligence in middle childhood as measured by the Moray House Test showed reasonably high stability at age 90. In other words, smart 11-year-olds tend to be smart 90-year-olds and not-so- smart 11-year-olds tend to be not-so-smart 90-year-olds.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Because the sample included only Scottish participants, it would be necessary to study people from other cultures to find out whether the results generalize across cultures. In addition, the use of only one test in the assessment of the participants at age 11 provides a narrow view of intelligence.

SOURCE: Deary, I. J., Pattie, A., & Starr, J. M. (2013). The stability of intelligence from age 11 to age 90 years: The Lothian birth cohort of 1921. Psychological Science, 24, 2361–2368. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486487.

229 Participants Lost: 88 Died 60 Withdrew 54 Were not well enough/no longer eligible 9 Lost contact 18 Were lost for other reasons

86 Participants Lost: 55 Died 7 Withdrew 19 Were not well enough/no longer eligible 5 Lost contact

95 Participants Lost: 47 Died 6 Withdrew 32 Were not well enough/no longer eligible 1 Lost contact 9 Were lost for other reasons

Wave 1 n = 550; Mean age ~ 79

Wave 2 n = 321; Mean age ~ 83

Wave 3 n = 235; Mean age ~ 87

Wave 4 n = 129; Mean age ~ 90

(+11 with dementia)

●◗ Figure 1.6 Number and mean age of participants and causes of attrition at each wave of testing in the Lothian Birth Cohort of 1921.

NOTE: “Withdrew” refers to participants who did not wish to continue to participate. “Not well enough/no longer eligible” refers to participants who withdrew for health reasons, including being too frail to travel, and participants who had severe visual impairments or dementia (except at Wave 4, when 11 people with dementia were examined at home by a physician, but not given tests of cognitive functioning). “Other reasons” for leaving the study include, for example, living too far away to travel to the testing site, caring for a spouse, refusal by the participant’s general medical practitioner, and missing an appointment.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

30 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

As powerful as it is in identifying what does—or does not—change over time, the longitudinal approach has disadvantages that frequently offset its strengths. One is cost: The expense of keeping up with a large sample of individuals can be staggering. A related problem is the need to keep the sample together over the course of the research. Main- taining contact with people over years or decades can be challenging. Even among those who do not move away, some lose interest and choose not to continue; others may die. These “dropouts” often differ significantly from their peers, and this fact may also distort the study’s outcome. For example, a study may seem to show that a group of older adults shows intellectual stability late in life. What may have happened, however, is that those who found earlier testing most difficult quit the study and thereby raised the group aver- age on the next round. Even when the sample remains constant, taking the same test many times may make people “test-wise.” Improvement over time may be attributed to develop- ment when it actually stems from practice with a particular test. Changing the test from testing session to testing session solves the practice problem but raises a new question: how to compare responses to different tests.

Because of these and other problems with the longitudinal method, human develop- ment researchers often use cross-sectional studies instead.

Cross-Sectional Studies In a cross-sectional study, developmental differences are identified by testing people of different ages. Development is charted by noting the differences between individuals of different ages at the same point in calendar time. The cross-sectional approach avoids the problems of repeated testing and the costs of tracking a sample over time. But cross- sectional research has its own weaknesses. Because people are tested at only one point in their lives, we learn nothing about the continuity of development. Consequently, we can- not tell whether an aggressive 14-year-old remains aggressive at age 30 because the person would be tested either at age 14 or age 30 but not both.

Cross-sectional studies are also affected by cohort effects, meaning that differences between age groups (cohorts) may result as much from environmental events as from devel- opmental processes. In a cross-sectional study, we typically compare people from two or more age groups. Differences that we find are attributed to the difference in age, but this needn’t be the case. Why? The cross-sectional study assumes that when the older people were younger, they resembled the people in the younger age group. This isn’t always true. Some factor other than difference in age may be responsible for differences between the groups. Suppose that young adults were found to be more imaginative than middle-aged adults. Should we conclude that imagination declines between these ages? Not necessar- ily. Perhaps a new curriculum to nourish creativity was introduced after the middle-aged adults completed school. Because the younger adults experienced the curriculum but the middle-aged adults did not, the difference between them is difficult to interpret.

Sequential Studies Some researchers use a more complex approach, called a sequential design, that is based on cross-sectional and longitudinal designs. A sequential design begins with a cross- sectional or longitudinal design. At some regular interval, the researcher then adds more cross-sectional or longitudinal studies, resulting in a sequence of these studies. For exam- ple, suppose a researcher wants to learn whether adults’ memory ability changes with age. One way to do this would be to start with a typical cross-sectional study in which 60- and 75-year-olds are tested. Then the two groups would be retested every three years, creating two separate longitudinal studies.

Although sequential designs are rare because they are so expensive, they are powerful because they allow researchers to distinguish age-related change from other effects (e.g., cohort effects, participant dropout). We will encounter examples of sequential designs when we consider some of the large studies examining the normal processes of aging in Chapters 10 and 14.

cross-sectional study Study in which developmental differences are identified by testing people of different ages.

cohort effects A problem with cross-sectional designs in which differences between age groups (cohorts) may result from environmental events not from developmental processes.

sequential design Developmental research design based on cross-sectional and longitudinal designs.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 31

◗ Table 1.5 summarizes the strengths and weaknesses of the general research designs and the developmental designs. You’ll read about each of these designs throughout this book, although the two cross-sectional types (cross-sectional experimental and cross- sectional correlational) occur more frequently than the other combinations of general and developmental designs. Why? The relative ease of conducting cross-sectional studies more than compensates for their limitations.

Integrating Findings from Different Studies Several times in the past few pages, we’ve emphasized the value of using different methods to study the same phenomenon. The advantage of this approach is that conclusions are most convincing when the results are consistent regardless of method.

However, sometimes findings are inconsistent. For example, suppose many research- ers find that people often share highly personal information with friends on social media, some researchers find that people share occasionally with friends, and a few researchers find that people never share with friends. What should we conclude? Meta-analysis allows researchers to synthesize the results of many quantitative studies to estimate actual rela- tions between variables (Schmidt & Hunter, 2014). In conducting a meta-analysis, investi- gators find all studies published on a topic over a substantial period of time (e.g., 10 to 20 years) and then record and analyze the results and important methodological variables.

The usefulness of meta-analysis is illustrated in a study by Kojima and colleagues (2016). After identifying 5,145 potential studies on the association between frailty and the quality of life of older adults living in the community, they focused on 11 cross-sectional and two longi- tudinal studies that used comparable measures. This meta-analysis indicated that frail older adults had lower quality of life. As a result, Kojima and colleagues suggest that interventions aimed at helping older adults deal with frailty may also improve their quality of life.

meta-analysis A tool that enables researchers to synthe- size the results of many studies to estimate relations between variables.

Table 1.5

Designs Used in Human Development Research

TYPE OF DESIGN DEFINITION STRENGTHS WEAKNESSES

General Designs

Correlational Observe variables as they exist in the world and determine their relations

Behavior is measured as it occurs naturally

Cannot determine cause and effect

Experimental Manipulate independent vari- able and determine effect on dependent variable

Control of variables allows con- clusions about cause and effect

Work is often laboratory-based, which can be artificial

Developmental Designs

Longitudinal One group of people is tested repeatedly as they develop

Only way to chart an individual’s development and look at the sta- bility of behavior over time

Expensive, participants drop out, and repeated testing can distort performance

Cross-sectional People of different ages are tested at the same time

Convenient—solves all problems associated with longitudinal studies

Cannot study stability of behav- ior; cohort effects complicate interpretation of differences between groups

Sequential Multiple groups of people are tested over time, based on either multiple longitudinal or cross- sectional designs

Best way to address limitation of single longitudinal and cross- sectional designs

Very expensive and time con- suming; may not completely solve limitations of longitudinal and cross-sectional designs

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

32 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

Meta-analysis is a particularly powerful tool because it allows scientists to determine whether a finding generalizes across many studies. In addition, meta-analysis can reveal the impact of different methods on results and further the development of specific theories of behavior.

Conducting Research Ethically Choosing a good research design involves more than just selecting a method. Researchers must determine whether the methods they plan to use protects the rights of people who participate. To verify that every research project incorporates these protections, local panels of experts and community representatives, called the Institutional Review Board, evaluate proposed studies before any data are collected. Only with the approval of this panel can scientists begin their study. If  the review panel objects to some aspects of the proposed study, then the researcher must revise those aspects and present them anew for the panel’s approval.

To guide review panels, professional organizations (e.g., the Amer- ican Psychological Association and government agencies (e.g., the National Institutes of Health) have codes of ethical conduct that specify the rights of research participants as well as procedures to protect these participants. The following essential guidelines are included in all of these codes:

●● Minimize risks to research participants Use methods that have the least potential for causing harm or stress for research participants. During the research, monitor the pro- cedures to ensure avoidance of any unforeseen stress or harm.

●● Describe the research to potential participants so that they can determine whether they want to participate Prospective participants must be told the purpose of the project, what they will be asked to do, whether there are any risks or potential harm or any benefits they may receive, that they are free to discontinue participation at any time without penalty, and that, after they have participated, the project will be described. After the study has been explained, each participant signs a document stating that he or she understands what he or she will do in the study. Special caution must be exercised in obtaining consent for the participation of children and adoles- cents, as well as people who have conditions that affect intellectual functioning (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, severe head injury). In these cases, consent from a parent, a legal guardian, or another responsible person—in addition to consent of the participant— is necessary.

●● Avoid deception; if participants must be deceived, provide a thorough explanation of the true nature of the experiment as soon as possible Providing complete information about a study in advance sometimes biases or distorts a person’s responses. Consequently, investigators may provide participants with partial information about the study or even mislead them about its true purpose. As soon as it is feasible—typically, just after the experiment—any false information that was given to research participants must be cor- rected and the reasons for the deception must be provided.

●● Results should be anonymous or confidential Research results should be anonymous, which means that people’s data cannot be linked to their name. When anonymity is not possible, research results should be confidential, which means that the identity of participants is known only to the investigator(s) conducting the study.

Conducting research ethically is an obligation of every investigator. If you con- duct a project, you should submit your procedures for review. If you are a participant in someone else’s project, make sure you are given complete information and read it thoroughly.

Informed consent is a necessary aspect of any research effort in human development.

A m

y Et

ra /P

ho to

E di

t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 33

Communicating Research Results When the study is complete and the data have been analyzed, researchers write a report of their work that describes what they did and why, their results, and the meanings of their results. The researchers will submit the report to one of several scientific journals that specialize in human development research. Some of these are Child Development, Devel- opmental Psychology, Psychology and Aging, and the Journals of Gerontology. If the journal editor accepts the report, it will appear in the journal, where other human development researchers can learn of the results.

These reports of research are the basis for virtually all the information we present in this book. You have already encountered many citations of research in the format of names in parentheses, followed by a date, like this:

(Smith & Jones, 2015)

This indicates the last name(s) of the person(s) who did the research and the year in which the research was published. By looking in the Reference section at the end of the book, which is organized alphabetically by the first author’s last name, you can find the title of the article and the journal in which it was published.

All of these steps in research may seem tedious and involved to you. However, for a human development researcher, much of the fun of research is planning a study that no one has done before and that will provide information useful to other specialists. This is one of the most creative and challenging parts of human develop- ment research.

Applying Research Results: Social Policy Some people question whether research really matters. Actually, research on human devel- opment has a strong influence on policy makers and politicians. For example, every state in the United States and many countries around the world have laws against child abuse and laws that govern child labor practices. Many countries have laws setting minimum ages for certain activities, such as consuming alcohol, voting, and driving. Some states in the United States are changing the way older drivers are screened when they renew their driver’s licenses. Human development research played a role in establishing all of these laws and regulations.

Other examples of how developmental research affects social policy include the elimi- nation of mandatory retirement, the Americans with Disabilities Act, many educational reform laws, indices courts use to decide whether an adolescent offender should be tried as a juvenile or an adult, and whether a person is mentally competent. Clearly, the research done by developmentalists influences many aspects of daily life that are governed by laws and societal rules.

At several points in the text, we will describe important connections between human development research and social policy. As you will see, these connections are broad- ranging and include areas that you may take for granted. For example, lead-based paint and lead water pipes can no longer be used in the United States, mainly because research by developmentalists showed that infants and young children who were exposed to lead (and who sometimes ate paint chips when they flaked off or drank water containing lead) suffered brain damage and learning problems. Research on human development not only provides many insights into what makes people tick but also can provide ways to improve the quality of life.

However, the views of scientists, ethicists, public citizens, and government sometimes collide in ways that result in significant debate concerning research. Such is the case with stem cell research, a hotly debated topic. We will consider controversial topics such as this throughout this text.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

SUMMARY

On page 24, we invited you to consider why asking children to judge emotions from photos may not be valid. Children’s judgments of the emotions depicted in photographs may be less accu- rate than they would be in real life because in real life (1) facial features are usually moving—not still as in the photographs—and movement may be one of the clues children naturally use to judge emotions; (2) facial expressions are often accompanied by sounds, and children use both sight and sound to judge emotions; and (3) children most often judge facial expressions of peo- ple they know (parents, siblings, peers, teachers), and knowing the “usual” appearance of a face may help children judge emotions accurately.

Problems with Using Photographs to Measure Understanding of Emotions

●◗ Figure 1.7

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

1. In _______________, people are observed as they behave spontaneously in a real-life setting.

2. A(n) _______________ is a group of individuals thought to be representative of some larger population of interest.

3. The _______________ variable is measured in an experiment to evaluate the impact of the variable that was manipulated.

4. Problems of longitudinal studies include the length of time to complete the work, loss of research participants over time, and _______________.

5. Human development researchers must submit their plans for research to a review board that determines whether the research _______________.

Interpret ●● How could a longitudinal design be used to test Piaget’s theory?

Apply ●● While at work, you notice that some people advance faster

in their careers than other people do. How could you deter- mine what factors might be responsible for or related to such success?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 1.3

34 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

1.1 Thinking About Development

What fundamental issues of development have scholars addressed throughout history?

●● Three main issues are prominent in the study of human development. The nature–nurture issue involves the degree to which genetics and the environment influence human development. Researchers view nature and nurture as mutually interactive influences; development is shaped by both. The continuity–discontinuity issue concerns whether the same explanations (continuity) or different explana- tions (discontinuity) must be used to explain changes in people over time. Continuity approaches emphasize quantitative change; discontinuity approaches empha- size qualitative change. In the issue of universal versus

context-specific development, the question is whether development follows the same general path in all people or fundamentally depends on the sociocultural context.

What are the basic forces in the biopsychosocial framework? how does the timing of these forces make a difference in their impact?

●● Development is based on the combined impact of four primary forces. Biological forces include all genetic and health-related factors that affect development. Many of these biological forces are determined by our genetic code.

●● Psychological forces include all internal cognitive, emo- tional, perceptual, and personality factors that influence development. Collectively, psychological forces explain the most noticeable differences in people.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T 35

●● Sociocultural forces include interpersonal, societal, cul- tural, and ethnic factors that affect development. Culture consists of the knowledge, attitudes, and behavior associ- ated with a group of people. Overall, sociocultural forces provide the context or backdrop for development.

●● Life-cycle forces provide a context for understanding how people perceive their current situation and what effects it has on them.

●● The biopsychosocial framework emphasizes that the four forces are mutually interactive; development cannot be understood by examining the forces in isolation. Further- more, the same event can have different effects, depending on when it happens.

how does neuroscience enhance our understanding of human development?

●● Neuroscience is the study of the brain and the nervous system, especially in terms of brain–behavior relationships. Identifying patterns of brain activity helps demonstrate how developmental forces interact.

1.2 Developmental Theories

What is a developmental theory?

●● Developmental theories organize knowledge so as to pro- vide testable explanations of human behaviors and the ways in which they change over time. Current approaches to developmental theory focus on specific aspects of behav- ior. At present, there is no single unified theory of human development.

how do psychodynamic theories account for development?

●● Psychodynamic theories propose that behavior is deter- mined by the way people deal with conflicts they face at different ages. Erikson proposed a life-span theory of psy- chosocial development consisting of eight universal stages, each characterized by a particular struggle.

What is the focus of learning theories of development?

●● Learning theory focuses on the development of observable behavior. Operant conditioning is based on the notions of reinforcement, punishment, and environmental control of behavior. Social learning theory proposes that people learn by observing and interpreting others’ behavior.

how do cognitive-developmental theories explain changes in thinking?

●● Cognitive-developmental theory focuses on thought pro- cesses. Piaget proposed a four-stage universal sequence based on the notion that throughout development, people create theories to explain how the world works. Accord- ing to information-processing theory, people deal with information like a computer does; development consists of increased efficiency in handling information. Vygotsky emphasized the influence of culture on development.

What are the main points in the ecological and systems approach?

●● Bronfenbrenner proposed that development occurs in the context of several interconnected systems of increasing complexity. The competence–environmental press theory postulates that there is a “best fit” between a person’s abili- ties and the demands of the person’s environment.

What are the major tenets of life-span and life-course theories?

●● According to the life-span perspective, human development is characterized by four critical developmental forces: mul- tidirectionality, plasticity, historical context, and multiple causation.

●● Selective optimization with compensation refers to the developmental trends to focus one’s efforts and abilities in successively fewer domains as one ages and to acquire ways to compensate for normative losses.

●● The life-course perspective refers to understanding human development within the context of the historical time period in which a generation develops, which creates unique sets of experiences.

1.3 Doing Developmental Research

how do scientists measure topics of interest in studying human development?

●● Research typically begins by determining how to mea- sure the topic of interest. Systematic observation involves recording people’s behavior as it takes place, in either a nat- ural environment (naturalistic observation) or a structured setting (structured observation). Researchers sometimes create tasks to obtain samples of behavior. In self-reports, people answer questions posed by the experimenter. Physi- ological measures provide a way to examine body–behavior relationships.

●● Researchers must determine that their measures are reliable and valid; they must also obtain a sample representative of a larger population.

What research designs are used to study human development?

●● In correlational studies, investigators examine relations among variables as they occur naturally. This relation is often measured by a correlation coefficient, which can vary from −1 (strong inverse relation) to 0 (no relation) to +1 (strong positive relation). Correlational studies cannot determine cause and effect, so researchers do experimental studies in which an independent variable is manipulated and the impact of this manipulation on a dependent vari- able is recorded. Experimental studies allow conclusions about cause and effect, but the required strict control of other variables often makes the situation artificial. The best approach is to use both experimental and correla- tional studies to provide converging evidence. Qualitative

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

36 C h A p T e R 1 : T H E S T U D Y O F H U M A N D E V E L O P M E N T

research permits more in-depth analysis of behavior and is often used as a preliminary step for, or in conjunction with, quantitative research.

●● To study development, some researchers use a longitudinal design in which the same people are observed repeatedly as they grow. This approach provides evidence concerning actual patterns of individual growth but is time-consuming, some people drop out of the project, and repeated testing can affect performance.

●● An alternative, the cross-sectional design, involves testing people of different ages. This design avoids the problems of the longitudinal design but provides no information about individual growth. Also, what appear to be age differences may be cohort effects. Because neither design is problem- free, the best approach involves using both to provide con- verging evidence.

●● Sequential designs are based on multiple longitudinal or cross-sectional designs.

how do researchers integrate results from multiple studies?

●● Meta-analysis provides a way for researchers to look for trends across multiple studies to estimate the relations among variables.

What ethical procedures must researchers follow?

●● Planning research also involves selecting methods that preserve the rights of research participants. Experiment- ers must minimize the risks to participants, describe the research so that candidates can make an informed deci- sion about participating, avoid deception, and keep results anonymous or confidential.

how do investigators communicate results from research studies?

●● Once research data are collected and analyzed, investiga- tors publish the results in scientific outlets such as journals and books. Such results form the foundation of knowledge about human development.

how does research affect public policy?

●● Research results are sometimes used to inform and shape public policy. The ban on lead-based paint is an example.

1.1 1. genetics 2. continuity 3. Biological 4. brain–behavior 1.2 1. Theories 2. psychosocial 3. observing others 4. cognitive- developmental 5. microsystem 6. life-span 1.3 1. naturalistic observation 2. sample 3. dependent 4. influence of repeated testing on a person’s performance 5. preserves the rights of research participants

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

human development (5) nature–nurture issue (6) continuity–discontinuity issue (6) universal and context-specific

development issue (7) biological forces (7) psychological forces (7) sociocultural forces (7) life-cycle forces (7) biopsychosocial framework (7) neuroscience (10) theory (12) psychodynamic theories (12) psychosocial theory (12) epigenetic principle (13) operant conditioning (13) reinforcement (13)

punishment (14) imitation or observational learning (14) self-efficacy (14) information-processing theory (16) ecological theory (17) microsystem (18) mesosystem (18) exosystem (18) macrosystem (18) competence (18) environmental press (18) life-span perspective (19) selective optimization with compensation

(SOC) model (19) life-course perspective (20) systematic observation (23) naturalistic observation (23)

structured observations (23) self-reports (24) reliability (25) validity (25) populations (25) sample (25) correlational study (26) correlation coefficient (26) experiment (27) independent variable (27) dependent variable (27) qualitative research (28) longitudinal study (28) cross-sectional study (30) cohort effects (30) sequential design (30) meta-analysis (31)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G W

Im ag

es /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

39

2Biological Foundations: Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth

If you ask parents to name the most memorable experiences of their lives, many immediately men- tion the events associated with the birth of their chil-

dren. From the initial exciting news that a woman is

pregnant through birth 9 months later, pregnancy

and birth evoke awe and wonder.

The period before birth is the foundation for all

human development and the focus of this chapter.

Pregnancy begins when egg and sperm cells unite

and exchange hereditary material. In the first sec-

tion, you’ll see how this exchange takes place and, in

the process, learn about inherited factors that affect

development. The second section of the chapter

traces the events that transform sperm and egg into

a living, breathing human being. You’ll learn about

the timetable that governs development before birth

and, along the way, get answers to common ques-

tions about pregnancy. We talk about some of the

problems that can occur during development before

birth in the third section of the chapter. The last section focuses on birth and

the newborn baby. You’ll find out how an expectant mother can prepare for

birth and what labor and delivery are like.

2.1 In the Beginning: 23 Pairs of Chromosomes 40 Mechanisms of Heredity 40 Genetic Disorders 43 Heredity, Environment, and Development 45

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT Ben and Matt Pick Their Niches 50

2.2 From Conception to Birth 51 Period of the Zygote (Weeks 1–2) 51

WHAT DO YOU THINK? Conception in the 21st Century 52

Period of the Embryo (Weeks 3–8) 53 Period of the Fetus (Weeks 9–38) 55

2.3 Influences on Prenatal Development 57 General Risk Factors 57 Teratogens: Drugs, Diseases, and Environmental

Hazards 59 Drugs 59 How Teratogens Influence Prenatal Development 62 Prenatal Diagnosis and Treatment 64

2.4 Labor and Delivery 67 Stages of Labor 67 Approaches to Childbirth 68 Adjusting to Parenthood 69

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Links Between Maternal Depression and Children’s Behavior Problems 70

Birth Complications 71 Infant Mortality 72

    Summary 74

    Key Terms 76

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

40 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Leslie and Glenn are excited at the thought of starting their own family. At the same time, they’re nervous because Leslie’s grandfather had sickle-cell disease and died when he was just 20 years old. Leslie is terrified that her baby may inherit the disease that killed her grand- father. She and Glenn wish that someone could reassure them that their baby will be okay.

How can we reassure Leslie and Glenn? For starters, we need to know more about sickle- cell disease. Red blood cells carry oxygen and carbon dioxide to and from the body. When a person has sickle-cell disease, the red blood cells are long and curved like a sickle. These stiff, misshapen cells cannot pass through small capillaries, so oxygen cannot reach all parts of the body. The trapped sickle cells also block the way of white blood cells that are the body’s natural defense against bacteria. As a result, many people with sickle-cell dis- ease are often tired, may experience acute pain for hours or days, and are prone to infec- tions. About 10% of people with the disease die by age 20 and 50% die by age 50 (Kumar et al., 2010). Sickle-cell disease is inherited, and because Leslie’s grandfather had the disor- der, it runs in her family. Will Leslie’s baby inherit the disease? To answer that question, we need to examine the mechanisms of heredity.

Mechanisms of Heredity At conception, egg and sperm unite to create a new organism that incorporates some characteristics of each parent. Each egg and sperm cell has 23 chromosomes, threadlike structures in the nucleus that contain genetic material. When a sperm penetrates an egg, their chromosomes combine to produce 23 pairs of chromosomes. The first 22 pairs of chromosomes are called autosomes. The 23rd pair determines the sex of the child, so these are known as the sex chromosomes. When the 23rd pair consists of an X and a Y chro- mosome, the result is a boy; two X chromosomes produce a girl.

chromosomes Threadlike structures in the nuclei of cells that contain genetic material.

autosomes First 22 pairs of chromosomes.

sex chromosomes 23rd pair of chromosomes; these determine the sex of the child.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are chromosomes and genes? How do they carry

hereditary information from one generation to the next?

●● What are common problems involving chromosomes, and what are their consequences?

●● How is children’s heredity influenced by the environment in which they grow up?

2.1 In the Beginning: 23 Pairs of Chromosomes

Red blood cells like the one on the left carry oxygen throughout the body. However, in sickle-cell disease, sickle-shaped cells like the one on the right cannot pass through the body’s small blood vessels.

Th e

Sc ie

nc e

Pi ct

ur e

Co m

pa ny

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 41

●◗ Figure 2.1 DNA is organized in a double helix, with strands of phosphates and sugars linked by nucleotide bases.

G

C

C

T

T

T

T

G

G

G

G

G

G

A

A

A

A

TA

A

C

C

C

C

C

G

T

G

G

G

A

C

C

C

T

G

C

G

A

T

C

T

G

C

Nucleotide bases (A = Adenine, T = Thymine, G = Guanine, C = Cytosine)

Strands of phosphates and sugars

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Each chromosome actually consists of one molecule of deoxyribonucleic acid— DNa for short. To understand the structure of DNA, imagine four different colors of beads placed on two strings. The strings complement each other precisely: Wherever a red bead appears on one string, a blue bead appears on the other; wherever a green bead appears on one string, a yellow one appears on the other. DNA is organized this way, except that the four colors of beads are four different chemical compounds: ade- nine, thymine, guanine, and cytosine. The strings, which are made up of phosphates and sugars, wrap around each other, creating the double helix shown in ◗ Figure 2.1.

The order in which the chemical compound “beads” appear is a code that causes the cell to create specific amino acids, proteins, and enzymes—important biological build- ing blocks. For example, three consecutive thymine “beads” make up the instruction to create the amino acid phenylalanine. Each group of compounds that provides a specific set of biochemical instructions is a gene. Thus, genes are the functional units of heredity because they determine the production of chemical substances that are, ultimately, the basis for all human characteristics and abilities.

Altogether, a person’s 46 chromosomes include roughly 20,500 genes. Chromosome 1 has the most genes (nearly 3,000), and the Y chromosome has the fewest (just over 200). Most of these genes are the same for all people—fewer than 1% of genes cause differences between people (Human Genome Project, 2003). Through biochemical instructions that are coded in DNA, genes regulate the development of all human characteristics and abil- ities. The complete set of genes makes up a person’s heredity and is known as the person’s genotype. Genetic instructions, in conjunction with environmental influences, produce a phenotype, an individual’s physical, behavioral, and psychological features.

How do genetic instructions produce the misshapen red blood cells of sickle-cell dis- ease? Genes come in different forms that are known as alleles. For example, in the case of red blood cells, two alleles can be present on chromosome 11. One allele has instructions for normal red blood cells; another allele has instructions for sickle-shaped red blood cells. The alleles in the pair of chromosomes are sometimes the same, which is known as being homozygous. The alleles sometimes differ, which is known as being heterozygous. Leslie’s baby would be homozygous if it had two alleles for normal cells or two alleles for sickle-shaped cells. The baby would be heterozygous if it had one allele of each type.

deoxyribonucleic acid (DNa) Molecule composed of four nucleotide bases that is the biochemical basis of heredity.

gene Group of nucleotide bases that provides a specific set of biochemical instructions.

genotype Person’s hereditary makeup.

phenotype Physical, behavioral, and psychological features that result from the interaction between one’s genes and the environment.

alleles Variations of genes.

homozygous When the alleles in a pair of chromosomes are the same.

heterozygous When the alleles in a pair of chromosomes differ from each other.

Humans have 23 pairs of chromosomes, including 22 pairs of autosomes and 1 pair of sex chromosomes.

Bi op

ho to

A ss

oc ia

te s/

Sc ie

nc e

So ur

ce

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

42 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

How does a genotype produce a phenotype? With sickle-cell disease, how do geno- types lead to specific kinds of blood cells? The answer is simple if a person is homozygous. When both alleles are the same—and therefore have chemical instructions for the same phenotype—that phenotype usually results. If Leslie’s baby had an allele for normal red blood cells on both of its 11th chromosomes, then the baby would almost be guaranteed to have normal cells. If, instead, the baby had two alleles for sickle-shaped cells, then it would almost certainly suffer from the disease.

When a person is heterozygous, the process is more complex. Often one allele is dominant, which means that its chemical instructions are followed, while those of the other recessive allele are ignored. In sickle-cell disease, the allele for normal cells is dominant and the allele for sickle-shaped cells is recessive. This is good news for Leslie: As long as either she or Glenn contributes the allele for normal red blood cells, their baby will not develop sickle-cell disease.

◗ Figure 2.2 summarizes what we’ve learned about sickle-cell disease: A denotes the allele for normal blood cells, and a denotes the allele for sickle-shaped cells. Depending on the alleles in Leslie’s egg and in the sperm that fertilizes that egg, three outcomes are possible. Only if the baby inherits two recessive alleles for sickle-shaped cells is it likely to develop sickle-cell disease. But this is unlikely in Glenn’s case: He is positive that no one in his family has had sickle-cell disease, so he almost certainly has the allele for normal blood cells on both of the chromosomes in his 11th pair.

Even though Glenn’s sperm will carry the gene for normal red blood cells, this doesn’t guarantee that their baby will be healthy. Why? Sometimes one allele does not dominate another completely, a situation known as incomplete dominance. In incomplete domi- nance, the phenotype that results often falls between the phenotype associated with either allele. This is the case for the genes that control red blood cells. Individuals with one domi- nant and one recessive allele have sickle-cell trait: In most situations they have no problems, but when seriously short of oxygen, they suffer a temporary, relatively mild form of the dis- ease. Thus, sickle-cell trait is likely to appear when the person exercises vigorously, becomes dehydrated, or is at high altitudes (Fidler, 2012). Leslie and Glenn’s baby would have sickle- cell trait if it inherits a recessive gene from Leslie and a dominant gene from Glenn.

dominant Form of an allele whose chemical instructions are followed.

recessive Allele whose instructions are ignored in the presence of a dominant allele.

incomplete dominance Situation in which one allele does not dom- inate another completely.

sickle-cell trait Disorder in which individuals show signs of mild anemia only when they are seriously deprived of oxygen; occurs in individuals who have one dominant allele for normal blood cells and one recessive sickle-cell allele.

●◗ Figure 2.2 In single-gene inheritance, a hetero- zygous father and a heterozygous mother can have a healthy child, a child with sickle-cell trait, or a child with sickle-cell disease.

Sickle-cell disease

Normal child

Sickle-cell trait

Sickle-cell trait

MotherFather

A A

A

A

A

A a a

a

a

a

a

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 43

The simple genetic mechanism responsible for sickle-cell disease—which involves a single gene pair with one dominant allele and one recessive allele—is also responsible for numerous other common traits, as shown in ◗ Table 2.1. In each of these instances, indi- viduals with the recessive phenotype have two recessive alleles, one from each parent. Individuals with the dominant phenotype have at least one dominant allele.

Most of the traits listed in ◗ Table 2.1 are biological and medical phenotypes. This same genetic mechanism can cause serious disorders, as we’ll see in the next section.

Genetic Disorders Genetics can derail development in two ways. First, some disorders are inherited. Sickle- cell disease is one example of an inherited disorder. Second, sometimes eggs or sperm do not include the usual 23 chromosomes but have more or fewer chromosomes instead. In the next few pages, we’ll see how inherited disorders and abnormal numbers of chromo- somes can alter a person’s development.

Inherited Disorders You know that sickle-cell disease is a disorder that affects people who inherit two recessive alleles. Another disorder that involves recessive alleles is phenylketonuria (PKU), a disorder in which babies are born lacking an important liver enzyme. This enzyme converts phenyl- alanine—an amino acid found in dairy products, bread, diet soda, and fish—into tyrosine (another amino acid). Without this enzyme, phenylalanine accumulates and produces poi- sons that harm the nervous system, resulting in mental retardation (Diamond et al., 1997; Mange & Mange, 1990).

Most inherited disorders are like sickle-cell disease and PKU in that they are car- ried by recessive alleles. Few serious disorders are caused by dominant alleles. Why? If the allele for the disorder is dominant, every person with at least one of these alleles would have the disorder. Individuals affected with these disorders typically do not live long enough to reproduce, so dominant alleles that produce fatal disorders soon vanish

phenylketonuria (PKU) Inherited disorder in which the infant lacks a liver enzyme.

Table 2.1

Some Common Phenotypes Associated with Single Pairs of Genes DOMINANT PHENOTYPE RECESSIVE PHENOTYPE Curly hair Straight hair

Normal hair Pattern baldness (men)

Dark hair Blond hair

Thick lips Thin lips

Cheek dimples No dimples

Normal hearing Some types of deafness

Normal vision Nearsightedness

Farsightedness Normal vision

Normal color vision Red–green color blindness

Type A blood Type O blood

Type B blood Type O blood

Rh-positive blood Rh-negative blood

Source: McKusick, 1995.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

44 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

from the species. An exception is huntington’s disease, a fatal disease characterized by progressive degeneration of the nervous system. Huntington’s disease is caused by a dominant allele found on chromosome 4. Individuals who inherit this disorder develop normally through childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. However, during middle age, nerve cells in the brain begin to deteriorate; by this time, many adults with Huntington’s have already had children, many of whom will develop the disease themselves.

Abnormal Chromosomes Sometimes individuals do not receive the normal complement of 46 chromosomes. If they are born with extra, missing, or damaged chromosomes, development is dis- turbed. The best example is Down syndrome. People with Down syndrome have almond-shaped eyes and a fold over the eyelid. Their head, neck, and nose are usually smaller than normal. During the first several months of life, development of babies with Down syndrome seems to be normal. Thereafter, their mental and behavioral develop- ment lags behind that of the average child. For example, a child with Down syndrome might first sit up without help at about 1 year, walk at 2, and talk at 3, reaching each of these developmental milestones months or even years behind children without Down syndrome. By childhood, most aspects of cognitive and social development are seri- ously retarded.

Rearing a child with Down syndrome presents special challenges. During the preschool years, children with Down syndrome need special programs to prepare them for school. Educational achievements of children with Down syndrome are likely to be limited, and their average life expectancy is about 50 years (Coppus, 2013). Nevertheless, as you’ll see in Chapter 6, many individuals with Down syndrome lead full, satisfying lives.

What causes Down syndrome? Individuals with Down syndrome typically have an extra 21st chromosome that is usually provided by the egg (Vraneković et al., 2012). Why

the mother provides two 21st chromosomes is unknown. However, the odds that a  woman  will bear a child with Down syndrome increase markedly as she gets older. For a woman in her late 20s, the risk of giving birth to a baby with Down syndrome is about 1 in 1,000; for a woman in her early 40s, the risk is about 1 in 50. Why? A woman’s eggs have been in her ovaries since her own prenatal development. Eggs may deteriorate over time as part of aging or because an older woman has a longer history of exposure to hazards in the environment, that may damage her eggs (e.g., X-rays).

An extra autosome (as in Down syndrome), a missing autosome, or a dam- aged autosome always has far-reaching consequences for development because the autosomes contain huge amounts of genetic material. In fact, nearly half of all fertilized eggs abort spontaneously within two weeks— primarily because of abnormal autosomes. Thus, most eggs that cannot develop normally are removed naturally (Moore & Persaud, 1993).

Abnormal sex chromosomes can also disrupt development. ◗ Table 2.2 lists four of the more frequent disorders associated with atypical numbers of X and Y chromosomes. Frequent is a relative term; although these disorders are more frequent than PKU or Huntington’s disease, most are uncommon. Notice that no disorders consist solely of Y chromosomes. The presence of an X chromosome appears to be necessary for life.

Fortunately, most of us receive the correct number of chromosomes and do not inherit life-threatening illnesses. For most people, heredity reveals its power in creating a unique individual—a person unlike any other.

Now that you understand the basic mechanisms of heredity, we can learn how heredity and environment work together to produce behavioral and psy- chological development.

huntington’s disease Progressive and fatal type of dementia caused by dominant alleles.

Children with Down syndrome typically have upward slanting eyes with a fold over the eyelid, a flattened facial profile, and a smaller than average nose and mouth.

M oo

db oa

rd S

to ck

P ho

to gr

ap hy

L td

./ Ca

no py

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 45

Heredity, Environment, and Development Many people mistakenly view heredity as a set of phenotypes unfolding automatically from the genotypes that are set at conception. Nothing could be further from the truth. Although genotypes are fixed when the sperm fertilizes the egg, phenotypes are not. Instead, phenotypes depend both on genotypes and on the environment in which individuals develop.

To begin our study of heredity and environment, we need to look first at the methods that developmental scientists use.

Behavioral Genetics: Mechanisms and Methods Behavioral genetics is the branch of genetics that deals with inheritance of behavioral and psychological traits. Behavioral genetics is complex, in part, because behavioral and psy- chological phenotypes are complex. Traits controlled by single genes are usually “either– or” phenotypes. A person either has dimpled cheeks or not; a person either has normal color vision or red–green color blindness; a person’s blood either clots normally or it does not. In contrast, most important behavioral and psychological characteristics are not of an “either–or” nature; rather, a range of different outcomes is possible. Consider extra- version as an example. You probably know a few extremely outgoing individuals and a few intensely shy persons, but most of your friends and acquaintances are somewhere in between; extroversion forms a continuum ranging from extreme extroversion at one end to extreme introversion at the other.

Many behavioral and psychological characteristics, including intelligence and aspects of personality, are distributed in this fashion, with a few individuals at the ends of the continuum and most near the middle. Phenotypes distributed like this often reflect the combined activity of many separate genes, a pattern known as polygenic inheritance. Because so many genes are involved in polygenic inheritance, we cannot trace the effects of each gene. But we can use a hypothetical example to show how many genes work together to produce a behavioral phenotype that spans a continuum. Suppose that four pairs of genes contribute to extroversion, that the allele for extrover- sion is dominant, and that the total amount of extroversion is simply the sum of the dominant alleles. If we continue to use uppercase letters to represent dominant alleles and lowercase letters to represent the recessive allele, then the four gene pairs would be Aa, Bb, Cc, and Dd.

These four pairs of genes produce 81 different genotypes and nine distinct phe- notypes. For example, a person with the genotype AABBCCDD has eight alleles for extroversion (the proverbial party animal). A person with the genotype aabbccdd has no alleles for extroversion (the proverbial wallflower). All other genotypes involve some combination of dominant and recessive alleles, so these are associated with phenotypes

behavioral genetics The branch of genetics that studies the inheritance of behavioral and psychological traits.

polygenic inheritance When phenotypes are the result of the combined activity of many separate genes.

Think About It Introversion–extroversion is an example of a psychological characteristic that defines a continuum. Think of other psychological characteristics like this, in which outcomes are not “either–or” but are distributed across a range.

Table 2.2

Common Disorders Associated with the Sex Chromosomes DISORDER SEX CHROMOSOMES FREQUENCY CHARACTERISTICS Klinefelter’s syndrome XXY 1 in 500 male births Tall, small testicles, sterile, below-normal intelligence, passive

XYY complement XYY 1 in 1,000 male births Tall, some cases apparently have below-normal intelligence

Turner’s syndrome X 1 in 2,500–5,000 female births

Short, limited development of secondary sex characteristics, problems perceiving spatial relations

XXX syndrome XXX 1 in 500–1,200 female births

Normal stature but delayed motor and language development

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

46 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

representing intermediate levels of extroversion. In fact, ◗ Figure 2.3 shows that the most common outcome is for people to inherit four dominant and four recessive alleles, and 19 of the 81 genotypes (e.g., AABbccDd, aaBbcCDd) produce this pattern. A few extreme cases (very outgoing or very shy), when coupled with many intermediate cases, produce the familiar bell-shaped distribution that characterizes many behavioral and psychologi- cal traits.

Remember, this example is hypothetical. Extroversion is not based on the combined influence of eight pairs of genes. However, the example shows how several genes work- ing together could produce a continuum of phenotypes. Something like our example is probably involved in the inheritance of numerous human behavioral traits, except that many more pairs of genes are involved and the environment also influences the phenotype ( Plomin, 2013).

If many behavioral phenotypes involve countless genes, how can we hope to unravel the influence of heredity? Twins and adopted children provide some important clues to the role of heredity. In twin studies, researchers compare identical and fraternal twins. Identical twins are called monozygotic twins because they come from a single fertilized egg that splits in two. Because identical twins come from the same fertilized egg, the same genes control their body structure, height, and facial features, which explains why iden- tical twins look alike. In contrast, fraternal or dizygotic twins come from two separate eggs fertilized by two separate sperm. Genetically, fraternal twins are just like any other siblings—on average, about half their genes are the same. In twin studies, scientists com- pare identical and fraternal twins to measure the influence of heredity. When identical twins are more alike than fraternal twins, this implicates heredity.

Adopted children are another important source of information about heredity. They are compared with their biological parents, who provide the children’s genes, and their adoptive parents, who provide the children’s environment. If an adopted child’s behavior

monozygotic twins The result of a single fertilized egg splitting to form two new individuals; also called identical twins.

dizygotic twins The result of two separate eggs fertilized by two sperm; also called fraternal twins.

●◗ Figure 2.3 Many behavioral phenotypes represent a continuum (with many people falling at the middle of the continuum), an outcome that can be caused by many genes working together.

2

0

20

4

12

14

16

18

8

6

10

0 1 2

AABBCCDD

AaBBCCDD

AABbCCDD

AABBCCDd

AABBCcDD

aaBBCCDD

AaBbCCDD

AaBBCcDD

AaBBCCDd

AAbbCCDD

AABbCcDD

AABbCCDd

AABBccDD

AABBCcDd

AABBCCdd

aaBbCCDDaabbCCDDaabbCcDDaabbccDDaabbccDd

aaBBCcDDaaBbCcDDaabbCcDd aabbCCDdaabbCcdd

aaBBCCDdaaBbCCDdaabbCCdd aaBbccDDaaBbccdd

AabbCCDDaaBBccDDaaBbccDd aaBbCcDd

AaBbCcDDaaBBCcDdaaBbCCddaaBbCcdd

AaBbCCDdaaBBCCddaaBBccDdaaBBccdd

AaBBccDDAabbCcDDaaBBCcddAabbccDd

AaBBCcDdAabbCCDdAabbccDDAabbCcdd

AaBBCCddAaBbccDDAabbCcDdAaBbccdd

AAbbCcDDAaBbCcDdAabbCCdd

AAbbCCDdAaBbCCddAaBbccDd

AABbccDDAaBBccDdAaBbCcdd

AABbCcDdAaBBCcddAaBBccdd

AABbCCddAAbbccDDAAbbccDd

AABBccDdAAbbCcDdAAbbCcdd

AABBCcddAAbbCCdd

AABbccDd

AABbCcdd

AABBccdd

AABbccdd

AAbbccdd

Aabbccdd

aabbccdd

3 4 5 6 7 8 Number of dominant alleles for extroversion (phenotype)

N u

m b

er o

f d

iff er

en t

ge n

ot yp

es

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 47

resembles that of his or her biological parents, this shows the impact of heredity; if the adopted child’s behavior resembles his or her adoptive parents, this shows the influence of the environment.

These and other methods are not foolproof. A potential flaw in twin studies is that parents and others may treat monozygotic twins more similarly than they treat dizygotic twins. This would make monozygotic twins more similar than dizygotic twins in their experiences as well as in their genes. Each method of study has its unique pitfalls, but if dif- ferent methods converge on the same conclusion about the influence of heredity, then we can be confident of that result. Throughout this book, you’ll see many instances where twin studies and adoption studies have pointed to genetic influences on human development.

Behavioral geneticists are now moving beyond twin and adop- tion studies to connect behavior to molecular genetics (Plomin, 2013; Trzaskowski et al., 2014). Today, researchers can obtain DNA by gather- ing cheek cells from inside a child’s mouth. A solution containing the DNA is placed on a microarray—a “chip” about the size of a postage stamp—that contains thousands of known sequences of DNA. Every match between the child’s DNA and the known sequences is recorded, creating a profile of the child’s genotype. Researchers then look to see if the genotype is associated with behavior phenotypes. For example, recent research has identified candidate genes that are linked to children’s dis- ability in reading and mathematics (Petrill, 2016).

This kind of molecular genetics research is challenging, in part because detecting the tiny effects of individual genes requires samples of thousands of children. But this research has the promise of linking individual genes to behavior. And when used with traditional methods of behavioral genetics (e.g., adoption studies), the new methods promise much greater understanding of how genes influence behavior and devel- opment (Plomin, 2013).

Throughout the rest of this book, you’ll encounter many instances that show the interactive influences of heredity and environment on human development. In the next few pages, however, we want to mention some general principles of heredity— environment interactions.

Paths from Genes to Behavior How do genes work together—for example, to make some children brighter than oth- ers and some more outgoing than others? That is, how does the information in strands of DNA influence a person’s behavioral and psychological development? In the next few pages, we’ll discover some general properties of the paths linking genes to behavior.

1. Heredity and environment interact dynamically throughout development. A tra- ditional but simple-minded view of heredity and environment is that heredity provides the clay of life and experience does the sculpting. In fact, genes and envi- ronments constantly interact to produce phenotypes throughout a person’s devel- opment (LaFreniere & MacDonald, 2013). Although we often think there is a direct link between a genotype and a phenotype—given a certain genotype, a specific phe- notype occurs, necessarily and automatically—in fact, the path from genotype to phenotype is more complicated and less direct than this. A more accurate descrip- tion would be that a genotype leads to a phenotype but only if the environment “cooperates” in the usual manner.

A good example of this genotype–phenotype link is seen in the disease phenyl- ketonuria (PKU for short). As we saw on page 43, PKU is a homozygous recessive trait in which phenylalanine accumulates in the child’s body, damaging the nervous system and leading to retarded mental development. Phenylalanine is abundant in many foods that most children eat regularly—meat, chicken, eggs, cheese—so that

Identical twins are called monozygotic twins because they came from a single fertilized egg that split in two; conse- quently, they have identical genes.

iS to

ck .c

om /d

ig ita

ls ki

lle t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

48 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

the environment usually provides the input (phenylalanine) necessary for the pheno- type (PKU) to emerge. However, in the middle of the 20th century, the biochemical basis for PKU was discovered, and now newborns are tested for the disorder. Infants who have the genotype for the disease are immediately placed on a diet that limits phenylalanine, and the disease does not appear; their nervous system develops nor- mally. In more general terms, a genotype is expressed differently (no disease) when it is exposed to a different environment (one lacking phenylalanine).

The effect can work in the other direction, too, with the environment trigger- ing genetic expression. That is, people’s experiences can help to determine how and when genes are activated. For instance, when infant girls who do not have a strong emotional attachment with their mothers also experience a stressful childhood, they begin to menstruate at a younger age (Sung et al., 2016). The exact pathway of influence is unknown (although it probably involves the hormones that are trig- gered by stress and those that initiate ovulation), but this is a clear case in which the environment advances the genes that regulate the developmental clock (Ellis, 2004).

We’ve used a rare disease (PKU) and a once-in-a-lifetime event (onset of men- struation) to show intimate connections between nature and nurture in human development. These examples may make it seem as if such connections are rare, but nothing could be further from the truth. At a biological level, genes always operate in a cellular environment. Genetic instructions interact constantly with the nature of the immediate cellular environment, which can be influenced by a host of much broader environmental factors (e.g., hormones triggered by a child’s experiences). This continuous interplay between genes and multiple levels of the environment (from cells to culture) that drives development is known as epigenesis.

Returning to the analogy of sculpting clay, an epigenetic view of molding would be that new and different forms of genetic clay are constantly being added to the sculpture, leading to resculpting by the environment, which causes more clay to be added, and the cycle continues. Hereditary clay and environmental sculpting are continuously interweaving and influencing each other.

Research in molecular genetics has begun to reveal ways in which experiences get “under the skin.” Sometimes experiences change the expression of DNA—the genetic code is preserved but some genes are “turned off.” This process is known as methylation because the chemical silencer is a methyl molecule (van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Ebstein, 2011). To illustrate, adversity early in life has been linked to increased methylation of a gene that helps regulate the body’s response to stress (Boyce & Kobor, 2015; Conradt, 2017). In other words, an experi- ence (early adversity) led to changes in heredity (a gene linked to stress regulation was “turned off ”).

Because of the epigenetic principle, you need to be wary when you read state- ments such as “X percent of a trait is due to heredity.” In fact, behavioral geneti- cists often use correlations from twin and adoption studies to calculate a heritability coefficient, which estimates the extent to which differences between people reflect heredity. For example, intelligence has a heritability coefficient of about 0.5, which means that about 50% of the differences in intelligence between people are due to heredity (Bouchard, 2004).

Why be cautious? One reason is that many people mistakenly interpret herita- bility coefficients to mean that 50% of an individual’s intelligence is due to heredity; this is incorrect because heritability coefficients apply to groups of people, not to a single person.

A second reason for caution is that heritability coefficients apply only to a spe- cific group of people living in a specific environment. They cannot be applied to other groups of people living in the same environment or to the same people living elsewhere. For example, a child’s height is certainly influenced by heredity, but the value of a heritability coefficient depends on the environment. When children grow

heritability coefficient A measure (derived from a correlation coefficient) of the extent to which a trait or characteristic is inherited.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 49

Children who are outgoing often like to be with other people and deliberately seek them out; this is an example of niche-picking.

iS to

ck .c

om /t

ra ck

5

in an environment that has ample nutrition—allowing all children to grow to their full genetic potential—heritability coefficients are large. But when some children receive inadequate nutrition, this aspect of their environment will limit their height and, in the process, reduce the heritability coefficient.

Similarly, the heritability coefficient for cognitive skill is larger for parents who are well educated than for parents who aren’t (Tucker-Drob, Briley, & Harden, 2013). Why? Well-educated parents more often provide the academically stimulating envi- ronment that fosters a child’s cognitive development; consequently, cognitive skill in this group usually reflects heredity. In contrast, less educated parents less often provide the needed stimulation; thus, cognitive skill reflects a mixture of genetic and environmental influences.

This brings us back to the principle that began this section: “Heredity and envi- ronment interact dynamically throughout development.” Both genes and environ- ments are powerful influences on development, but we can understand one only by considering the other, too. This is why it is essential to expand research beyond the middle-class, European American participants who have dominated the samples of scientists studying human development. Only by studying diverse groups of people can we understand the many ways in which genes and environments propel children along their developmental journeys (Tucker-Drob et al., 2013).

2. Genes can influence the kind of environment to which a person is exposed. In other words, “nature” can help to determine the kind of “nurturing” that a child receives (Scarr, 1992; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). A person’s genotype can lead others to respond in a specific way. For example, imagine someone who is bright and outgo- ing as a result, in part, of her genes. As a child, she may receive plenty of attention and encouragement from teachers. In contrast, someone who is not as bright and is more withdrawn (again, due in part to heredity) may be easily overlooked by teach- ers. In addition, as children grow and become more independent, they actively seek environments that fit their genetic makeup. Children who are bright may actively seek peers, adults, and activities that strengthen their intellectual development. Similarly, people who are outgoing may seek the company of other people, particu- larly extroverts like themselves. This process of deliberately seeking environments that fit one’s heredity is called niche-picking. Niche-picking is first seen in child- hood and becomes more common as children grow older and can control their environments. Through niche-picking, the environment amplifies genetic differ- ences as, for example, bright children seek intellectually stimulating environments that make them even smarter and extroverted children seek socially stimulating environments that make them even more outgoing (Tucker-Drob et al., 2013). The Real People feature on page 50 shows niche-picking in action.

3. Environmental influences typically make children within a family different. One of the fruits of behavioral genetic research is greater understanding of the man- ner in which environments influence people (Harden, 2013). Traditionally, scientists considered some environ- ments beneficial and others detrimental for people. This view has been especially strong in regard to family envi- ronments. Some parenting practices are thought to be more effective than others, and parents who use these effective practices are believed to have children who are, on average, better off than children of parents who don’t use these practices. This view leads to a simple predic- tion: Children within a family should be similar because they all receive the same type of effective (or ineffective) parenting. However, dozens of behavioral genetic stud- ies show that, in reality, siblings are not very much alike in their cognitive and social development (Plomin & Spinath, 2004).

niche-picking Process of deliberately seeking environments that are compatible with one’s genetic makeup.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

50 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Does this mean that family environment is not important? No. These findings point to the importance of nonshared environmental influences, the forces within a family that make children different from one another. Although the family environment is important, it usually affects each child in a unique way, making siblings different. Each child is likely to have different experiences in daily family life. For example, parents may be more affec- tionate with one child than another, they may use more physical punishment with  one

child than another, or they may have higher expectations for school achievement by one child than another. All these contrast- ing parental influences tend to make siblings different, not alike (Liang & Eley, 2005). Family environments are important, but— as we describe their influence throughout this book—you should remember that families create multiple unique environments, one for each person in the family.

Much of what we have said about genes, environment, and development is summarized in ◗ Figure 2.4 (Lytton, 2000). Parents are the source of children’s genes and, at least for young children, the primary source of children’s experiences. Children’s genes also influence the experiences they have and the impact of those experi- ences on them. However, to capture the idea of nonshared environ- mental influences, we would need a separate diagram for each child, reflecting the fact that parents provide unique genes and a unique family environment for each of their offspring.

Most of this book explains the links between nature, nurture, and development. We can see the interaction of nature and nurture during prenatal development, which we examine in the next section of this chapter.

nonshared environmental influences Forces within a family that make siblings different from one another.

●◗ Figure 2.4 Parents influence their children by providing genes and experiences; children’s genes and their environ- ments work together to shape development.

Child’s environment

Child’s phenotype

Parents’ genes

Child’s genes

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Children’s experiences within a family typically make them different from one another, not more alike.

Pe te

r C ad

e/ Th

e Im

ag e

Ba nk

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Ben and Matt Kail were born 25 months apart. Even as a young baby, Ben was always a “people person.” He relished con- tact with other people and preferred play that involved others. From the beginning, Matt was different. He was more withdrawn and was quite happy to play alone. The first separa- tion from parents was harder for Ben than for Matt because Ben relished parental contact more. When they entered school, Ben enjoyed

increasing the scope of his friendships; Matt liked all the different activities that were available and barely noticed the new faces. Although brothers, Ben and Matt are quite dissimilar in terms of their sociability, a char- acteristic known to have important genetic components (Braungart et al., 1992).

As Ben and Matt have grown up (they’re now adults), they have consistently sought environments that fit their different needs for

social stimulation. Ben was involved in team sports and now enjoys teaching. Matt took art and photography classes and now is happy working at his computer. Ben and Matt have chosen very different niches, and their choices have been driven in part by the genes that regulate sociability.

Real People Applying Human Development

Ben and Matt Pick Their Niches

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 51

Eun Jung has just learned that she is pregnant with her first child. Like many other parents- to-be, she and her husband are ecstatic. But they also realize how little they know about “what happens when” during pregnancy. Eun Jung is eager to visit her obstetrician to learn more about the timetable of events during pregnancy.

The many changes that transform an egg fertilized by a sperm cell into a newborn human constitute prenatal development. Prenatal development takes an aver- age of 38 weeks, which are divided into three periods: the period of the zygote, the period of the embryo, and the period of the fetus.* Each period gets its name from the scientific term used to describe the baby-to-be at that point in its pre- natal development.

In this section, we’ll trace the major developments of each of these periods. As we do, you’ll learn the answers to the “what happens when” question that intrigues Eun Jung.

Period of the Zygote (Weeks 1–2) The teaspoon or so of seminal fluid produced during a fertile male’s ejaculation contains from 200 to 500 million sperm. Of the sperm released into the vagina, only a few hundred complete the six- or seven-inch journey to the fallopian tubes.

prenatal development The many changes that turn a fertilized egg into a newborn human.

recall 1. The first 22 pairs of chromosomes are called

_______________.

2. _______________ reflects the combined activity of a number of distinct genes.

3. Individuals with _______________ have an extra 21st chromo- some, usually inherited from the mother.

4. When a fertilized egg has defective autosomes, the usual result is that _______________.

5. Nonshared environmental influences tend to make siblings _______________.

Interpret ●● Explain how niche-picking shows the interaction between

heredity and environment.

apply ●● Leslie and Glenn, the couple concerned that their baby could

have sickle-cell disease, are already charting their baby’s life course. Leslie, who has always loved to sing, is confident that her baby will be a fantastic musician and imagines a routine of music lessons, rehearsals, and concerts. Glenn, a pilot, is just as confident that his child will share his love of flying; he is already planning trips the two of them can take together. What advice might you give to Leslie and Glenn about factors they are ignoring?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 2.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What happens to a fertilized egg in the first two weeks

after conception?

●● When do body structures and internal organs emerge in prenatal development?

●● When do body systems begin to function well enough to support life?

2.2 From Conception to Birth

In this photo, the tail of the sperm can be seen clearly but the sperm has burrowed so deeply that the head is barely visible.

Ey e

of S

ci en

ce /S

ci en

ce S

ou rc

e

*Perhaps you’ve heard that pregnancy lasts 40 weeks and wonder why we say that prenatal development lasts 38 weeks. The reason is that the 40 weeks of pregnancy are measured from the start of a woman’s last menstrual period, which typically is about 2 weeks before conception.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

52 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

An egg arrives there monthly, hours after it is released by an ovary. If an egg is present, many sperm simultaneously begin to burrow their way through the cluster of nurturing cells that surround the egg. When one sperm finally penetrates the cellular wall of the egg, chemical changes occur in the wall immediately, blocking out all other sperm. Then the nuclei of the egg and sperm fuse, and the two independent sets of 23 chromosomes are interchanged. The development of a new human being is under way.

For nearly all of history, sexual intercourse was the only way for egg and sperm to unite and begin the development that results in a human being. This is no longer the only way, as we see in the What Do You Think? feature.

Whether by artificial means or by natural means, fertilization begins the period of the zygote, the term for the fertilized egg. This period ends when the zygote implants itself in the wall of the uterus. During these two weeks, the zygote grows rapidly through cell division. ◗ Figure 2.5 traces the egg cell from the time it is released from the ovary until the zygote becomes implanted in the wall of the uterus. The zygote travels down the fal- lopian tube toward the uterus. Within hours, the zygote divides for the first time; it then continues to do so every 12 hours. Occasionally, the zygote separates into two clusters that develop into identical twins. Fraternal twins, which are more common, are created when two eggs are released and each is fertilized by a different sperm cell.

zygote Fertilized egg, created when a sperm cell penetrates an egg.

in vitro fertilization Process by which sperm and an egg are mixed in a petri dish to create a zygote, which is then placed in a woman’s uterus.

preimplantation genetic screening (PGS) A procedure used to test the heredity of an egg fertilized with assisted reproductive technology, typically to determine the pres- ence of genetic disorders.

eugenics Effort to improve the human species by letting only people whose characteristics are valued by a society mate and pass along their genes.

About 40 years ago, Louise Brown captured the world’s attention as the first test-tube baby—conceived in a petri dish instead of in her mother’s body. Today, assisted reproductive technology is no longer experimental; it is used nearly 200,000 times annually with American women, producing more than 65,000 babies (Centers for Dis- ease Control and Prevention, 2015). Many new techniques are available to couples who cannot conceive a child through sexual inter- course. The best-known technique, in vitro fertilization, involves mixing sperm and egg in a petri dish and then placing several fertilized eggs in the mother’s uterus, with the hope that they become implanted in the uterine wall. Other methods include inject- ing many sperm directly into the fallopian tubes or a single sperm directly into an egg ( Ramalingam, Durgadevit, & Mahmood, 2016).

The sperm and egg usually come from the prospective parents, but sometimes they are provided by donors. Typically, the fertilized eggs are placed in the uterus of the prospec- tive mother, but sometimes they are placed in the uterus of a surrogate mother who carries the baby to term. This means that a baby could have as many as five “parents”: the man and woman who provided the sperm and egg; the surrogate mother who carried the baby; and the mother and father who rear the baby.

New reproductive techniques offer hope for couples who have long wanted a child but have been unable to conceive, and studies of the first generation of children conceived

via these techniques indicates that their social and emotional development is normal (Golombok, 2013). But there are difficulties. For women under 35, about 40% of attempts at in vitro fertilization succeed; for older women, success is less likely. What’s more, when a woman becomes pregnant, she is more likely to have twins or triplets because multiple eggs are transferred to increase the odds that at least one fertilized egg will implant in the mother’s uterus (Ramalingam et al., 2016). She is also at greater risk for giving birth to a baby with low birth weight (Luke et al., 2016) or birth defects. Finally, the procedure is expensive—the average cost in the United States of a single cycle of treat- ment is between $10,000 and $17,000—and often is not covered by health insurance.

These problems emphasize that although technology has increased the alternatives for infertile couples, pregnancy on demand is still in the realm of science fiction. At the same time, the new technologies have led to much controversy because of complex ethical issues associated with their use. One concerns the prospective parents’ right to select particular egg and sperm cells; another involves who should be able to use this technology.

Designer embryos? Today, parents can ask that the genes of a fertilized egg be analyzed before it is implanted in a woman’s uterus. This procedure, known as preimplantation genetic screening (PGS), is usually used with couples known to be at risk for genetic disorders (e.g., sickle-cell anemia). But PGS

can also be used to determine a child-to-be’s gender or, for that matter, hair color (Ethics Committee of the American Society for Repro- ductive Medicine, 2015). If parents aren’t pleased with the test results, they can have the embryo discarded.

Some claim that prospective parents have a right to be fully informed about the proper- ties of the embryo that will be implanted in the woman’s uterus. Others argue that this amounts to eugenics, which is the effort to improve the human species by allowing only certain people to mate and pass along their genes.

Available to all? About 75% of women who use assisted reproductive technology are in their 30s, but about 1% involve women who are 45 and older and cannot conceive naturally because they have gone through menopause. The American Society for Repro- ductive Medicine (2016) recommends that women older than 55 be discouraged from using reproductive technology because preg- nancy could pose risks to their health. But what about 45- to 55-year-old women? Some argue that it is unfair for a child to have par- ents who may not live until the child reaches adulthood. Others point out that people are living longer and that middle-aged (or older) adults make better parents. (We discuss this issue in more depth in Chapter 13.)

What do you think? Should prospective parents be allowed to “pick and choose” their embryos? Should new reproductive technolo- gies be available to all, regardless of age?

Conception in the 21st CenturyWhat Do You Think?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 53

After about four days, the zygote includes about 100 cells and resembles a hollow ball. The inner part of the ball is destined to become the baby. The outer layer of cells will form a number of structures that provide a life-support system throughout prenatal development.

By the end of the first week, the zygote reaches the uterus. The next step is implanta- tion, in which the zygote burrows into the uterine wall and establishes connections with the woman’s blood vessels. Implantation takes about a week to complete and triggers hormonal changes that prevent menstruation, letting the woman know that she has conceived.

The implanted zygote is less than a millimeter in diameter, yet its cells have already begun to differentiate. A small cluster of cells near the center of the zygote, the germ disc, eventually develop into the baby. The other cells become structures that support, nour- ish, and protect the developing organism. The layer of cells closest to the uterus becomes the placenta, a structure through which nutrients and wastes are exchanged between the mother and the developing organism.

Implantation and differentiation of cells mark the end of the period of the zygote. Sheltered in the uterus, the zygote is well prepared for the remaining 36 weeks of the jour- ney to birth.

Period of the Embryo (Weeks 3–8) After the zygote is completely embedded in the uterine wall, it is called an embryo. This new period typically begins the third week after conception and lasts until the end of the eighth week. During the period of the embryo, body structures and internal organs develop. At the beginning of this period, three layers form in the embryo. The outer layer, or ectoderm, becomes hair, the outer layer of skin, and the nervous system; the middle layer, or mesoderm, forms muscles, bones, and the circulatory system; the inner layer, or endoderm, forms the digestive system and the lungs.

A dramatic way to see these changes is to compare a 3-week-old embryo with an 8-week-old embryo. The 3-week-old embryo is about 2 millimeters long and looks more like a salamander than a human being. But growth and specialization proceed so rap- idly that the 8-week-old embryo shown in the right photo on page 54 looks distinctively

implantation Step in which the zygote burrows into the uterine wall and establishes connections with the woman’s blood vessels.

germ disc Small cluster of cells near the center of the zygote that eventually develop into the baby. placenta Structure through which nutrients and wastes are exchanged between the mother and the developing child.

embryo Term given to the zygote once it is com- pletely embedded in the uterine wall.

ectoderm Outer layer of the embryo, which becomes the hair, outer layer of skin, and nervous system.

mesoderm Middle layer of the embryo, which becomes the muscles, bones, and circulatory system.

endoderm Inner layer of the embryo, which becomes the lungs and digestive system.

●◗ Figure 2.5 The period of the zygote spans 14 days, beginning with fertilization of the egg in the fallopian tube and ending with implanta- tion of the fertilized egg in the wall of the uterus.

36 hours after fertilization: 2 cells

5 48 hours after fertilization: 4 cells

6 3 days: A cluster of 16–32 cells7

4 days: A hollow ball of about 100 cells

8

4–5 days: Zygote enters the uterus

9

6–7 days: Zygote begins to attach to the wall of the uterus

10

12–14 days: Zygote is completely implanted in the uterine wall

Cavity of uterus

Inner wall of uterus

Ovary

Fallopian tube leading to uterus

11

Egg cell divides for the first time

4

24–30 hours after fertilization male (sperm) and female (egg) chromosome material unite

3

Fertilization usually takes place in the upper third of the tube, within 24 hours after ovulation

2

Ovulation: An egg cell from the ovary enters the Fallopian tube at 9–16 days of the menstrual cycle

1

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

54 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

human with eyes, arms, and legs. The brain and the nervous system are developing rap- idly, and the heart has been beating for nearly a month. Most of the organs found in a mature human are in place in some form. (The sex organs are a notable exception.) Yet because it is only an inch long and weighs a fraction of an ounce, the embryo is too small for the mother to feel its presence.

The embryo’s environment is shown in ◗ Figure 2.6. The embryo rests in a sac called the amnion, which is filled with amniotic fluid that cushions the embryo and maintains a constant temperature. The embryo is linked to the mother via two structures, the placenta and the umbilical cord. The umbilical cord houses blood vessels that join the embryo to the placenta. In the placenta, the blood vessels from the umbilical cord run close to the mother’s blood vessels, allowing nutrients, oxygen, vitamins, and waste products to be exchanged between mother and embryo.

Growth in the period of the embryo follows two important principles. First, the head develops before the rest of the body. Such growth, from the head to the base of the spine, illustrates the cephalocaudal principle. Second, arms and legs develop before hands and feet. Growth of parts near the center of the body before those that are more distant illus- trates the proximodistal principle. Growth after birth also follows these principles.

amnion Inner sac in which the developing child rests.

amniotic fluid Fluid that surrounds the fetus.

umbilical cord Structure containing veins and arteries that connects the developing child to the placenta.

cephalocaudal principle A principle of physical growth that states that structures nearest the head develop first.

proximodistal principle Principle of physical growth that states that structures nearest the center of the body develop first.

By the end of the period of the zygote, the fertilized egg has been implanted in the wall of the uterus and has begun to make connections with the mother’s blood vessels.

A na

to m

ic al

T ra

ve lo

gu e

/ Sc

ie nc

e So

ur ce

At 3 weeks after conception, the fertilized egg is about 2  millimeters long and resembles a salamander.

Su pe

rS to

ck /S

ci en

ce F

ac tio

n Im

ag es

/A ge

F ot

os to

ck

At 8 weeks after conception near the end of the period of the embryo, the fertilized egg is obvi- ously recognizable as a baby-to-be.

M ed

ic al

RF .c

om /G

et ty

Im ag

es

●◗ Figure 2.6 The fetus is wrapped in the amniotic sac and connected to the mother by the umbilical cord.

Amniotic fluid

Placenta

Uterine wall

Uterine wallMother’s blood

Umbilical cord

Amniotic sac Fetus

Blood vessels in umbilical cord

Chorionic villi

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 55

Period of the Fetus (Weeks 9–38) The final and longest phase of prenatal development, the period of the fetus, begins at the ninth week and ends at birth. During this period, the baby-to-be becomes much larger and its bodily systems begin to work. The increase in size is remarkable. At the beginning of this period, the fetus weighs less than an ounce. At about 4 months, the fetus weighs roughly 4 to 8 ounces, which is large enough for the mother to feel its movements. In the last 5 months of pregnancy, the fetus gains an additional 7 or 8 pounds before birth. ◗ Figure 2.7, which depicts the fetus at one-eighth of its actual size, shows these incredible increases in size.

During the fetal period, the finishing touches are placed on the many systems essen- tial to human life, such as respiration, digestion, and vision. Some highlights of this period include the following:

●● At 4 weeks after conception, a flat set of cells curls to form a tube. One end of the tube swells to form the brain; the rest forms the spinal cord. By the start of the fetal period, the brain has distinct structures and has begun to regulate body functions. During the period of the fetus, all regions of the brain grow—particularly the cerebral cortex, the wrinkled surface of the brain that regulates many important human behaviors.

●● Near the end of the embryonic period, male embryos develop testes and female embryos develop ovaries. In the third month, the testes in a male fetus secrete a hormone that causes a set of cells to become a penis and scrotum; in a female fetus, this hormone is absent, so the same cells become a vagina and labia.

●● By about 4 months after conception, the fetus moves: it stretches, yawns, swallows, and moves its limbs. Initially, these movements are global and uncoordinated. But over the last few months of pregnancy, they become more differentiated and fluid. However, fetuses differ in their level of activity: Some seem to be moving constantly but others are much less active (DiPietro, Costigan, & Voegtline, 2015).

●● During the fifth and sixth months after conception, eyebrows, eyelashes, and scalp hair emerge. The skin thickens and is covered with a thick greasy substance, or vernix, that protects the fetus during its long bath in amniotic fluid.

●● Perceptual systems begin to work during the period of the fetus. The fetus responds to touch at 14 weeks after conception and responds to light at 26 weeks after conception.

period of the fetus Longest period of prenatal development, extending from the 9th until the 38th week after conception.

cerebral cortex Wrinkled surface of the brain that regulates many functions that are distinctly human.

vernix Substance that protects the fetus’s skin during development.

●◗ Figure 2.7 The baby-to-be becomes much larger during the period of the fetus, and its bodily systems start to work.

9 12 16 20 24 28 32 36 38 Full term

D iff

er en

ti at

io n

o f

ov ar

ie s

an d

t es

te s

C ir

cu la

to ry

s ys

te m

w or

k in

g

M ov

em en

ts f

el t

b y

m ot

h er

Hair forming

Sucking and swallowing

Brain specialization

Age of viability

Rapid weight gain

Birth

Weeks since conception

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

56 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Most impressive, however, is the fetus’s sensitivity to sound. The fetus responds to sound at 29 weeks after conception and by 34 weeks can distinguish different speech sounds, including different vowel sounds and different languages (Hepper, 2015; Kisilevsky, 2016).

●● The fetus can learn and remember. When a fetus is exposed repeatedly to distinct sounds (e.g., its mother’s voice) or distinct tastes (e.g., its mother eats garlic), the fetus recognizes those stimuli after birth (Hepper, 2015). For example, after women drank carrot juice frequently during the last month of pregnancy, as 5- and 6-month-olds, their infants preferred cereal flavored with carrot juice (Mennella, Jagnow, & Beauchamp, 2001). These skills are adaptive in helping newborns to recognize mother (by her voice and smell) and in helping them to learn language (because the sounds are familiar).

With these and other rapid changes, by 22 to 28 weeks, most systems function well enough that a fetus born at this time has a chance to survive, which is why this age range is called the age of viability. By this age, the fetus has a distinctly baby-like look, but babies born this early have trouble breathing because their lungs are not yet mature. Also, they can- not regulate their body temperature very well because they lack the insulating layer of fat that appears in the eighth month after conception. With modern neonatal intensive care, infants born this early can survive, but they face other challenges, as we’ll see later in this chapter.

age of viability age at which a fetus can survive outside the womb because most of its bodily systems function adequately; typically at 7 months after conception

recall 1. The period of the zygote ends _______________.

2. Body structures and internal organs are created during the period of the _______________.

3. _______________ is called the age of viability because this is when most body systems function well enough to support life.

4. Behaviors that emerge in the fetal period include movement, perception of touch, light, and sound, and _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare the events of prenatal development that precede the

age of viability with those that follow it.

apply ●● In the last few months before birth, the fetus has some basic

perceptual and motor skills; a fetus can hear, see, taste, and move. What are the advantages of having these skills in place months before they’re needed?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 2.2

Think About It Healthcare professionals often divide pregnancy into three 3-month trimesters. How do these three trimesters cor- respond to the periods of the zygote, embryo, and fetus?

At 22 to 28 weeks after conception, the fetus has achieved the age of viability, meaning that it has a chance of surviving if born prematurely. Pe

tit F

or m

at /S

ci en

ce S

ou rc

e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 57

Chloe was 2 months pregnant at her first prenatal checkup. As her appointment drew near, she began making a list of questions to ask her obstetrician. “I use my cell phone a lot. Is radiation from the phone harmful to my baby?” “When my husband and I get home from work, we have a glass of wine to help us unwind from the stress of the day. Is moderate drinking like that okay?” “I’m 38. I know older women are more likely to give birth to babies with mental retardation. Will I be able to find out if my baby is mentally retarded?”

Each of Chloe’s questions concerns the possibility of harm befalling her baby-to-be. She worries about the safety of her cell phone, about her nightly glass of wine, and about her age. Chloe’s concerns are well founded. Many factors influence the course of prenatal development, and they are the focus of this section. If you can answer all of Chloe’s ques- tions, then skip this section and go directly to page 66. Otherwise, read on to learn about problems that sometimes arise in pregnancy.

General Risk Factors As the name implies, general risk factors can have widespread effects on prenatal devel- opment. Scientists have identified three general risk factors: nutrition, stress, and a mother’s age.

Nutrition The mother is the developing child’s sole source of nutrition, so a balanced diet that includes foods from each of the five major food groups is vital. Most pregnant women need to increase their intake of calories by about 10% to 20% to meet the needs of prenatal development. In addition, pregnant women need to be sure to consume key nutrients: folic acid (found in green leafy vegetables) is essential for the spinal cord to develop properly; iron (found in beef, chicken, beans, spinach, and tofu) is necessary to make additional hemoglobin, which carries oxygen to the body’s cells, and calcium (found in milk, yogurt, and cheese) is required to develop strong teeth and bones as well as a healthy heart, mus- cles, and nerves.

A diet that lacks these nutrients can harm the developing child. For example, if moth- ers do not consume adequate amounts of folic acid, their babies are at risk for spina bifida, a disorder in which the embryo’s neural tube does not close properly during the first month of pregnancy. When the neural tube does not close properly, the result is permanent dam- age to the spinal cord and the nervous system; consequently, many children with spina bifida use crutches, braces, or wheelchairs (National Institute of Neurological Disor- ders and  Stroke, 2013). To ensure that pregnant women receive adequate amounts of folic acid and other nutrients, health care providers often recommend a vitamin/mineral supplement (Kohn, 2015).

spina bifida Disorder in which the embryo’s neural tube does not close properly.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How is prenatal development influenced by a pregnant

woman’s age, her nutrition, and the stress she experi- ences while pregnant?

●● How do diseases, drugs, and environmental hazards sometimes affect prenatal development?

●● What general principles affect the ways that prenatal development can be harmed?

●● How can prenatal development be monitored? Can abnormal prenatal development be corrected?

2.3 Influences on Prenatal Development

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

58 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Stress Does a pregnant woman’s mood affect the zygote, embryo, or fetus in her uterus? Is a woman who is happy during pregnancy more likely to give birth to a happy baby? Is a har- ried office worker more likely to give birth to an irritable baby? These questions address the impact on prenatal development of chronic stress, which refers to a person’s physical and psychological responses to threatening or challenging situations. Women who report greater anxiety during pregnancy more often give birth early or have babies who weigh less than average (Staneva et al, 2015; Tegethoff et al., 2010). What’s more, when preg- nant women are anxious, their children are less able to pay attention and more prone to behavioral problems as preschoolers (Grizenko et al., 2015). Similar results emerged from studies of pregnant women exposed to disasters, such as the September 11 attacks on the World Trade Center: Their children’s physical, cognitive, and language development was affected (Engel et al., 2005; King et al., 2012). Finally, the harmful effects of stress are not linked to anxiety in general but are specific to worries about pregnancy, particularly in the first few months (Davis & Sandman, 2010; DiPietro et al., 2006).

Increased stress can harm prenatal development in several ways. First, when a preg- nant woman experiences stress, her body secretes hormones that reduce the flow of oxygen to the fetus while increasing its heart rate and activity level (Monk et al., 2000). Second, stress can weaken a pregnant woman’s immune system, making her more suscep- tible to illness (Cohen & Williamson, 1991) that can, in turn, damage fetal development. Third, pregnant women under stress are more likely to smoke or drink alcohol and are less likely to rest, exercise, and eat properly (DiPietro et al., 2004). Fourth, stress may produce epigenetic changes (described on page 48) in which genes that help children regulate their behavior are made less effective (Monk, Spicer, & Champagne, 2012). All these behaviors endanger prenatal development.

We want to emphasize that the results described here apply to women who experience prolonged, extreme stress. Virtually all women become anxious or upset sometime during their pregnancy. Occasional, relatively mild anxiety is not thought to have any harmful consequences for prenatal development.

Mother’s Age Traditionally, the 20s were thought to be the prime childbearing years. Teenage women as well as women who were 30 and older were considered less fit for the rigors of pregnancy. Is being a 20-something really important for a successful pregnancy? Let’s answer that question separately for teenagers and older women. Compared with women in their 20s, teenage girls are at greater risk to give birth early and to give birth to babies low in birth weight (Khashan, Baker, & Kenny, 2010). This is largely because pregnant teenagers are more likely to be living in poverty and do not receive good prenatal care.

Nevertheless, even when a teenager receives adequate pre- natal care and gives birth to a healthy baby, all is not rosy. Children of teenage mothers generally do less well in school and more often have behavioral problems (D’Onofrio et al., 2009; Fergusson & Woodward, 2000). For example, as adoles- cents, they’re more likely to be convicted of crimes (Coyne et al., 2013).

Of course, not all teenage mothers and their infants follow this dismal life course. Some teenage mothers finish school, find good jobs, and have happy marriages; their children do well in school academically and socially. These “success stories” are more likely when teenage moms live with a relative—typically, the child’s grandmother (Gordon, Chase-Lansdale, & Brooks- Gunn, 2004). However, teenage pregnancies with “happy end- ings” are the exception; for most teenage mothers and their children, life is a struggle.

stress Physical and psychological responses to threatening or challenging conditions.

When pregnant women experience chronic stress, they’re more likely to give birth early or have smaller babies. This may be because women who are stressed are more likely to smoke or drink and less likely to rest, exercise, and eat properly.

Zu rij

et a/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 59

And teen parenthood may become even more challenging: In the U. S., the safety net for teen parents has been shrinking, which means they have fewer resources than before. In addition, the average age at first birth is increasing, so teenage moms are becoming increasingly dissimilar to the typical U. S. mom and perhaps more likely to be stigmatized (Mollborn, 2017).

Are older women better suited for pregnancy? That is an important question because today’s American woman is wait- ing longer than ever to have her first child. Completing an edu- cation and beginning a career often delay childbearing. In fact, the birth rate in the early 2000s among 40- to 44-year-olds was at its highest rate since the 1960s (Hamilton, Martin, & Ventura, 2010).

Traditionally, older women were thought to have more dif- ficult pregnancies and more complicated labor and deliveries. Today, we know that older women have more difficulty getting pregnant and are less likely to have successful pregnancies. Women in their 20s are twice as fertile as women in their 30s (Dunson, Colombo, & Baird, 2002), and past 35 years of age, the risks of miscarriage and stillbirth increase rapidly. For example, among 40- to 45-year-olds, pregnancies are much more likely to result in miscarriage or in babies with low birth weight (Khalil et al., 2013). What’s more, women in their 40s are more likely to give birth to babies with Down syndrome. However, as mothers, older women are quite effective. For example, they are just as able to provide the sort of sensitive, responsive care- giving that promotes a child’s development (Bornstein et al., 2006).

In general then, prenatal development is most likely to proceed normally when women are between 20 and 35 years of age, are healthy and eat right, get good health care, and lead lives that are free of chronic stress. But even in these optimal cases, prenatal develop- ment can be disrupted, as we’ll see in the next section.

Teratogens: Drugs, Diseases, and Environmental Hazards In the late 1950s, many pregnant women in Germany took thalidomide, a drug that helped them sleep. Soon, however, came reports that many of these women were giving birth to babies with deformed arms, legs, hands, or fingers. Thalidomide is a powerful teratogen, an agent that causes abnormal prenatal development. Ultimately, more than 7,000 babies world- wide were harmed before thalidomide was withdrawn from the market (Kolberg, 1999).

Prompted by the thalidomide disaster, scientists began to study teratogens extensively. Today, we know a great deal about many teratogens that affect prenatal development. Most teratogens fall into one of three categories: drugs, diseases, or environmental hazards.

Drugs Thalidomide illustrates the harm that drugs can cause during prenatal development. ◗ Table 2.3 lists several other drugs that are known teratogens. Most of the drugs in the list are substances you may use routinely—Accutane (used to treat acne), alcohol, aspirin, caffeine, and nicotine. Nevertheless, when consumed by pregnant women, they present special dangers (Behnke & Eyler, 1993).

Cigarette smoking is typical of the potential harm from teratogenic drugs (Cornelius et al., 1995; Espy et al., 2011). The nicotine in cigarette smoke constricts blood vessels and thus reduces the oxygen and nutrients that can reach the fetus through the placenta. There- fore, pregnant women who smoke are more likely to miscarry (abort the fetus spontane- ously) and to bear children who are smaller than average at birth (D’Onofrio et al., 2014). Most of these harmful effects depend on degree of exposure—heavy smoking is more

teratogen An agent that causes abnormal prenatal development.

For teenage mothers and their babies, life is often a struggle because the mothers are unable to complete their education and often live in poverty.

M ic

he ko

P ro

du ct

io ns

, I nh

. M ic

he le

V itu

cc i/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

60 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Table 2.3

Teratogenic Drugs and Their Consequences DRUG POTENTIAL CONSEQUENCES Alcohol Fetal alcohol syndrome, cognitive deficits, heart damage, retarded

growth

Aspirin Deficits in intelligence, attention, and motor skills

Caffeine Lower birth weight, decreased muscle tone

Cocaine and heroin Retarded growth, irritability in newborns

Marijuana Lower birth weight, less motor control

Nicotine Retarded growth, possible cognitive impairments

When pregnant women drink large amounts of alcohol, their children often have fetal alcohol syndrome; these children tend to have a small head and a thin upper lip as well as retarded mental development.

harmful than moderate smoking—and on the fetal genotype: Some children inherit genes that are more effective in defending, in utero, against the toxins in cigarette smoke (Price et al., 2010).

Even secondhand smoke is harmful: When pregnant women don’t smoke but their environment is filled with tobacco smoke, their babies tend to be smaller at birth, to be born early, and to be at risk for birth defects such as cleft palate or spina bifida (Hoyt et al., 2016; Meeker & Benedict, 2013). These harmful effects are reduced when smoking is banned in public places and workplaces (Bakolis et al., 2016).

Alcohol also carries serious risk. Pregnant women who regu- larly consume alcoholic beverages may give birth to babies with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FaSD). The most extreme form, fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), is most likely in pregnant women who are heavy drinkers—for example, they drink 15 or more cans of beer over a weekend (May et al., 2013). Chil- dren with FAS usually grow more slowly than normal and have heart problems and misshapen faces. Like the child in the photo, youngsters with FAS often have a small head, a thin upper lip, a short nose, and widely spaced eyes. FAS is the leading cause of developmental disabilities in the United States, and children with FAS have serious attentional, cognitive, and behavioral problems (Davis et al., 2013).

Is there any amount of drinking that’s safe during pregnancy? Maybe, but scientists have yet to determine one. This inconclu- siveness stems from two factors. First, drinking is often esti- mated from women’s responses to interviews or questionnaires. These replies may be incorrect, leading to inaccurate estimates of the harm associated with drinking. Second, any safe level of consumption is probably not the same for all women. Based on their health and heredity, some women may be able to consume more alcohol safely than others.

These factors make it impossible to offer guaranteed state- ments about safe levels of alcohol or any of the other drugs listed in ◗ Table 2.3. For this reason, the best policy is for women to avoid all drugs during pregnancy.

Diseases Sometimes women become ill while pregnant. Most diseases, such as colds and many strains of the flu, do not affect the fetus. However, many bacterial and viral infections can be harmful; several are listed in ◗ Table 2.4.

fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FaSD) Disorder affecting babies whose mothers consumed large amounts of alcohol while they were pregnant.

Be tt

y U

de se

n KR

T/ N

ew sc

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 61

Some of these diseases pass from the mother through the placenta to attack the embryo or fetus directly. They include cytomegalovirus (a type of herpes), rubella, and syphilis. Other diseases attack during birth: The virus is present in the lining of the birth canal, and babies are infected as they pass through the canal. AIDS and genital herpes are two such diseases.

The only way to guarantee that these diseases will not harm prenatal development is for a woman not to contract them before or during her pregnancy. Medicines that may help to treat a woman after she becomes ill do not prevent the disease from damaging the fetus.

Environmental Hazards As a by-product of life in an industrialized world, people are often exposed to toxins in food they eat, fluids they drink, and air they breathe. Chemicals associated with industrial waste are the most common form of environmental teratogens. The quantity involved is usually minute; however, as with drugs, amounts that go unnoticed in an adult can cause serious damage to the fetus.

Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) illustrate the danger of environmental teratogens. These were used in electrical transformers and paints until the U.S. government banned them in the 1970s. However, PCBs (like many industrial by-products) seeped into the waterways, where they contaminated fish and wildlife. The amount of PCBs in a typical contaminated fish does not affect adults, but when pregnant women ate large numbers of PCB-contaminated fish, their children’s cognitive skills and reading achievement were impaired (Jacobson & Jacobson, 1996; Winneke, 2011).

Several environmental hazards that are known teratogens are listed in ◗ Table 2.5. In developed nations, the most common teratogen is polluted air. Exposure to highly polluted air is associated with greater risk for premature births and lower birth weight ( Currie, 2013). For example, in a clever natural experiment (Currie & Walker, 2011), researchers studied pregnant women living near highway toll plazas. When devices are installed that allow drivers to pay tolls electronically without stopping, air pollution drops substantially (because cars neither wait to pay nor accelerate back to highway speed). Collecting tolls electronically produced a 10% drop in prematurity and low birth weight among pregnant women living near the toll plazas.

You may be wondering about one ubiquitous feature of modern environments that doesn’t appear in ◗ Table 2.5: cell phones. Is the use of a cell phone by a pregnant woman hazardous to the health of her fetus? Although the radiofrequency radiation that cell phones generate has sometimes been linked to health risks in adults (e.g., cancer), the findings for pregnant women are inconsistent. Some studies reported greater risk for behavioral problems in children whose mothers used cell phones frequently during preg- nancy (Divan et al., 2012), but other studies find no harm associated with using cell phones during pregnancy (Baste et al., 2015). Of course, one way in which cell phones represent a

Table 2.4

Teratogenic Diseases and Their Consequences DISEASE POTENTIAL CONSEQUENCES AIDS Frequent infections, neurological disorders, death

Chlamydia Premature birth, low birth weight, eye inflammation

Chicken pox Spontaneous abortion, developmental delays, mental retardation

Cytomegalovirus Deafness, blindness, abnormally small head, mental retardation

Genital herpes Encephalitis, enlarged spleen, improper blood clotting

Rubella (German measles) Mental retardation; damage to eyes, ears, and heart

Syphilis Damage to the central nervous system, teeth, and bones

Toxoplasmosis Damage to the eyes and brain; learning disabilities

Think About It A pregnant woman reluctant to give up her morning cup of coffee and nightly glass of wine says, “I drink so little coffee and wine that it couldn’t possibly hurt my baby.” What do you think?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

62 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

huge risk for pregnant women is talking while driving, which increases the odds of being in an accident by more than 50% (Asbridge, Brubacher, & Chan, 2013). So while we wait for research to provide more information, the best advice for a pregnant woman is to keep a cell phone at a distance when it’s not being used and never use it while driving.

Environmental teratogens are treacherous because people are unaware of their pres- ence in the environment. For example, the women studied by Jacobson, Jacobson, and Humphrey (1990) did not realize they were eating PCB-laden fish. The invisibility of some environmental teratogens makes it more difficult for a pregnant woman to protect herself from them. Pregnant women need to be particularly careful of the foods they eat and the air they breathe. They should clean all foods thoroughly to rid them of insecticides and should avoid convenience foods, which often contain chemical additives. And they should stay away from air that’s been contaminated by household products such as cleansers, paint strippers, and fertilizers. Women in jobs that require contact with potential teratogens (e.g., housecleaners, hairdressers) should switch to less potent chemicals. For example, they should use baking soda instead of more chemically laden cleansers. They also should wear protective gloves, aprons, and masks to reduce their contact with potential terato- gens. Finally, because environmental teratogens continue to increase, pregnant women should check with a health care provider to learn if they should avoid other materials.

How Teratogens Influence Prenatal Development By assembling all the evidence on the harm caused by drugs, diseases, and environmental hazards, scientists have identified five important general principles about how teratogens usually work (Hogge, 1990; Jacobson & Jacobson, 2000; Vorhees & Mollnow, 1987).

1. The impact of a teratogen depends on the genotype of the organism. A substance may be harmful to one species but not to another. To determine its safety, thalidomide was tested on pregnant rats and rabbits, and their offspring had normal limbs. Yet when pregnant women took the same drug in comparable doses, many had children with deformed limbs. Moreover, some women who took thalidomide gave birth to babies with normal limbs, whereas others who took comparable doses of thalidomide at the same time in their pregnancies gave birth to babies with deformed arms and legs. Apparently, heredity makes some individuals more susceptible to a teratogen.

2. The impact of teratogens changes over the course of prenatal development. The timing of exposure to a teratogen is very important. ◗ Figure 2.8 shows how the consequences of teratogens differ for the periods of the zygote, embryo, and fetus. During the period of the zygote, exposure to teratogens usually results in sponta- neous abortion of the fertilized egg. During the period of the embryo, exposure to teratogens produces major defects in bodily structure. For instance, women who took thalidomide during the period of the embryo had babies with ill-formed or missing limbs, and women who contract rubella during the period of the embryo have babies with heart defects. During the period of the fetus, exposure to terato- gens either produces minor defects in bodily structure or causes body systems to

Table 2.5

Environmental Teratogens and Their Consequences HAZARD POTENTIAL CONSEQUENCES Air pollution Low birth weight, premature birth, lower test scores

Lead Mental retardation

Mercury Retarded growth, mental retardation, cerebral palsy

PCBs Impaired memory and verbal skills

X-rays Retarded growth, leukemia, mental retardation

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 63

function improperly. For example, when women drink large quantities of alcohol during this period, the fetus develops fewer brain cells.

Even within the different periods of prenatal development, developing body parts and systems are more vulnerable at some times than others. The orange shading in the chart indicates a time of maximum vulnerability; yellow shading indicates a time when the developing organism is less vulnerable. The heart, for example, is most sensitive to teratogens during the first half of the embryonic period. Exposure to teratogens before this time rarely produces heart damage, and exposure after this time results in relatively mild damage.

3. Each teratogen affects a specific aspect (or aspects) of prenatal development. Teratogens do not harm all body systems; instead, damage is selective. When women contract rubella, their babies often have problems with their eyes, ears, and heart but have nor- mal limbs. When mothers consume PCB-contaminated fish, their babies typically have normal body parts and normal motor skills but below-average verbal and memory skills.

4. The impact of teratogens depends on the dose. Just as a single drop of oil won’t pol- lute a lake, small doses of teratogens may not harm the fetus. In research on PCBs, for example, cognitive skills were affected only among children who had the great- est prenatal exposure to these by-products. In general, the greater the exposure, the greater the risk for damage (Adams, 1999). An implication of this principle is that researchers should be able to determine safe levels for a teratogen. In reality, this is extremely difficult because sensitivity to teratogens is not the same for all people (and it’s not practical to establish safe amounts for each person). Hence, the safest rule is zero exposure to teratogens.

5. Damage from teratogens is not always evident at birth but may appear later in life. In the case of malformed limbs, the effects of a teratogen are obvious immediately. Sometimes, however, the damage from a teratogen becomes evident only as the

●◗ Figure 2.8 The effects of a teratogen on an unborn child depend on the stage of prenatal development.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 16 20–26 38

BrainEarEar

Teeth

Eye Eye Heart

Heart

Central nervous system

Period of dividing zygote, implantation,

and embryo Palate

Indicates common site of action of teratogen

Arm

Leg External genitalia

Usually not susceptible to

teratogens

Prenatal death Major structural abnormalities (orange) Physiological defects and minor structural abnormalities (gold)

Age of embryo (in weeks) Fetal period (in weeks) Full term

Central nervous system

Eyes

Teeth

Palate

External genitalia

Ear

Legs

Heart

Arms

Based on Before We Are Born, 4th ed., by K. L. Moore and T. V. N. Persaud, p. 130.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

64 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

child develops. For example, when women ate PCB-contaminated fish, their babies were normal at birth. Their below-average cognitive skills were not evident until several months later.

An even more dramatic example of the delayed impact of a teratogen involves the drug diethylstilbestrol (DES). Between 1947 and 1971, many pregnant women took DES, a synthetic version of the female hormone estrogen, to prevent miscar- riages. Their babies were apparently normal at birth. As adults, however, daughters of women who took DES are more likely to have breast cancer or a rare cancer of the vagina. And they sometimes have abnormalities in their reproductive tract that make it difficult to become pregnant. Sons of women who took DES are at risk for testicular abnormalities and for testicular cancer (National Cancer Institute, 2006). In this case, the impact of the teratogen is not evident until decades after birth.

The Real World of Prenatal Risk We have discussed risk factors individually as if each factor were the only potential threat to prenatal development. In reality, infants are sometimes exposed to multiple general risks and multiple teratogens. Pregnant women who drink alcohol often smoke and drink coffee (Baron et al., 2013). Pregnant women who are under stress often drink alcohol and may self-medicate with aspirin or other over-the-counter drugs. Many of these same women live in poverty, which means that they may have inadequate nutrition and receive minimal medical care during pregnancy. When all the risks are combined, prenatal devel- opment is rarely optimal (Yumoto, Jacobson, & Jacobson, 2008).

This pattern explains why it’s often challenging for human development researchers to determine the harm associated with individual teratogens. Cocaine is a perfect example. You may remember stories in newspapers and magazines about “crack babies” and their developmental problems. Children exposed to cocaine during prenatal development suf- fer from a range of problems in physical growth, cognitive development, behavioral regu- lation, and psychopathology (e.g., Buckingham-Howes et al., 2013; Schuetze, Molnar,  & Eiden, 2012). However, many of the problems associated with cocaine reflect, in part, the impact of concurrent smoking and drinking during pregnancy and the inadequate parent- ing these children receive (Lambert & Bauer, 2012). Similarly, harmful effects attributed to smoking during pregnancy may also stem from the fact that pregnant women who smoke are more likely to be less educated and to have a history of psychological problems, includ- ing antisocial behavior (D’Onofrio et al., 2013).

Of course, findings such as these don’t mean that pregnant women should feel free to light up (or for that matter, to shoot up). Instead, they highlight the difficulties involved in determining the harm associated with a single risk factor (e.g., smoking) when it usually occurs alongside many other risk factors (e.g., inadequate parenting, continued exposure to smoke after birth).

From what we’ve said so far in this section, you may think that the developing child has little chance of escaping harm. But most babies are born in good health. Of course, a good policy for pregnant women is to avoid diseases, drugs, and environmental haz- ards that are known teratogens. This, coupled with thorough prenatal medical care and adequate nutrition, is the best recipe for normal prenatal development.

Prenatal Diagnosis and Treatment “I really don’t care whether I have a boy or girl, just as long as it’s healthy.” Legions of parents worldwide have felt this way, but until recently, all they could do was hope for the best. However, advances in technology give parents a much better idea of whether their baby is developing normally.

Genetic Counseling Often the first step in deciding whether a couple’s baby is likely to be at risk is genetic counseling. A counselor asks about family medical history and constructs a family tree for each parent to assess the odds that their child will inherit a disorder. If the family tree

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 65

●◗ Figure 2.9 In amniocentesis, a sample of fetal cells is extracted from the fluid in the amniotic sac.

Uterine wall

Placenta

Ultrasound scanner

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

●◗ Figure 2.10 In chorionic villus sampling, fetal cells are extracted from the placenta.

Ultrasound scanner

Chorionic villi

Uterine wall

Vagina ©

2 01

9 Ce

ng ag

e

A standard part of prenatal care is ultra- sound, in which sound waves are used to generate an image of the fetus that can be used to determine its position in the uterus.

Ke ith

B ro

fs ky

/P ho

to di

sc /G

et ty

Im ag

es

suggests that a parent is likely to be a carrier of the disorder, blood tests can determine the parent’s genotype. With this information, a genetic counselor then advises prospective parents about their choices. A couple might go ahead and attempt to conceive a child “naturally,” use sperm or eggs from donors, or adopt a child.

Prenatal Diagnosis After a woman is pregnant, how can we know whether prenatal devel- opment is progressing normally? Traditionally, obstetricians tracked the progress of prenatal development by feeling the size and position of the fetus through a woman’s abdomen. This technique was not very precise and couldn’t be done at all until the fetus was large enough to feel. However, several new techniques have revolutionized our abil- ity to monitor prenatal growth and development. A standard part of prenatal care in the United States is ultrasound, in which sound waves are used to generate a picture of the fetus. In this procedure, a tool about the size of a hair dryer is rubbed over the woman’s abdomen, and the image appears on a nearby computer monitor. The pictures generated are hardly portrait quality; they are grainy, and it takes an expert’s eye to distinguish what’s what. Nevertheless, parents are often thrilled to see their baby and to watch it move.

Ultrasound typically can be used as early as 4 or 5 weeks after conception; prior to this time, the embryo is not large enough to generate an interpretable image. Ultrasound pictures are useful for determining the position of the fetus within the uterus and, at 16 to 20 weeks after conception, its sex. Ultrasound is also helpful in detecting twins or triplets. Finally, ultrasound is used to identify gross physical deformities, such as abnormal growth of the head.

In pregnancies where a genetic disorder is suspected, three other techniques are particularly valuable because they provide a sample of fetal cells that can be analyzed. In amniocentesis, a needle is inserted through the mother’s abdomen to obtain a sample of the amniotic fluid that surrounds the fetus. As you can see in ◗ Figure 2.9, ultrasound is used to guide the needle into the uterus. The fluid contains skin cells that can be grown in a labora- tory and then analyzed to determine the genotype of the fetus.

A procedure that can be used much earlier in pregnancy is chorionic villus sampling (CVS) in which a sample of tissue is obtained from part of the placenta. ◗  ●Figure  2.10

ultrasound Prenatal diagnostic technique that uses sound waves to generate an image of the fetus.

amniocentesis Prenatal diagnostic technique that uses a syringe to withdraw a sample of amniotic fluid through the mother’s abdomen.

chorionic villus sampling (CVS) Prenatal diagnostic technique that involves taking a sample of tissue from the placenta.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

66 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

shows that a small tube—typically inserted through the vagina and into the uterus but sometimes through the abdomen—is used to collect a small plug of cells from the pla- centa. This procedure can be used within 9 to 12 weeks after conception, much earlier than amniocentesis.

With the samples obtained from either technique, roughly 200 different genetic disorders, including Down syndrome, can be detected. These procedures are virtually free of errors but miscarriages are slightly—1% or 2%—more likely after amniocentesis or chorionic villus sampling (Wilson, 2000). However, a new test is available to help women evaluate the value of these procedures. The placenta releases genetic material that circulates in a pregnant woman’s blood stream, genetic material that reflects the fetal genotype. In non-invasive prenatal testing (NIPT), this genetic material is analyzed in a routine sample of blood from a pregnant woman. Genetic disorders such as hemo- philia and cystic fibrosis can be detected; any findings suggesting genetic disorders are confirmed via amniocentesis or CVS (American College of Obstetricians and Gynecolo- gists, 2015).

Fetal Medicine Ultrasound, amniocentesis, chorionic villus sampling, and noninvasive prenatal test- ing have made it much easier to determine whether prenatal development is progressing normally. But what happens when it is not? Traditionally, a woman’s options have been limited: She could continue the pregnancy or end it. Today, the list of options is expand- ing. A new field called fetal therapy is concerned with treating prenatal problems before birth. Many tools are now available to solve problems that are detected during pregnancy (Rodeck & Whittle, 2009). One approach is to treat disorders medically by administering drugs or hormones to the fetus. For example, in fetal hypothyroidism, the fetal thyroid gland does not produce enough hormones. This can lead to retarded physical and mental development, but the disorder can be treated by injecting the necessary hormones directly into the amniotic cavity, resulting in normal growth.

Another way to correct prenatal problems is fetal surgery (Warner, Altimier, & Crombleholme, 2007). For instance, spina bifida can be corrected with fetal surgery in the seventh or eighth month of pregnancy. Surgeons cut through the mother’s abdominal wall to expose the fetus and then cut through the fetal abdominal wall; the spinal cord is repaired, and the fetus is returned to the uterus. However, the procedure is far from fool- proof: The best techniques and the ideal times to use them are still unknown (Deprest et al., 2016).

Yet another approach is genetic engineering. A new technique called CRISPR allows scientists to edit hereditary material: genes can be removed, turned off, or replaced off. Consider sickle-cell disease as an example. Recall from page 42 that when a baby inherits the recessive allele for sickle-cell disease from both parents, the child has mis- shaped red blood cells that can’t pass through capillaries. In principle, CRISPR could edit the genome, replacing the recessive alleles from the 11th pair of chromosomes with dominant genes. CRISPR is still experimental. The first clinical trial is underway to determine whether CRISPR can be used to edit cancer-causing genes; many scientific, technical, and ethical answers need to be addressed before gene-editing becomes a rou- tine part of prenatal care (Reardon, 2016). Nevertheless, CRISPR offers hope for pro- spective parents who fear that their offspring may suffer fatal or debilitating diseases (Hayden, 2016).

Answers to Chloe’s Questions. Now you can return to Chloe’s questions in the section- opening vignette (page 57) and answer them. If you’re not certain, here are the pages in this chapter where the answers appear:

●● About her cell phone—page 61 ●● About her nightly glass of wine—page 60 ●● About giving birth to a baby with mental retardation—page 44

non-invasive prenatal testing (NIPT) A prenatal diagnostic technique that ana- lyzes genetic material released from the pla- centa that circulates in a pregnant woman’s blood stream.

fetal therapy Field of medicine concerned with treating prenatal problems before birth.

Think About It Imagine that you are 42 years old and pregnant. Would you want to have amniocentesis or chorionic villus sam- pling to determine the genotype of the fetus? Why or why not?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 67

Dominique is 6 months pregnant; soon she and her partner will begin childbirth classes at the local hospital. She is relieved that the classes are finally starting because this means that her pregnancy is nearly over. But all the talk she has heard about “breathing exercises” and “coaching” sounds mysterious to her. Dominique wonders what’s involved and how the classes will help her during labor and delivery.

As women such as Dominique near the end of pregnancy, they find that sleeping and breathing become more difficult, that they tire more rapidly, and that their legs and feet swell. Women look forward to birth, both to relieve their discomfort and, of course, to meet their baby. In this section, you’ll see the different steps involved in birth, review dif- ferent approaches to childbirth, and look at problems that can arise. Along the way, we’ll look at classes like those Dominique will take and the exercises she’ll learn.

Stages of Labor Labor is an appropriate name for childbirth, which is the most intense, prolonged physi- cal effort that humans experience. Labor is usually divided into the three stages shown in ◗ Figure 2.11.

●● In stage 1, which may last from 12 to 24 hours for a first birth, the uterus starts to con- tract. The first contractions are weak and irregular. Gradually, they become stronger and more rhythmic, enlarging the cervix (the opening from the uterus to the vagina) to approximately 10 centimeters.

●● In stage 2, the baby passes through the cervix and enters the vagina. The mother helps push the baby along by contracting muscles in her abdomen. Soon the top of the baby’s head appears, an event known as crowning. Within about an hour, the baby is delivered.

crowning Appearance of the top of the baby’s head during labor.

recall 1. General risk factors in pregnancy include a woman’s

nutrition, _______________, and her age.

2. _______________ are some of the most dangerous teratogens because a pregnant woman is often unaware of their presence.

3. During the period of the zygote, exposure to a teratogen typically results in _______________.

4. Three techniques used to determine whether a fetus has a hereditary disorder are amniocentesis, chorionic villus sam- pling, and _______________.

Interpret ●● Explain how the impact of a teratogen changes over the

course of prenatal development.

apply ●● What would you say to a 45-year-old woman who is eager to

become pregnant but is unsure about the risks associated with pregnancy at this age?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 2.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the different phases of labor and delivery?

●● What are “natural” ways of coping with the pain of child- birth? Is childbirth at home safe?

●● What adjustments do parents face after a baby’s birth?

●● What are some complications that can occur during birth?

●● What contributes to infant mortality in developed and least developed countries?

2.4 Labor and Delivery

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

68 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● In stage 3, which lasts only minutes, the mother pushes a few more times to expel the placenta (also called, appropriately, the afterbirth).

The times given for each of the stages are only approximations; the actual times vary greatly among women. For most women, labor with their second and subsequent children is more rapid; stage 1 may last 4 to 6 hours, and stage 2 may be as brief as 20 minutes.

Approaches to Childbirth When your authors were born in the 1950s, women in labor were admitted to a hospital and administered a general anesthetic. Fathers waited anxiously in a nearby room for news of the baby. These were standard hospital procedures, and virtually all American babies were born this way.

But childbirth has changed. In the middle of the 20th century, two European physicians—Grantly Dick-Read (1959) and Fernand Lamaze (1958)—criticized the tradi- tional view in which labor and delivery had come to involve elaborate medical procedures

that were often unnecessary and often left women afraid of giving birth. This fear led them to be tense, thereby increasing the pain they experienced during labor. These physicians argued for a more “natural” or prepared approach to childbirth, viewing labor and delivery as life events to be celebrated rather than medical procedures to be endured.

Today, many varieties of prepared childbirth are available to pregnant women. However, most share some fundamental beliefs. One is that birth is more likely to be problem-free and rewarding when mothers and fathers understand what’s happen- ing during pregnancy, labor, and delivery. Consequently, prepared childbirth means going to classes to learn basic facts about pregnancy and childbirth (like the material presented in this chapter).

A second common element is that natural methods of dealing with pain are emphasized over medical procedures, which involve possible side effects and com- plications. For example, the most common procedure in the United States is epi- dural analgesia, in which drugs are injected into the space below the spinal cord. These reduce the pain of childbirth but sometimes cause women to experience headaches or decreased blood pressure (American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, 2011b). One key to reducing birth pain without drugs is relaxation. Because pain often feels greater when a person is tense, pregnant women learn to relax during labor by deep breathing or by visualizing a reassuring, pleasant scene or experience. Whenever they begin to experience pain during labor, they use these methods to relax.

●◗ Figure 2.11 Labor includes three stages, beginning when the uterus contracts and ending when the placenta is expelled.

Umbilical cord

Dilated cervix

Detached placenta

Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

During childbirth preparation classes, pregnant women learn exercises that help them relax and reduce the pain associated with childbirth.

Ja m

es M

ar sh

al l/

Th e

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 69

A third common element of prepared childbirth is the involvement of a supportive adult, who may be the father-to-be, a relative, a close friend, or a trained birth assistant (known as a doula). These people provide emotional support, act as advocates (communi- cating a woman’s wishes to health care personnel), and help a woman use techniques for managing pain. When pregnant women are supported in this manner, their labor tends to be shorter, they use less medication, and they report greater satisfaction with childbirth (Hodnett. et al., 2012).

Another element of the trend to natural childbirth is the idea that birth need not take place in a hospital. Virtually all babies in the United States are born in hospitals, with only 1% born at home (Martin et al., 2013). For Americans accustomed to hospital delivery, home delivery can seem like a risky proposi- tion and some medical professionals remain skeptical (Declercq, 2012). However, many women are more relaxed during labor at home, and they enjoy the greater control they have over labor and birth in a home delivery. That said, women should consider birth at home only if they are healthy, their pregnancy has been problem-free, labor and delivery are expected to be problem- free, a trained healthcare professional is present to assist, and comprehensive medical care is readily available should the need arise (Wax, Pinette, & Cartin, 2010).

Adjusting to Parenthood For parents, the time immediately after a trouble-free birth is full of excitement, pride, and joy—the much-anticipated baby is finally here! But it is also a time of adjustments for parents. A woman experiences many physical changes after birth. Her breasts begin to produce milk, and her uterus gradually becomes smaller, returning to its normal size in five or six weeks. And levels of female hormones (e.g., estrogen) drop.

Parents must also adjust psychologically. They reorganize old routines, particularly for first-born children, to fit the young baby’s sleep–wake cycle. In the process, fathers sometimes feel left out when mothers devote most of their attention to the baby.

Becoming a parent can be a huge adjustment, so it’s not surprising that roughly half of all new mothers find that their initial excitement gives way to irritation, resentment, and crying spells—the so-called baby blues. These feelings usually last a week or two and prob- ably reflect both the stress of caring for a new baby and the physiological changes that take place as a woman’s body returns to a nonpregnant state (Brockington, 1996).

For 10% to 15% of new mothers, however, irritability continues for months and is often accompanied by feelings of low self-worth, disturbed sleep, poor appetite, and apathy—a condition known as postpartum depression. Postpartum depression does not strike ran- domly. Biology contributes: Change in hormonal levels following birth place some women at risk for postpartum depression (O’Hara & McCabe, 2013). Experience also contributes: Women are more likely to experience postpartum depression when they are single, were depressed before pregnancy, are coping with other life stresses (e.g., dealing with the death of a loved one or moving to a new residence), did not plan to become pregnant, or lack other adults (e.g., the father) to support their adjustment to motherhood (Edwards et al., 2012; O’Hara, 2009).

Women who are lethargic and emotionless do not mother warmly and enthusiasti- cally. They don’t touch and cuddle their new babies much or talk to them. And depressed moms are less effective in the common but essential tasks of feeding and sleep routines (Field, 2010). When postpartum depression persists over years, children’s development is affected (Goodman et al., 2011). In the Spotlight on Research feature, for example, you’ll see how maternal depression can lead children to have behavioral problems.

Findings like those in the Spotlight on Research feature show that postpartum depres- sion should not be taken lightly: If a mother’s depression doesn’t lift after a few weeks, she should seek help. Home visits by trained health care professionals can be valuable

In many countries around the world, a  midwife delivers the baby.

Je nn

y M

at th

ew s/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

70 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

On ResearchSpotlight

Links Between Maternal Depression and Children’s Behavior Problems

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? When mothers are depressed, they don’t parent effectively. However, this might not be due to depression per se because the same factors that put women at risk for experienc- ing postpartum depression—for example, being single, lacking social support, and experiencing stress—may contribute to their ineffective parenting. Edward Barker and his colleagues (Barker et al., 2012) hoped to better understand how maternal depression affects children’s development.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Barker and colleagues were interested in three variables: maternal depres- sion, maternal risk factors associated with depression that might impair children’s devel- opment, and children’s behavioral problems. They measured the first two with question- naires: When children were 1½ years old, moms completed a depression questionnaire; at various points between birth and their child’s second birthday, moms completed questionnaires measuring exposure to risk factors such as being single, being exposed to stressful events such as cruelty from a partner, and having an inadequate support network. When children were 7 or 8 years old, their behavioral problems were diagnosed by experienced clinicians from teachers’ and parents’ reports of children’s behavior.

Who were the participants in the study? The sample was drawn from the Avon Longi- tudinal Study of Parents and Children, a proj- ect conducted in England that investigates children’s health and development. Data on all three variables were available for 7,429 moth- ers and children.

What was the design of the study? The study was correlational because the

investigators were interested in the relation that existed naturally among depression, risk factors, and children’s problem behaviors. The study was longitudinal because chil- dren and parents were tested multiple times (and are still being tested as the study is ongoing).

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. The measures were ones com- monly used with parents and children; they posed no known risks. The investigators obtained permission from the parents and their children to participate.

What were the results? Using the moth- ers’ replies to the depression questionnaire, Barker and his colleagues distinguished moms who had been depressed when their child was 1½ years old from moms who were not depressed. Then they compared rates of behavioral problems in children of the two groups of moms; those results are shown in ◗ Figure 2.12. For each of the five disorders, 7- and 8-year-olds were more likely to have behavioral problems if their mom had been depressed; overall, they were 2.56 times more likely to have problems. Next, Barker and his colleagues confirmed that, as expected, risk rates were greater for depressed moms. For example, depressed moms were five times more likely to have experienced cruelty from a partner and four times more likely to have inadequate sup- port. Nevertheless, when these differences in exposure to risk were equated statisti- cally, children with depressed moms were still 1.92 times more likely to have behavioral problems.

What did the investigators conclude? The depression that some women experience following childbirth influences children in two ways. One is that depression symptoms per se are harmful for children: Depressed moms are less able to parent effectively, and this may lead to behavioral problems. A second path is that the same factors that put moms

at risk for depression (e.g., inadequate sup- port) can impair a child’s development, per- haps because they lead moms to parent less effectively.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? These find- ings are based largely on mothers’ reports of their depression, their risk factors, and their children’s behavior. It would be valuable to have independent estimates of these vari- ables (e.g., observation of children’s antisocial behavior that contributes to the diagnosis of conduct disorder). In addition, nearly one- third of the mothers dropped out of the study and those dropping out were more likely to have been exposed to risk. The present findings would be strengthened if they were replicated in a sample that was more stable over time.

●◗ Figure 2.12 When mothers are depressed, their children are at risk for ADHD, disobedient and antisocial behavior, anxiety, and depression.

Type of Disorder

AD H

D

H os

til e d

iso be

di en

ce

An tis

oc ia

l b eh

av io

r

An xi

et y

De pr

es sio

n

Children of depressed moms

Children of nondepressed moms

P er

ce n

ta ge

o f

ch il

d re

n 8

6

4

2

0

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

(O’Hara & McCabe, 2013). During these visits, the visitors show mom better ways to cope with the many changes that accompany her new baby. They also provide emotional sup- port by being a caring, sensitive listener, and they can refer the mother to other community resources if needed. Finally, one simple way to reduce the risk of postpartum depression is worth mentioning—breast-feeding. Moms who breast-feed are less likely to become depressed, perhaps because breast-feeding releases hormones that act as antidepressants (Gagliardi, 2005).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 71

Birth Complications Women who are healthy when they become pregnant usually have a normal pregnancy, labor, and delivery. When women are not healthy or don’t receive adequate prenatal care, problems can surface during labor and delivery. (Of course, even healthy women can have problems, but not as often.) The more common birth complications are listed in ◗ Table 2.6.

Some of these complications, such as a prolapsed umbilical cord, are dangerous because they disrupt the flow of blood through the umbilical cord. If this flow of blood is disrupted, then infants do not receive adequate oxygen, a condition known as hypoxia. Hypoxia sometimes occurs during labor and delivery because the umbilical cord is pinched or squeezed shut, cutting off the flow of blood. Hypoxia is serious because it can lead to mental retardation or death (Hogan et al., 2006).

To guard against hypoxia, fetal heart rate is monitored during labor by ultrasound or with a tiny electrode that is passed through the vagina and attached to the scalp of the fetus. An abrupt change in heart rate can be a sign that the fetus is not receiving enough oxygen. If the heart rate does change suddenly, a health care professional will try to con- firm that the fetus is in distress, perhaps by measuring fetal heart rate with a stethoscope on the mother’s abdomen.

When a fetus is in distress or when the fetus is in an irregular position or is too large to pass through the birth canal, a physician may decide to remove it from the mother’s uterus surgically (American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, 2011a). In a cesarean section (or C-section), an incision is made in the abdomen to remove the baby from the uterus. A C-section is riskier than a vaginal delivery for mothers because of increased bleeding and greater danger of infection. A C-section poses little risk for babies, although they are often lethargic briefly from the anesthesia the mother receives before the opera- tion. And mother–infant interactions are much the same for babies delivered vaginally or by planned or unplanned C-sections (Durik, Hyde, & Clark, 2000).

Birth complications are hazardous not just for a newborn’s health; they have long- term effects, too. When babies experience many birth complications, they are at risk for becoming aggressive or violent and for developing schizophrenia (de Haan et al., 2006; Fazel et al., 2012). This is particularly true for newborns with birth complications who later experience family adversity, such as living in poverty (Arseneault et al., 2002). These outcomes underscore the importance of excellent health care for the mother through pregnancy and labor and the need for a supportive environment throughout childhood.

Problems also arise when babies are born too early or too small. Normally, a baby spends about 38 weeks developing before being born. Babies born before the 36th week are called preterm or premature. In the first year or so, premature infants often lag behind full-term infants in many facets of development. However, by 2 or 3 years of age, such differences have vanished, and most premature infants develop normally (Greenberg & Crnic, 1988).

hypoxia A birth complication in which umbilical blood flow is disrupted and the infant does not receive adequate oxygen.

cesarean section (C-section) Surgical removal of an infant from the uterus through an incision made in the mother’s abdomen.

preterm or premature Babies born before the 36th week after conception.

Table 2.6

Common Birth Complications COMPLICATION FEATURES Cephalopelvic disproportion The infant’s head is larger than the pelvis, making it impossible

for the baby to pass through the birth canal.

Irregular position In shoulder presentation, the baby is lying crosswise in the uterus and the shoulder appears first; in breech presentation, the buttocks appear first.

Preeclampsia A pregnant woman has high blood pressure, protein in her urine, and swelling in her extremities (due to fluid retention).

Prolapsed umbilical cord The umbilical cord precedes the baby through the birth canal and is squeezed shut, cutting off oxygen to the baby.

Think About It A friend of yours has just given birth six weeks prematurely. The baby is average size for a baby born prematurely and seems to be faring well, but your friend is concerned nonetheless. What would you say to reassure your friend?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

72 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Prospects are usually not as bright for babies who are “small for date.” These infants are most often born to women who smoke or drink alcohol frequently during pregnancy or  who do not eat enough nutritious food (Chomitz, Cheung, & Lieberman, 1995). Newborns who weigh 2,500 grams (5.5 pounds) or less are said to have low birth weight, newborns weighing less than 1,500 grams (3.3 pounds) are said to have very low birth weight, and those weighing less than 1,000 grams (2.2 pounds) are said to have extremely low birth weight.

Babies with very or extremely low birth weight do not fare well. Many do not sur- vive, and those who live often lag behind in the development of intellectual and motor skills (Kavsek & Bornstein, 2010). Newborns who weigh more than 1,500 grams have bet- ter prospects if they receive appropriate care. These babies are placed in special sealed beds where temperature and air quality are regulated carefully. These beds isolate infants, depriving them of environmental stimulation. Consequently, they often receive auditory stimulation, such as a recording of soothing music or their mother’s voice, or visual stimu- lation from a mobile placed over the bed. Infants also receive tactile stimulation—they are “massaged” several times daily. These forms of stimulation foster physical and cognitive development in small-for-date babies (Field, Diego, & Hernandez-Reif, 2010).

This special care should continue when infants leave the hospital to go home. Con- sequently, intervention programs for small-for-date babies typically include training pro- grams designed for parents of infants and young children. In these programs, parents learn to respond appropriately to their child’s behaviors. For example, they are taught the signs that a baby is in distress, overstimulated, or ready to interact. Parents also learn how to use games and activities to foster their child’s development. In addition, children are enrolled in high-quality child care centers where the curriculum is coordinated with par- ent training. This sensitive care promotes development in low birth weight babies; for example, sometimes they catch up to full-term infants in terms of cognitive development (Hill, Brooks-Gunn, & Waldfogel, 2003).

Long-term positive outcomes for these infants depend critically on providing a supportive and stimulating home envi- ronment. Unfortunately, not all at-risk babies have these opti- mal experiences. Many experience stress or disorder in their family lives. In these cases, development is usually affected ( Poehlmann et al., 2011).

The importance of a supportive environment for low birth weight babies is underscored by the results of a 30-year longi- tudinal study by Werner (1989, 1995) covering all children born on the Hawaiian island of Kauai in 1955. When low birth weight children grew up in stable homes—defined as having two men- tally healthy parents throughout childhood—they were indis- tinguishable from children born without birth complications. However, when low birth weight children experienced an unsta- ble family environment—defined as including divorce, parental alcoholism, or parental mental illness—they lagged behind their peers in intellectual and social development.

Thus, when both biological and sociocultural forces are harmful—low birth weight plus inadequate medical care or family stress—the prognosis for babies is grim. The mes- sage to parents of low birth weight newborns is clear: Do not despair because excellent caregiving can compensate for all but the most severe birth problems (Werner, 1994; Werner & Smith, 1992).

Infant Mortality If you were the proud parent of a newborn and a citizen of Afghanistan, the odds are 1 in 6 that your baby would die before his or her first birthday—worldwide, Afghanistan has the highest infant mortality rate, defined as the percentage of infants who die before

infant mortality The number of infants out of 1,000 births who die before their first birthday.

low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 2,500 grams (5.5 pounds).

very low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 1,500 grams (3.3 pounds).

extremely low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 1,000 grams (2.2 pounds).

Small-for-date babies often survive, but their cognitive and motor development usually is delayed.

A bi

d Ka

tib /G

et ty

Im ag

es N

ew s/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 73

their first birthday. In contrast, if you were a parent and a citizen of the Czech Republic, Iceland, Finland, or Japan, the odds are less than 1 in 300 that your baby would die within a year because these countries have some of the lowest infant mortality rates.

The graphs in ◗ Figure 2.13 put these numbers in a global context, depicting infant mortality rates for 15 developed nations as well as for 15 least developed countries. Not surprisingly, risks to infants are far greater—about 20 times, on average—in the least developed nations compared with developed nations (Central Intelligence Agency, 2013). In fact, the differences are so great that the graphs for the two groups of nations must be drawn on different scales.

If you’re an American, you may be surprised to see that the United States ranks near the bottom of the list of developed nations. The difference is small, but if the United States were to reduce its infant mortality rate to the 4% that’s common in European countries, this would mean that 8,000 American babies who now die annually before their first birthday would live.

What explains these differences in infant mortality rates? The United States has more babies with low birth weight than virtually all other developed countries, and we’ve already seen that low birth weight places an infant at risk. Low birth weight can usually be prevented when a pregnant woman gets regular prenatal care, but many pregnant women in the United States receive inadequate or no prenatal care because they have no health insurance (Cohen, Martinez, & Ward, 2010). Virtually all countries that rank ahead of the United States provide complete prenatal care at little or no cost. Many of these countries also provide for paid leaves of absence for pregnant women (OECD, 2006).

In least developed countries, inadequate prenatal care is common and mothers often have inadequate nutrition. After birth, infants in these countries face the twin challenges of receiving adequate nutrition and avoiding disease. However, with improved prenatal care and improved health care and nutrition for infants, the global infant mortality has been cut in half since 1990 (UNICEF, 2007).

●◗ Figure 2.13 The infant mortality rate in least developed countries is much greater than in developed countries.

0

5

10

15

20

25

Ja pa

n

Sw ed

en Ita

ly

Fr an

ce Sp

ai n

Ge rm

an y

Ne th

er la

nd s

Ir ela

nd Is

ra el

Au str

al ia

Un ite

d Ki

ng do

m

Ne w

Z ea

la nd

Ca na

da

Un ite

d St

at es

Tu rk

ey

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

140

Bh ut

an

M ad

ag as

ca r

Ca m

bo di

a

Se ne

ga l

Su da

n

M au

rit an

ia

Bu ru

nd i

Rw an

da

Ug an

da

Za m

bi a

M oz

am bi

qu e

Si er

ra L

eo ne

An go

la

Af gh

an ist

an

So m

al ia

In fa

n t

m or

ta li

ty In

fa n

t m

or ta

li ty

Developed nations

Least Developed Nations

SOURCE: Data from Central Intelligence Agency 2013.

recall 1. In the third stage of labor, the _______________ is delivered.

2. Prepared childbirth includes communicating basic facts of labor and delivery, minimizing the use of medication to relieve pain, and _______________.

3. Home delivery is safe when a pregnant woman is healthy, has had a problem-free pregnancy, and expects to have a problem-free delivery and when _______________.

4. When the supply of oxygen to the fetus is disrupted because the umbilical cord is squeezed shut, _______________ results.

Interpret ●● Explain why some at-risk newborns develop normally but oth-

ers do not.

apply ●● Lynn is pregnant with her first child and would like to give birth

at home. Her husband is against the idea, claiming that it’s much too risky. What advice would you give them?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 2.4

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

74 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

2.1 In the Beginning: 23 Pairs of Chromosomes

What are chromosomes and genes? how do they carry hereditary information from one generation to the next?

●● At conception, the 23 chromosomes in the sperm merge with the 23 chromosomes in the egg. Each chromosome is one molecule of DNA; a section of DNA that provides spe- cific biochemical instructions is called a gene.

●● All of a person’s genes make up a genotype; the phenotype refers to the physical, behavioral, and psychological char- acteristics that develop when the genotype is exposed to a specific environment.

●● Different forms of the same gene are called alleles. A per- son who inherits the same allele on a pair of chromosomes is homozygous; in this case, the biochemical instructions on the allele are followed. A person who inherits differ- ent alleles is heterozygous; in this case, the instructions of the dominant allele are followed and those of the recessive allele ignored.

What are common problems involving chromosomes, and what are their consequences?

●● Most inherited disorders are carried by recessive alleles. Examples include sickle-cell disease and phenylketonuria, in which toxins accumulate and cause mental retardation.

●● Sometimes fertilized eggs do not have 46 chromosomes. Usually they are aborted spontaneously soon after concep- tion. An exception is Down syndrome, in which individuals typically have an extra 21st chromosome. Down syndrome individuals have a distinctive appearance and are mentally retarded. Disorders of the sex chromosomes are more common because these chromosomes contain less genetic material than do autosomes.

how is children’s heredity influenced by the environment in which they grow up?

●● Behavioral and psychological phenotypes that reflect an underlying continuum (such as intelligence) often involve polygenic inheritance. In polygenic inheritance, the pheno- type reflects the combined activity of many distinct genes. Traditionally, polygenic inheritance has been examined by studying twins and adopted children and, more recently, by identifying DNA markers.

●● The impact of heredity on a child’s development depends on the environment in which the genetic instructions are carried out, and these heredity–environment interac- tions occur throughout a child’s life. A child’s genotype can affect the kinds of experiences the child has; children and adolescents often actively seek environments related to their genetic makeup. Family environments affect

siblings differently (nonshared environmental influence); parents provide a unique environment for each child in the family.

2.2 From Conception to Birth

What happens to a fertilized egg in the first two weeks after conception?

●● The first period of prenatal development lasts two weeks. It begins when the egg is fertilized by the sperm in the fal- lopian tube and ends when the fertilized egg has implanted itself in the wall of the uterus. By the end of this period, cells have begun to differentiate.

When do body structures and internal organs emerge in  prenatal development?

●● The second period of prenatal development begins two weeks after conception and lasts until the end of the eighth week. This is a period of rapid growth in which most major body structures are created. Growth in this period is cepha- locaudal (the head develops first) and proximodistal (parts near the center of the body develop first).

When do body systems begin to function well enough to support life?

●● The third period of prenatal development begins nine weeks after conception and lasts until birth. The highlights of this period are a remarkable increase in the size of the fetus and changes in body systems that are necessary for life. By 7 months, most body systems function well enough to support life.

2.3 Influences on Prenatal Development

how is prenatal development influenced by a pregnant woman’s age, her nutrition, and the stress she experiences while pregnant?

●● The woman’s age can affect prenatal development. Teenag- ers often have problem pregnancies, mainly because they rarely receive adequate prenatal care. After age 35, preg- nant women are more likely to have a miscarriage or to give birth to a child with mental retardation. Prenatal develop- ment can also be harmed if a pregnant mother has inad- equate nutrition or experiences considerable stress.

how do diseases, drugs, and environmental hazards some- times affect prenatal development?

●● Teratogens are agents that can cause abnormal prenatal development. Many drugs that adults take are teratogens. For most drugs, scientists have not established amounts that can be consumed safely.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E r 2 : B I O L O G I C A L F O U N D AT I O N S : H E R E D I T Y, P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, A N D B I R T H 75

●● Several diseases are teratogens. Only by avoiding these dis- eases entirely can a pregnant woman escape their harmful consequences.

●● Environmental teratogens are particularly dangerous because a pregnant woman may not know that these sub- stances are present in the environment.

What general principles affect the ways that prenatal devel- opment can be harmed?

●● The impact of teratogens depends on the genotype of the organism, the period of prenatal development when the organism is exposed to the teratogen, and the amount of exposure. Sometimes the effect of a teratogen is not evident until later in life.

how can prenatal development be monitored? Can abnormal prenatal development be corrected?

●● Many techniques are used to track the progress of prenatal development. A common component of prenatal care is ultrasound, which uses sound waves to generate a picture of the fetus. This picture can be used to determine the position of the fetus; its sex; and gross physical deformities, if any.

●● When genetic disorders are suspected, amniocentesis, cho- rionic villus sampling, and noninvasive prenatal testing are used to determine the genotype of the fetus.

●● Fetal therapy is a new field in which problems of prenatal development are corrected medically via surgery or genetic engineering.

2.4 Labor and Delivery

What are the different phases of labor and delivery?

●● Labor consists of three stages. In stage 1, the muscles of the uterus contract. The contractions, which are weak at first and gradually become stronger, cause the cervix to enlarge. In stage 2, the baby moves through the birth canal. In stage 3, the placenta is delivered.

What are “natural” ways of coping with the pain of child- birth? Is childbirth at home safe?

●● Natural or prepared childbirth is based on the assumption that parents should understand what takes place during pregnancy and birth. In prepared childbirth, women learn to cope with pain through relaxation, imagery, and the help of a supportive coach.

●● Most American babies are born in hospitals, but home delivery can be safe when the mother is healthy, when preg- nancy and birth are trouble-free, and when a health care professional is present to deliver the baby.

What adjustments do parents face after a baby’s birth?

●● Following the birth of a child, a woman’s body undergoes several changes: her breasts fill with milk, her uterus becomes smaller, and hormone levels drop. Both parents also adjust psychologically, and sometimes fathers feel left out. After giving birth, some women experience postpar- tum depression: they are irritable, have poor appetite and disturbed sleep, and are apathetic.

What are some complications that can occur during birth?

●● During labor and delivery, the flow of blood to the fetus can be disrupted because the umbilical cord is squeezed shut. This causes hypoxia, a lack of oxygen to the fetus. Some babies are born prematurely, and others are “small for date.” Premature babies develop more slowly at first but catch up by 2 or 3 years of age. Small-for-date babies often do not fare well, particularly when they weigh less than 1,500 grams at birth and their environment is stressful.

What contributes to infant mortality in developed and least developed countries?

●● Infant mortality is relatively high in many countries around the world, primarily because of inadequate care before birth and disease and inadequate nutrition after birth.

Test Yourself: Recall Answers 2.1 1. autosomes 2. Polygenic inheritance 3. Down syndrome 4. the fertilized egg is aborted spontaneously 5. different from each other 2.2 1. at two weeks after conception (when the zygote is completely implanted in the wall of the uterus) 2. embryo 3. Between 22 and  28 weeks 4. learning and remembering 2.3 1. prolonged stress 2. Environmental hazards 3. spontaneous abortion of the fertilized egg 4. non-invasive prenatal testing 2.4 1. placenta 2. the presence of a supportive adult 3. trained health care professionals are present to deliver the baby 4. Hypoxia

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

76 Pa rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

chromosomes (40) autosomes (40) sex chromosomes (40) deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) (41) gene (41) genotype (41) phenotype (41) alleles (41) homozygous (41) heterozygous (41) dominant (42) recessive (42) incomplete dominance (42) sickle-cell trait (42) phenylketonuria (PKU) (43) Huntington’s disease (44) behavioral genetics (45) polygenic inheritance (45) monozygotic twins (46) dizygotic twins (46) heritability coefficient (48)

niche-picking (49) nonshared environmental influences (50) prenatal development (51) zygote (52) in vitro fertilization (52) preimplantation genetic screening

(PGS) (52) eugenics (52) implantation (53) germ disc (53) placenta (53) embryo (53) ectoderm (53) mesoderm (53) endoderm (53) amnion (54) amniotic fluid (54) umbilical cord (54) cephalocaudal principle (54) proximodistal principle (54) period of the fetus (55)

cerebral cortex (55) vernix (55) age of viability (56) spina bifida (57) stress (58) teratogen (59) fetal alcohol spectrum disorder

(FASD) (60) ultrasound (65) amniocentesis (65) chorionic villus sampling (CVS) (65) non-invasive prenatal testing (NIPT) (66) fetal therapy (66) crowning (67) hypoxia (71) cesarean section (C-section) (71) preterm (premature) (71) low birth weight (72) very low birth weight (72) extremely low birth weight (72) infant mortality (72)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

La ur

a D

w ig

ht /P

ho to

Ed it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

79

3Tools for Exploring the WorldPhysical, Perceptual, and Motor Development T hink about what you were like 2 years ago. Whatever you were doing, you probably look, act, think, and feel in much the same way today as

you did then. Two years in an adult’s life usually

doesn’t result in profound changes, but two years

makes a big difference early in life. The changes that

occur in the first few years after birth are incredible.

In less than two years, an infant is transformed from

a seemingly helpless newborn into a talking, walking,

havoc-wreaking toddler. No changes at any other

point in the life span come close to the drama and

excitement of these early years.

In this chapter, our tour of these two years

begins with the newborn and then moves to physical

growth—changes in the body and the brain. The third

section of the chapter examines motor skills. You’ll

discover how babies learn to walk and how they learn to use their hands to

hold and then manipulate objects. In the fourth section, we’ll examine changes

in infants’ sensory abilities that allow them to comprehend their world.

As children begin to explore their world and learn more about it, they also

learn more about themselves. They learn to recognize themselves and begin

to understand more about their thoughts and others’ thoughts. We’ll explore

these changes in the last section of the chapter.

3.1 The Newborn 80 The Newborn’s Reflexes 80 Assessing the Newborn 80 The Newborn’s States 82 Temperament 85

3.2 Physical Development 87 Growth of the Body 87 The Emerging Nervous System 91

3.3 Moving and Grasping: Early Motor Skills 96 Locomotion 96 Fine Motor Skills 100 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Benefits of Training

Babies to Grasp 101

3.4 Coming to Know the World: Perception 102 Smell, Taste, and Touch 103 Hearing 103 Seeing 104 Integrating Sensory Information 108

3.5 Becoming Self-Aware 109 Origins of Self-Concept 110 Theory of Mind 111 REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

“Seeing Is Believing . . .” for 3-Year-Olds 112

    Summary 113

    Key Terms 115

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

80 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do reflexes help newborns interact with the world?

●● How do we determine whether a baby is healthy and adjusting to life outside the uterus?

●● What behavioral states are common among newborns?

●● What are the different features of temperament? Do they change as children grow?

3.1 The Newborn

Lisa and Steve, proud but exhausted parents, are astonished at how their lives revolve around 10-day-old Dan’s eating and sleeping. Lisa feels as if she is feeding Dan around the clock. When Dan naps, Lisa thinks of many things she should be doing but usually naps herself because she is so tired. Steve wonders when Dan will start sleeping through the night so that he and Lisa can get a good night’s sleep.

The newborn baby who thrills parents like Lisa and Steve is actually rather homely. New- borns arrive covered with blood and vernix, a white-colored “wax” that protected the skin during the many months of prenatal development. In addition, the baby’s head is temporar- ily distorted from its journey through the birth canal, and the newborn has a beer belly and is bowlegged.

What can newborns like Dan do? We’ll answer that question in this section, and as we do, you’ll learn when Lisa and Steve can expect to get a full night’s sleep again.

The Newborn’s Reflexes Most newborns are well prepared to begin interacting with their world; they are endowed with a rich set of reflexes, unlearned responses that are triggered by a specific form of stimulation. Table 3.1 shows the variety of reflexes commonly found in newborn babies.

Some reflexes pave the way for newborns to get the nutrients they need to grow: The rooting and sucking reflexes ensure that the newborn is prepared to begin a new diet of life-sustaining milk. Other reflexes protect the newborn from danger in the environment. The eye blink, for example, helps newborns avoid unpleasant stimulation.

Still other reflexes are the foundation for larger, voluntary patterns of motor activity. For example, the stepping reflex motions look like precursors to walking, so it’s not surprising that babies who practice the stepping reflex often learn to walk earlier than those who don’t practice (Zelazo, 1993).

Reflexes are also important because they can help determine whether the newborn’s nervous system is working properly. For exam- ple, infants with damage to the sciatic nerve, which is found in the spinal cord, do not show the withdrawal reflex. Infants who have prob- lems with the lower part of the spine do not show the Babinski reflex. If these or other reflexes are weak or missing altogether, a thorough physical and behavioral assessment is necessary. Similarly, many of these reflexes normally vanish during infancy; if they linger, this, too, indicates the need for a thorough physical examination.

Assessing the Newborn Imagine that a mother has just asked you if her newborn baby is healthy. How would you decide? You would probably check to see whether the baby seems to be breathing and if the baby’s heart seems to be beating.

reflexes Unlearned responses triggered by specific stimulation.

This newborn baby (Ben Kail at 20 seconds old) is covered with vernix and is bow-legged; his head is distorted from the journey down the birth canal.

Ro be

rt V

. K ai

l

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 81

In fact, breathing and heartbeat are two vital signs included in the Apgar score, which provides a quick assessment of the newborn’s sta- tus by focusing on the body systems needed to sustain life. The other vital signs are muscle tone, presence of reflexes such as coughing, and skin tone. Each of the five vital signs receives a score of 0, 1, or 2, where 2 is optimal. For example, a newborn whose muscles are completely limp receives a 0; a baby who moves arms and legs vigorously receives a 2. The five scores are summed, with a total score of 7 or more indi- cating that a baby is in good physical condition. A score of 4 to 6 means that a newborn needs special attention and care. A score of 3 or less signals a life-threatening situation requiring emergency medical care (Apgar, 1953).

For a comprehensive evaluation of the newborn’s well-being, pediatricians and other child development specialists sometimes administer the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale, or NBAS (Brazelton & Nugent, 1995). The NBAS includes 28 behavioral items (e.g., the baby’s response to light, sound, and touch) along with 18 items that test reflexes such as those we just described. The baby’s performance is used to evaluate the functioning of these four systems:

●● Autonomic: the baby’s ability to control body functions such as breathing and temperature regulation

●● Motor: the baby’s ability to control body movements and activity level

●● State: the baby’s ability to maintain a state (e.g., staying alert or staying asleep) ●● Social: the baby’s ability to interact with people

The NBAS is based on the view that newborns are remarkably competent individuals who are well prepared to interact with the environment. Reflecting this view, examiners go to great lengths to bring out a baby’s best performance by making a baby feel comfort- able and secure during testing. And if an infant does not at first succeed on an item, the examiner assists (Alberts, 2005).

Table 3.1

Some Major Reflexes Found in Newborns

NAME RESPONSE AGE WHEN REFLEX DISAPPEARS SIGNIFICANCE

Babinski A baby’s toes fan out when the sole of the foot is stroked from heel to toe

8–12 months Perhaps a remnant of evolution

Blink A baby’s eyes close in response to bright light or loud noise Permanent Protects the eyes

Moro A baby throws its arms out and then inward (as if embracing) in response to loud noise or when its head falls

6 months May help a baby cling to its mother

Palmar A baby grasps an object placed in the palm of its hand 3–4 months Precursor to voluntary walking

Rooting When a baby’s cheek is stroked, it turns its head toward the stroking and opens its mouth

3–4 weeks (replaced with voluntary head turning)

Helps a baby find the nipple

Stepping A baby who is held upright by an adult and is then moved forward begins to step rhythmically

2–3 months Precursor to voluntary walking

Sucking A baby sucks when an object is placed in its mouth 4 months (replaced with voluntary sucking)

Permits feeding

Think About It Newborns seem to be extremely well prepared to begin to interact with their environment. Which of the theories described in Chapter 1 predict such preparedness? Which do not?

Newborns exhibit the Moro reflex, opening their arms and then bringing them inward in response to loud noise or when their head falls.

Pi ct

ur e

Pa rt

ne rs

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

82 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Besides the NBAS being useful to clinicians in evaluating the well-being of individ- ual babies, researchers also have found it a valuable tool. Sometimes performance on the NBAS is used as a dependent variable. For example, infants whose mothers were depressed during pregnancy have lower scores on the NBAS (e.g., Suri et al., 2014). Researchers also use scores on the NBAS to predict later development; for example, in one study infants with greater scores on the NBAS were, as 10-year-olds, more empathic and less prone to accidents (Feldman, 2015).

The Newborn’s States Newborns spend most of each day alternating between four different states (St. James- Roberts & Plewis, 1996; Wolff, 1987):

●● Alert inactivity—The baby is calm with eyes open and attentive; the baby seems to be deliberately inspecting the environment.

●● Waking activity—The baby’s eyes are open, but they seem unfocused; the arms or legs move in bursts of uncoordinated motion.

●● Crying—The baby cries vigorously, usually accompanied by agitated but uncoordinated motion.

●● Sleeping—The baby alternates between being still and breathing regularly to moving gently and breathing irregularly; eyes are closed throughout.

Of these states, crying and sleeping have captured the attention of parents and researchers alike.

Crying Newborns spend two to three hours each day crying or on the verge of crying. If you’ve not spent much time around newborns, you might think that all crying is pretty much alike. In fact, scientists and parents can identify three distinctive types of cries (Snow, 1998). A basic cry starts softly and gradually becomes more intense; it usually occurs when a baby is hungry or tired. A mad cry is a more intense version of a basic cry; a pain cry begins with a sudden, long burst of crying followed by a long pause and gasping. Thus, crying repre-

sents the newborn’s first venture into interpersonal communication. By crying, babies tell their parents that they are hungry or tired, angry or hurt. By responding to these cries, parents are encouraging their newborn’s efforts to communicate.

Parents are naturally concerned when their baby cries, and if they can’t quiet a crying baby, their concern mounts and can easily give way to frustration and annoyance. It’s no surprise then that parents develop little tricks for soothing their babies. Many Western parents lift a baby to the shoulder and walk or gently rock the baby. Sometimes they also sing lullabies, pat the baby’s back, or give the baby a pacifier. Yet another method is to put a newborn in a car seat and go for a drive; this technique was used once, as a last resort, at 2 a.m. with Ben Kail when he was 10 days old. After about the 12th time around the block, he finally stopped crying and fell asleep.

Another useful technique is swaddling, in which an infant is wrapped tightly in a blanket. Swaddling is used in many cultures around the world, including Turkey and Peru, as well as by Native Americans. Swaddling provides warmth and tactile stimu- lation that usually works well to soothe a baby (Delaney, 2000).

Parents are sometimes reluctant to respond to their crying infant for fear of producing a baby who cries constantly. Yet they hear their baby’s cry as a call for help that they shouldn’t ignore. What to do? Should parents respond? Yes, until their baby is about 3 months old. However, with older babies, parents should consider why their infant is crying and the intensity of the crying (St. James-Roberts, 2007). When an older baby wakes during the night and cries quietly, a parent should wait before responding, giving the baby a chance to calm herself. Of course, if parents hear a loud noise from an infant’s bedroom followed by a mad cry, they should respond

alert inactivity State in which a baby is calm with eyes open and attentive; the baby seems to be deliberately inspecting the environment.

waking activity State in which a baby’s eyes are open but seem unfocused while the arms or legs move in bursts of uncoordinated motion.

crying State in which a baby cries vigorously, usu- ally accompanied by agitated but uncoordi- nated movement.

sleeping State in which a baby alternates between being still and breathing regularly to mov- ing gently and breathing irregularly; the eyes are closed throughout.

basic cry Cry that starts softly and gradually becomes more intense; often heard when babies are hungry or tired.

mad cry More intense version of a basic cry.

pain cry Cry that begins with a sudden long burst, followed by a long pause and gasping.

In many countries worldwide, infants are wrapped tightly in blankets as a way to keep them calm.

Ey e

U bi

qu ito

us /N

ew sc

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 83

immediately. Parents need to remember that crying is the newborn’s first attempt to com- municate. They need to decide what the infant is trying to tell them and whether that war- rants a quick response or whether they should let the baby soothe herself.

Sleeping Crying may get parents’ attention, but sleep is what newborns do more than anything else. They sleep 16 to 18 hours daily. The problem for tired parents is that newborns sleep in naps taken around the clock. Newborns typically go through a cycle of wake- fulness and sleep about every four hours. That is, they will be awake for about an hour, sleep for three hours, and then start the cycle anew. During the hour when newborns are awake, they regularly move between the different waking states several times. Cycles of alert inactivity, waking activity, and crying are common.

As babies grow older, the sleep–wake cycle gradually begins to correspond to the day–night cycle (St. James- Roberts & Plewis, 1996). By 3 or 4 months, many babies sleep for 5 to 6 hours straight, and by 6 months, many are sleeping for 10 to 12 hours at night, a major milestone for bleary-eyed parents such as Lisa and Steve.

By 6 months, most North American infants are sleeping in a crib in their own room. Although this practice seems “natural” to North American parents, in much of the rest of the world (and for much of human history), children sleep with their parents throughout infancy and the preschool years. Such parent–child “co-sleeping” is more common in Africa and in Asia than in Europe and North America—a regional split that coincides approximately with cultures where people are seen as interdependent (Africa, Asia) versus cultures that see people as independent and self-reliant (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2016; Tan, 2009).

How does co-sleeping work? Infants may sleep in a cradle placed next to their parents’ bed or in a basket that’s in their parents’ bed. When they outgrow this arrangement, they sleep in the bed with their mother; depending on the culture, the father may sleep in the same bed, in another bed in the same room, in another room, or in another house altogether.

Perhaps you’re skeptical about co-sleeping because you’ve heard reports about babies who died while sleeping with their parents. In fact, research suggests that co-sleeping is dangerous only when parents smoke, drink, or sleep with babies in sofas or chairs, and with babies who were born prematurely or with low birth weight (Fleming & Blair, 2015). And one advantage of co-sleeping is that it facilitates breastfeeding: moms who breastfeed often decide to co-sleep and this simplifies breastfeeding (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2016).

Beyond safety and breastfeeding, scientists, healthcare professionals, and parents have wondered how co-sleeping affects whether babies sleep well, become emotion- ally attached to their mothers, and become autonomous. Unfortunately, research pro- vides no solid answers to these questions. They’re difficult to answer, in part, because researchers must consider whether co-sleeping is a well-established cultural practice or just a “new trend” and whether parents choose to co-sleep because they believe in its benefits or if they resort to co-sleeping to address a baby’s sleep-related problems (Mileva-Seitz et al., 2016).

Roughly half of newborns’ sleep is irregular or rapid-eye-movement (rEM) sleep, a time when the body is quite active. During REM sleep, newborns move their arms and legs; they may grimace, and their eyes may dart beneath their eyelids. Brain waves register fast activity, the heart beats more rapidly, and breathing is more rapid. In regular or nonrEM sleep, breathing, heart rate, and brain activity are steady and newborns lie quietly without the twitching associated with REM sleep. Newborns spend about equal amounts of time in REM and non-REM sleep. REM sleep becomes less frequent as infants grow: By the first birthday, REM sleep drops to about 33%, not far from the adult average of 20% ( Lushington et al., 2013).

irregular or rapid-eye-movement (rEM) sleep Irregular sleep in which an infant’s eyes dart rapidly beneath the eyelids while the body is quite active.

regular or nonrEM sleep Sleep in which heart rate, breathing, and brain activity are steady.

Think About It When Mary’s 4-month-old son cries, she rushes to him immediately and does everything possible to console him. Is this a good idea?

Co-sleeping, in which infants and young children sleep with their parents, is common in many countries around the world.

Pe r-A

nd er

s Pe

tt er

ss on

/G et

ty Im

ag es

N ew

s/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

84 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

The function of REM sleep is still debated. Older children and adults dream during REM sleep, and brain waves during REM sleep resemble those of an alert, awake per- son. Consequently, many scientists believe that REM sleep provides stimulation for the brain that fosters growth in the nervous system (Halpern et al., 1995; Roffwarg, Muzio, & Dement, 1966).

By the toddler and preschool years, sleep routines are well established. Most 2-year- olds spend about 13 hours sleeping, compared to just under 11 hours for 6-year-olds. By age 4, most youngsters give up their afternoon nap and sleep longer at night to compen- sate. This can be a challenging time for parents and caregivers who use naptime as an opportunity to complete some work or to relax.

Following an active day, most preschool children drift off to sleep easily. However, most children have an occasional night when bedtime is a struggle. Furthermore, for approximately 20% to 30% of preschool children, bedtime struggles occur nightly (Lozoff, Wolf, & Davis, 1985). More often than not, these bedtime problems reflect the absence of a regular bedtime routine. The key to a pleasant bedtime is to establish a nighttime rou- tine that helps children to “wind down” from busy daytime activities. This routine should start at about the same time every night (“It’s time to get ready for bed.”) and end at about the same time (when the parent leaves the child and the child tries to fall asleep). This nighttime routine may be anywhere from 15 to 45 minutes long, depending on the child. Also, as children get older, parents can expect them to perform more of these tasks inde- pendently. A 2-year-old will need help all along the way, but a 5-year-old can do many of these tasks alone. But parents must remember to follow the routine consistently; this way, children know that each step is getting them closer to bedtime and falling asleep.

Sudden Infant Death Syndrome For many parents of young babies, however, sleep is a cause of concern. In sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS), a healthy baby dies suddenly for no apparent reason. Approxi- mately 2 of every 1,000 American babies die from SIDS. Most of them are between 2 and 4 months of age (Wegman, 1994).

Scientists don’t know the exact causes of SIDS, but one idea is that 2- to 4-month- old infants are particularly vulnerable to SIDS because many newborn reflexes are wan- ing during these months and thus infants may not respond effectively when breathing becomes difficult. They may not reflexively move their head away from a blanket or pillow that is smothering them (Lipsitt, 2003).

Researchers have also identified several risk factors associated with SIDS (Carpenter et  al., 2013; Sahni, Fifer, & Myers, 2007). Babies are more vulnerable if they were born prematurely or with low birth weight. They are also more vulnerable when their parents smoke. SIDS is more likely when a baby sleeps on its stomach (face down) than when it sleeps on its back (face up). Finally, SIDS is more likely during winter, when babies some- times become overheated from too many blankets and sleepwear that is too heavy (Carroll & Loughlin, 1994). Evidently, SIDS infants, many of whom were born prematurely or with low birth weight, are less able to withstand physiological stresses and imbalances that are brought on by cigarette smoke, breathing that is temporarily interrupted, or overheating (Simpson, 2001).

In 1992, based on mounting evidence that SIDS occurred more often when infants slept on their stomachs, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) began advising par- ents to put babies to sleep on their backs or sides. In 1994, the AAP joined forces with the U.S. Public Health Service to launch a national program to educate parents about the dangers of SIDS and the importance of putting babies to sleep on their backs. The “Back to Sleep” campaign was widely publicized through brochures, posters like the one in ◗ Figure 3.1, and videos.

Since the “Back to Sleep” campaign began, the incidence of SIDS has been cut in half, but it still remains the leading cause of death in 1- to 12-month-olds (Hakeem et al., 2015). Consequently, in the 21st century, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) focused on groups in which SIDS is more common, including African Americans and Native

sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) When a healthy baby dies suddenly for no apparent reason.

●◗ Figure 3.1 This poster is one part of an effective campaign to reduce SIDS by encourag- ing parents to put their babies to sleep on their backs.

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 85

Americans. The NIH developed ways to convey the “Back to Sleep” message in a culturally appropriate manner to African American communities (NICHD, 2004). In addition, the NIH developed educational programs for nurses and pharmacists. In 2012, the campaign was named Safe to Sleep and included additional recommendations to keep infants safe while asleep. Through these policies, the NIH hopes to spread the word to parents and others who care for infants: The keys to safe sleeping include keeping babies away from smoke, putting them on a firm mattress on their backs to sleep, and not overdressing them or wrapping them too tightly in blankets.

Temperament So far, we’ve talked as if all babies are alike. But if you’ve seen a number of babies together, you know that isn’t true. Perhaps you’ve seen some babies who were quiet most of the time alongside others who cried often and impatiently? Maybe you’ve known infants who responded warmly to strangers next to others who seemed shy? These characteristics of infants indicate a consistent style or pattern to an infant’s behavior, and collectively they define an infant’s temperament.

Alexander Thomas and Stella Chess (Thomas & Chess, 1977; Thomas, Chess, & Birch, 1968) pioneered the study of temperament with the New York Longitudinal Study, in which they traced the lives of 141 individuals from infancy through adulthood. Thomas and Chess interviewed parents about their babies and had individuals unfamiliar with the children observe them at home. From these interviews and observations, Thomas and Chess suggested that infants’ behavior varied along nine temperamental dimensions. One was activity, which referred to an infant’s typical level of motor activity. A second was per- sistence, which referred to the amount of time an infant devoted to an activity, particularly when obstacles were present.

The New York Longitudinal Study launched research on infant temperament, but today we know that Thomas and Chess overestimated the number of temperamental dimensions. Instead of nine dimensions, scientists now propose from two to six dimen- sions. For example, Mary K. Rothbart (2011) devised an influential theory of temperament that includes three different dimensions:

●● Surgency/extroversion refers to the extent to which a child is generally happy, active, and vocal and regularly seeks interesting stimulation.

●● Negative affect refers to the extent to which a child is angry, fearful, frus- trated, shy, and not easily soothed.

●● Effortful control refers to the extent to which a child can focus attention, is not readily distracted, and can inhibit responses.

These dimensions of temperament emerge in infancy, continue into childhood, and are related to dimensions of personality that are found in adolescence and adulthood (Gartstein, Knyazev, & Slobodskaya, 2005). However, the dimen- sions are not independent: Infants who are high on effortful control tend to be high on surgency/extroversion and low on negative affect. In other words, babies who can control their attention and inhibit responses tend to be happy and active but not angry or fearful.

Hereditary and Environmental Contributions to Temperament Temperament reflects both heredity and experience. The influence of heredity is shown in twin studies and adoption studies. For example, the three dimen- sions of Rothbart’s model—surgency, negative affect, and effortful control— are strongly influenced by heredity; if one identical twin is skilled at effortful control, the odds are that the other twin will also be skilled (Saudino & Micalizzi, 2015).

temperament Consistent style or pattern of behavior.

Twin studies show the impact of heredity on temperament: If one identical twin is active, the other one usually is, too.

St ua

rt M

on k/

Fo to

lia

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

86 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

The environment also contributes to children’s temperament. Positive emotionality—youngsters who laugh often, seem to be generally happy, and express pleasure often—seems to reflect environmental influences (Goldsmith, Buss, & Lemery, 1997). What’s more, infants are less emotional when parents are responsive (Gudmund- son & Leerkes, 2012; Leerkes, Blankson, & O’Brien, 2009). Conversely, infants become increasingly fearful when their mothers are depressed (Gartstein et al., 2010). And some temperamental characteristics are more common in some cultures than in oth- ers. For example, compared with toddlers from North America and Europe, toddlers in Asia often have greater effortful control but lower levels of positive emotion (Krassner et al., 2017).

Of course, in trying to determine the contributions of heredity and environ- ment to temperament, the most likely explanation is that both contribute (Henderson & Wachs, 2007). In fact, one view is that temperament may make some children par- ticularly susceptible to environmental influences—either beneficial or harmful (Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007). For example, in one study (Kochanska, Aksan, & Joy, 2007), emotionally fearful children were more likely to cheat in a game when their parents’ discipline emphasized asserting power (e.g., “Do this now and don’t argue!”). Yet these children were least likely to cheat when parents were nurturing and supportive. In other words, temperamentally fearful preschoolers can become dishonest or honest, depending on their parents’ disciplinary style.

There’s no question that heredity and experience cause babies’ temperaments to dif- fer, but how stable is temperament? We’ll find out in the next section.

Stability of Temperament Do calm, easygoing babies grow up to be calm, easygoing children, adolescents, and adults? Are difficult, irritable infants destined to grow up to be cranky, whiny children? In fact, temperament is moderately stable throughout infancy, childhood, and adoles- cence (Janson & Mathiesen, 2008; Wachs & Bates, 2001). For example, 6-month-olds who laugh and smile often tend to become 12-month-olds who laugh and smile often

(Bornstein et al., 2015). In addition, when inhibited toddlers are adults, they respond more strongly to unfamiliar stimuli (Schwartz et al., 2003).

Thus, evidence suggests that temperament is at least somewhat stable throughout infancy and the toddler years (Lemery et al., 1999). Of course, the links are not perfect. Sam, an emotional 1-year-old, is more likely to be emotional as a 12-year-old than Dave, an unemotional 1-year-old. However, it’s not a “sure thing” that Sam will still be emo- tional as a 12-year-old. Instead, think of temperament as a predisposi- tion. Some infants are naturally predisposed to be sociable, emotional, or active; others can act in these ways, too, but only if the behaviors are nurtured by parents and others.

Although temperament is only moderately stable during infancy and toddlerhood, it can still shape development in important ways. For example, an infant’s temperament may determine the experiences that parents provide. Parents may read more to quiet babies but play more physical games with their active babies. These different experiences, driven by the infants’ temperament, contribute to each infant’s devel- opment despite the fact that the infants’ temperament may change over the years. Thus, although infants have many features in common, tem- perament characteristics remind us that each baby also seems to have a unique personality from the very start.

Think About It How would a learning theorist explain why children have different temperaments?

Children’s temperament influences the way that adults treat them; for example, parents engage in more vigorous play when their children are temperamentally active.

Ri ch

ar d

Lo rd

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 87

While crossing the street, 4-year-old Martin was struck by a passing car. He was in a coma for a week but then gradually became more alert. Now he seems to be aware of his sur- roundings. Needless to say, Martin’s mother is grateful that he survived the accident, but she wonders what the future holds for her son.

For parents and children alike, physical growth is a topic of great interest and a source of pride. Parents marvel at the speed with which babies add pounds and inches, and 2-year- olds proudly proclaim, “I bigger now!” In this section, we examine some of the basic features of physical growth, see how the brain develops, and discover how the accident affected Martin’s development.

Growth of the Body Growth is more rapid in infancy than during any other period after birth. Typically, infants double their birth weight by 3 months of age and triple it by their first birthday. This rate of growth is so rapid that if continued throughout childhood, a typical 10-year-old boy would be nearly as long as a jumbo jet and weigh almost as much (McCall, 1979).

Average heights and weights for young children are represented by the lines marked 50th percentile in ◗ Figure 3.2. An average girl weighs about 7 pounds at birth, about 21  pounds at 12 months, and about 26 pounds at 24 months. If perfectly average, she would be 19 to 20 inches long at birth and grow to 29 to 30 inches at 12 months and 34  to  35  inches at 24 months. Figures for an average boy are similar, but weights are slightly greater at ages 12 and 24 months.

recall 1. Some reflexes help infants get necessary nutrients, other

reflexes protect infants from danger, and still other reflexes _______________.

2. The _______________ is based on five vital functions and pro- vides a quick indication of a newborn’s physical health.

3. A baby lying calmly with his or her eyes open and focused is in a state of _______________.

4. Newborns spend more time asleep than awake, and about half this time asleep is spent in _______________, a time thought to foster growth in the central nervous system.

5. The campaign to reduce SIDS emphasizes that infants should _______________.

6. Research on the stability of temperament in infants and young children typically finds that _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare the Apgar and the NBAS as measures of a newborn

baby’s well-being.

Apply ●● Based on what you know about the stability of tempera-

ment, what would you say to a parent who’s worried that her 15-month-old seems shy and inhibited?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 3.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do height and weight change from birth to 2 years of

age?

●● What nutrients do young children need? How are nutrients best provided?

●● What are the consequences of malnutrition? How can it be treated?

●● What are nerve cells, and how are they organized in the brain?

●● How does the brain develop? When does it begin to function?

3.2 Physical Development

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

88 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

These charts also highlight how much children of the same age vary in weight and height. The lines marked 90th percentile in ◗ Figure 3.2 represent heights and weights for children who are larger than 90% of their peers; the lines marked 10th percentile represent heights and weights for children who are smaller than 90% of their peers. Any heights and weights between these lines are considered normal. At age 1, for example, normal weights for boys range from about 19 to 27 pounds. This means that an extremely light but normal boy weighs only two-thirds as much as his extremely heavy but normal peer.

The important message here is that average height and normal height are not one and the same. Many children are much taller or shorter than average but are still perfectly normal. This applies to all of the age norms that we mention in this book. Whenever we provide a typical or average age for a developmental milestone, remember that the normal range for passing the milestone is much wider.

Whether an infant is short or tall depends largely on heredity. Both parents contribute to their children’s height. In fact, the correlation between the average of the two parents’ heights and their child’s height at 2 years of age is about 0.7 (Plomin, 1990). As a general rule, two tall parents will have tall offspring, two short parents will have short offspring, and one tall parent and one short parent will have offspring of medium height.

So far we have emphasized the quantitative aspects of growth, such as height. This ignores an important fact: Infants are not simply scaled-down versions of adults. ◗ Figure 3.3

●◗ Figure 3.2 Boys and girls grow taller and heavier from birth to 3 years of age, but the range of normal heights and weights is wide.

42

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

26

28

30

32

34

36

38

40

L en

gt h

( in

.)

B 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33 36

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

26

28

30

32

34

36

38

40

42

44

46

48

W ei

gh t

(l b

)

Age (months)

42

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

26

28

30

32

34

34

38

40

L en

gt h

( in

.)

B 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33 36

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

26

28

30

32

34

36

38

40

42

44

46

48

W ei

gh t

(l b

)

Age (months)

LengthLength

90th percentile90th percentile

90th percentile90th percentile

50th percentile

50th percentile

10th percentile10th percentile

10th percentile10th percentile

50th percentile

50th percentile

WeightWeight

Boys Girls

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Think About It In Chapter 2, we explained how poly- genic inheritance is often involved when phenotypes form a continuum. Height is such a phenotype. Propose a simple polygenic model to explain how height might be inherited.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 89

shows that compared with adolescents and adults, infants and young children look top-heavy because their heads and trunks are disproportionately large. As growth of the hips, legs, and feet catches up later in childhood, their bodies take on more adult proportions. This pattern of growth, in which the head and trunk develop first, follows the cephalocaudal principle introduced in Chapter 2 (page 54).

Growth of this sort requires energy. Let’s see how food and drink provide the fuel to grow.

“You Are What You Eat”: Nutrition and Growth In a typical 2-month-old, roughly 40% of the body’s energy is devoted to growth. Most of the remaining energy is used for basic bodily functions such as digestion and respira- tion. A much smaller portion is consumed in physical activity. Because growth requires so much high energy, young babies must consume about 50 calories per pound of weight (compared with 15 to 20 calories per pound for an adult).

Breastfeeding is the best way to ensure that babies get the nourishment they need. Human milk contains the proper amounts of carbohydrates, fats, protein, vitamins, and minerals for babies. Breastfeeding also has several other advantages compared with bottle- feeding. First, breastfeeding has short- and long-term health-related benefits for babies. In the short-term, they are less likely to be ill (because breast milk contains a mother’s antibodies) and less likely to die (because they are less prone to diarrhea, a leading cause of death for infants in developing countries). In the long-term, breastfed babies are less prone to become obese or to develop diabetes (Victora et al., 2016). Second, breastfed babies typically transition to solid foods more easily, apparently because they are accus- tomed to changes in the taste of breast milk that reflect a mother’s diet. Third, breast milk cannot be contaminated, which is a significant problem in developing countries when for- mula is used to bottle-feed babies.

Experts recommend that children be breastfed until they are 2 years old and that they be breastfed exclusively for the first six months (UNICEF, 2016). In fact, in many developing nations in African, Asia, and Latin America, mothers approximate these guidelines, but in the United States and other developed nations, most mothers stop breastfeeding by 12 months, often because it is inconvenient when they return to full- time work (Victora et  al., 2016). Consequently, health experts have devised interven- tions to encourage mothers to breastfeed from birth through 2 years of age. These interventions (e.g., guidance from a lactation counselor) are effective (Rollins et al., 2016). If they were successful worldwide in meeting guidelines for breastfeeding, this would reduce childhood mortality by more than 800,000 deaths annually (Victora et al., 2016).

●◗ Figure 3.3 The head and trunk develop before the hips, legs, and feet, which gives young children a top-heavy appearance.

2 months (fetal)

5 months (fetal)

Age

Newborn 18 months 7 years 13 years 16 years

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

90 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

In developing nations, bottle-feeding is potentially disastrous. Often the only water available to prepare formula is contaminated; the result is that infants have chronic diarrhea, leading to dehydration and sometimes death. Or in an effort to conserve valuable formula, parents may ignore instructions and use less formula than indicated when making milk; the resulting “weak” milk leads to malnutri- tion. For these reasons, the World Health Organization strongly advocates breast- feeding as the primary source of nutrition for infants and toddlers in developing nations.

By 2 years, growth slows; so children need less to eat. This is also a time when many children become picky eaters, and toddlers and preschool children may find that foods they once ate willingly are now “yucky.” As a toddler, Laura Kail loved green beans. When she reached 2, she decided that green beans were awful and refused to eat them. Although such finickiness can be annoying, it may actually be adaptive for increasingly independent preschoolers. Because toddlers don’t know what is and isn’t safe to eat, eating only familiar foods protects them from poten- tial harm (Aldridge, Dovey, & Halford, 2009).

Parents should not be overly concerned about this finicky period. Although some children eat less than before (in terms of calories per pound), virtually all picky eaters get adequate food for growth. Nevertheless, several methods can be used to encourage youngsters to eat more healthfully:

●● Reward children when they eat healthy foods; in one study (Cooke et al., 2011), when 4- to 6-year-olds received a sticker after they tasted a vegetable, consumption increased sixfold and persisted for 3 months after rewards were dropped.

●● Parents can model eating unfamiliar foods; when parents seem to be enjoy- ing novel fruits and vegetables, young children are more willing to try them ( Edelson, Mokdad, & Martin, 2016).

Toddlers and preschool children often become picky eaters. This can be annoying but should not con- cern parents.

●● Teach children about nutrition, emphasizing that different body functions require a diverse diet that includes a variety of nutrients; in one study (Gripshover & Markman, 2013), preschoolers taught these concepts from story books ate twice as many vegetables.

●● Offer children new foods over several meals, in small amounts; encourage but don’t force children to eat new foods; and when children reject a new food, continue to offer it over several meals so that it will become familiar (Lam, 2015).

Collectively, these methods can help children receive the nutrition they need to grow.

Malnutrition An adequate diet is only a dream to many of the world’s children. Worldwide, about one in four children under age 5 is malnourished, as indicated by being small for their age. Many, like the children in the photo on page 91, are from developing countries in Southern Asia (e.g., Bangladesh, India, Pakistan) and Africa (UNICEF-WHO-The World Bank, 2016). But malnutrition is regrettably common in industrialized countries, too. Many American children growing up homeless and in poverty are malnourished. Approximately 13% of American households have difficulty at some point in providing adequate food for all fam- ily members (Coleman-Jensen et al., 2016).

Malnourishment is especially damaging during infancy because growth is rapid dur- ing these years. By the school-age years, children with a history of infant malnutrition are easily distracted in school. Malnutrition during rapid periods of growth apparently dam- ages the developing brain, affecting a child’s abilities to pay attention and learn (Nyaradi et al., 2013).

Malnutrition would seem to have a simple cure—an adequate diet. But the solution is more complex than you might expect. Malnourished children are often listless and inactive, behaviors that are useful because they conserve limited energy. Unfortunately, when children are routinely unresponsive and lethargic, parents often come to believe that their actions have little impact on the children. Over time, parents tend to provide

malnourished Being small for one’s age because of inad- equate nutrition.

D av

id P

ag e

Ph ot

og ra

ph y/

Ph ot

ol ib

ra ry

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 91

fewer experiences that foster their children’s development. In other words, a bio- logical influence (lethargy stemming from insufficient nourishment) causes a pro- found change in the experiences (parental teaching) that foster a child’s development (Aboud & Yousafzai, 2015).

Thus, to counter the effects of malnutrition, children need more than an improved diet. Parents must be taught how to foster their children’s development and must be encouraged to do so. Programs that combine dietary supplements with par- ent training offer promise in treating malnutrition. Children in these programs often improve their physical and intellectual growth, showing that the best way to reduce the effect of malnutrition on psychological forces is by addressing both biological and sociocultural forces (Yousafzai et al., 2016).

The Emerging Nervous System The physical changes we see as infants grow are impressive. Even more awe-inspiring are the changes we cannot see—those involving the brain and the nervous system. An infant’s feelings of hunger or pain, the infant’s smiles or laughs, and his or her efforts to sit upright or to hold a rattle all reflect the functioning of the brain and the rest of the emerging nervous system.

How does the brain accomplish these many tasks? To answer this question, we need to look at the organization of the brain. The basic unit in the brain and the rest of the nervous system is the neuron, a cell that specializes in receiving and transmitting information. Neurons have the basic elements shown in ◗ Figure 3.4. The cell body, in the center of the cell, contains the basic biological machinery that keeps the neuron alive. The receiving end of the neuron, the dendrite, looks like a tree with its many branches. This structure allows one neuron to receive input from thousands of other neurons (Morgan & Gibson, 1991). The tubelike structure that emerges from the other side of the cell body, the axon, transmits information to other neurons. At the end of the axon are small knobs called terminal buttons, which release chemicals called neu- rotransmitters. These neurotransmitters are the messengers that carry information to nearby neurons.

With 50 to 100 billion neurons such as these, you have the beginnings of a human brain. An adult’s brain weighs a little less than 3 pounds and easily fits in your hands. The wrinkled surface of the brain is the cerebral cortex; made up of 10 billion neurons, the

neuron Basic cellular unit of the brain and nervous system that specializes in receiving and transmitting information.

cell body Center of the neuron that keeps the neuron alive.

dendrite End of the neuron that receives informa- tion; it looks like a tree with many branches.

axon Tubelike structure that emerges from the cell body and transmits information to other neurons.

terminal buttons Small knobs at the end of the axon that release neurotransmitters.

neurotransmitters Chemicals released by the terminal buttons that allow neurons to communicate with each other.

cerebral cortex Wrinkled surface of the brain that regulates many functions that are distinctly human.

Many children around the world are malnourished.

Bu rg

er /P

ha ni

e/ Su

pe rs

to ck

●◗ Figure 3.4 A nerve cell includes dendrites that receive information; a cell body that has life-sustaining machinery; and for sending information, an axon that ends in terminal buttons.

Cell body

Dendrites

Terminal buttons

Direction of information flow

Axon

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

92 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

cortex regulates many of the functions that we think of as distinctly human. The cortex consists of left and right halves, called hemispheres, linked by a thick bundle of neurons called the corpus callosum. The characteristics you value the most—your engaging per- sonality, your “way with words,” or your uncanny knack for “reading” others’ emotions— are all controlled by specific regions in the cortex (◗ Figure 3.5). For example, your personality and your ability to make and carry out plans are largely centered in an area in the front of the cortex called the frontal cortex. For most people, the ability to produce and understand language is mainly housed in neurons in the left hemisphere of the cor- tex. When you recognize that others are happy or sad, neurons in your right hemisphere are usually at work.

Now that we know how the mature brain is organized, let’s look at how it grows and begins to function.

Emerging Brain Structures The brain weighs only 12 ounces at birth, which is roughly 25% of the weight of an adult brain. But the brain grows rapidly during infancy and the preschool years. However, brain weight doesn’t tell us much about the fascinating changes that take place to create a work- ing brain. Instead, we need to look back at prenatal development.

The beginnings of the brain can be traced to the period of the zygote. At roughly three weeks after conception, a group of cells form a flat structure known as the neural plate. At four weeks, the neural plate folds to form a tube that ultimately becomes the brain and spinal cord. When the ends of the tube fuse shut, neurons are produced in one small region of the neural tube. Production of neurons begins about 10 weeks after conception, and by 28 weeks, the developing brain has virtually all the neurons it will ever have. During these weeks, neurons form at the incredible rate of more than 3,000 per second. Surprisingly, many of these newly formed neurons are short-lived: they are programmed to die, creating space for nearby neurons to form connections (Stiles, 2008).

From the neuron-manufacturing site in the neural tube, neurons migrate to their final positions in the brain. The brain is built in stages, beginning with the innermost layers. Neurons in the deepest layer are positioned first, followed by neurons in the second layer, and so on. This layering process continues until all six layers of the mature brain are in place, which occurs about seven months after conception (Rakic, 1995).

In the fourth month of prenatal development, axons begin to acquire myelin—the fatty wrap that speeds neural transmission. This process continues through infancy into child- hood and adolescence (Paus, 2010). Neurons that carry sensory information are the first to acquire myelin; neurons in the cortex are among the last. You can see the effect of more

hemispheres Right and left halves of the cortex.

corpus callosum Thick bundle of neurons that connects the two hemispheres.

frontal cortex Brain region that regulates personality and goal-directed behavior.

neural plate Flat group of cells present in prenatal devel- opment that becomes the brain and spinal cord.

myelin Fatty sheath that wraps around neurons and enables them to transmit information more rapidly.

●◗ Figure 3.5 The brain on the left, viewed from above, shows the left and right hemispheres. The brain on the right, viewed from the side, shows the major regions of the cortex and their primary functions.

Parietal cortex

Frontal cortex (planning, personality,

some aspects of emotion) Parietal cortex

(body sensations)

Occipital cortex

Occipital cortex (vision)

Temporal cortex (hearing, advanced visual processing)

© C

en ga

ge L

ea rn

in g®

Co ur

te sy

o f D

r. D

an a

Co pe

la nd

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 93

myelin in improved coordination and reaction times. The older the infant and (later) the child, the more rapid and coordinated his or her reactions.

In the months after birth, the brain grows rapidly. Axons and dendrites grow longer, and like a maturing tree, dendrites quickly sprout new limbs. As the number of dendrites increases, so does the number of synapses, reaching a peak at about the first birthday. Soon after, synapses begin to disappear gradually, a phenomenon known as synaptic pruning. Thus, beginning in infancy and continuing into early adolescence, the brain goes through its own version of “downsizing,” weeding out unnecessary connections between neurons. This pruning depends on the activity of the neural circuits: Synapses that are active are preserved, but those that aren’t active are eliminated (Webb, Monk, & Nelson, 2001). Pruning is completed first for brain regions associated with sensory and motor functions. Regions associated with basic language and spatial skills are completed next, followed by regions associated with attention and planning (Casey et al., 2005).

Growth of a Specialized Brain Because the mature brain is specialized, with different psychological functions localized in particular regions, developmental researchers have had a keen interest in determining the origins and time course of the brain’s specialization. For many years, the only clues to spe- cialization came from children who had suffered brain injury. The logic linked the location of the injury to the resulting impairment: If a region of the brain regulates a particular func- tion (e.g., understanding speech), then damage to that region should impair the function.

Fortunately, relatively few children suffer brain injury. But this meant that scientists needed other methods to study brain development. One of them, electroencephalography, involves measuring the brain’s electrical activity from electrodes placed on the scalp, as shown in the photo at the top of page 94. If a region of the brain regulates a function, then the region should show distinctive patterns of electrical activity while a child is using that function. A newer technique, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMrI), uses mag- netic fields to track the flow of blood in the brain. With this method, shown in the photo at the bottom of page 94, the research participant’s brain is literally wrapped in an incredibly powerful magnet that can track blood flow in the brain as participants perform different cognitive tasks (Casey et al., 2005). The logic is that active brain regions need more oxygen, which increases blood flow to those regions.

None of these methods is perfect; each has drawbacks. For example, fMRI is used sparingly because it’s expensive and participants must lie still for several minutes at a time. Despite these limitations, the combined outcome of research using these different approaches has identified some general principles that describe the brain’s specialization as children develop.

synaptic pruning Gradual reduction in the number of syn- apses, beginning in infancy and continuing until early adolescence.

electroencephalography The study of brain waves recorded from electrodes that are placed on the scalp.

functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMrI) Method of studying brain activity by using magnetic fields to track blood flow in the brain.

From birth to 2 years, neurons grow and create many new synapses with other neurons.

At birth 1 month 3 months 5 months 24 months

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

A rg

os y

Pu bl

is hi

ng In

c.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

94 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

parietal cortex, which is directly behind the frontal cortex—that responds to changes in number (Edwards et al., 2016).

2. Specialization takes two specific forms. First, with development, the brain regions active during processing become more focused, much like the difference between a weak thunderstorm that covers a large areas versus a more powerful storm that covers a smaller region (Haist et al., 2013). Second, the kinds of stimuli that trig- ger brain activity shift from being general to being specific (Johnson, Grossman, & Cohen Kadosh, 2009). Processing of facelike stimuli by the brain shows both trends: It becomes focused in a particular area (the fusiform gyrus) and becomes tuned narrowly to faces (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2013; Haist et al., 2013).

3. Different brain systems specialize at different rates. Think of a new housing devel- opment involving construction of many multistory homes. In each house, the first floor is completed before higher floors, but some houses are finished before others are even started. In this same way, brain regions involving basic sensory and per- ceptual processes specialize well before those regions necessary for higher-order processes (Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010). Similarly, some brain systems that are sensitive to reward (especially to rewards from peers) may reach maturity in ado- lescence, but the systems responsible for self-control aren’t fully specialized until adulthood (Casey & Caudle, 2013; Galván, 2013).

4. Successful specialization requires stimulation from the environment. To return to the analogy of the brain as a house, the newborn’s brain is perhaps best conceived as a partially finished, partially furnished house: A general organizational framework is

present, with preliminary neural pathways designed to perform cer- tain functions. The left hemisphere has some language pathways, and the frontal cortex has some emotion-related pathways. However, completing the typical organization of the mature brain requires input from the environment (Greenough & Black, 1992). In this case, environmental input influences experience-expectant growth—over the course of evolution, human infants have typically been exposed to some forms of stimulation that are used to adjust brain wiring, strengthening some circuits and eliminating others. For example, under normal conditions, healthy human infants experience moving visual patterns (e.g., faces) and varied sounds (e.g., voices). Just as a newly planted seed depends on a water-filled environment for growth, a developing brain depends on environmental stimulation to fine-tune circuits for vision, hearing, and other systems (Black, 2003). And when this expected stimulation is missing, brain

experience-expectant growth Process by which the wiring of the brain is organized by experiences that are common to most humans.

Electrodes placed on an infant’s scalp can detect electrical activity that is used to cre- ate an electroencephalogram, a pattern of the brain’s response to stimulation.

db lig

ht /E

+/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

In fMRI, a magnet is used to track the flow of blood to different regions of the brain as children and adults perform cognitive tasks.

PH IL

IP PE

M ER

LE /A

FP /G

et ty

Im ag

es

1. Specialization is evident early in development. Maybe you expect the brain to be completely unspecialized? In fact, many regions are already specialized very early in infancy. For example, early specialization of the frontal cortex is shown by the finding that damage to this region in infancy results in impaired decision making and abnormal emotional responses (Anderson et al., 2001). Similarly, studies using electroen- cephalography show that a newborn infant’s left hemisphere generates more electrical activity in response to speech than does the right hemi- sphere (Maitre et al., 2014). Thus, by birth, the cortex of the left hemisphere is already special- ized for language processing. As we’ll see in Chapter 4, this specialization allows language to develop rapidly during infancy. Finally, studies tracking the flow of blood in the infant’s brain indicate a region in the right hemisphere—the

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 95

development can go awry: For example, young children reared in institutions receive far less of the cognitive and social stimulation that most parents provide; this often leads to underdevelopment of the brain regions that support processing of complex cognitive and social information (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014).

Of course, experiences later in life also sculpt the brain (and we’ll see this in several chapters later in this book). Experience-dependent growth denotes changes in the brain that are not linked to specific points in development and that vary across individuals and across cultures. Experience-dependent growth is illustrated by a preschool child’s learning of a classmate’s name, an elementary school child’s discovery of a shortcut home from school, and an adolescent’s mastery of the func- tions of a new cell phone. In each case, brain circuits are modified in response to an individual’s experiences. With today’s technology, we can’t see these daily changes in the brain. But when they accumulate over many years—as when individuals acquire expertise in a skill—brain changes can be detected. For example, skilled cel- lists have extensive brain regions devoted to controlling the fingers of the left hand as they are positioned on the strings (Elbert et al., 1995). And years of driving a taxicab produces changes in the hippocampus, a region of the brain implicated in navigation and way-finding (Maguire, Woollett, & Spiers, 2006).

5. The immature brain’s lack of specialization confers a benefit—greater plasticity. Just as the structures in a housing development follow a plan that specifies the location of each house and its design, brain development usually follows a predict- able course that reflects epigenetic interactions (page 48) between the genetic code and required environmental input. Sometimes, however, the normal course is disrupted. A person may experience events harmful to the brain (e.g., injured in an accident) or may be deprived of some essential ingredients of successful “brain building” (e.g., necessary experiences).

Research that examines the consequences of these atypical experiences shows that the brain has some flexibility: It is plastic. Remember Martin, the child in the vignette whose brain was damaged when he was struck by a car? His language skills were impaired after the accident. This was not surprising because the left hemi- sphere of Martin’s brain had absorbed most of the force of the collision. But within several months, Martin had completely recovered his language skills. Apparently other neurons took over language-related processing from the damaged neurons. This recovery of function is not uncommon—particularly for young children—and shows that the brain is plastic. In other words, young children often recover more skills after brain injury than older children and adults, apparently because functions are more easily reassigned in the young brain (Kolb & Teskey, 2012; Demir, Levine, & Goldin-Meadow, 2010).

experience-dependent growth Process by which an individual’s unique experiences over a lifetime affect brain structures and organization.

recall 1. Compared to older children and adults, an infant’s head and

trunk are _______________.

2. Because of the high demands of growth, infants need _______________ calories per pound than do adults.

3. The most effective treatment for malnutrition is improved diet and _______________.

4. The _______________ is the part of the neuron that contains the basic machinery to keep the cell alive.

5. The frontal cortex is the seat of personality and regulates _______________.

6. Human speech typically elicits the greatest electrical activity from the _______________ of an infant’s brain.

7. A good example of brain plasticity is that although children with brain damage often have impaired cognitive processes, _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare growth of the brain before birth with growth of the

brain after birth.

Apply ●● How does malnutrition illustrate the influence on development

of life-cycle forces in the biopsychosocial framework?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 3.2

Think About It What’s an example of experience- expectant growth for U.S. 3-year-olds? What’s a common type of experience- dependent growth at this age?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

96 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Nancy is 14 months old and a world-class crawler. Using hands and knees, she can go just about anywhere she wants. Nancy does not walk and seems uninterested in learning how. Nancy’s dad wonders whether he should be doing something to help Nancy progress beyond crawling. Deep down, he worries that perhaps he was negligent in not providing more exercise for Nancy when she was younger. Do you remember what it was like to learn to type, to drive a car with a stick shift, to play a musical instrument, or to play a sport? Each of these activities involves motor skills: coor- dinated movements of the muscles and limbs. Success demands that each movement be done precisely, in exactly the right sequence, and at exactly the right time. For example, in the few seconds that it takes you to type human development, if you don’t move your fin- gers in the correct sequence to the precise location on the keyboard, you might get jinsj drveo;nrwnt.

These activities are demanding for adults, but think about similar challenges for infants. Infants must learn to move about in the world, to locomote. Newborns are rela- tively immobile, but infants soon learn to crawl, stand, and walk. Learning to move through the environment upright leaves the arms and hands free, which allows infants to grasp and manipulate objects. Infants must learn the fine motor skills associated with grasping, hold- ing, and manipulating objects. In the case of feeding, for example, infants progress from being fed by others, to holding a bottle, to feeding themselves with their fingers, to eating with utensils.

Together, locomotion and fine motor skills give children access to an enormous vari- ety of information about shapes, textures, and features in their environment. In this sec- tion, we’ll see how locomotion and fine motor skills develop. As we do, we’ll see whether Nancy’s dad should worry about her lack of interest in walking.

Locomotion Advances in posture and locomotion transform the infant in little more than a year. ◗ ●Figure 3.6 shows the age by which most infants achieve important milestones in motor development. By about 5 months of age, most babies have rolled from back to front and can sit upright with support. By 7 months, infants can sit alone, and by 10 months, they can creep. A typical 14-month-old is able to stand alone briefly and walk with assistance. This early, unsteady form of walking is called toddling (hence the term toddler). Of course, not all children walk at the same age. Some walk before their first birthday; others—like Nancy, the world-class crawler in the vignette—take their first steps as late as 18 or 19 months of age. By 24 months, most children can climb steps, walk backward, and kick a ball.

Researchers once thought these developmental milestones reflected maturation (e.g., McGraw, 1935). Walking, for example, emerged naturally when the necessary muscles and neural circuits matured. However, today locomotion—and, in fact, all of motor development—is viewed from a new perspective. According to dynamic systems theory, motor development involves many distinct skills that are organized and reorganized over time to meet the demands of specific tasks. For example, walking includes maintaining bal- ance, moving limbs, perceiving the environment, and having a reason to move. Only by understanding each of these skills and how they are combined to allow movement in a specific situation can we understand walking (Thelen & Smith, 1998).

motor skills Coordinated movements of the muscles and limbs.

locomote To move around in the world.

fine motor skills Motor skills associated with grasping, hold- ing, and manipulating objects.

toddling Early, unsteady form of walking done by infants.

toddler Young children who have just learned to walk.

dynamic systems theory Theory that views motor development as involving many distinct skills that are orga- nized and reorganized over time to meet specific needs.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the component skills involved in learning

to walk? At what age do infants master them? ●● How do infants learn to coordinate the use

of their hands?

3.3 Moving and Grasping: Early Motor Skills

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 97

Posture and Balance The ability to maintain an upright posture is fundamental to walking. But upright posture is virtually impossible for newborns and young infants because of they are top-heavy. Con- sequently, as soon as a young infant starts to lose her balance, she tumbles over. Only with growth of the legs and muscles can infants maintain an upright posture. After infants can stand upright, they adjust their posture to avoid falling down, relying on visual cues to stay upright (Adolph & Berger, 2015).

Balance is not, however, something that infants master just once. Instead, infants must relearn balancing for sitting, crawling, walking, and other postures. Why? The body rotates around different points in each posture (e.g., the wrists for crawling versus the ankles for walking), and different muscle groups are used to generate compensating motions when infants begin to lose their balance. Consequently, it’s hardly surprising that infants who easily maintain their balance when sitting still topple over time after time when crawling. And after they walk, infants must adjust their posture further when they carry objects because these affect balance (Garciaguirre, Adolph, & Shrout, 2007). Infants must recalibrate the balance system as they take on each new posture, just as basketball players recalibrate their muscle movements when they move from dunking to shooting a three-pointer (Adolph, 2000, 2002).

●◗ Figure 3.6 Locomotor skills improve rapidly in the 15 months after birth, and progress can be measured by many developmental milestones.

Pull to stand by furniture 12 months

Fetal posture 0 months

Walk when led 11 months

Creep 10 months

Stand holding furniture 9 months

Stand with help 8 months

Sit alone 7 months

Sit on high chair; grasp dangling object 6 months

Chin up 1 month

Chest up 2 month

Reach and miss 3 month

Sit with support 4 month

Sit on lap; grasp object 5 month

Climb stair steps 13 months

Stand alone 14 months

Walk alone 15 months

B as

ed o

n B

ay le

y, 1

96 9;

S hi

rl ey

, 1 93

1.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

98 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Stepping Another essential element of walking is moving the legs alternately, repeatedly transferring the weight of the body from one foot to the other. Children don’t step spontaneously until approximately 10 months because they must be able to stand in order to step.

Can younger children step if they are held upright? Thelen and Ulrich (1991) devised a clever procedure to answer this question. Infants were placed on a treadmill and held upright by an adult. When the belt on the treadmill started to move, infants could respond in one of several ways. They might simply let both legs be dragged rearward by the belt. Or they might let their legs be dragged briefly, then move them forward together in a hopping motion. Many 6- and 7-month- olds demonstrated the mature pattern of alternating steps on each leg. Apparently, the alternate stepping motion that is essential for walking is evident long before infants walk alone. Walking unassisted is not possible, though, until other component skills are mastered.

Environmental Cues Many infants learn to walk in the relative security of flat, uncluttered floors at home. But they soon discover that the environment offers a variety of surfaces, some more conducive to walking than others. Infants use cues in the environment to judge whether a surface is suitable for walking. For example, they are more likely to cross a bridge when it’s wide and has a rigid handrail than when it is narrow and has a wob- bly handrail (Berger, Adolph, & Lobo, 2005; Kretch & Adolph, 2013b). And when walking down stairs, if a step is too large to descend safely, novice walkers often continue (and fall), but older experienced walkers either stop or slide down on their backs; only experienced walkers rec- ognize the cues that signal steps are safe for walking (Kretch & Adolph, 2013a). Finally, expert walkers use a parent’s social cues (“Come on!”

“No, stop!”) but mainly when they can’t decide for themselves if a surface is safe (Karasik, Tamis-LeMonda, & Adolph, 2016). Results like these show that infants use cues from the environment to decide whether a surface is safe for walking.

Coordinating Skills Dynamic systems theory emphasizes that learning to walk demands orchestration of many individual skills. Each component skill must first be mastered alone and then integrated with the other skills (Werner, 1948). That is, mastery of intricate motions requires both differentiation (mastery of component skills) and integration—combining the motions in proper sequence into a coherent, working whole. In the case of walking, not until 12 to 15 months of age have children mastered the component skills to be coordinated and so allow independent, unsupported walking.

Mastering individual skills and coordinating them well does not happen overnight. Novice walkers take nearly 1,500 steps per hour, covering about one-fifth of a mile, and falling more than 30 times; infants obviously get much natural practice (along with feed- back from falls) as they master walking (Adolph et al., 2012).

These findings from laboratory research are not the only evidence that practice pro- motes motor development; cross-cultural research points to the same conclusion. In Europe and North America, most infants typically walk alone near their first birthday. But infants in other cultures often begin to walk (and reach the other milestones listed on page  97) at an earlier age because childcare practices allow children to practice their emerging motor skills. For example, in some traditional African cultures, infants sit and walk at younger ages. Why? Infants are commonly carried by their parents piggyback style, which helps develop muscles in the infants’ trunk and legs.

differentiation Distinguishing and mastering individual motions.

integration Linking individual motions into a coherent, coordinated whole.

Infants are capable of stepping—moving the legs alternately—long before they can walk alone.

D ex

te r G

or m

le y

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 99

Some cultures even go a step further. They believe that practice is essential for motor skills to develop normally, so they (or siblings) provide daily training sessions. For example, the Kipsigis of Kenya help chil- dren learn to sit by having them sit while propped up (Super, 1981). Among the West Indians of Jamaica, mothers have an exercise routine that allows babies to practice walking (Hopkins & Westra, 1988). Not sur- prisingly, infants with these opportunities learn to sit and walk earlier, findings that are confirmed by experi- mental work in which parents participate in activities that let babies practice controlling their bodies (Lobo & Galloway, 2012).

You may be surprised that some cultures do just the opposite—they have practices that discourage motor development. The Ache, an indigenous group in Para- guay, protect infants and toddlers from harm by carry- ing them constantly (Kaplan & Dove, 1987). In Chinese cities, parents often allow their children to crawl only on a bed surrounded by pillows, in part because they don’t want their children crawling on a dirty floor (Campos et al., 2000). In both cases, infants reach motor milestones a few months later than the ages listed in the chart on page 97.

Even European and North American infants are reaching motor milestones at older ages today than did previous generations (Miller et al., 2011). This difference reflects the effectiveness of the Back to Sleep campaign described on page 84. Because today’s babies spend less time on their tummies, they have fewer opportunities to discover that they can propel themselves by creeping, which would otherwise prepare them for crawling.

Thus, cultural practices can accelerate or delay the early stages of motor development, depending on the nature of practice that infants and toddlers receive (Adolph & Robinson, 2013). In the long run, however, the age of mas- tering various motor milestones is not critical for children’s development. All healthy children learn to walk, and whether this happens a few months before or after the “typical” ages shown on page 97 has no bearing on children’s later development (Lung & Shu, 2011).

Beyond Walking If you can recall the feeling of freedom that accompanied your first driv- er’s license, you can imagine how the world expands for infants and tod- dlers as they learn to move independently. The first tentative steps soon are followed by others that are more skilled. With more experience, infants take longer, straighter steps. Like adults, they begin to swing their arms, rotating the left  arm forward as the right leg moves, then repeating with the  right arm  and left leg (Ledebt, 2000; Ledebt, van Wieringen, & Savelsbergh, 2004).

Children’s growing skill is evident in their running and hopping. Most 2-year-olds have a hurried walk instead of a true run; they move their legs stiffly (rather than bending them at the knees) and are not airborne as is the case when running. By 5 or 6 years, children run easily, quickly changing directions or speed.

Infants use their new walking skills to get distant objects—a favorite toy that’s in a different room of the house—and carry them to share with other people (Karasik, Tamis-Lemonda, & Adolph, 2011). In the next section, we’ll see how infants’ fine motor skills allow them to grasp objects.

In many African cultures, infants are routinely carried piggyback style; this strengthens the infant’s legs, which allows them to walk at a younger age.

Br itt

a/ Ve

tt a/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Locomotor skills develop rapidly in preschool children, making it possible for them to play vigorously.

iS to

ck .c

om /p

om ar

in us

Think About It How does learning to hop on one foot demonstrate differentiation and integra- tion of motor skills?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

100 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Fine Motor Skills A major accomplishment of infancy is skilled use of the hands. Newborns have little apparent control of their hands, but 1-year- olds are extraordinarily talented.

Reaching and Grasping At about 4 months, infants can successfully reach for objects (Ber- tenthal & Clifton, 1998). These early reaches often look clumsy, because infants don’t move their arm and hand smoothly to the desired object. Instead, the infant’s hand follows a zigzag path: It moves a short distance, slows, then moves again in a slightly dif- ferent direction—a process that’s repeated until the hand finally contacts the object. As infants grow, their reaches have fewer movements, although they still are not as continuous and smooth as reaches by older children and adults (Adolph & Berger, 2015).

Reaching requires that an infant move the hand to the location of a desired object. Grasping poses a different challenge: Now the infant must coordinate movements of indi- vidual fingers to grab an object. Over the first year, infants become more skilled in orient- ing their hand and positioning their fingers to grasp an object. For example, if trying to grasp a long thin rod, infants place their fingers perpendicular to the rod, which is the best position for grasping. And babies learn when to grip for power (using all fingers) versus when to grip for precision (using only the thumb and forefinger). By 12 months, babies have become skilled at grasping, an accomplishment that allows them to enjoy playing with objects.

Infants’ growing control of each hand is accompanied by greater coordination of the two hands. Although 4-month-olds use both hands, their motions are not coordinated; rather, each hand seems to have a mind of its own. Infants may hold a toy motionless in one hand while shaking a rattle in the other. At roughly 5 to 6 months of age, infants can coordinate the motions of their hands so that each hand performs different actions that serve a common goal. So a child might, for example, hold a toy animal in one hand and pet it with the other (Karniol, 1989). These skills continue to improve after the child’s first birthday: One-year-olds reach for most objects with one hand; by 2 years, they reach with one or two hands, as appro- priate, depending on the size of the object (van Hof, van der Kamp, & Savelsbergh, 2002).

These gradual changes in fine motor coordination are well illustrated by the ways chil- dren feed themselves. Beginning at roughly 6 months of age, many infants experiment with “finger foods” such as sliced bananas and green beans. Infants can easily pick up such foods, but getting them into their mouths is another story. The hand grasping the food may be raised to the cheek, then moved to the edge of the lips, and finally shoved into the mouth. Mission accomplished, but only after many detours along the way! However, infants’ eye–hand coordination improves rapidly, and foods varying in size, shape, and texture are soon placed directly in the mouth.

At about the first birthday, many parents allow their children to try eating with a spoon. Youngsters first play with the spoon, dipping it in and out of a dish filled with food or sucking on an empty spoon. Soon they learn to fill the spoon with food and place it in their mouth, but the motions are awkward. For example, most 1-year-olds fill a spoon by first placing it directly over a dish. Then they lower the spoon until its bowl is full. In contrast, 2-year-olds typically scoop food from a dish by rotating their wrist, which is the same motion adults use.

As preschoolers, children become more dexterous and are able to make many pre- cise and delicate movements with their hands and fingers. Greater fine motor skill means that preschool children begin to care for themselves. No longer must they rely primar- ily on parents to feed and clothe them; instead, they become increasingly skilled at feed- ing and dressing themselves. A 2- or 3-year-old, for example, can put on simple clothing and use zippers but not buttons; by 3 or 4 years, children can fasten buttons and take off their clothes when going to the bathroom; and most 5-year-olds can dress and undress themselves—except for tying shoes, which children typically master at about age 6.

A typical 4-month-old grasps an object with fingers alone.

Lu ca

re lli

T em

is to

cl e/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 101

On ResearchSpotlight

Benefits of Training Babies to Grasp

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? By 4 to 6 months, babies can reach and grasp objects, actions that allow them to explore objects and, in the process, foster their cognitive, social, and linguistic skills. Before 4 months, babies have little success when trying to grasp objects. If they were better able to grasp, would they benefit from exploring objects? Answering this question was the aim of research by Klaus Libertus and his colleagues, Amy Joh and Amy Needham (2010, 2016).

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Libertus and colleagues created “sticky” mittens for 3-month-olds. The mittens were covered with Velcro® as were interesting toys. Wearing the mittens, any contact allowed babies to “grasp” and explore the object. For 2 weeks, the babies wore the mittens daily for 10 minutes. In one control condition, babies wore mittens that had no Velcro®; these babies played with toys and, during the daily play sessions, par- ents held the toys against the mittens (simu- lating the experience of babies wearing sticky mittens). After 2 weeks of training, babies came to the researchers’ lab, where a color- ful rattle was placed within their reach; the researchers recorded babies’ efforts to grasp the rattle. About one year later, the procedure was repeated, with the rattle replaced by a bead maze. In two other control conditions, babies received no training at all but were just tested in the laboratory, either as 3-month- olds or as 15-month-olds.

Who were the participants in the study? The study included 55 3-month-olds and 15 15-month-olds.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental. The independent variable was the nature of the training expe- rience (sticky mittens, regular mittens, no training). The dependent variable was the per- centage of time that the babies tried to grasp the novel toys. The study included a longitu- dinal component (babies in the mitten condi- tions who were tested as 3-month-olds and again as 15-month-olds) as well as cross- sectional component (babies who received no training and were tested as 3-month-olds or as 15-month-olds).

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. There was no obvious harm asso- ciated with wearing the mittens or playing with toys.

What were the results? The researchers calculated the percentage of time that babies reached for the toys. As you can see in ◗ Figure 3.7, at 3 and 15 months, babies who wore the sticky mittens were more likely than babies in the other groups to grasp objects. In contrast, wearing regular mittens did not affect babies’ reaching.

What did the investigators conclude? Training babies to reach had immediate and long-term benefits: Babies who wore the sticky mittens were more likely to reach immediately after training and approximately one year later. Training may have affected babies directly—they learned the benefits of reaching and grasping—and may have affected them indirectly—when parents see babies interested in objects around them,

they more often engage their children in object-oriented play.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? One useful extension of this work would be to see if the impact of training persists beyond 15 months and if it extends to domains beyond grasping (e.g., learning the names of objects). Another extension would be to determine whether the training affects parent-child play, particularly play that involves objects.

●◗ Figure 3.7 When babies were trained to reach using “sticky” mittens, they were more likely to reach for objects, both imme- diately after training and one year later.

Sticky mittens Regular mittens No mittens

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

3 months 15 months Age

P er

ce n

ta ge

o f

ti m

e re

ac h

in g

an d

gr as

p in

g

Babies wearing sticky mittens grasped more often.

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

All these actions illustrate the principles of differentiation and integration that were introduced in our discussion of locomotion. Complex acts involve simple movements that must be performed correctly and in the proper sequence. Development involves first mastering the separate elements and then assembling them into a smoothly functioning whole. And, as we’ll see in the Spotlight on Research feature, having infants practice these movements can have long-lasting consequences.

Handedness Before birth, most children are emergent right handers: fetuses are more likely to suck their right thumb than their left, to move the right arm than the left, and to turn the head to the right rather than to the left. After birth, babies reach more often and more accurately with their right hand than with the left (Fagard, 2013). By the first birthday, most young- sters use their left hand to steady the toy while the right hand manipulates the object.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

102 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

This early preference for one hand becomes stronger and more consistent during the preschool years and is well established by kindergarten (Mars- chik et al., 2008; Nelson, Campbell, & Michel, 2013).

What determines whether children become left- or right-handed? Not surprisingly, heredity and environment both contribute. Scientists believe that as many as 30 to 40 different genes may play a role in deter- mining whether children are left or right handed (Ocklenburg, Beste, & Güntürkün, 2013). But experience also contributes. Many cultures have traditionally viewed left-handedness as evil and have punished children for using their left hand to eat or write. Not surprisingly, left-handed children are rare in these cultures. Similarly, in many developed nations, elementary school teachers used to urge left-handed children to write with their right hands. As this practice was abandoned, the percentage of left-handed children has increased (Provins, 1997). Thus, handedness is influenced by both heredity and environment.

By age 5, fine motor skills are developed to the point that most youngsters can dress themselves.

M ed

io im

ag es

/P ho

to di

sc /G

et ty

Im ag

es

recall 1. According to _______________, motor development involves

many distinct skills that are organized and reorganized over time, depending on task demands.

2. Skills important in learning to walk include maintaining upright posture and balance, stepping, and _______________.

3. Akira uses both hands simultaneously, but not in a coordi- nated manner; each hand seems to be “doing its own thing.” Akira is probably _______________ months old.

4. At one year, most infants use their left hand to steady a toy and their right hand to _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast the milestones of locomotor develop-

ment in the first year with the fine motor milestones.

Apply ●● Describe how the mastery of a fine motor skill—such as learn-

ing to use a spoon or a crayon—illustrates the integration of biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces in the bio- psychosocial framework.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 3.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Are infants able to smell, taste, and experience pain?

●● How well do infants hear?

●● How well can infants see? Can they see color and depth?

●● How do infants coordinate information between different sensory modalities, such as between vision and hearing?

3.4 Coming to Know the World: Perception

Darla is mesmerized by her newborn daughter, Olivia. Darla loves holding Olivia, talking to her, and simply watching her. Darla is certain that Olivia is already getting to know her and is coming to recognize her face and the sound of her voice. Darla’s husband, Steve, thinks she is crazy: “Everyone knows that babies are born blind, and they probably can’t hear much either.” Darla doubts Steve and wishes someone could tell her the truth about Olivia’s vision and hearing.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 103

To answer Darla’s questions, we need to define what it means for an infant to experi- ence or sense the world. Humans have several kinds of sense organs, each receptive to a different kind of physical energy. For example, the retina at the back of the eye is sensi- tive to some types of electromagnetic energy, and sight is the result. The eardrum detects changes in air pressure, and hearing is the result. Cells at the top of the nasal passage detect the passage of airborne molecules, and smell is the result. In each case, the sense organ translates the physical stimulus into nerve impulses that are sent to the brain. The process by which the brain receives, selects, modifies, and organizes these impulses is known as perception. This is the first step in the complex process of accu- mulating information that eventually results in “knowing.”

Darla’s questions are really about her newborn daughter’s perceptual skills. By the end of this section, you’ll be able to answer her questions because we’re going to look at how infants use different senses to experience the world.

Smell, Taste, and Touch Newborns have a keen sense of smell. Infants respond positively to pleasant smells and negatively to unpleasant smells (Mennella & Beauchamp, 1997). They have a relaxed and contented facial expression when they smell honey or chocolate, but they frown or turn away when they smell rotten eggs or ammonia. Young babies also recognize familiar odors. Newborns look in the direction of a pad that is saturated with their own amniotic fluid (Schaal, Marlier, & Soussignan, 1998). They also turn toward a pad saturated with the odor of their mother’s breast or her perfume (Porter & Winberg, 1999).

Newborns also have a highly developed sense of taste. They readily differentiate salty, sour, bitter, and sweet tastes (Schwartz, Issanchou, & Nicklaus, 2009). Most infants prefer sweet and salty substances—they react to them by smiling, sucking, and licking their lips (Beauchamp & Mennella, 2011) but grimace when fed bitter or sour substances (Kaijura, Cowart, & Beauchamp, 1992). Infants are also sensitive to changes in the taste of breast milk that reflect a mother’s diet and will nurse more after their mother has consumed a sweet-tasting substance such as vanilla (Mennella & Beauchamp, 1996).

Newborns are sensitive to touch. As we saw earlier in this chapter, many areas of the newborn’s body respond reflexively when touched. Touching an infant’s cheek, mouth, hand, or foot produces reflexive movements, documenting that infants perceive touch. What’s more, babies’ behavior in response to apparent pain-provoking stimuli suggests that they experience pain. Look, for example, at the baby in the photo who is receiving an inoculation. She’s opened her mouth to cry, and although we can’t hear her, the sound of her cry is probably the unique pattern associated with pain. The pain cry begins suddenly, is high-pitched, and is not easily soothed. Her heart rate has jumped; and she’s trying to move her hands, arms, and legs (Warnock et al., 2014). Collectively, these signs strongly suggest that babies experience pain.

Perceptual skills are extraordinarily useful to newborns and young babies. Smell and touch help them recognize their mothers. Smell and taste make it much easier for them to learn to eat. Early development of smell, taste, and touch prepares newborns and young babies to learn about the world.

Hearing We saw in Chapter 2 that the fetus can hear at seven or eight months after conception. As you would expect from these results, newborns typically respond to sounds in their sur- roundings. If a parent is quiet and then coughs, an infant may startle, blink his eyes, and move his arms or legs. These responses may seem natural, but they indicate that infants are sensitive to sound.

Overall, adults can hear better than infants, but infants eliminate much of the gap by 6 months, particularly for higher-pitched sounds. Infants can pick out a sound from

perception Processes by which the brain receives, selects, modifies, and organizes incoming nerve impulses that are the result of physi- cal stimulation.

Infants and toddlers do not like bitter and sour tastes!

D io

n O

gu st

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

An infant’s response to an inoculation— a distinctive facial expression coupled with a distinctive cry—clearly suggests that the baby feels pain.

Je ni

fe r H

ar rin

gt on

/S to

ck by

te /J

up ite

r I m

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

104 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

background noise, though not as skillfully as adults (Litovsky, 2015). More interesting, infants best hear sounds that have pitches in the range of human speech: neither very high-pitched nor very low-pitched. Infants can distinguish speech sounds, such as vowels from consonant sounds, and by 4 or 5 months, they can recognize their own names (Jusc- zyk, 1995; Mandel, Jusczyk, & Pisoni, 1995).

Infants also can distinguish different musical sounds. They can distinguish different melodies and prefer melodies that are pleasant-sounding over those that are unpleasant- sounding or dissonant (Trainor & Heinmiller, 1998). And infants are sensitive to the rhyth- mic structure of music: After hearing a sequence of notes, 5-month-olds notice when the rhythmic structure of the notes changes (Hannon, Soley, & Levine, 2011). This early sensi- tivity to music is remarkable but perhaps not so surprising when you consider that music is (and has been) central in all cultures.

Thus, by the middle of the first year, infants respond to much of the information that is provided by sound. In Chapter 4, we will reach the same conclusion when we examine the perception of language-related sounds.

Seeing If you’ve ever watched infants, you probably noticed that they spend much of their wak- ing time looking around. Sometimes they scan their environment, and sometimes they focus on nearby objects. What do they see as a result? Perhaps their visual world is a sea of confusing gray blobs. Or maybe they see the world as adults do. Actually, neither of these descriptions is entirely accurate, but the second is closer to the truth.

The various elements of the visual system—the eye, the optic nerve, and the brain— are relatively well developed at birth. Newborns respond to light and can track moving objects with their eyes. How well do infants see? The clarity of vision, called visual acuity, is defined as the smallest pattern that can be distinguished dependably. You’ve undoubtedly had your acuity measured, probably by being asked to read rows of progressively smaller letters or numbers from a chart. To assess newborns’ acuity, researchers use a similar approach, testing infants’ looking at patterns (e.g., stripes). This work reveals that newborns and 1-month-olds see at 20 feet what normal adults would see at 200 to 400 feet, but 1-year-olds have the acuity of an adult with normal vision (Kellman & Arterberry, 2006). This improvement reflects changes in the eye: receptors move  to  the center of the eye and become longer, allowing them to collect more light ( Johnson, 2013).

Color Infants not only see the world with greater acuity during the first year, they also begin to see it in color! How do we perceive color? The wavelength of light is the basis of color per- ception. In ◗ Figure 3.8, light that we see as red has a relatively long wavelength, whereas violet (at the other end of the color spectrum) has a much shorter wavelength. Concen- trated in the back of the eye along the retina are specialized neurons called cones. Some cones are sensitive to short-wavelength light (blues and violets). Others are sensitive to medium-wavelength light (greens and yellows), and still others are sensitive to long- wavelength light (reds and oranges). These different kinds of cones are linked by com- plex circuits of neurons, and this circuitry is responsible for our ability to see the world in colors.

These circuits gradually begin to function in the first few months after birth. New- borns and young babies can perceive few colors, but by 3 months, the three kinds of cones and their associated circuits are working and infants are able to see the full range of colors (Kellman & Arterberry, 2006). In fact, by 4 to 6 months, infants’ color percep- tion seems similar to that of adults: Like adults, infants see categories of color. They see the spectrum as a group of reds, a group of yellows, a group of greens, and the like (Yang et al., 2016).

visual acuity Smallest pattern that one can distinguish reliably.

cones Specialized neurons in the back of the eye that sense color.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 105

Depth People see objects as having three dimensions: height, width, and depth. The retina of the eye is flat, so height and width can be represented directly on its two-dimensional surface. But the third dimension, depth, cannot be represented directly on this flat surface; so how do we perceive depth? We use perceptual processing to infer depth.

Depth perception tells us whether objects are near or far, which was the basis for some classic research by Eleanor Gibson and Richard Walk (1960) on the origins of depth per- ception. In their work, babies were placed on a glass-covered platform, a device known as the visual cliff. On one side of the platform, a checkerboard pattern appeared directly under the glass; on the other side, the pattern appeared several feet below the glass. The result was that the first side looked shallow but the other looked deep, like a cliff.

Mothers stood on each side of the visual cliff and tried to coax their infants across the deep side or the shallow side. Most babies willingly crawled to their mothers when they stood on the shallow side. In contrast, babies refused to cross the deep side, even when the mothers called them by name and tried to lure them with an attractive toy. Clearly, infants can perceive depth by the time they are old enough to crawl.

What about younger babies who cannot yet crawl? When babies as young as 6 weeks are placed on the visual cliff, their hearts beat more slowly when they are placed on the deep side of the cliff. Heart rate often decelerates when people notice something interesting, so this would suggest that 6-week-olds notice that the deep side is different. At 7 months, infants’ heart rates accelerate, a sign of fear. Thus, although young babies can detect a difference between the shallow and deep sides of the visual cliff, only older crawling babies are afraid of the deep side (Campos et al., 1978).

How do infants infer depth on the visual cliff or anywhere else? They use several kinds of cues. Among the first are kinetic cues, in which motion is used to estimate depth. Visual expansion refers to the fact that as an object moves closer, it fills an ever- greater proportion of the retina. Visual expansion is why we flinch when someone unex- pectedly tosses a soda can toward us, and it’s what allows a batter to estimate when a baseball will arrive over the plate. Another cue, motion parallax, refers to the fact that nearby moving objects move across our visual field faster than those at a distance. Motion parallax is in action when you look out the side window in a moving car: Trees next to the road move rapidly across the visual field, but mountains in the distance move more slowly. Babies use these cues in the first weeks after birth; for example, a 1-month-old baby will blink if a moving object looks as if it’s going to hit him in the face (Nánez & Yonas, 1994).

visual cliff Glass-covered platform that appears to have a “shallow” and a “deep” side; used to study infants’ depth perception.

kinetic cues Cues to depth perception in which motion is used to estimate depth.

visual expansion Kinetic cue to depth perception that is based on the fact that an object fills an ever-greater proportion of the retina as it moves closer.

motion parallax Kinetic cue to depth perception based on the fact that nearby moving objects move across our visual field faster than do distant objects.

●◗ Figure 3.8 The visible portion of light ranges from a wavelength of about 400 nano- meters (which looks violet) to nearly 700 nanometers (which looks red).

500 600

Visible light

X-rays

.1 10 100 1000

Ultra- violet rays

Infrared rays

Wavelength of light in nanometers (billionths of a meter)

400 700

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Despite their mother’s coaxing, infants avoid the “deep side” of the visual cliff, indicating that they perceive depth.

M ar

k Ri

ch ar

ds /P

ho to

Ed it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

106 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Another cue becomes important at about 4 months. Binocular disparity is based on the fact that the left and right eyes often see slightly different versions of the same scene. You can demonstrate binocular disparity by touching your nose with your finger. If you look at your finger with one eye (closing the other eye), each eye has a very different view of your finger. But if you hold your finger at arm’s length from your nose and repeat the demon- stration, each eye has similar views of your fingers. Thus, greater disparity in retinal images signifies that an object is close. By about 4 months, infants use binocular disparity as a depth cue, correctly inferring that objects are nearby when disparity is great (Kavšek & Braun, 2016).

By about 6 months, infants use several cues for depth that depend on the arrangement of objects in the environment (Kavšek, Yonas, & Granrud, 2012). These are sometimes called pictorial cues because they’re the same cues that artists use to convey depth in draw- ings and paintings. Here are two examples of pictorial cues that 7-month-olds use to infer depth.

●● Linear perspective: Parallel lines come together at a single point in the distance. Thus, we use the space between the lines as a cue to distance and consequently decide that the tracks close together are farther away than the tracks far apart.

●● Texture gradient: The texture of objects changes from distinct for nearby objects to finer and less distinct for distant objects. We judge that distinct flowers are close and that blurred ones are distant.

Infants not only use visual cues to judge depth but also use sound. Infants correctly judge quieter objects to be more distant than louder objects. Given such an assortment of cues, it is not surprising that infants gauge depth so accurately.

Perceiving Objects Perceptual processes enable us to interpret patterns of lines, textures, and colors as objects. That is, our perception actually creates an object from sensory stimulation. This is particularly challenging because we often see only parts of objects—nearby objects often obscure parts of more distant objects. Nevertheless, we recognize these objects despite this complexity in our visual environment.

Perception of objects is limited in newborns, but it develops rapidly in the first few months after birth (Johnson, 2001). By 4 months, infants use a number of cues to deter- mine which elements go together to form objects. One important cue is motion: Elements

binocular disparity Way of inferring depth based on differences in the retinal images in the left and right eyes.

pictorial cues Cues to depth perception that are used to convey depth in drawings and paintings.

linear perspective A cue to depth perception based on the fact that parallel lines come together at a single point in the distance.

texture gradient Perceptual cue to depth based on the fact that the texture of objects changes from distinct for nearby objects to finer and less distinct for distant objects.

Texture gradient is used to infer depth: We interpret the distinct flowers as being closer than the flowers with the blurry texture.

T. W

. v an

U rk

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Think About It Psychologists often refer to “perceptual- motor skills,” which implies that the two are closely related. Based on what you’ve learned in this chapter, how might motor skills influence perception? How could perception influence motor skills?

Linear perspective is one cue to depth: We interpret the railroad tracks that are close together as being more distant than the tracks that are far apart.

Ro be

rt G

lu si

c/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 107

that move together are usually part of the same object (Kellman & Arterberry, 2006). For example, at the left of ◗ Figure 3.9, a pencil appears to be moving back and forth behind a colored square. If the square were removed, you would be surprised to see a pair of pencil stubs, as shown on the right side of the diagram. The common movement of the pencil’s eraser and point leads us to believe that they’re part of the same pencil.

Young infants, too, are surprised by demonstrations such as this. If they see a display such as the moving pencils, they will then look briefly at a whole pencil, apparently because they expected it. In contrast, if after seeing the moving pencil they’re shown the two pencil stubs, they look much longer—as if trying to figure out what happened (Amso & Johnson, 2006; Eizenman & Bertenthal, 1998). Babies use common motion to identify objects, and given the right conditions, newborns do, too (Valenza & Bulf, 2011).

Motion is one clue to object unity, but infants use others, including color, texture, and aligned edges. As you can see in ◗ Figure 3.10, infants more often group features together (i.e., believe they’re part of the same object) when they’re the same color, they have the same texture, and their edges are aligned (Bremner et al., 2013; Johnson, 2001).

Perceiving Faces Babies depend on other people to care for them, so it’s not surprising that young babies are attuned to human faces. For example, newborns prefer (1) faces with normal features over faces in which features are scrambled (Easterbrook et al., 1999), (2) upright faces over inverted faces (Mondloch et al., 1999), and (3) attractive faces over unattractive faces (Slater et al., 2000). Findings such as these lead some scientists to claim that babies are innately attracted to moving stimuli that are facelike (e.g., consist of three high-contrast blobs close together). In other words, newborns’ face perception may be reflexive based on primitive circuits in the brain. At about 2 or 3 months of age, different circuits in the brain’s cortex begin to control infants’ looking at faces, allowing infants to learn about faces and to distinguish different faces (Morton & Johnson, 1991).

Through the first few months after birth, infants have a very general prototype for a face—one that includes human and nonhuman faces (Pascalis, de Haan, & Nelson, 2002). However, over the first year, infants fine-tune their prototype of a face so that it reflects faces that are familiar in their environments. For example, 3-month-olds prefer to look at faces from their own race, but they can recognize faces from other races (and other spe- cies). In contrast, 6-month-olds often fail to recognize faces of individuals from other unfa- miliar races (Anzures et al., 2013). And by 9 months, infants lump together faces from all other races: For example, European American babies create an “other race” category that includes Asian and African faces (Quinn et al., 2016).

●◗ Figure 3.9 After infants have seen the pencil ends moving behind the square, they are surprised to see two pencils when the square is removed; this shows that babies use common motion as a way to determine what makes up an object.

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Think About It When 6-month-old Sebastian watches his mother type on a keyboard, how does he know that her fingers and the keyboard are not simply one big unusual object?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

108 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Apparently, older infants’ greater familiarity with faces of their own race leads to a more precise configuration of faces, one that includes faces of familiar racial and ethnic groups. Consis- tent with this interpretation, infants born in Asia but adopted by European parents recognize European faces better than Asian faces ( Sangrigoli et al., 2005). What’s more, if older infants receive exten- sive experience with other-race faces, they can learn to recognize them (Anzures et al., 2012).

These changes in face-recognition skill show the role of expe- rience in fine-tuning infants’ perception, a theme that will emerge again in the early phases of language learning (Chapter 4). And these improved face-recognition skills are adaptive, for they provide the basis for social relationships that infants form during the rest of the first year, which we’ll examine in Chapter 5.

Integrating Sensory Information So far, we have discussed infants’ sensory systems separately. In reality, most infant experiences are better described as “multime- dia events.” For example, a nursing mother provides visual and taste cues to her baby. A rattle stimulates vision, hearing, and touch. In fact, much stimulation is not specific to one sense, but spans mul- tiple senses. Temporal information, such as duration or tempo, can be seen or heard. For example, you can detect the rhythm of a person clapping by seeing the hands meet or by hearing the sound of hands striking. Similarly, the texture of a surface—whether it’s rough or smooth—can be detected by sight or by feel.

Infants readily perceive many of these relations. For example, infants can recognize visually an object that they have only touched previously (Sann & Streri, 2007). Similarly, they can detect rela- tions between information presented visually and auditorily. Babies look longer when an object’s motion matches its sound (it makes higher-pitched sounds while rising but lower-pitched sounds while falling) than when it doesn’t (Walker et al., 2010). And they can link the temporal properties of visual and auditory stimulation, such as duration and rhythm (Lewkowicz, 2000). Finally, they link their own body movement to their perceptions of musical rhythm, giving new meaning to the phrase “feel the beat, baby!” (Gerry, Faux,  & Trainor, 2010).

Traditionally, coordinating information from different senses (e.g., vision with hearing, vision with touch) was thought to be a challenging task for infants. But a new view is that cross-modal

perception is easier for infants because regions in the brain devoted to sensory process- ing are not yet specialized in infancy. For example, regions in an adult’s brain that respond only to visual stimuli respond to visual and auditory input in the infant’s brain (Spector & Maurer, 2009). And some researchers have argued that the infant’s sensory systems are particularly attuned to intersensory redundancy—that is, to information that is presented simultaneously to different sensory modes (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2002; 2012). Perception is best when information is presented redundantly to multiple senses. When an infant sees and hears the mother clapping (visual, auditory information), he focuses on the information conveyed to both senses and pays less attention to informa- tion that’s only available in one sense, such as the color of the mother’s nail polish or the sounds of her humming along with the tune (Bahrick et al., 2015). Or the infant can learn that the mom’s lips are chapped from seeing the flaking skin and by feeling the roughness as the mother kisses him. According to intersensory redundancy theory, it’s

intersensory redundancy Infants’ sensory systems are attuned to information presented simultaneously to different sensory modes.

●◗ Figure 3.10 In addition to common motion, infants use common color, common texture, and aligned edges as clues to object unity.

Cue

Color

Infants believe this display has one pencil…

…but believe this display has two pencils

Texture

Aligned edges

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 109

as if infants follow the rule “Any information that’s presented in mul- tiple senses must be important, so pay attention to it!”

Integrating information from different senses is yet another variation on the theme that has dominated this chapter: Infants’ sensory and perceptual skills are impressive. Darla’s newborn daughter, from the opening vignette, can smell, taste, and feel pain. She can distinguish sounds. Her vision is a little blurry now but will improve rapidly; in a few months, she’ll see the full range of col- ors and perceive depth. In short, Darla’s daughter, like most infants, is exceptionally well prepared to begin to make sense out of her environment.

A mother who breastfeeds provides her baby with a multimedia event: the baby sees, smells, hears, feels, and tastes her!

N an

cy N

ey /P

ho to

di sc

/G et

ty im

ag es

recall 1. Infants respond negatively to substances that taste sour or

_______________.

2. Infants respond to _______________ with a high-pitched cry that is hard to soothe.

3. Infants’ hearing is best for sounds that have the pitch of _______________.

4. At age _______________, infants’ acuity is like that of an adult with normal vision.

5. _______________ are specialized neurons in the retina that are sensitive to color.

6. _______________ refers to the fact that images of an object in the left and right eyes differ for nearby objects.

7. When elements consistently move together, infants decide that they are _______________.

8. Infants readily integrate information from different senses, and their sensory systems seem to be particularly attuned to _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare the impact of nature and nurture on the develop-

ment of infants’ sensory and perceptual skills.

Apply ●● Perceptual skills are quite refined at birth and mature rap-

idly. What evolutionary purposes are served by this rapid development?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 3.4

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● When do children begin to realize that they exist?

●● What are toddlers’ and preschoolers’ self-concepts like?

●● When do preschool children begin to acquire a theory of mind?

When ximena brushes her teeth, she puts her 20-month-old son, Christof, in an infant seat facing the bathroom mirror. She’s been doing this for months, and Christof enjoys looking at the images in the mirror. Lately, he seems to pay special attention to his own reflection. ximena thinks that sometimes Christof deliberately frowns or laughs just to see what he looks like. Is this possible, ximena wonders, or is her imagination simply running wild?

3.5 Becoming Self-Aware

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

110 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

As infants’ physical, motor, and perceptual skills grow, they learn more  and more about the world around them. As part of this learning, infants and toddlers begin to realize that they exist independently of other people and objects in the environment and that their existence con- tinues over time. In this last section, you’ll see how children become self- aware and learn what Christof knows about himself.

Origins of Self-Concept When do children begin to understand that they exist? Measuring the onset of this awareness is not easy. Obviously, we can’t simply ask a 3-year-old, “So tell me, when did you first realize you existed and weren’t just part of the furniture?” Investigators need a less direct approach, and a mirror offers one route. Babies sometimes touch the face in the mirror or wave at it, but none of their behaviors indicate that they recognize themselves in the mirror. Instead, babies act as if the face in the mirror is simply a very interesting stimulus.

How would we know that infants recognize themselves in a mirror? One clever approach is to have mothers place a red mark on their infant’s nose; they do this sur- reptitiously, while wiping the baby’s face. Then the infant is returned to the mirror. Many 1-year-olds touch the red mark on the mirror, showing that they notice the mark on the face in the mirror. By 15 months, however, an important change occurs: Babies see the red mark in the mirror, then reach up and touch their own noses. By age 2, virtually all children do this (Bullock & Lütkenhaus, 1990; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979). When these older children notice the red mark in the mirror, they understand that the funny-looking nose in the mirror is their own!

Other features of young children’s behavior suggest that self-awareness emerges between 18 and 24 months. During this same period, toddlers look more at photographs of themselves than at photos of other children. They also refer to themselves by name or with a personal pronoun, such as I or me, and sometimes they know their age and gender. These changes often occur together and they suggest that self-awareness is well estab- lished in most children by age 2, although it develops faster in cultures that encourage toddlers to be independent (Lewis, 2011; Ross et al., 2016).

Soon toddlers and young children begin to recognize continuity in the self over time; the “I” in the present is linked to the “I” in the past (Nelson, 2001). Awareness of a self that is extended in time is fostered by conversations with parents about the past and the future. Through such conversations, a 3-year-old celebrating a birthday understands that she’s an older version of the same person who had a birthday a year ago.

Children’s growing awareness of a self extended in time is also revealed by their understanding of ownership (Fasig, 2000). When a toddler sees his favorite toy and says “mine,” this implies awareness of continuity of the self over time: “In the past, I played with that.” And when toddlers say “Mine,” rather than being selfish, they’re indicating ownership as a way of defining themselves. They are not trying to deny the toy to another, but simply asserting that playing with this toy is part of who they are (Levine, 1983).

Once children fully understand that they exist, they begin to wonder who they are. They want to define themselves. Throughout the preschool years, possessions continue to be one of the ways in which children define themselves. Preschoolers are also likely to mention physical characteristics (“I have blue eyes”), their preferences (“I like spaghetti”), and their competencies (“I can count to 50”). What these features have in common is a focus on a child’s characteristics that are observable and concrete (Harter, 2006).

As children enter school, their self-concepts become even more elaborate (Harter, 1994), changes that we’ll explore in Chapter 9.

Not until 15 to 18 months of age do babies recognize themselves in the mirror, which is an important step in becoming self-aware.

Pa ul

in e

Br ei

je r/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 111

Theory of Mind As youngsters gain more insights into themselves as thinking beings, they begin to realize that people have thoughts, beliefs, and intentions. They also understand that thoughts, beliefs, and intentions often cause people to behave as they do. Amazingly, even infants understand that people’s behavior is often intentional—designed to achieve a goal. Imagine a father who says “Where are the crackers?” in front of his 1-year-old daughter and then begins opening kitchen cabinets, mov- ing objects to look behind them. Finding the box of crackers, he says, “There they are!” An infant who understands intentionality would real- ize how her father’s actions (searching, moving objects) were related to the goal of finding the crackers.

Many clever experiments have revealed that 1-year-olds do indeed understand intentionality (Baillargeon, Scott, & Bian, 2016). For exam- ple, in one study, infants observed a woman who consistently seemed interested in one of two stuffed animals: she looked at it intently and seemed happy. Next, the woman picked up either that stuffed ani- mal or another one that she had ignored. By 12 months, infants were surprised to see the woman pick up the animal that she had ignored instead of that animal that had seemed interesting. Infants appar- ently inferred from the women’s looking that she intended to pick up one animal and were surprised when she picked up the other instead ( Dunphy-Lelii et al., 2014).

From this early understanding of intentionality, young children’s naïve psychology expands rapidly. Between 2 and 5 years of age, chil- dren develop a theory of mind, a naïve understanding of the relations between mind and behavior. One of the leading researchers on theory of mind, Henry Wellman (2014), believes that children’s theory of mind moves through several phases during the preschool years. In the earli- est phase, preschoolers understand that people can have different desires: One child might want raisins for a snack while another child wants crackers. In the next phase, children know that people can have different beliefs: In trying to find a missing shoe, one child might believe that the shoe is in the kitchen while another child believes that it’s in the car. In the third phase, children understand that different experiences can lead to different states of knowledge: A child who has seen a toy hidden in a drawer knows what’s in the now-closed drawer, but a child who did not see the toy hidden does not.

The next phase represents a fundamental shift in children’s theory of mind: Children understand that behavior is based on a person’s beliefs about events and situations, even when those beliefs are wrong. Children’s understanding of the influ- ence of such false beliefs is revealed in tasks such as the one shown in ◗ Figure 3.11. Anne knows that the marble has been moved to the box, but Sally believes that the marble is still in the basket. Not until 4 years of age do most children correctly say that Sally will look for the  marble in the basket (acting on her false belief ); 4-year-olds understand that Sally’s behavior is based on her beliefs even though her belief is wrong.

In the final phase, children understand that people may feel one emotion but show another. For example, a child who is disappointed by a birthday present smiles anyway because she doesn’t want her parents to know how she really feels.

Thus, children’s theory of mind becomes more sophisticated over the preschool years. This general pattern is found for children around the world, with one twist: The five-phase sequence we described here is common in many Western nations, but in China and Iran, preschoolers typically understand differences in knowledge (phase 3) before differences in beliefs (Shahaeian et al., 2011; Wellman, Fang, & Peterson, 2011). One explanation for this difference is that compared to Western parents, parents in China and Iran emphasize

theory of mind Ideas about connections between thoughts, beliefs, intentions, and behavior that cre- ate an intuitive understanding of the link between mind and behavior.

●◗ Figure 3.11 In a false-belief task, most 3-year-olds say that Sally will look for the ball in the box, showing that they do not understand how people can act on their beliefs (where the ball is) even when those beliefs are wrong.

Sally goes out for a walk.

Anne takes the ball from the basket and puts it into the box.

Now Sally comes back. She wants to play with her ball. Where will she look for her ball?

This is Sally. Sally has a basket.

Sally puts her ball in her basket.

This is Anne. Anne has a box.

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

112 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

knowledge to their young children (“knowing the right things”) and are less tolerant of dif- ferent beliefs (Wellman, 2012).

You can see preschool children’s growing understanding of false belief in the Real People feature.

The early stages of children’s theory of mind seem clear. But just how this happens is a matter of debate. One view emphasizes the contribution of language, which develops rap- idly during the same years that theory of mind emerges (as we’ll see in Chapter 4). Some scientists believe that children’s language skills contribute to growth of theory of mind, perhaps reflecting the benefit of an expanding vocabulary that includes verbs describ- ing mental states, such as think, know, and believe (Pascual et al., 2008). Or the benefits may reflect children’s mastery of grammatical forms that can be used to describe a setting where a person knows that another person has a false belief (Farrant, Maybery, & Fletcher, 2012).

A different view is that a child’s theory of mind emerges from interactions with other people, interactions that provide children with insights into different mental states (Dunn  & Brophy, 2005; Slaughter, 2015). Through conversations with parents and older siblings that focus on other people’s mental states, children learn facts of mental life, and this helps children to see that others often have different perspectives than they do. In other words, when children frequently participate in conversations that focus on other people’s moods, feelings, and intentions, they learn that people’s behavior is based on their beliefs, regardless of the accuracy of those beliefs.

These different views are also used to explain why theory of mind develops very slowly in children with autism, which is our last topic in this chapter.

Theory of Mind in Children with Autism Autism is the most serious of a family of disorders known as autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Individuals with ASD acquire language later than usual, and their speech often echoes what others say to them; also, they sometimes become intensely interested in objects (e.g., making the same actions with a toy over and over). In addi- tion, they often seem uninterested in other people, and when they do interact, those exchanges are often awkward, as if individu- als with ASD aren’t following the rules that govern social interac- tions. Symptoms usually emerge early in life, typically by 2 years of age. Roughly 8% of children worldwide are diagnosed with ASD; about 80% of them are boys (Baxter et al., 2015). ASD has many possible causes, including genetics, being born prematurely or to older mothers, and being exposed prenatally to toxins (Lyall et al., 2017). Many studies point to atypical brain functioning, perhaps due to an imbalance in excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters ( Robertson, Ratai, & Kanwisher, 2016).

Children with autism master language later than usual and are often more interested in objects than in people.

Se an

S pr

ag ue

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Think About It Suppose you believe that a theory of mind develops faster when preschoolers spend much time with other children. What sort of correlational study would you devise to test this hypothesis? How could you do an experimental study to test the same hypothesis?

Real People Applying Human Development

Preschoolers gradually recognize that people’s behavior is sometimes guided by mistaken beliefs. We once witnessed an episode at a day care center that documented this growing understanding. After lunch, Karen, a 2-year-old, saw ketchup on the floor and squealed, “blood, blood!” Lonna, a 3-year-old,

said in a disgusted tone, “It’s not blood—it’s ketchup.” Then Shenan, a 4-year-old, inter- jected, “Yeah, but Karen thought it was blood.” A similar incident took place a few weeks later on the day after Halloween. This time Lonna put on a monster mask and scared Karen. When Karen began to cry, Lonna said,

“Oh stop. It’s just a mask.” Shenan broke in again, saying, “You know it’s just a mask. But she thinks it’s a monster.” In both cases, only Shenan understood that Karen’s behavior was based on her beliefs (that the ketchup is blood and that the monster is real) even though her beliefs were false.

“Seeing Is Believing …” for 3-Year-Olds

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 113

Children with ASD grasp false belief very slowly, and this performance leads some researchers to conclude that the absence of a theory of mind—sometimes called “mind- blindness” (Baron-Cohen, 2005)—is the defining characteristic of ASD (Leekam, 2016). Other scientists aren’t convinced. Although no one doubts that autistic children find false-belief tasks puzzling, some scientists say that mindblindness is a by-product of other deficits and not the cause of the symptoms associated with ASD. One idea is that ASD reflects problems in inhibiting irrelevant actions and in shifting smoothly between actions ( Pellicano, 2013). Another idea emphasizes a focused processing style that is common in ASD. For example, children with ASD find hidden objects faster than typically developing children do (Chen et al., 2012). But this emphasis on perceptual details usually comes at the expense of maintaining a coherent overall picture. Consequently, in social interactions, children with ASD may focus on one facet of another person’s behavior (e.g., gestures) but ignore other verbal and nonverbal cues (e.g., speech, facial expressions, body language) that collectively promote fluid interactions. Research to evaluate these claims is still ongo- ing; the answers will likely indicate that multiple factors contribute to ASD.

ASD can’t be cured. However, therapy can be used to improve language and social skills in children with autism. In addition, medications can be used to reduce some of the symptoms, such as compulsive behavior and depression (National Institutes of Health, 2015). When ASD is diagnosed early and autistic children grow up in supportive, respon- sive environments and receive appropriate treatments, they can lead satisfying and pro- ductive lives.

recall 1. Apparently children are first self-aware at age 2 because this

is when they first recognize themselves in a mirror and in photographs and when they first use _______________.

2. During the preschool years, children’s self-concepts empha- size _______________, physical characteristics, preferences, and competencies.

3. Unlike 4-year-olds, most 3-year-olds don’t understand that other people’s behavior is sometimes based on _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast different explanations of the growth of

theory of mind during the preschool years.

Apply ●● Self-concept emerges over the same months that toddlers

show rapid gains in locomotor skills. How might changes in locomotor skill contribute to a toddler’s emerging sense of self?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 3.5

SUMMARY

3.1 The Newborn

how do reflexes help newborns interact with the world?

●● Babies are born with a number of different reflexes. Some help them adjust to life outside the uterus, some help pro- tect them from danger, and some serve as the basis for later voluntary motor behavior.

how do we determine whether a baby is healthy and adjust- ing to life outside the uterus?

●● The Apgar scale measures five vital signs to determine a newborn baby’s physical well-being. The Neonatal Behav- ioral Assessment Scale provides a comprehensive evalua- tion of a baby’s behavioral and physical status.

What behavioral states are common among newborns?

●● Newborns spend their day in one of four states: alert inac- tivity, waking activity, crying, and sleeping. A newborn’s crying includes a basic cry, a mad cry, and a pain cry. The best way to calm a crying baby is to put the baby on the shoulder and rock him or her.

●● Newborns spend approximately two-thirds of every day asleep and go through a complete sleep–wake cycle once every four hours. By 3 or 4 months, babies sleep through the night. Newborns spend about half of their time asleep in REM sleep, an active form of sleep that may stimulate growth in the nervous system.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

114 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● Some healthy babies die from sudden infant death syn- drome (SIDS). Factors that contribute to SIDS are pre- maturity, low birth weight, and smoking. Also, babies are vulnerable to SIDS when they sleep on their stomach and when they are overheated. The Back to Sleep campaign was designed to prevent SIDS by encouraging parents to have infants sleep on their backs.

What are the different features of temperament? Do they change as children grow?

●● Temperament refers to a consistent style or pattern to an infant’s behavior. Modern theories list two to six dimen- sions of temperament, including, for example, extroversion and negative affect. Temperament is influenced by both heredity and environment and is a reasonably stable char- acteristic of infants and young children.

3.2 Physical Development

how do height and weight change from birth to 2 years of age?

●● Physical growth is particularly rapid during infancy, but babies of the same age differ considerably in their height and weight. Size at maturity is largely determined by heredity.

●● Growth follows the cephalocaudal principle, in which the head and trunk develop before the legs. Consequently, infants and young children have disproportionately large heads and trunks.

What nutrients do young children need? how are nutrients best provided?

●● Infants must consume a large number of calories relative to their body weight, primarily because of the energy required for growth. Breast-feeding is the best way to provide infants with nourishment and has several other advantages com- pared with bottle-feeding (e.g., health benefits).

●● Malnutrition is a worldwide problem that is particularly harmful during infancy, when growth is so rapid. Treat- ing malnutrition adequately requires improving children’s diets and training their parents to provide stimulating environments.

What are nerve cells, and how are they organized in the brain?

●● A nerve cell, called a neuron, includes a cell body, a den- drite, and an axon. The mature brain consists of billions of neurons organized into nearly identical left and right hemi- spheres connected by the corpus callosum. The cerebral cortex regulates most of the functions we think of as distinc- tively human. The frontal cortex is associated with person- ality and goal-directed behavior; the left hemisphere of the cortex, with language; and the right hemisphere of the cor- tex, with nonverbal processes such as perceiving emotions.

how does the brain develop? When does it begin to function?

●● Brain specialization is evident in infancy; further special- ization involves more focused brain areas and narrowing of stimuli that trigger brain activity. Different systems special- ize at different rates. Specialization depends on stimulation from the environment. The relative lack of specialization in the immature brain makes it better able to recover from injury.

3.3 Moving and Grasping: Early Motor Skills

What component skills are involved in learning to walk? At what age do infants master them?

●● Infants acquire a series of locomotor skills during their first year, culminating in walking a few months after the first birthday. Like most motor skills, learning to walk involves differentiation of individual skills, such as maintaining bal- ance and using the legs alternately, and then integrating those skills into a coherent whole.

how do infants learn to coordinate the use of their hands?

●● Infants first use one hand at a time; then both hands inde- pendently; then both hands in common actions; and finally, at about 5 months of age, both hands in different actions with a common purpose.

●● Most people are right-handed, a preference observed in prenatal development that becomes stronger during the preschool years. Handedness is determined by heredity but can also be influenced by cultural values.

3.4 Coming to Know the World: Perception

Are infants able to smell, taste, and experience pain?

●● Newborns are able to smell, and some can recognize their mother’s odor; they also taste, preferring sweet substances and responding negatively to bitter and sour tastes.

●● Infants respond to touch. They probably experience pain because their responses to painful stimuli resemble those of older children.

how well do infants hear?

●● Babies can hear. More importantly, they can distinguish dif- ferent speech sounds and different musical sounds.

how well can infants see? Can they see color and depth?

●● A newborn’s visual acuity is relatively poor, but 1-year-olds can see as well as an adult with normal vision.

●● Color vision develops as different sets of cones begin to function, a process that seems to be complete by 4 to 6 months of age. Infants perceive depth based on kinetic cues, binocular disparity, and pictorial cues. They also use motion to recognize objects.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 3 : T O O L S F O R E x P L O R I N G T H E W O R L D 115

●● Infants are attracted to faces, and experience leads infants to form a prototypic face based on faces they see often.

how do infants process and combine information from differ- ent sensory modalities, such as between vision and hearing?

●● Infants coordinate information from different senses. They can recognize by sight an object they felt previously. Infants are often particularly attentive to information presented redundantly to multiple senses.

3.5 Becoming Self-Aware

When do children begin to realize that they exist?

●● Beginning at about 15 months, infants begin to recognize themselves in the mirror, which is one of the first signs of self-recognition. They also begin to prefer looking at

pictures of themselves, begin referring to themselves by name (or using personal pronouns), and sometimes know their age and gender. Evidently, by 2 years of age, most chil- dren are self-aware.

What are toddlers’ and preschoolers’ self-concepts like?

●● Preschoolers often define themselves in terms of observable characteristics such as possessions, physical characteristics, preferences, and competencies.

When do preschool children begin to acquire a theory of ind?

●● During the preschool years, children’s theory of mind becomes progressively more sophisticated. One landmark is the understanding that people’s behavior is based on beliefs about events and situations, even when those beliefs are wrong. Children with autism have a limited theory of mind.

3.1 1. serve as the basis for later motor behaviors 2. Apgar score 3. alert inactivity 4. REM sleep 5. sleep on their backs 6. temperament is moderately stable in these years 3.2 1. disproportionately large 2. more 3. parent training 4. cell body 5. planning 6. left hemisphere 7. they often regain their earlier skills over time 3.3 1. dynamic systems theory 2. using perceptual information 3. 4 4. manipulate the toy 3.4 1. bitter 2. pain 3. human speech 4. 1 year 5. Cones 6. binocular disparity 7. part of the same object 8. information presented redundantly to multiple senses 3.5 1. personal pronouns such as I and me 2. possessions 3. false beliefs

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

reflexes (80) alert inactivity (82) waking activity (82) crying (82) sleeping (82) basic cry (82) mad cry (82) pain cry (82) irregular or rapid-eye-movement

(REM) sleep (83) regular or nonREM sleep (83) sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) (84) temperament (85) malnourished (90) neuron (91) cell body (91) dendrite (91) axon (91)

terminal buttons (91) neurotransmitters (91) cerebral cortex (91) hemispheres (92) corpus callosum (92) frontal cortex (92) neural plate (92) myelin (92) synaptic pruning (93) electroencephalography (93) functional magnetic resonance imaging

(fMRI) (93) experience-expectant growth (94) experience-dependent growth (95) motor skills (96) locomote (96) fine motor skills (96) toddling (96)

toddler (96) dynamic systems theory (96) differentiation (98) integration (98) perception (103) visual acuity (104) cones (104) visual cliff (105) kinetic cues (105) visual expansion (105) motion parallax (105) binocular disparity (106) pictorial cues (106) linear perspective (106) texture gradient (106) intersensory redundancy (108) theory of mind (111)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Ra w

pi xe

l.c om

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

117

4The Emergence of Thought and Language Cognitive Development in Infancy and Early Childhood

On the TV show Family Guy, Stewie is a 1-year-old who can’t stand his mother (Stewie: “Hey, mother. I come bearing a gift. I’ll give you a

hint. It’s in my diaper, and it’s not a toaster.”) and

hopes to dominate the world. Much of the humor, of

course, turns on the idea that babies are capable

of sophisticated thinking and just can’t express it.

But what thoughts actually lurk in the mind of an

infant who is not yet speaking? How does cognition

develop during infancy and early childhood? What

makes these changes possible?

These questions provide the focus of this chapter.

We begin with what has long been considered the

definitive account of cognitive development, Jean

Piaget’s theory, as well as a later variant, the core

knowledge approach. The next two sections of the

chapter concern alternative accounts of cogni-

tive development. One account, the information-

processing perspective, traces children’s emerging

cognitive skills in many specific domains, including

memory skills. The other, Lev Vygotsky’s theory, emphasizes the cultural origins

of cognitive development and explains why children sometimes talk to them-

selves as they play or work.

4.1 The Onset of Thinking: Piaget’s Account 118 Basic Principles of Cognitive Development 118

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Learning about Butterflies: Accommodation and Assimilation in Action 119

Sensorimotor Thinking 120 Preoperational Thinking 122 Evaluating Piaget’s Theory 125 Extending Piaget’s Account: Children’s Naïve

Theories 126

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Have a Heart! Preschoolers’ Essentialist Thinking 129

4.2 Information Processing During Infancy and Early Childhood 130 General Principles of Information Processing 130 Attention 131 Learning 131 Memory 132 Learning Number Skills 134

4.3 Mind and Culture: Vygotsky’s Theory 136 The Zone of Proximal Development 137 Scaffolding 137 Private Speech 138

4.4 Language 139 The Road to Speech 139 First Words and Many More 142 Speaking in Sentences: Grammatical Development 146 Communicating with Others 149

    Summary 151

    Key Terms 153

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

118 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Throughout development, children express their thoughts in oral

and written language. In the last section of this chapter, you’ll see

how children master the sounds, words, and grammar of their native

language.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● According to Piaget, how do schemes, assimilation, and

accommodation provide the foundation for cognitive development throughout the life span?

●● How does thinking become more advanced as infants progress through the sensorimotor stage?

●● What are the distinguishing characteristics of thinking during the preoperational stage?

●● What are the strengths and weaknesses of Piaget’s theory?

●● How have contemporary researchers extended Piaget’s theory?

4.1 The Onset of Thinking: Piaget’s Account

Three-year-old Jamila loves talking to her grandmother (“Gram”) on the telephone. Some- times these conversations don’t succeed because Gram asks questions and Jamila replies by nodding her head “yes” or “no.” Jamila’s dad has explained that Gram (and others on the phone) can’t see her nodding—that she needs to say “yes” or “no.” But Jamila invariably returns to head-nodding. Her dad can’t understand why such a bright and talkative child doesn’t realize that nodding is meaningless over the phone.

Why does Jamila insist on nodding her head when she’s talking on the phone? This behav- ior is quite typical according to the famous Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget (1896–1980). In Piaget’s theory, children’s thinking progresses through four qualitatively different stages. In this section, we’ll begin by describing some of the general features of Piaget’s theory, then examine Piaget’s account of thinking during the first two stages (those spanning infancy and the preschool years), and finally consider some of the strengths and weak- nesses of the theory.

Basic Principles of Cognitive Development Piaget believed that children are naturally curious. They constantly want to make sense of their experience and, in the process, construct their understanding of the world. For Piaget, children at all ages are like scientists in creating theories about how the world works. Of course, children’s theories are often incomplete but they are valuable in making the world seem more predictable to children.

According to Piaget, children understand the world with schemes, psychological struc- tures that organize experience. Schemes are mental categories of related events, objects, and knowledge. During infancy, most schemes are based on actions. Infants group objects based on the actions they can perform on them. For example, infants suck and grasp, and they use these actions to create categories of objects that can be sucked and objects that can be grasped.

Schemes are just as important after infancy, but they are now based primarily on functional or conceptual relationships, not action. For example, preschoolers learn that forks, knives, and spoons form a functional category of “things I use to eat.” And they learn that dogs, cats, and goldfish form a conceptual category of “pets.”

Like preschoolers, older children and adolescents have schemes based on functional and conceptual schemes. But they also have schemes that are based on increasingly

schemes according to Piaget, mental structures that organize information and regulate behavior.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 119

abstract properties. For example, an adolescent might put fascism, racism, and sexism in a category of “ideologies I despise.”

Thus, schemes of related objects, events, and ideas are present throughout develop- ment. But as children develop, their rules for creating schemes shift from physical activity to functional, conceptual, and, later, abstract properties of objects, events, and ideas.

Assimilation and Accommodation Schemes change constantly, adapting to children’s experiences. In fact, intellectual adapta- tion involves two processes working together: assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation occurs when new experiences are readily incorporated into existing schemes. Imagine a baby who has the familiar grasping scheme. The baby soon discovers that the grasping scheme also works well on blocks, toy cars, and other small objects. Extending the existing grasping scheme to new objects illustrates assimilation. Accommodation occurs when schemes are modified based on experience. Soon the infant learns that some objects can only be lifted with two hands and that some can’t be lifted at all. Changing the scheme so that it works for new objects (e.g., using two hands to grasp heavy objects) illus- trates accommodation.

Assimilation and accommodation are easier to understand when you remember Piaget’s belief that infants, children, and adolescents create theories to try to understand events and objects around them. An infant whose theory is that objects can be lifted with one hand finds the theory confirmed when she tries to pick up small objects, but she’s in for a surprise when she tries to pick up a heavy book. The unexpected result forces the infant, like a good scientist, to revise her theory to include this new finding.

The Real People feature shows how accommodation and assimilation allow young children to understand their worlds.

Equilibration and Stages of Cognitive Development Assimilation and accommodation are usually in balance, or equilibrium. Children find that they can readily assimilate many experiences into their schemes but that they some- times need to accommodate their schemes to adjust to new experiences. This balance between assimilation and accommodation was illustrated by our infant with a theory about lifting objects. However, periodically this balance is upset, producing a state of dis- equilibrium. That is, children discover that their schemes are not adequate because they are spending too much time accommodating and much less time assimilating. When dis- equilibrium occurs, children reorganize their schemes to return to a state of equilibrium, a process that Piaget called equilibration. To restore the balance, current but outmoded ways of thinking are replaced with a qualitatively different, more advanced set of schemes.

assimilation according to Piaget, taking in information that is compatible with what one already knows.

accommodation according to Piaget, changing existing knowledge based on new knowledge.

equilibration according to Piaget, the process by which children reorganize their schemes to return to a state of equilibrium when disequilib- rium occurs.

When Ethan, an energetic 2½-year-old, saw a monarch butterfly for the first time, his mother, Kat, told him, “Butterfly, but- terfly; that’s a butterfly, Ethan.” A few minutes later, a zebra swallowtail butterfly landed on a nearby bush and Ethan shouted in excite- ment, “Butterfly, Mama, butterfly!” A little later, a moth flew out of another bush; with even greater excitement in his voice, Ethan shouted, “Butterfly, Mama, more butterfly!” As Kat was telling Ethan, “No, honey. That’s

a moth, not a butterfly,” she marveled at how rapidly Ethan seemed to grasp new concepts with so little direction from her. How was this possible?

Piaget’s explanation would be that when Kat named the monarch butterfly for Ethan, he formed a simple theory, something like “but- terflies are bugs with big wings.” The second butterfly differed in color but was still a bug with big wings, so it was readily assimilated into Ethan’s new theory of butterflies. However,

when Ethan referred to the moth as a but- terfly, Kat corrected him. Ethan was forced to accommodate to this new experience. He changed his theory of butterflies to make it more precise; the new theory might be some- thing like “butterflies are bugs with thin bodies and big, colorful wings.” He also created a new theory, something like “a moth is a bug with a bigger body and plain wings.” Accom- modation and assimilation work together to help Ethan make sense of his experiences.

Real People Applying Human Development

Learning about Butterflies: Accommodation and Assimilation in Action

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

120 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

One way to understand equilibration is to return to the metaphor of the child as a scientist. As we discussed in Chapter 1, good scientific theories readily explain some phenomena but usually must be revised to explain others. Children’s theories allow them to understand many experiences by predicting, for example, what will happen (“It’s morning, so it’s time for break- fast”) or who will do what (“Mom’s gone to work, so Dad will take me to school”), but the theories must be modified when predic- tions go awry (“Dad thinks I’m old enough to walk to school, so he won’t take me”).

Sometimes scientists find that their theories contain critical flaws that can’t be fixed simply by revising; instead, they must create a new theory that draws on the older theory but differs fundamentally. For example, when the astronomer Copernicus realized that the earth-centered theory of the solar system was

fundamentally wrong, his new theory built on the assumption that the sun is the center of the solar system. In much the same way, periodically children reach states in which their current theories seem to be wrong much of the time, so they abandon these theories in favor of more advanced ways of thinking about their physical and social worlds.

According to Piaget, these revolutionary changes in thought occur three times over the life span, at approximately 2, 7, and 11 years of age. This divides cognitive development into the following four stages:

Period of develoPment Age rAnge Sensorimotor period Infancy (0–2 years)

Preoperational period Preschool and early elementary school years (2–7 years)

Concrete operational period Middle and late elementary school years (7–11 years)

Formal operational period Adolescence and adulthood (11 years and up)

This baby will learn that many objects can be grasped easily with one hand— illustrating assimilation—but will also discover that bigger, heavier objects can be grasped only with two hands—illustrating accommodation.

Ki m

Pi nP

ho to

gr ap

hy /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

The ages listed are only approximate. Some youngsters move through the periods more rapidly than other children do. However, the only route to formal operations— the  most sophisticated type of thought—is through the first three periods, in sequence. Sensorimotor thinking always gives rise to preoperational thinking; a child cannot “skip” preoperational thinking and move directly from the sensorimotor to the concrete operational period.

In the next few pages of this chapter, we consider Piaget’s account of sensorimotor and preoperational thinking, the periods from birth to approximately 7 years of age. In Chapter 6, we will return to Piaget’s theory to examine his account of concrete and formal operational thinking in older children and adolescents.

Sensorimotor Thinking Piaget (1951, 1952, 1954) believed that the first two years of life form a distinct phase in human development. The sensorimotor period, from birth to roughly 2 years of age, is the first of Piaget’s four periods of cognitive development. In the 24 months of this stage, infants’ thinking progresses remarkably along three important fronts.

Adapting to and Exploring the Environment Newborns respond reflexively to many stimuli, but between 1 and 4 months, reflexes are first modified by experience. An infant may inadvertently touch his lips with his thumb, thereby initiating sucking and the pleasing sensations associated with sucking. Later, the

sensorimotor period first of Piaget’s four stages of cognitive development, which lasts from birth to approximately 2 years.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 121

infant tries to re-create these sensations by guiding his thumb to his mouth. Sucking no longer occurs only reflexively when a mother places a nipple at the infant’s mouth; instead, the infant can initiate sucking.

At about 8 months, infants reach a watershed: the onset of deliberate, intentional behavior. For the first time, the “means” and “end” of activities are distinct. For example, if a father places his hand in front of a toy, an infant will move his father’s hand to be able to play with the toy. “The moving the hand” scheme is the means to achieve the goal of “grasping the toy.” Using one action as a means to achieve another end is the first indica- tion of purposeful, goal-directed behavior during infancy.

Beginning at about 12 months, infants become active experimenters. An infant may deliberately shake a number of different objects, trying to discover which produce sounds. Or an infant may decide to drop different objects to see what happens. An infant will dis- cover that stuffed animals land quietly, whereas bigger toys often make a more satisfying “clunk” when they hit the ground. These actions represent a significant extension of inten- tional behavior; now babies repeat actions with different objects solely for the purpose of seeing what will happen.

Understanding Objects The world is filled with animate objects such as dogs, spiders, and college students, as well as inanimate objects such as cheeseburgers, socks, and this book. But they all share a fun- damental property—they exist independently of our actions and thoughts toward them. Piaget’s term for this understanding that objects exist independently is object permanence. And Piaget made the astonishing claim that infants lacked this understanding for much of the first year. He proposed that an infant’s understanding of objects could be summarized as “out of sight, out of mind.” For infants, objects exist when in sight and no longer exist when out of sight.

Piaget showed that 4- to 8-month-olds will grasp for an interesting object that is placed in front of them. However, if the object is then hidden by a barrier or covered with a cloth, the infant will neither reach nor search. Instead, the infant seems to lose all inter- est in the object, as if the now hidden object no longer exists. Paraphrasing the familiar phrase, “out of sight is out of existence.”

Beginning at about 8 months, infants search for an object that an experimenter has covered with a cloth. In fact, many 8- to 12-month-olds enjoy playing this game: An adult covers the object, and the infant sweeps away the cover, laughing and smil- ing all the while! But despite this accomplishment, their understanding of object permanence remains incomplete, according to Piaget. If 8- to 10-month-olds see an object hidden under one container several times and then see it hidden under a second container, they usually reach for the toy under the first container. Piaget claimed that this behavior shows fragmentary understanding of objects because infants do not distinguish the object from the actions they use to locate it, such as reaching for a particular container.

Piaget argued that not until approximately 18 months do infants have full understanding of object permanence. However, in a few pages, we’ll see that infants know more about objects than Piaget claimed.

Using Symbols By 18 months, most infants have begun to talk and gesture, evidence of their emerging capacity to use symbols. Words and gestures are symbols that stand for something else. When a baby waves, it’s a symbol that’s just as effective as saying “good-bye” to bid farewell. Children also begin to engage in pretend play, another use of symbols. At 20 months, children may move their hand back and forth in front of their mouth, pretending to brush their teeth.

Once infants can use symbols, they can anticipate the consequences of actions mentally instead of having to perform them. Imagine that an infant and a par- ent construct a tower of blocks next to an open door. Leaving the room, a 12- to

object permanence understanding, acquired in infancy, that objects exist independently of oneself.

Toddlers frequently gesture, a sign of their growing competence at using symbols.

D av

id Y

ou ng

-W ol

ff /P

ho to

Ed it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

122 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

18-month-old might close the door, knocking over the tower because he cannot foresee this outcome of closing the door. But an 18- to 24-month-old can anticipate the conse- quence of closing the door and move the tower beforehand.

In just 2 years, the infant progresses from responding reflexively to actively exploring the world, understanding objects, and using symbols. These achievements are remarkable and set the stage for preoperational thinking, which we’ll examine next.

Preoperational Thinking Once young children have crossed into preoperational thinking, the magical power of symbols is available to them. Of course, mastering this power is a lifelong process; the pre- school child’s efforts are tentative and sometimes incorrect. Piaget identified a number of characteristic shortcomings in preschoolers’ symbolic skills. Let’s look at three.

Egocentrism Preoperational children typically believe that others see the world—literally and figuratively— exactly as they do. Egocentrism is difficulty in seeing the world from another’s outlook. When  youngsters stubbornly cling to their own way, they are not simply being contrary. Preoperational children do not comprehend that other people differ in their ideas, convic- tions, and emotions.

One of Piaget’s famous experiments, the three-mountains problem, demonstrates preoperational children’s egocentrism (Piaget & Inhelder, 1956, Chap. 8). Youngsters were seated at a table like the one shown in ◗Figure 4.1. When preoperational children were asked to choose the photograph that corresponded to another person’s view of the mountains, they usually picked the photograph that showed their own view of the mountains, not the other person’s. Preoperational youngsters evidently suppose that the mountains are seen

egocentrism Difficulty in seeing the world from anoth- er’s point of view; typical of children in the preoperational period.

●◗ Figure 4.1 Egocentrism: When asked to select the photograph that shows the mountains as the adult sees them, preschool children often select the photograph that shows how the mountains look to them.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 123

the  same way by all; they presume that theirs is the only view, not one of many possible views. According to Piaget, only concrete operational children fully understand that all people do not experience an event in exactly the same way.

In the vignette, 3-year-old Jamila nods her head during phone conversations with her grandmother. This, too, reflects preoperational egocentrism. Jamila assumes that because she is aware that her head is moving up and down (or side to side), her grandmother must be aware of it, too. Because of this egocentrism, preoperational youngsters often attribute their own thoughts and feelings to others. They may even credit inanimate objects with life and lifelike properties, a phenomenon known as animism (Piaget, 1929). A preschool child may think that the sun is unhappy on a cloudy day or that a car hurts when it’s in an acci- dent. Caught up in their egocentrism, preoperational youngsters believe that inanimate objects have feelings just as they do.

Centration A second characteristic of preoperational thinking is that children seem to have the psy- chological equivalent of tunnel vision: They often concentrate on one aspect of a problem but ignore other, equally relevant aspects. Centration is Piaget’s term for this narrowly focused thought that characterizes preoperational youngsters.

Piaget demonstrated centration in his experiments involving conservation. In these experiments, Piaget wanted to determine when children realize that important character- istics of objects (or sets of objects) stay the same despite changes in their physical appear- ance. Some tasks that Piaget used to study conservation are shown in ◗ Figure 4.2. Each begins with identical objects (or sets of objects). Then one of the objects (or sets) is trans- formed, and children are asked if the objects are the same in terms of some important feature.

A typical conservation problem involves conservation of liquid quantity. Children are shown identical beakers filled with the same amount of juice. After children agree that the two beakers have the same amount of juice, the juice is poured from one beaker into a taller, thinner beaker. The juice looks different in the tall, thin beaker—it rises higher—but of course the amount is unchanged. Nevertheless, preoperational children claim that the tall, thin beaker has more juice than the original beaker. (And if the juice is poured into a wider beaker, they believe it has less.)

What is happening here? According to Piaget, preoperational children center on the level of the juice in the beaker. If the juice is higher after it is poured, preoperational chil- dren believe that there must be more juice now than before. Because preoperational think- ing is centered, these youngsters ignore the fact that the change in the level of the juice is always accompanied by a change in the diameter of the beaker.

Appearance as Reality A final feature of preoperational thinking is that preschool children believe that an object’s appearance tells what the object is really like. For instance, many a 3-year-old has watched with fascination as an older brother or sister put on a ghoulish costume only to erupt in frightened tears when their sibling put on scary makeup. The scary made-up face is reality, not just something that looks frightening but really isn’t.

Confusion between appearance and reality is not limited to cos- tumes and masks. It is a general characteristic of preoperational thinking. Consider the following cases where appearances and reality conflict:

●● A boy is angry because a friend is being mean but smiles because he’s afraid the friend will leave if he reveals his anger.

●● A glass of milk looks brown when seen through sunglasses. ●● A piece of hard rubber looks like food (e.g., a piece of pizza).

animism Phenomenon of crediting inanimate objects with life and lifelike properties such as feelings.

centration according to Piaget, narrowly focused type of thought characteristic of preoperational children.

In conservation problems, preschool chil- dren typically do not believe that the quan- tity of a liquid remains the same when it is poured into a taller, more slender beaker.

Le w

M er

rim /S

ci en

ce S

ou rc

e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

124 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Older children and adults know that the boy looks happy, the milk looks brown, and the object looks like food but that the boy is really angry, the milk is really white, and the object is really rubber. In contrast, preoperational children confuse appearance and real- ity, thinking the boy is happy, the milk is brown, and the piece of rubber is edible.

The defining characteristics of preoperational thought are summarized in ◗ Table 4.1.

Think About It Children with low birth weight often have delayed intellectual development. According to Piaget, what form might the delay take?

●◗ Figure 4.2 Children in the preoperational stage of development typically have difficulty solving conservation problems, in which important features of an object (or objects) stay the same despite changes in physical appearance.

Liquid quantity

Pour water from one glass into a shorter, wider glass.

Is there the same amount of water in each glass?

Are there the same number of pennies in each row?

Are these sticks the same length?

Does each ball have the same amount of clay?

Now does each piece have the same amount of clay, or does one have more?

Does each cow have the same amount of grass to eat?

Now does each cow have the same amount to eat, or does one cow have more?

Now are the sticks the same length, or is one longer?

Now are there the same number of pennies in each row, or does one row have more?

Now is there the same amount of water in each glass, or does one glass have more?

Stretch out the top row of pennies, push together the bottom row.

Move one stick to the left and the other to the right.

Roll one ball so that it looks like a sausage.

Spread out the squares in one field.

Number

Length

Mass

Area

Type of conservation Starting configuration Final configurationTransformation

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 125

Evaluating Piaget’s Theory Because Piaget’s theory is so comprehensive, it has stimulated much research. Much of this work supports Piaget’s view that children actively try to understand the world around them and to organize their knowledge and that cognitive development includes major qualitative changes (Brainerd, 1996; Flavell, 1996). One important contribution of Piaget’s theory is that many teachers and parents have found it a rich source of ideas about ways to foster children’s development.

Guidelines for Fostering Cognitive Development Piaget’s theory has several straightforward implications for conditions that promote cognitive growth.

●● Cognitive growth occurs as children construct their own understanding of the world, so the teacher’s role is to create environments where children can discover how the world works. A teacher shouldn’t simply try to tell children how addition and subtrac- tion are complementary, but should provide children with materials that allow them to discover the complementarity themselves.

●● Children profit from experience only when they can interpret the experience with their current cognitive structures. Consequently, the best teaching experiences are slightly ahead of the children’s current level of thinking. As youngsters begin to mas- ter basic addition, don’t jump directly to subtraction, but go to slightly more difficult addition problems.

●● Cognitive growth can be particularly rapid when children discover inconsistencies and errors in their own thinking. Therefore, teachers should encourage children to look at the consistency of their thinking, but then let children take the lead in sorting out the inconsistencies. If a child is making mistakes in borrowing on subtraction problems, a teacher shouldn’t correct the error directly, but should encourage the child to look at a large number of these errors to discover what he or she is doing wrong.

Criticisms of Piaget’s Theory Although Piaget’s contributions to child development are legendary, some elements of his theory have held up better than others (Miller, 2011; Newcombe, 2013; Siegler & Alibali, 2005).

●● Piaget’s theory underestimates cognitive competence in infants and young children and overestimates cognitive competence in adoles- cents. In Piaget’s theory, cognitive development is steady in early childhood but not particularly rapid. In contrast, a main theme

Table 4.1

Characteristics of Preoperational Thinking ChArACteristiC definition exAmPle Egocentrism Child believes that all people see the world as he or she

does A child gestures during a telephone conversation, not realizing that the listener cannot see the gestures

Centration Child focuses on one aspect of a problem or situation but ignores other relevant aspects

In conservation of liquid quantity, child pays attention to the height of the liquid in the beaker but ignores the diameter of the beaker

Appearance as reality Child assumes that an object really is what it appears to be

Child believes that a person smiling at another person is really happy even though the other person is being mean

According to Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, children need to learn by doing.

Ja ck

.Q /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

126 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

of modern child development science is that infants and toddlers are extraordinarily competent. By using more sensitive tasks than Piaget’s, modern investigators have shown that infants and toddlers are vastly more capable than expected based on Piaget’s theory. For example, in a few pages, we’ll see that infants have a much greater understanding of objects than Piaget believed. However, Piaget overestimated cogni- tive skill in adolescents, who often fail to reason according to formal operational prin- ciples and revert to less sophisticated reasoning.

●● Piaget’s theory is vague concerning processes and mechanisms of change. Many of the key components of the theory, such as accommodation and assimilation, are too vague to test scientifically. Consequently, scientists abandoned them in favor of other cog- nitive processes that could be evaluated more readily and hence could provide more convincing accounts of children’s thinking.

●● Piaget’s stage model does not account for variability in children’s performance. In Piaget’s view, each stage of intellectual development has unique characteristics that leave their mark on everything a child does. For example, preoperational thinking is defined by egocentrism and centration. Consequently, children’s performance on different tasks should be consistent. On the conservation and the three-mountains tasks, for instance, according to Piaget, 4-year-olds should always respond in a preop- erational way: They should say that the water is not the same after pouring and that another person sees the mountains the same way they do. In fact, children’s thinking is not this consistent. A child’s thinking may be sophisticated in some domains but naïve in others (Siegler, 1981). This inconsistency does not support Piaget’s view that children’s thinking should always reflect the distinctive imprint of their current stage of cognitive development.

●● Piaget’s theory undervalues the influence of the sociocultural environment on cogni- tive development. Returning to the metaphor of the child as scientist, Piaget describes the child as a lone scientist, constantly trying to figure out by herself how her theory coordinates with data. In reality, a child’s effort to understand her world is far more social than Piaget described. Her growing understanding of the world is profoundly influenced by interactions with family members, peers, and teachers, and it takes place against the backdrop of cultural values. Piaget did not ignore these social and cultural forces entirely, but they are not prominent in his theory.

These criticisms do not mean that Piaget’s theory is invalid or should be abandoned. As noted previously, it remains the most complete account of cognitive development. How- ever, in recent years, researchers have attempted to round out our understanding of cogni- tive development using other theoretical perspectives, such as the information-processing approach examined later in this chapter.

Extending Piaget’s Account: Children’s Naïve Theories Piaget believed that children, like scientists, formulate theories about how the world works. Children’s theories are usually called “naïve theories” because, unlike real scien- tific theories, they are not created by specialists and are rarely evaluated by real experi- ments. Nevertheless, naïve theories are valuable because they allow children (and adults) to understand new experiences and predict future events.

In Piaget’s view, children formulate a grand, comprehensive theory that attempts to explain an enormous variety of phenomena—including reasoning about objects, people, and morals, for example—within a common framework. More recent views retain the idea of children as theorists but propose that children, like real scientists, develop specialized theories about much narrower areas. For example, according to the core knowledge hypothesis, infants are born with rudimentary knowledge of the world; this knowledge is elaborated based on children’s experiences (Newcombe, 2013; Spelke & Kinzler, 2007). Some of the theories young children first develop concern physics, psychology, and biol- ogy. That is, infants and toddlers rapidly develop theories that organize their knowledge about properties of objects, people, and living things (Wellman & Gelman, 1998).

core knowledge hypothesis Infants are born with rudimentary knowl- edge of the world, which is elaborated based on experiences.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 127

We examined children’s developing theory of mind in Chapter 3; in the next few pages, we’ll look at children’s naïve theories of physics and biology.

Naïve Physics As adults, we know much about objects and their properties. For example, we know that if we place a coffee cup on a table, it will remain there unless another person moves it; it will not move by itself or disappear. And we don’t release a coffee cup in midair because we know that an unsupported object will fall. Child development researchers have long been interested in young children’s understanding of objects, in part because Piaget claimed that understanding of objects develops slowly and takes many months to become com- plete. However, by devising some clever procedures, other investigators have shown that babies understand objects much earlier than Piaget claimed. Renée Baillargeon (1987, 1994), for example, assessed object permanence by using a procedure in which infants first saw a silver screen that appeared to be rotating back and forth. After an infant became familiar with this display, one of two new displays was shown. In the “realistic” event, a red box appeared in a position behind the screen, making it impossible for the screen to rotate as far back as it had previously. Instead, the screen moved away from the infant until it made contact with the box, then moved back toward the infant. In the “unrealistic” event, shown in ◗ Figure 4.3, the red box appeared but the screen continued to move as before. The screen moved away from the infant until it was flat, then moved forward, again revealing the red box. The illusion was possible because the box was mounted on a mov- able platform that allowed it to drop out of the way of the moving screen. However, from the infant’s perspective, the box seemed to vanish behind the screen, only to reappear.

●◗ Figure 4.3 Object permanence in 3½- and 4½-month-old infants. Infants are surprised to see the silver screen rotate flat, which suggests that they understand the “permanence” of the red box. box (Baillargeon, R., 1987).

1. The silver screen is lying flat on the table and the red box is fully visible.

3. The silver screen is now vertical, blocking the red box.

4. The silver screen continues to rotate, blocking the red box, which has started to drop through the trap door.

6. The silver screen is rotating back toward the infant but still blocks the red box.

7. The silver screen is again flat and the box fully visible to the infant.

5. The silver screen is completely flat, apparently having “rotated through” the red box which is actually now under the table.

2. The silver screen has begun to rotate, but the red box is largely visible.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

128 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

The disappearance and reappearance of the box violates the idea that objects exist permanently. Consequently, an infant who under- stands that objects are permanent should find the unrealistic event a novel stimulus and look at it longer than the realistic event. Baillargeon found that 4½-month-olds consistently looked longer at the unrealistic event than at the realistic event. Infants apparently thought that the unrealistic event was novel, just as we are surprised when an object vanishes from a magician’s scarf. Evidently, infants have some under- standing of object permanence early in the first year of life.

Of course, infants’ understanding of objects is rudimentary and research has begun to reveal the specific features of events that lead infants to believe that objects do not vanish (Bremner, Slater, & Johnson, 2015). And infants know more about objects than perma- nence. They know that objects move continuously on a path, not magi-

cally moving from one spot to another; they know that objects are solid and that objects cannot “pass through” each other; they know that one object must contact another to cause movement; and they know that unsupported objects will fall (Hespos & vanMarle, 2012; Wang, Zhang, & Baillargeon, 2016). In short, infants rapidly create a simple but use- ful theory of basic properties of objects, a theory that helps them expect that objects such as toys will act predictably.

Naïve Biology Fundamental to adults’ naïve theories is the distinction between living and nonliving things. Adults know that living things, for example, are made of cells, inherit properties from parents, and move spontaneously. Adults’ theories of living things begin in infancy, when youngsters first distinguish animate objects (e.g., people, insects, other animals) from inanimate objects (e.g., rocks, plants, furniture, tools). Motion is critical in infants’ understanding of the difference between animate and inanimate objects. That is, by about 9 months, infants have determined that animate objects are self-propelled, sometimes move in irregular paths, and act to achieve goals (Opfer & Gelman, 2011).

During the preschool years, children’s naïve theories of biology expand to include many specific properties associated with living things, particularly when they have daily contact with animals (Geerdts, Van de Walle, & LoBue, 2015). Many 4-year-olds’ theo- ries of biology include the ideas that only animals can grow, become ill, and heal when damaged (Legare, Wellman, & Gelman, 2009; Margett & Witherington, 2011). And their understanding is so sophisticated that children aren’t fooled by lifelike robots: 4-year-olds know that robots are machines that (a) do not eat or grow and (b) are made by people and can break (Jipson & Gelman, 2007). Nevertheless, preschoolers believe that robots rep- resent a special kind of machine because youngsters attribute humanlike traits to robots, such as being friendly (Kahn, Gary, & Shen, 2013).

A fundamental part of young children’s theory of living things is a commitment to teleological explanations: Children believe that livings things, including their parts and their actions, exist for a purpose. For example, young children believe that lions exist so that people can see them in a zoo (Kelemen, 2003). And they believe that a child’s pet ran away to teach the child to be more responsible (Banerjee & Bloom, 2015). Teleological thinking resembles animism (described on page 123) and may reflect a child’s tendency to extend to animals the knowledge that objects such as tools are made for a specific purpose (Kelemen & DiYanni, 2005).

Young children’s theories of living things are also rooted in essentialism: Children believe that many living things have an essence that can’t be seen but gives a living thing its identity. Specifically, essentialism assumes that certain natural categories (e.g., turtles, girls) have sharp boundaries that can’t be changed, that members of these categories share many features, and that these shared features are caused by an underlying essence present in all category members (Gelman, 2003). For example, essentialism says that birds form a category that is clearly distinguishable from other categories (i.e., there are no animals that

teleological explanations Children’s belief that living things, includ- ing their parts and their actions, exist for a purpose.

essentialism Children’s belief that all living things have an essence that can’t be seen but gives a living thing its identity.

Toddlers distinguish animate objects, such as goats, from inanimate objects, such as furniture and tools.

Va le

ry M

in ya

ev /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 129

are part bird and part squirrel), they share many features (e.g., they fly, sing, and lay eggs), and they have an underlying essence—“birdness”—that is responsible for the shared fea- tures. Young children’s essentialism explains why 4-year-olds believe that a baby kangaroo adopted by goats will still hop and have a pouch and why they believe that a watermelon seed planted in a cornfield will produce watermelons (Solomon & Zaitchik, 2012).

One way to see children’s essentialism in action is through “transplant studies” in which children are told about internal parts transplanted from one person to another and children are asked whether the organ recipient takes on the properties of the organ donor. One of these studies is the subject of the Spotlight on Research feature.

On ResearchSpotlight

Have a Heart! Preschoolers’ Essentialist Thinking

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Imagine that your heart is failing and that you need a transplant to live. The only heart available is Adolf Hitler’s. Would you take it? Many adults would not because they fear that organ recipients might take on the properties of the organ donor, even though there is no evidence that transplanting a heart would transfer properties of the donor. This shows essentialism: The heart is seen as part of the essence of “human-ness” so a transplant transfers other properties to the recipient. Would preschool children show this sort of essentialism? Meredith Meyer and her col- leagues, Susan Gelman, Steven Roberts, and Sarah-Jane Leslie, conducted a study to find out (2016).

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Meyer and colleagues created scenarios in which children were described as being nice, mean, smart, or not smart. They also included scenarios that described pigs and monkeys acting naturally. Children participating in the study were asked to imagine that they had received $1 from the child or animal in the scenario or that they had received a heart from the child or animal in the scenario. Then they were asked if they had received $1 or a heart, whether they would become like the child or animal in the scenario (i.e., become nice, mean, smart, not smart, piglike, monkeylike).

Who were the children in the study? The researchers tested preschool children and elementary school children. For simplicity, we describe only the findings for the 4- and 5-year-olds.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental: Meyers and

colleagues manipulated the kind of transfer— heart versus money—and examined its impact on children’s judgments that they would take on characteristics of the donor. The study was cross-sectional, but we’re only considering the findings from the younger children.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. The tasks posed no danger to the children.

What were the results? The results are shown in ◗ Figure 4.4. A score of 0 corre- sponds to “no transfer”—in other words, chil- dren judged that they would not take on the characteristics of the donor. Scores greater than 0 indicate ever greater judgments that children would take on the features of the donor. The left bars of Figure 4.4 show that overall preschoolers believed they would be unaffected by receiving $1 but would be affected by receiving a heart.

One potential problem is that nice people are sometimes referred to as “warm hearted” and mean people are sometimes described as “cold hearted,” so children’s responses might not reflect essentialist thinking. Consequently, to provide a more stringent test of chil- dren’s essentialist beliefs, the research- ers repeated the analyses but excluding nice and mean (i.e., including only smart, not smart, piglike, and monkeylike). The results, shown in the right bars, are nearly identical to the full analyses. Children do not believe that they would become not smart or monkeylike if they were to receive $1, but they think it would be possible if they were to receive a heart.

What did the investigators conclude? The findings illustrate a key element of essentialism: “children appeal to an internal . . . essence when making

predictions about outward features” (p. 15). In other words, children seem to view the heart as an internal essence that could cause a recipient of that heart to take on properties of the donor, such as being nice, smart, or like a monkey.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Meyer and her colleagues examined transfer of a single internal organ: It would be useful to extend the work to other internal organs such as the stomach or the lungs. And researchers, by asking children about transplants involving hair or skin (biological but not internal) or a pacemaker (internal but not biological), could describe more precisely what preschool chil- dren consider to be essences.

●◗ Figure 4.4 Preschool children believe that they might take on the features of a heart donor, a finding that reveals essentialist thinking.

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

All features Not including mean, not mean

C h

il d

re n

’s j

u d

gm en

ts t

h at

t h

ey w

ou ld

t ak

e on

f ea

tu re

s of

d on

or s

$1 Heart

Children believe they might take on features of heart donors.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

130 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

mental hardware mental and neural structures that are built in and allow the mind to operate.

mental software mental “programs” that are the basis for performing particular tasks.

Of course, although preschoolers’ naïve theories of biology are complex, their theories have some flaws. The most striking is that although preschoolers know that plants grow and heal, they don’t consider plants to be living things, apparently because they see goal- directed motion as a key property of living things (Opfer & Siegler, 2004). Nevertheless, children’s naïve theories of biology, when joined with their naïve theory of physics, provide powerful tools for making sense of their world and for understanding new experiences.

recall 1. The term refers to modification of schemes

based on experience.

2. According to Piaget, are psychological struc- tures that organize experience.

3. Piaget believed that infants’ understanding of objects could be summarized as .

4. By 18 months, most infants talk and gesture, which shows that they have the capacity .

5. Preschoolers are often , meaning that they are unable to take another person’s viewpoint.

6. Preoperational children sometimes attribute thoughts and feelings to inanimate objects; this is called .

7. One criticism of Piaget’s theory is that it underestimates cognitive competence in .

8. Most 4-year-olds know that living things move, , become ill, and heal when injured.

Interpret ●● Piaget championed the view that children participate

actively in their own development. How do the sensorimotor child’s contributions differ from the formal-operational child’s contributions?

Apply ●● Based on what you know about Piaget’s theory, what would

his position have been on the continuity–discontinuity issue discussed in Chapter 1?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 4.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is the basis of the information-processing approach?

●● How well do young children pay attention?

●● What kinds of learning take place during infancy?

●● Do infants and preschool children remember?

●● What do infants and preschoolers know about numbers?

4.2 Information Processing During Infancy and Early Childhood

When Claire, a bubbly 3-year-old, is asked how old she’ll be on her next birthday, she proudly says, “Four!” while holding up five fingers. Asked to count four objects, whether they’re can- dies, toys, or socks, Claire usually says, “1, 2, 6, 7 . . . SEVEN!” Claire’s older brothers find this very funny, but her mother thinks that, the mistakes notwithstanding, Claire’s behavior shows that she knows a great deal about numbers and counting. But what exactly does Claire understand? That question has her mother stumped!

Today, many developmentalists borrow from computer science to formulate their ideas about human thinking and how it develops (Plunkett, 1996). As you recall from Chapter 1, this approach is called information processing. In this section, we’ll see what information processing has revealed about young children’s thinking and, along the way, see what to make of Claire’s counting.

General Principles of Information Processing In the information-processing view, human thinking is based on both mental hardware and mental software. Mental hardware refers to mental and neural structures that are built in and allow the mind to operate. Mental software refers to mental “programs” that

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 131

are the basis for performing particular tasks. Information-processing psychologists claim that as children develop, their mental software becomes more complex, more powerful, and more efficient.

In the next few pages, we’ll look at the development of information-processing skills in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers, beginning with attention.

Attention Hannah was only 3 days old and was often startled by the sounds of traffic outside her family’s apartment. Hannah’s parents worried that she might not get enough sleep. Yet within a few days, traffic sounds no longer disturbed Hannah; she slept blissfully. Why was a noise that had been so troubling no longer a problem? The key is attention, a process that determines which sensory information receives additional cognitive processing.

Hannah’s response was normal not only for infants but also for children and adoles- cents. When presented with a strong or unfamiliar stimulus, an orienting response usu- ally occurs: A person startles, fixes the eyes on the stimulus, and shows changes in heart rate and brain-wave activity. Collectively, these responses indicate that the infant has noticed the stimulus. Remember, too, that Hannah soon ignored the sounds of traffic. When a stimulus is presented repeatedly, people recognize it as familiar and the orienting response gradually disappears. habituation is the diminished response to a stimulus as it becomes familiar.

The orienting response and habituation are both useful to infants. Orienting makes the infant aware of potentially important or dangerous events in the environment. How- ever, responding constantly to insignificant stimuli is wasteful, so habituation keeps infants from devoting too much energy to insignificant events (Rovee-Collier, 1987).

Preschool children gradually learn how to focus their attention, but when compared with older children and adults, they are often not very attentive (Hatania & Smith, 2010). In the meantime, teachers and parents can help young children pay attention better. For example, Tools of the Mind is a curriculum for preschool and kindergarten children that uses pretend play to improve attentional processes (Diamond & Lee, 2011). Pretend play may seem like a surprising way to improve attention, but staying “in character” while pretending teaches children to inhibit inappropriate “out-of-character” behavior. And it encourages thinking flexibly as children respond to their playmates’ improvisation. Teachers also contribute by providing visual reminders of the need to pay attention, such as showing a drawing of an ear to remind children to listen. Tools of the Mind improves attention, particularly in children who are at risk for problems in school (Blair, 2016).

Parents can also help promote their children’s attentional skills. In one study (Neville et al., 2013), parents and their preschool children attended an after-school program that included activities designed to improve children’s attention. For exam- ple, in one task, children were taught how to color carefully while being distracted by nearby peers playing with balloons. Parents were taught ways to support their chil- dren’s attention, for example by monitoring how they spoke to their children. After children participated in this program, their attention improved.

Learning From birth, babies rapidly learn about the people and the objects in their environ- ment. This learning reflects several processes:

●● You may recall experiments in which dogs learned to salivate when they heard a bell. This learning represents classical conditioning, in which a neutral stimulus (a bell) elicits a response (salivation) that was originally produced by another stim- ulus ( food). Similarly, when infants repeatedly hear a tone just before sugar water is placed in their mouth with a dropper, they learn to suck when they hear the tone (Lipsitt, 1990). Classical conditioning gives infants a sense of order in their

attention Processes that determine which infor- mation will be processed further by an individual.

orienting response an individual views a strong or unfamiliar stimulus, and changes in heart rate and brain-wave activity occur.

habituation act of becoming unresponsive to a stimulus that is presented repeatedly.

classical conditioning a form of learning that involves pairing a neutral stimulus and a response originally produced by another stimulus.

Infants (and older children) pay attention to loud stimuli at first but then ignore them if they aren’t interesting or dangerous.

LI U

J IN

/A FP

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

132 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● Operant conditioning focuses on the relation between the consequences of behavior and the likelihood that the behavior will recur. When a child’s behavior leads to pleasant conse- quences, the child will probably behave similarly in the future; when the child’s behavior leads to unpleasant conse- quences, the child will probably not repeat the behavior. When a baby smiles, an adult may hug the baby in return; this pleasing consequence makes the baby more likely to smile in the future. When a baby grabs a family heirloom, an adult may become angry and shout at the baby; these unpleasant consequences make the baby less likely to grab the heirloom in the future.

●● In imitation, children, adolescents, and young adults learn much simply by watching others behave. For example, children learn new sports moves by watching pro ath- letes, they learn gender roles by watching TV, and they learn new computer games by watching peers. Some scientists believe that imitation begins at birth (Meltzoff & Moore, 1994) but the evidence is inconsistent and open to different interpretations (Oostenbroek et al., 2013, 2016). However, by 6 months, infants definitely imitate (Barr & Hayne, 1999). For example, a baby may imitate an adult waving her finger.

Processes like classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and imitation allow babies to make sense of the world around them.

Memory Young babies remember events for days and even weeks at a time, an ability that was revealed by experiments in which a ribbon from a mobile is attached to a 2- or 3-month- old’s leg (Rovee-Collier, 1997, 1999). Within minutes, the baby learns to kick to make the mobile move. When the ribbon is attached several days or a couple of weeks later, babies would still kick to make the mobile move. However, after several weeks passed, most babies forgot that kicking moved the mobile. But when babies were reminded—a researcher moved the mobile without attaching the ribbon to the baby’s foot—the next day most kicked to move the mobile. Thus, this work shows that three important features of memory exist by 2 and 3 months of age: (1) an event from the past is remembered, (2) over time, the event can no longer be recalled, and (3) a cue can serve to retrieve a memory that seems to have been forgotten.

From these humble origins, memory improves rapidly in older infants and toddlers. Youngsters require fewer exposure to information to remember it and they can remem- ber it longer (Lukowski & Bauer, 2014). When shown novel actions with toys and later asked to imitate what they saw, compared with infants, toddlers require fewer exposures to the actions to remember them and remember them longer. As children master language and are asked to describe past events aloud, older preschoolers provide more elaborate descriptions and often highlight the unique features of events (e.g., a birthday party with a Star Wars theme).

These improvements in memory can be traced, in part, to growth in the brain regions that support memory (Lukowski & Bower, 2014; Olson & Newcombe, 2014). The hippocampus, a brain structure that plays a key role in storing information in memory, develops rapidly during the first year although some regions are not mature until about two years. The prefrontal cortex, a structure responsible for retrieving these stored memories, develops more slowly, into the preschool and elementary school years. Thus, development of memory during the first two years reflects growth in these two different brain regions.

operant conditioning form of learning that emphasizes the con- sequences of reward and punishment.

Toddlers who have fun playing in the water will welcome the sound of the bathtub being filled.

sh up

ia n/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Several days after infants learn that kicking moves a mobile, they kick when they see the mobile, showing that they remember the connection between kicking and the mobile’s movements.

Co ur

te sy

o f D

r. Ca

ro ly

n Ro

ve e-

Co lli

er

environment; they learn that a stimulus is a signal for what will happen next. For example, a toddler may smile when she hears the family dog’s collar because she knows the dog is coming to play with her.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 133

Autobiographical Memory Do you remember the name of your teacher in fourth grade or where your high school graduation was held? In answering those questions, you searched memory, just as you would to answer questions such as “What is the capital of Canada?” and “Who invented the sewing machine?” However, answers to questions about Canada and sewing machines are based on gen- eral knowledge that you have not experienced personally; in contrast, answers to questions about your fourth-grade teacher and your high school graduation are based on knowledge unique to your own life. Autobiographical memory refers to people’s memory of the significant events and experiences of their own lives. Autobiographical memory is important because it helps people construct a personal life history. In addition, autobio- graphical memory allows people to relate their experiences to others, creating socially shared memories.

Autobiographical memory originates early. Infants and toddlers have the basic mem- ory skills that enable them to remember past events, but they forget so rapidly that many experiences are not represented in long-term memory. However, preschoolers’ memories are more robust, supporting autobiographical memory (Bauer, 2015). Layered on top of these memory skills is a child’s sense of self (Howe, 2014). Toddlers come to understand that they exist independently in space and time, an understanding that provides coherence and continuity to children’s experience. Children realize that the self who went to the park a few days ago is the same self who is now at a birthday party and is the same self who will read a book with dad before bedtime.

Also contributing to autobiographical memory are young children’s improved lan- guage skills, skills that allow children to converse with parents about past and future events (Fivush, 2014). Parents may talk about what the child did today at day care or remind the child about what she will be doing this weekend. Children’s autobiographical memories are richer when parents talk about past events in detail and, specifically, when they encourage children to expand their description of past events by asking open-ended questions such as “Where did Mommy go last night?” (Fivush, 2014). The elaborative style is more com- mon among European American parents than Asian parents, a finding that explains why European American children often report more specific details of events than Chinese children do (Wang, 2014).

Preschoolers as Eyewitnesses Research on children’s autobiographical memory has played a central role in cases of sus- pected child abuse. When abuse is suspected, the victim is usually the sole witness. To prosecute the alleged abuser, the child’s testimony is needed. But can preschoolers accu- rately recall these events? Sometimes. One obstacle to accurate testimony is that young children are often interviewed repeatedly, a practice that can cause them to confuse what actually happened with what others suggest may have happened. When the questioner is an adult in a position of authority, children often believe that what is suggested by the adult actually happened (Candel et al., 2009; Ceci & Bruck, 1998). They will tell a con- vincing tale about “what really happened” simply because adults have led them to believe things must have happened that way. Young children’s storytelling can be so convincing that professionals often cannot distinguish true and false reports (Gordon, Baker-Ward, & Ornstein, 2001).

Preschool children are particularly suggestible. Why? One idea is that preschool children are more suggestible because they don’t remember the sources of information that they remember (Poole & Lindsay, 1995; Roberts, Evans & Duncanson, 2016). For example, they may know that a cousin dressed as a ghost for Halloween, but they can’t remember whether they actually saw the cousin in her costume or learned about the cos- tume from a parent. When confused in this manner, preschoolers frequently assume they

autobiographical memory memories of the significant events and experiences of one’s own life

Asian parents are less likely than Europeans or North Americans to talk to their children about events that foster autobiographical memory.

sz ef

ei /1

23 RF

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

134 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

must have experienced something personally. Consequently, when preschool children are asked leading questions (e.g., “When the man touched you, did it hurt?”), children store this information but later have trouble distinguishing what they actually experienced from what interviewers imply that they experienced (Ghetti, 2008).

Although preschoolers are easily misled, they can provide reliable testimony if inter- viewers follow several guidelines derived from research on children’s memory. Specifically, interviewers should:

●● Interview children as soon as possible after the event in question. ●● Encourage children to tell the truth, to feel free to say “I don’t know” to questions, and

to correct interviewers when they say something that’s incorrect. ●● Avoid nonverbal cues such as gestures or facial expressions because these may lead

children to believe they should respond in a certain way. Also, avoid selectively rein- forcing responses that are consistent with allegations.

●● Start by asking children to describe the event in their own words (“Tell me what hap- pened after school.”), follow up with open-ended questions (“Can you tell me more about what happened while you were walking home?”), and avoid yes/no and multiple- choice questions (because they lead to fewer details as well as more mistakes and inconsistencies).

●● Allow children to understand and feel comfortable in the interview format by begin- ning with a neutral event (e.g., a birthday party or holiday celebration) before moving to the event of interest.

●● Ask questions that consider other explanations of the event (i.e., explanations that don’t involve abuse).

Following guidelines such as these can foster the conditions under which children are likely to recall past events more accurately and thereby be better witnesses (Brown & Lamb, 2015; Cleveland, Quas, & Lyon, 2016; Poole & Bruck, 2012).

Learning Number Skills Powerful learning and memory skills allow infants and preschoolers to learn much about their worlds. This rapid growth is well illustrated by research on children’s understanding of the concept of number. Basic number skills originate in infancy, long before babies learn the names of numbers. Many babies experience daily variation in quantity. They play with two blocks and see that another baby has three; they watch as a father sorts laundry and finds two black socks but only one blue sock; and they eat 6 grapes for lunch while an older brother eats 15.

From these experiences, babies apparently come to appreciate that quantity or amount is one of the ways in which objects in the world can differ. In fact, infants may represent number using two distinct systems (Mussolin et al., 2016). One system is used for sets of one to four objects and is very precise; it allows babies to distinguish one object from two and two objects from three (Opfer & Siegler, 2012).

This system also allows babies to perform simple addition and subtraction. In experi- ments using the method shown in ◗ Figure 4.5, infants view a stage with one mouse. A screen hides the mouse and then a hand appears with a second mouse, which is placed behind the screen. When the screen is removed and reveals one mouse, 5-month-olds look longer than when two mice appear. Apparently, 5-month-olds expect that one mouse plus another mouse should equal two mice, and they look longer when this expectancy is violated (Wynn, 1992). Likewise, when the stage first has two mice and one of them is removed, infants are surprised when the screen is removed and two mice are still on the stage.

The other system—known as the approximate number system (ANS)—estimates quantities and is used to distinguish larger sets. This system allows babies to distinguish 8  objects from 16 but not 8 objects from 12, because the ANS isn’t sufficiently precise (Opfer & Siegler, 2012). Representations of number in the ANS become sharper with development and this allows 6-year-olds to discriminate larger sets (e.g., 50 teddy bears

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 135

versus 60 teddy bears) that 3-year-olds cannot (Halberda & Feigenson, 2008). And num- ber representations in the ANS have been linked to a specific region in the brain’s parietal lobe (Emerson & Cantlon, 2015).

Learning to Count By 2 years of age, most youngsters know some number words and have begun to count. However, this counting is usually full of mistakes. They might count “1, 2, 6, 7”—skipping 3, 4, and 5. Gelman and Meck (1986) charted preschoolers’ understanding of counting. They simply placed several objects in front of a child and asked, “How many?” By analyz- ing children’s answers to many of these questions, Gelman and Meck discovered that by age 3, most children have mastered three basic principles of counting—at least for count- ing up to five objects.

●● One-to-one principle: There must be one and only one number name for each object that is counted. A child who counts three objects as “1, 2, a” understands this principle because the number of names matches the number of objects to be counted, even though the third name is a letter.

●● Stable-order principle: Number names must be counted in the same order. A child who counts in the same sequence—for example, consistently counting four objects as “1, 2, 4, 5”—understands this principle.

●● Cardinality principle: The last number name differs from the previous ones in a counting sequence by denoting the number of objects. Typically, 3-year-olds reveal their understanding of this principle by repeating the last number name, often with emphasis: “1, 2, 4, 8 . . . EIGHT!”

During the preschool years, children master these basic prin- ciples and apply them to ever-larger sets of objects. By age 5, most youngsters apply these counting principles to as many as nine objects. And children are more likely to master counting principles when parents mention numbers in their speech, such as counting objects with their children or simply stating the number of objects present ( Gunderson  & Levine, 2011). By age 4, most youngsters know the numbers to 20, and some can count to 99 (Siegler & Robinson, 1982).

one-to-one principle Counting principle that states that there must be one and only one number name for each object counted.

stable-order principle Counting principle that states that number names must always be counted in the same order.

cardinality principle Counting principle that states that the last number name denotes the number of objects being counted.

●◗ Figure 4.5 Infants are surprised when they see objects added or removed but the original number of objects is still present when the screen is removed; this pattern suggests some basic understanding of addition and subtraction.

Sequence of events 1 + 1 = 1 or 2

1. Object placed on stage 2. Screen comes up 3. Second object added 4. Hand leaves empty

or: impossible outcomeThen either: possible outcome

5. Screen drops… revealing 2 objects 5. Screen drops… revealing 1 object

By age 5, children have mastered the three counting principles and can apply them to large sets of objects.

A la

n O

dd ie

/P ho

to Ed

it

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

136 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

intersubjectivity mutual, shared understanding among participants in an activity.

guided participation Children’s involvement in structured activi- ties with others who are more skilled, typi- cally producing cognitive growth.

Thus far, we have not considered the impact of social context on children’s thinking. In the next section, we’ll examine a theory developed by Vygotsky, who believed that cog- nitive development has its roots in social interactions.

recall 1. One way to improve preschool children’s attention is to have

them engage in play.

2. Four-month-old Tanya has forgotten that kicking moves a mobile. To remind her of the link between kicking and the mobile’s movement, we could .

3. Preschoolers may be particularly suggestible because they are less skilled at .

4. When a child who is counting a set of objects repeats the last number, usually with emphasis, this indicates the child understands the principle of counting.

Interpret ●● Do the developmental mechanisms in the information-

processing perspective emphasize nature, nurture, or both? How?

Apply ●● Describe how research on children’s eyewitness testimony

illustrates connections among emotional, cognitive, and social development.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 4.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is the zone of proximal development? How does it

help explain how children accomplish more when they collaborate with others?

●● Why is scaffolding a particularly effective way of teaching youngsters new concepts and skills?

●● When and why do children talk to themselves as they solve problems?

4.3 Mind and Culture: Vygotsky’s Theory

Victoria, a 4-year-old, enjoys solving jigsaw puzzles, coloring, and building towers with blocks. While busy with these activities, she often talks to herself. For example, once as she was coloring a picture, she said, “Where’s the red crayon? Stay inside the lines. Color the blocks blue.” These remarks were not directed at anyone else—Victoria was alone. Why did she say these things? What purpose did they serve?

Human development is often referred to as a journey that takes people along many dif- ferent paths. For Piaget and for information-processing psychologists, children make the journey alone. Other people (and culture in general) influence the direction that children take, but fundamentally the child is a solitary adventurer-explorer, boldly forging ahead. Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934), a Russian psychologist, proposed a very different account: Development is an apprenticeship in which children advance when they collaborate with others who are more skilled. According to Vygotsky (1978), children rarely make much headway on the developmental path when they walk alone; they progress when they walk hand in hand with an expert.

For Vygotsky and other sociocultural theorists, the social nature of cognitive develop- ment is captured in the concept of intersubjectivity, which refers to mutual, shared under- standing among participants in an activity. For example, when parents and children play board games together, they share an understanding of the goals of their activity and of their roles in playing the games. Such shared understanding allows parents and children to work together in complementary fashion on the puzzles. Such interactions typify guided participation, in which cognitive growth results from children’s involvement in  structured activities with others who are more skilled than they. Through guided

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 137

participation, children learn from others how to connect new experiences and new skills with what they already know (Rogoff, 2003). Guided participation is shown when a child learns a new video game from a peer or an adolescent learns a new karate move from a partner.

Vygotsky died when he was only 37 years old, so he did not develop his theory fully. However, his ideas are influential, largely because they fill some gaps in the Piagetian and information-processing accounts. In the next few pages, we’ll look at three of Vygotsky’s most important contributions—the zone of proximal development, scaffolding, and pri- vate speech—and learn more about why Victoria talks to herself.

The Zone of Proximal Development Four-year-old Ian and his father often solve puzzles together. Although Ian does most of the work, his father encourages him, sometimes finding a piece that he needs or showing Ian how to put parts together. When Ian tries to assemble the same puzzles by himself, he rarely can complete them. The difference between what Ian can do with assistance and what he does alone defines his zone of proximal development. That is, the zone is the area between the level of performance a child can achieve when working independently and a higher level of performance that is possible when working under the guidance or direction of more skilled adults or peers (Daniels, 2011; Wertsch & Tulviste, 1992). For example, many elementary school children have difficulty solving arithmetic story problems because they don’t know where to begin. By structuring the task for them—“first decide what you’re supposed to figure out; then decide what information you’re told in the problem”— teachers can help children accomplish what they cannot do by themselves. Thus, just as training wheels help children learn to ride a bike by allowing them to concentrate on certain aspects of bicycling, collaborators help children perform more effectively by providing structure, hints, and reminders.

The idea of a zone of proximal development follows natu- rally from Vygotsky’s basic premise: Cognition develops first in a social setting and only gradually comes under the child’s inde- pendent control. What factors aid this shift? This leads us to the second of Vygotsky’s key contributions.

Scaffolding Have you had the good fortune to work with a master teacher, one who knew exactly when to say something to help you over an obstacle but otherwise let you work uninterrupted? Scaffolding is a style in which teachers gauge the amount of assistance they offer to match the learner’s needs. Early in learn- ing a new task, children know little; so teachers give much direct instruction about how to do all the different elements of a task. As the children catch on, teachers need to provide less direct instruction; they are more likely to give reminders.

Worldwide, parents scaffold their children’s learning, but not always using the same methods. In cultures that value chil- dren’s independence (e.g., many countries in North America and Europe), parents allow children to play at a distance (to encour- age their independence) and thus parents rely on language to scaffold their children. In contrast, in cultures that value chil- dren’s interdependence with other people (e.g., many countries in Latin America and Asia), parents keep children close physi- cally and rely on gesture and touch to scaffold (Kuhl & Keller, 2008; Luo & Tamis-LeMonda, 2016).

zone of proximal development Difference between what children can do with assistance and what they can do alone.

scaffolding a style in which teachers gauge the amount of assistance they offer to match the learner’s needs.

Young children can often accomplish far more with some adult guidance than they can accomplish alone; Vygotsky referred to this difference as the zone of proximal development.

Jo hn

B ird

sa ll

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

138 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

The defining characteristic of scaffolding—giving help but not more than is needed—clearly promotes learning (Cole, 2006). Youngsters do not learn readily when they are con- stantly told what to do or when they are simply left to struggle through a problem unaided. However, when teachers collabo- rate with children, allowing them to take on more and more of a task as they master its different elements, they learn more effectively (Murphy & Messer, 2000). Scaffolding is an impor- tant technique for transferring skills from others to the child, both in formal settings such as schools and in informal set- tings such as the home and playground (Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, 2010).

Private Speech Remember Victoria, the 4-year-old in the vignette who talked to herself as she colored? Her behavior demonstrates private speech: comments that are not intended for others but are designed to help children regulate their own behavior. Thus, Victoria’s remarks are sim- ply an effort to help her color the picture.

Vygotsky viewed private speech as an intermediate step toward self-regulation of cognitive skills (Fernyhough, 2010). At first, children’s behavior is regulated by speech from other people that is directed toward them. When youngsters first try to control their own behavior and thoughts without others present, they instruct themselves by speaking aloud. Private speech seems to be children’s way of guiding themselves, of mak- ing sure they do all the required steps in solving a problem. Finally, as children gain ever greater skill, private speech becomes inner speech, which was Vygotsky’s term for thought.

If private speech functions in this way, then children should use private speech more often on difficult tasks than on easy tasks because children are most likely to need extra guidance on harder tasks. Also, children should be more likely to use private speech after a mistake than after a correct response. These predictions are supported by research (Berk, 2003), findings that suggest the power of language in helping children learn to control their own behavior and thinking.

Thus, Vygotsky’s work has characterized cognitive development not as a solitary undertaking, but as a collaboration between expert and novice. His work reminds us of the importance of language, which we’ll examine in detail in the last section of this chapter.

private speech a child’s comments that are not intended for others but are designed to help regulate the child’s own behavior.

Think About It Vygotsky emphasized cognitive develop- ment as collaboration. How could such collaboration be included in Piaget’s theory? In information processing?

Young children often regulate their own behavior by talking to themselves, particu- larly while performing difficult tasks.

recall 1. The is the difference between the level of

performance that youngsters can achieve with assistance and the level they can achieve alone.

2. The term refers to a style in which teachers adjust their assistance to match a child’s needs.

3. According to Vygotsky, is an intermediate step between speech from others and inner speech.

Interpret ●● How would scaffolding appropriate for infants differ from the

scaffolding appropriate for preschool children?

Apply ●● Review Piaget’s description of the conditions that foster cog-

nitive development (on page 125). How would an analogous list derived from Vygotsky’s theory compare?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 4.3

is to

ck .c

om /F

at ih

ho ca

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 139

Nabina is just a few weeks away from her first birthday. For the past month, she has seemed to understand much of her mother’s speech. If her mom asks, “Where’s Garfield?” (the family cat), Nabina scans the room and points toward Garfield. Yet Nabina’s own speech is still gibberish: She “talks” constantly, but her mom can’t understand a word of it. If Nabina apparently understands others’ speech, why can’t she speak herself?

Soon after the first birthday, most children say their first word, which is followed in the ensuing months by hundreds more. This marks the beginning of a child’s ability to com- municate orally with others. Through speech, youngsters impart their ideas, beliefs, and feelings to family, friends, and others.

The first spoken words represent the climax of a year’s worth of language growth. To tell the story of language acquisition properly and explain Nabina’s seemingly strange behavior, we begin with the months preceding the first words.

The Road to Speech When a baby is upset, a concerned mother tries to console him. Unable to talk, the infant conveys his unhappiness by crying. The mother uses both verbal and nonverbal measures to cheer her baby. In this situation, do babies understand any of the speech directed at them? And how do infants progress from crying to more effective methods of oral com- munication, such as speech? Let’s start by answering the first question.

Perceiving Speech Even newborn infants hear remarkably well (page 103); the left hemisphere of a newborn’s brain is particularly sensitive to language (page 94); and babies prefer to listen to speech over comparably complex nonspeech sounds (Vouloumanos et al., 2010). Critically, babies can distinguish consonant sounds, such as the sound of t in toe and tap, as well as vowel sounds, such as the sound of e in get and bed (Cristia et al., 2016; Galle &McMurray, 2014).

The Impact of Language Exposure. Not all languages use the same set of language sounds, phonemes, so a distinction that is important in one language may be ignored in another. For example, Japanese does not distinguish the consonant sound of r in rip from the sound of l in lip, and Japanese adults trying to learn English have great difficulty distinguishing these sounds. At about 6 to 8 months, Japanese and American infants can dis- tinguish these sounds equally well. However, by 10 to 12 months, perception of r and l improves for American infants—presumably because they hear these sounds frequently—but declines for Japa- nese babies (Werker & Hensch, 2015).

Thus, young babies can distinguish a range of sounds but, as babies are more exposed to a particular language, they only notice the linguistic distinctions that are meaningful in that language. In other words, specializing in one language apparently comes at the cost of making it more difficult to hear sounds in other languages. And this pattern of greater specialization in speech perception resembles the profile for face perception described on page 108.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● When do infants first hear and make speech sounds?

●● When do children start to talk? How do they learn word meanings?

●● How do young children learn grammar?

●● How well do youngsters communicate?

4.4 Language

A baby’s first form of communication— crying—is soon joined by other language- based ways of communicating.

Ca tc

hl ig

ht V

is ua

l S er

vi ce

s/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

140 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

With greater exposure to human faces, babies develop a more refined notion of a human face, just as they develop a more refined notion of the sounds (and signs) that are impor- tant in their native language (Pascalis et al., 2014).

Identifying Words. Of course, identifying language sounds is just the first step in perceiv- ing speech. One of the biggest challenges for infants is identifying words. Imagine, for example, an infant overhearing this conversation between a parent and an older sibling:

SIBLING: Jerry got a new bike. PARENT: Was his old bike broken? SIBLING: No. He’d saved his money to buy a new mountain bike.

An infant listening to this conversation hears bike three times. Can the infant learn from this experience? Yes. When 7- to 8-month-olds hear a word repeatedly in different sentences, they later pay more attention to this word than to words they haven’t heard previously (Houston & Jusczyk, 2003; Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996). By 6 months, infants pay more attention to content words (e.g., nouns, verbs) than to function words (e.g., articles, prepositions), and they look at the correct parent when they hear “mommy” or “daddy” (Shi & Werker, 2001; Tincoff & Jusczyk, 1999).

In normal conversation, there are no silent gaps between words, so how do infants pick out words? Stress is one important clue. English contains many one-syllable words that are stressed and many two-syllable words that have a stressed syllable followed by an unstressed syllable (e.g., dough’-nut, tooth’-paste, bas’-ket). Infants pay more attention to stressed syllables than unstressed syllables, which is a good strategy for identifying the beginnings of words (Bortfeld & Morgan, 2010; Thiessen & Saffran, 2003). And infants learn words more readily when they appear at the beginnings and ends of sentences, prob- ably because the brief pause between sentences makes it easier to identify first and last words (Seidl & Johnson, 2006).

Another way to identify words in speech is statistical. Infants notice syllables that go together frequently (Jusczyk, 2002). For example, in many studies, 8-month-olds heard the following sounds, which consisted of four 3-syllable artificial words, said over and over in random order:

pa bi ku go la tu da ro pi ti bu do da ro pi go la tu pa bi ku da ro pi . . .

We’ve underlined the words and inserted gaps between them so that you can see them more easily, but in the studies, there were no breaks—just a steady flow of syllables for three minutes. Later, infants listened to these words less than to new words that were novel combinations of the same syllables. They had detected pa bi ku, go la tu, da ro pi, and ti bu do as familiar patterns and hence listened to them less than to new “words” such as tu da ro even though the latter were made up from syllables they’d already heard (Aslin, 2017).

Yet another way that infants identify words is through their emerging knowledge of how sounds are used in their native language. For example, s followed by t and s followed by d are quite common at the end of one word and the beginning of the next: bus takes, kiss took; this dog, pass directly. However, s and t occur frequently within a word (stop, list, pest, stink) but s and d do not. Consequently, when d follows an s, it probably starts a new word. In fact, 9-month-olds follow rules such as this one: When they hear novel words embedded in continuous speech, they’re more likely to identify the novel word when the final sound in the preceding word occurs infrequently with the first sound of the novel word (Mattys & Jusczyk, 2001).

Another strategy that infants use is to rely on familiar function words, such as the arti- cles a and the, to break up the speech stream. These words are common in adults’ speech; by 6 months, most infants recognize these words and use them to determine the onset of a new word (Shi, 2014). For example, for infants familiar with a, the sequence aballabata- glove becomes a ball a bat a glove. The new words are isolated by the familiar ones.

Thus, infants use many powerful tools to identify words in speech. Of course, they don’t yet understand the meanings of these words; at this point, they simply recognize

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 141

a word as a distinct configuration of sounds. Nevertheless, these early perceptual skills are important because infants who are more skilled at identifying words within the speech stream know more words as tod- dlers (Newman, Rowe, & Ratner, 2016).

Parents (and other adults) often help infants master language sounds by talking in a distinctive style. In infant-directed speech, adults speak slowly with exaggerated changes in pitch and loudness.1 If you listen to a mother talking to her baby, you will notice that she speaks slowly, in a  higher and more variable pitch, and with exaggerated facial expres- sions, and her sentences are brief. Infant-directed speech is common in most cultures that have been studied, although the specific features often vary with the language (Saint-Georges et al., 2013).

Babies benefit from infant-directed speech, for two reasons. First, the higher pitch, the variability in pitch and the exaggerated facial expres- sions, of infant-directed speech grab a baby’s attention. In short, babies pay more attention to the speech they’re hearing when it’s expressed in infant-directed speech. Second, infant-directed speech has features that help babies to learn language. The slow pace and variable pitch of infant- directed speech seems to help babies to segment the speech stream. In addition, infant-directed speech is beneficial in providing excellent examples of lan- guage  sounds, examples that help babies to learn different language sounds (Golinkoff et al., 2015).

Steps to Speech As any new parent can testify, newborns and young babies make many sounds—they cry, burp, and sneeze. Language-based sounds don’t appear immediately. At 2 months, infants begin to produce vowellike sounds such as “ooooooo” and “ahhhhhh,” a phenomenon known as cooing. Sometimes infants become quite excited as they coo, perhaps reflecting the joy of simply playing with sounds.

After cooing comes babbling, speechlike sound that has no meaning. A typical 6-month- old might say “dah” or “bah,” utterances that sound like a single syllable consisting of a consonant and a vowel. Over the next few months, babbling becomes more elaborate as babies apparently experiment with more complex speech sounds. Older infants some- times repeat a sound, as in “bahbahbah,” and begin to combine different sounds, such as “dahmahbah” (Hoff, 2014).

Babbling is not just mindless playing with sounds; it is a precursor to real speech. We know this, in part, from video records of people’s mouths while speaking. When an adult speaks, the mouth opens somewhat wider on the right side than on the left side, reflect- ing the left hemisphere’s control of language and muscle movements on the body’s right side (Graves & Landis, 1990). Infants do the same when they babble but not when mak- ing other nonbabbling sounds, which suggests that babbling is fundamentally linguistic (Holowka & Petitto, 2002).

Other evidence for the linguistic nature of babbling comes from studies of develop- mental change in babbling: At roughly 8 to 11 months, infants’ babbling sounds more like real speech because infants stress some syllables and vary the pitch of their speech (Snow, 2006). In English declarative sentences, for example, pitch first rises and then falls toward the end of the sentence. However, in questions the pitch is level and then rises toward the end of the question. Older babies’ babbling reflects these patterns: Babies who are brought up by English-speaking parents have both the declarative and question patterns of intona- tion in their babbling. Babies exposed to a language with different patterns of intonation, such as Japanese and French, reflect their language’s intonation in their babbling (Levitt & Utman, 1992).

1Infant-directed speech was once known as motherese because this form of speaking was first noted in mothers, but we know now that most caregivers talk this way to infants.

infant-directed speech speech that adults use with infants that is slow and has exaggerated changes in pitch and volume; it helps children master language.

cooing Early vowellike sounds that babies produce.

babbling speechlike sounds that consist of vowel– consonant combinations; common at about 6 months.

When mothers and other adults talk to young children, they often use infant- directed speech in which they speak slowly with exaggerated changes in pitch and loudness.

A nd

y Co

x/ Th

e Im

ag e

Ba nk

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

142 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

The appearance of intonation in babbling indicates a strong link between percep- tion and production of speech: Infants’ babbling is influenced by the characteristics of the speech that they hear (Goldstein & Schwade, 2008). Beginning in the middle of the first year, infants try to reproduce the sounds of language that others use in trying to communicate with them (or in the case of deaf infants with deaf parents, the signs that others use). Hear- ing dog, an infant may first say “dod” and then “gog” before finally saying “dog” correctly. In the same way that beginning typists gradually link movements of their fingers with par- ticular keys, through babbling, infants learn to use their lips, tongue, and teeth to produce specific sounds, gradually making sounds that approximate real words (Poulson et al., 1991).

These developments in production of sound, coupled with the 1-year-old’s advanced ability to perceive speech sounds, set the stage for the infant’s first true words.

First Words and Many More Recall that Nabina, the 1-year-old in the vignette, looks at the family cat when she hears its name. This phenomenon is common in 10- to 14-month-olds. They appear to understand what others say despite the fact that they have yet to speak. In response to “Where is the book?” children will find the book. They grasp the question even though their own speech is limited to advanced babbling (Fenson et al., 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1997). Evidently, chil- dren have linked speech sounds and particular objects even though they cannot yet manu- facture the sounds themselves. As fluent adult speakers, we forget that speech is a motor skill requiring perfect timing and tremendous coordination.

A few months later, most youngsters utter their first words. In many languages, those words are similar (Nelson, 1973; Tardif et al., 2008) and include terms for mother and father, greetings (hi, bye-bye), and foods and toys (juice, ball). By age 2, most youngsters have a vocabulary of a few hundred words, and by age 6, a typical child’s vocabulary includes more than 10,000 words (Bloom, 2000). Worldwide, nouns are common in chil- dren’s early vocabularies, perhaps because they refer to objects that infants can perceive easily; verbs are less common than nouns, but this difference is smaller (and sometimes disappears) in languages such as Korean and Chinese, perhaps because verbs are more prominent in these languages or because East Asian cultures emphasize actions more than objects (Waxman et al., 2013).

The Grand Insight: Words as Symbols To make the transition from babbling to real speech, infants need to learn that speech is more than just entertaining sound. They need to know that particular sounds form words that can refer to objects, actions, and properties. Put another way, infants must recog- nize that words are symbols—entities that stand for other entities. By the first birthday, children’s experiences cause them to form concepts such as “round, bouncy things” and “furry things that bark.” With the insight that speech sounds can denote these concepts, infants begin to identify a word that goes with each concept (Reich, 1986).

If this argument is correct, then we should find that children use symbols in other areas, not just in language. They do. Gestures are symbols, and infants begin to gesture shortly before their first birthday (Goodwyn & Acredolo, 1993). Young children may open and close their hands to request an object or wave “bye-bye” when leaving. Infants’ vocabularies of gestures and spoken words expand at about the same rate, consistent with the idea that words and gestures reflect the infant’s emerging understanding of symbols (Caselli et al., 2012).

What’s more, gestures sometimes pave the way for language. Before knowing an object’s name, infants often point to it or pick it up for a listener, as if saying, “I want this!” or “What’s this?” In one study, youngsters first referred to nearly 50% of objects by gesture and, about three months later, by word (Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005). Given this con- nection between early gestures and first spoken words, it’s not surprising that toddlers who are more advanced in their use of gesture tend to have, as preschoolers, more com- plex spoken language (Kuhn et al., 2014; Rowe, Raudenbush, & Goldin-Meadow, 2012).

Think About It Compare and contrast the steps in learn- ing to make speech sounds with Piaget’s account of the sensorimotor period.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 143

What’s What? Fast Mapping of Words After children know that a word can symbolize an object or action, their vocabularies grow, but slowly at first. A typical 15-month-old, for exam- ple, may learn about 10 words each month. However, between 18 and 24 months, many children experience a naming explosion during which they learn new words—particularly names of objects—much more rap- idly than before. Children now learn 25 to 30 new words each month (Samuelson & McMurray, 2017).

This rapid rate of word learning is astonishing when we realize that most words have many plausible but incorrect referents. To illustrate, imagine what’s going through the mind of a child whose mother points to a flower and says, “Flower. This is a flower. See the flower.” This all seems crystal clear to you and incredibly straightforward. But what might the child learn from this episode? Perhaps the correct referent for “flower.” But a youngster could conclude that “flower” refers to a petal, to the color of the flower, or to the mother’s actions in pointing at the flower.

Surprisingly, though, most youngsters learn the proper meanings of simple words in just a few presentations. Children’s ability to connect new words to referents so rapidly that they cannot be considering all possible meanings for the new word is termed fast mapping. How can young children learn new words so rapidly? Many distinct factors contribute (Hollich, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2000).

Joint Attention. Parents encourage word learning by carefully watching what interests their children. When toddlers touch or look at an object, parents often label it for them. When a youngster points to a banana, a parent may say, “Banana. That’s a banana.” Such labeling in the context of joint attention promotes word learning, particularly when infants and toddlers participate actively, directing their parents’ attention (Beuker et al., 2013).

Of course, to take advantage of this help, infants must be able to tell when parents are labeling instead of just conversing. In fact, when adults label an unfamiliar object, 18- to 20-month-olds assume that the label is the object’s name only when adults show signs that they are referring to the object (Liebal et al., 2009; Nurmsoo & Bloom, 2008). Young children also consider an adult’s credibility as a source: Preschoolers are less likely to learn words from adults who seem uncertain, have given incorrect names for words in the past, or speak with a foreign accent (Corriveau, Kinzler, & Harris, 2013; Jaswal & Konrad, 2016). Thus, beginning in the tod- dler years, parents and children work together to create conditions that foster word learning: Parents label objects, and youngsters rely on adults’ behavior to interpret the words they hear.

Constraints on Word Names. Joint attention simplifies word learning for children, but the problem still remains: How does a toddler know that banana refers to the object that she’s touching as opposed to her activity (touching) or to the object’s color? Young children follow several simple rules that constrain their inferences about a word’s meaning. One simple rule is that if an unfamiliar word is heard in the presence of objects that already have names and objects that don’t, the word refers to one of the objects that doesn’t have a name. Suppose a shelf includes a book, an apple, and a camera, and that a child knows the names of the first two. If an adult says, “Look at the camera,” preschoolers will infer that “camera” refers to the third, unnamed object (Au & Glasman, 1990).

Another simple but useful rule is that a name refers to a whole object, not its parts or its relation to other objects, and refers not just to this particular object, but to all objects of the same type (Hollich, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2007). For example, when a grandpar- ent points to a stuffed animal on a shelf and says “dinosaur,” children conclude that dino- saur refers to the entire dinosaur, not just its ears or nose, not to the fact that the dinosaur is on a shelf, and not to this specific dinosaur, but to all dinosaurlike objects.

Rules such as these make it possible for children like Nabina, the child in the vignette, to learn words rapidly because they reduce the number of possible referents. The child being shown a flower follows these rules to decide that flower refers to the entire object, not its parts or the action of pointing to it.

fast mapping a child’s connections between words and referents that are made so quickly that he or she cannot consider all possible meanings of the word.

One of the challenges for theories of lan- guage learning is to explain how children figure out that the parent’s words refer to the object, not to its color or texture.

iS to

ck .c

om /P

at ric

k H

ea gn

ey

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

144 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Sentence Cues. Children hear many unfamiliar words embedded in sentences containing words they already know. The other words and the overall sentence structure can be help- ful clues to a word’s meaning (Yuan & Fisher, 2009). For example, when a parent describes an event using familiar words but an unfamiliar verb, children often infer that the verb refers to the action performed by the subject of the sentence (Arunachalam et al., 2013). When youngsters hear “The man is juggling the bats,” they will infer that juggling refers to the man’s actions because they already know the actor (man) and the object of the action (bats). Similarly, toddlers know that a and the often precede nouns and that he, she, and they precede verbs. Thus, they will conclude that “a boz” refers to an object but “she boz” refers to an action (Cauvet et al., 2014).

Cognitive Factors. The naming explosion coincides with a time of rapid cognitive growth, and children’s increased cognitive skill helps them learn new words. As children’s thinking becomes more sophisticated and, in particular, as they start to have goals and intentions, language becomes a means to express those goals and to achieve them. Thus, intention provides children with an important motive to learn language—to help achieve their goals (Bloom & Tinker, 2001).

In addition, young children’s improving attentional and perceptual skills also promote word learning. Infants and young children learn that objects that have the same shape have the same name, and they learn that paying attention to shape is an easy way to learn names (Smith, 2000, 2009). And their improving memory skills forge stronger links between objects and newly discovered names (Samuelson & McMurray, 2016).

Developmental Change in Word Learning. Some of the word-learning tools described in the past few pages are particularly important at different ages (Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 2008). Before 18 months, infants learn words relatively slowly—often just one new word each day. At this age, children rely heavily on simple attentional processes (e.g., shape) to learn new words. But by 24 months, most children are learning many new words daily. This faster learning reflects children’s greater use of language cues (e.g., constraints on names) and a speaker’s social cues. At any age, infants and toddlers rely on a mixture of word-learning tools, but with age, they gradually move away from attentional cues to lan- guage and social cues.

Naming Errors. Of course, the mechanisms for learning new words are not perfect; initial mappings of words onto meanings are often only partially correct (Hoff & Naigles, 2002). A common mistake is underextension, defining a word too narrowly. Using car to refer only to the family car and ball to a favorite toy ball are examples of underextension. Between 1 and 3 years, children sometimes make the opposite error, overextension, defin- ing a word too broadly. Children may use car also to refer to buses and trucks or use doggie to refer to all four-legged animals.

Overextension errors occur more frequently when children are producing words than when they are comprehending words. Two-year-old Jason may say “doggie” to refer to a goat but nevertheless correctly point to a picture of a goat when asked. Because overexten- sion is more common in word production, it may reflect another fast-mapping rule that children follow: “If you can’t remember the name for an object, say the name of a related object” (Naigles & Gelman, 1995). Both underextension and overextension disappear gradually as youngsters refine meanings for words after increased exposure to language.

Individual Differences in Word Learning The naming explosion typically occurs at about 18 months, but like many developmental milestones, the timing of this event varies widely for individual children. Some young- sters have a naming explosion as early as 14 months, but for others, it may be as late as 22 months (Goldfield & Reznick, 1990). Another way to make this point is to look at varia- tion in the size of children’s vocabulary at a specific age. At 18 months, for example, an average child knows about 75 words, but a child in the 90th percentile would know nearly 250 words and a child in the 10th percentile fewer than 25 words (Fenson et al., 1994).

underextension when children define words more narrowly than adults do.

overextension when children define words more broadly than adults do.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 145

This range in vocabulary size for typical 18-month-olds is huge—from 25 to 250 words. What can account for this difference? Two factors contribute. One is phonological memory, the ability to remember speech sounds briefly. This is often measured by saying a nonsense word to children—“ballop” or “glistering”—and asking them to repeat it imme- diately. Children who remember such words accurately tend to have larger vocabularies (Newbury et al., 2016). Children who cannot remember speech sounds accurately find word learning particularly challenging, an outcome that’s not surprising because word learning involves associating meaning with an unfamiliar sequence of speech sounds.

However, the single most important factor in growth of vocabulary is the child’s lan- guage environment. Children have larger vocabularies when they are exposed to much high-quality language. That is, children learn more words when their parents’ speech is rich in different words and is grammatically sophisticated (Huttenlocher et al., 2010; Rowe, 2012) and when parents respond promptly and appropriately to their children’s talk (Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko, & Song, 2014).

Bilingualism. Millions of American children grow up in bilingual households; these young- sters usually speak English and another language. When children are exposed simultane- ously to two (or more) languages from birth, they pass through the same milestones in each language as monolingual children, but somewhat more slowly. For example, in each lan- guage, their vocabulary is often slightly smaller and their grammar somewhat less complex. But their total vocabulary (i.e., words known in both languages and words known in either language but not both) is greater than that of monolingual children (Hoff et al., 2012).

These patterns depend critically on the circumstances in which children experience multiple languages. Children’s language skills progress more rapidly in the language they hear the most (Hoff et al., 2012). If a child hears English from mom and at day care but hears Croatian only from dad, the child’s skills in English will probably surpass her skills in Croatian. And children’s language develops more rapidly when their language expo- sure comes from a native speaker (Place & Hoff, 2011). Language acquisition in bilingual children can also be affected by the relative prestige of the two languages as well as the cultures associated with the two languages (Hoff, 2014).

Being bilingual also has some important language and cognitive advantages. Bilingual children better understand that words are simply arbitrary symbols. Bilingual youngsters, for instance, are more likely than monolingual children to understand that as long as all English speakers agreed, dog could refer to cats and cat could refer to dogs (Bialystok, 1988; Campbell & Sais, 1995). And they are more skilled at switching back and forth between tasks and often are better able to inhibit inappropriate responses (Barac & Bialystok, 2012; Bialystok, 2010), perhaps reflecting their experience of switching between languages and inhibiting relevant words from the “other” language (e.g., when shown a photo of a dog and asked “What’s this?” preschoolers bilingual in French and English must respond “dog” while suppressing “chien”).

Word Learning Styles. As vocabularies grow, some youngsters adopt a distinctive style of learning language (Bates, Bretherton, & Snyder, 1988; Nelson, 1973). Some children have a referential style; their vocabularies mainly consist of words that name objects, persons, or actions. For example, Rachel, a referential child, has 41 name words in her 50-word vocab- ulary, but only two words for social interaction or questions. Other children have an expressive style; their vocabularies include some names but also many social phrases that are used like a single word, such as “go away,” “what’d you want?” and “I want it.” Elizabeth, an expressive child, has a more balanced vocabulary, with 14 words for social interactions and questions and 24 name words.

Referential and expressive styles represent end points on a continuum; most children are somewhere in between. For children with referential emphasis, language is primar- ily an intellectual tool: a means of learning and talking about objects (Masur, 1995). In contrast, for children with expressive emphasis, language is more of a social tool: a way of enhancing interactions with others. Of course, both of these functions—intellectual and social—are important functions of language, which explains why most children blend the referential and expressive styles of learning language.

phonological memory ability to remember speech sounds briefly; an important skill in acquiring vocabulary.

referential style language-learning style of children whose vocabularies are dominated by names of objects, persons, or actions.

expressive style language-learning style of children whose vocabularies include many social phrases that are used like one word.

Think About It Gavin and Heath are both 16-month- olds. Gavin’s vocabulary includes about 14 words, but Heath’s has about 150 words, more than 10 times as many as Gavin. What factors contribute to this difference?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

146 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Encouraging Language Growth For children to expand their vocabularies, they need to hear others speak. Not sur- prisingly, children have a larger vocabu- lary when parents’ speech includes more words and more varied words (Newman et al., 2016). Parents can foster word learn- ing by naming objects that are the focus of a child’s attention, such as the products in a grocery or objects seen on a walk ( Dunham, Dunham, & Curwin, 1993). Reading books with children also helps, particularly when parents describe pictures carefully, ask chil- dren questions while reading, talk about the meanings of words that appear, or reread stories (Reese & Cox, 1999; Wasik, Hindman, & Snell, 2016).

Of course, video is an essential part of the lives of young children. Does it help them to learn new words? Sometimes. On the one hand, watching Sesame Street regu- larly promotes word learning (Wright et al.,

2001) as do programs that tell a story (e.g., Thomas the Tank Engine) and those that ask questions of the viewer (e.g., Blue’s Clues and Dora the Explorer). On the other hand, most cartoons have no benefit for language learning (Linebarger & Vaala, 2010). And videos claiming to promote word learning in infants (e.g., Baby Einstein, Brainy Baby) typically are not effective, often because 12- to 18-month-olds have difficulty relating what they see in the video to their own experiences (DeLoache et al., 2010; Linebarger & Vaala, 2010).

Of course, today video is available on demand via touchscreen tablets and smart- phones. Many infants and toddlers have daily access to these devices, whether in a car en route to day care, waiting in a dentist’s office, or sitting in a shopping cart at Walmart (Barr  & Linebarger, 2017). Because this technology is relatively new, scientists are still studying its impact on young children’s word learning. However, the emerging evidence suggests that apps for new technology are most likely to be effective when they require children to actively engage with meaningful content and when they use apps with parents who can support children’s learning (Zosh et al., 2017).

The research we’ve described in this section points to a simple but powerful conclu- sion: Children are most likely to learn new words when they participate in activities that force them to understand the meanings of new words and use those new words.

Speaking in Sentences: Grammatical Development Within months after saying the first words, children begin to form simple two-word sen- tences. In their two-word speech, children follow rules to express different meanings. For example, the sentences truck go and Daddy eat both follow the rule is “agent + action.” In contrast, my truck follows the rule “possessor + possession.” Regardless of the language they learn, children’s two-word sentences follow a common set of rules that are useful to describe people and objects, their actions, and their properties (Tager-Flusberg, 1993).

From Two Words to Complex Sentences Children rapidly move beyond two-word sentences, first by linking two-word state- ments together: “Rachel kick” and “Kick ball” become “Rachel kick ball.” Even longer sen- tences soon follow; sentences with 10 or more words are common in 3-year-olds’ speech. For example, at 1½ years, Laura Kail would say “Gimme juice” or “Bye-bye Ben.” As a

Parents are more effective than videos in teaching new words to their children.

Co ur

te sy

o f R

ob er

t V . K

ai l

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 147

2½-year-old, she had progressed to “When I finish my ice cream, I’ll take a shower, okay?” and “Don’t turn the light out—I can’t see better!”

Children’s two- and three-word sentences often fall short of adults’ standards of gram- mar. Youngsters will say “He eating” rather than “He is eating” or “two cat” rather than “two cats.” This sort of speech is called telegraphic because, like telegrams of days gone by, children’s speech includes only words directly relevant to meaning and nothing more. Before texts and tweets, people sent urgent messages by telegraph, and the cost was based on the number of words. Consequently, telegrams were brief and to the point, containing only important nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs—much like children’s two-word speech. The missing elements, grammatical morphemes, are words or endings of words (such as -ing, -ed, and -s) that make a sentence grammatical. During the preschool years, children gradually acquire the grammatical morphemes, first mastering those that express simple relations, such as -ing, which is used to denote that the action expressed by the verb is ongoing. More complex forms, such as appropriate use of the various forms of the verb to be, are mastered later (Peters, 1995).

Children’s use of grammatical morphemes is based on their growing knowledge of grammatical rules, not simply memory for individual words. This was first demonstrated in a landmark study by Berko (1958) in which preschoolers were shown pictures of non- sense objects like the one in ◗ Figure 4.6. The experimenter labeled it, saying, “This is a wug.” Then youngsters were shown pictures of two of the objects, and the experimenter said, “These are two.” Most children spontaneously said, “wugs.” Because both the singular and plural forms of this word were novel for these youngsters, they could have generated the correct plural form only by applying the familiar rule of adding -s.

Children growing up in homes where English is spoken face the problem that their native language is irregular, with many exceptions to the rules. Sometimes children apply rules to words that are exceptions to the rule, errors called overregularizations. With plu- rals, for example, youngsters may incorrectly add -s instead of using an irregular plural— two “mans” instead of two “men.” With the past tense, children may add -ed instead of using an irregular past tense: “I goed home” instead of “I went home” (Marcus et al., 1992; Mervis & Johnson, 1991).

These examples give some insight into the complexities of mastering the grammati- cal rules of one’s language. Children not only must learn an extensive set of specific rules they also must absorb—on a case-by-case basis—all the exceptions. Despite the enormity of this task, most children have mastered the basics of their native tongue by the time they enter school. How do they do it? Biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces all contribute.

How Do Children Acquire Grammar? Most youngsters can neither read nor do arithmetic when they enter kindergarten, but virtually all have mastered the fundamentals of grammar of their native tongue. How do they do it? Theorists have proposed several different answers to this question.

The Behaviorist Answer. B. F. Skinner (1957) and other learning theorists once claimed that all aspects of language—sounds, words, grammar, and communication— are learned through imitation and reinforcement (Moerk, 2000; Whitehurst & Vasta, 1975). But critics were quick to point to flaws in this explanation. One problem is that most of children’s sentences are novel, an outcome difficult to explain in terms of simple imitation of adults’ speech. For example, when young children create questions by inserting a wh word at the beginning of a sentence (“What she doing?”), who are they imitating? Also troublesome is that even when children imitate adult sentences, they do not imitate adult grammar. In simply trying to repeat “I am drawing a picture,” young children say “I draw picture.”

The Linguistic Answer. Many scientists believe that children are born with mechanisms that simplify the task of learning grammar (Slobin, 1985). According to this view, chil- dren are born with neural circuits in the brain that allow them to infer the grammar

telegraphic speech speech used by young children that con- tains only the words necessary to convey a message.

grammatical morphemes words or endings of words that make a sentence grammatical.

overregularization grammatical usage that results from apply- ing rules to words that are exceptions to the rule.

●◗ Figure 4.6 When shown the two birds, young chil- dren usually refer to them as two “wugs,” spontaneously adding an s to “wug” to make it plural.

This is a wug.

Now there is another one. There are two of them. There are two ____________.

Berko, 1958.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

148 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

of the language that they hear. That is, grammar itself is not built into the child’s nervous system, but processes that guide the learning of grammar are. Many findings indirectly support this view.

1. If children are born with a “grammar-learning processor,” then specific regions of the brain should be involved in learning grammar. As we discussed on page 92, the left hemisphere of the brain plays a critical role in understanding language and language-specific areas in the left hemisphere are evident in infancy (Dehaene- Lambertz & Spelke, 2015).

2. If learning grammar depends on specialized neural mechanisms that are unique to humans, then efforts to teach grammar to nonhumans should fail. This prediction has been tested by trying to teach grammar to chimpanzees, the species closest to humans on the evolutionary ladder. The result: Chimps master a handful of gram- matical rules governing two-word speech, but only with massive effort that is com- pletely unlike the preschool child’s learning of grammar (Hoff, 2014).

3. The period from birth through adolescence is a critical period for acquiring language generally and mastering grammar particularly. If children do not acquire language in this period, they never truly master language later (DeKeyser, Alfi-Shabtay, & Ravid, 2010).

Although these findings are consistent with the idea that children have innate grammar- learning mechanisms, they do not prove the existence of such mechanisms. Consequently, scientists have looked for other explanations.

The Cognitive Answer. Some theorists believe that children learn grammar through powerful cognitive skills that help them rapidly detect regularities in their environment, including patterns in the speech they hear. According to this approach, it’s as if children establish a huge spreadsheet that has the speech they’ve heard in one column and the context in which they heard it in a second column; periodically infants scan the columns looking for recurring patterns (Maratsos, 1998). For example, children might be confused the first time they hear -s added to the end of a familiar noun. However, as the database expands to include many instances of familiar nouns with an added -s, children discover that -s is always added to a noun when there are multiple instances of an object. Thus, they create the rule: plural = noun + -s. With this view, children learn language by using power- ful analytical tools to detect regularities across many examples that are stored in memory, not through an inborn grammar-learning device (Bannard & Matthews, 2008). Scientists who subscribe to this view argue that infants’ impressive ability to extract regularities in the speech sounds that they hear (described on page 140) would work just as effectively to extract regularities in sentence structure (Kidd, 2012).

The Social-Interaction Answer. This approach is eclectic, drawing on each of the views we’ve considered. From the behaviorist approach, that an environment filled with lan- guage is essential; from the linguistic approach, that language learning is distinct; and from the cognitive view, that children have powerful cognitive skills they can use to master language. The unique contribution of this perspective is emphasizing that much language learning takes place in the context of interactions between children and adults, with both parties eager for better communication (Bloom & Tinker, 2001). Children have an ever- expanding repertoire of ideas and intentions that they want to convey to others, and caring adults want to understand their children; so both parties work to improve language skills as a means toward better communication. Thus, improved communication provides an incentive for children to master language and for adults to help them.

None of these accounts provides a comprehensive explanation of how grammar is mastered. But many scientists believe the final explanation will include contribu- tions from the linguistic, cognitive, and social-interaction accounts. That is, chil- dren’s learning of grammar will be explained in terms of some mechanisms specific to learning grammar, children actively seeking to identify regularities in their envi- ronment, and linguistically rich interactions taking place between children and adults ( MacWhinney, 1998).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 149

Communicating with Others When preschoolers argue, they often talk at the same time, their remarks are rambling and incoherent, and neither both- ers to listen to the other. For effective oral communication children need to follow a few simple guidelines:

●● People should take turns, alternating as speaker and listener.

●● A speaker’s remarks should be clear from the listener’s perspective.

●● A listener should pay attention and let speakers know when their remarks don’t make sense.

Complete mastery of these elements is a lifelong pursuit. After all, even adults often miscommunicate with one another, violat- ing each of these prescriptions in the process. However, young- sters grasp many of the basics of communication early in life.

Taking Turns Many parents begin to encourage turn-taking long before infants have said their first words (Field & Widmayer, 1982):

PARENT: Can you see the bird?

INFANT: (cooing) ooooh.

PARENT: It is a pretty bird.

INFANT: ooooh.

PARENT: You’re right. It’s a cardinal.

Soon after 1-year-olds begin to speak, parents encourage their youngsters to participate in conversational turn-taking. To help their children along, parents often carry both sides of the conversation to show how the roles of speaker and listener are alternated (Hoff, 2014):

PARENT: (initiating conversation) What’s Kendra eating?

PARENT: (illustrating reply for child) She’s eating a cookie.

Help of this sort is needed less often by age 2, when spontaneous turn-taking is common in conversations between youngsters and adults (Barton & Tomasello, 1991). By 3 years of age, children have progressed to the point that they can anticipate when speakers are about to finish their turn (Keitel & Daum, 2015) and if a listener fails to reply promptly, the child often repeats his or her remarks to elicit a response and keep the conversation moving (Garvey & Berninger, 1981).

Speaking Effectively When do children first try to initiate communications with others? In fact, what appear to be the first deliberate attempts to communicate typically emerge at 10 months (Golinkoff, 1993). Infants at this age may touch or point to an object while simultaneously looking at another per- son. They continue this behavior until the person acknowledges them. It’s as if the child is saying, “This is a neat toy! I want you to see it, too.”

Beginning at 10 months, an infant may point, touch, or make noises to get an adult to do something. An infant in a playpen who wants a toy that is out of reach may make noises while pointing to the toy. The noises capture an adult’s attention, and the pointing indicates what the baby wants (Tomasello, Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007). The

Arguments can often be traced to people’s failure to follow the fundamental conversational rules of taking turns, speaking clearly, and listening carefully.

Su sa

n Jo

hn s/

Sc ie

nc e

So ur

ce

In early parent–child “conversations,” par- ents usually carry both sides of the conver- sation, alternating as speaker and listener.

To ny

F re

em an

/P ho

to Ed

it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

150 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

communication may be primitive by adult standards, but it works for babies! And mothers typically translate their baby’s pointing into words so that gesture paves the wave for learn- ing words (Goldin-Meadow, Mylander, & Franklin, 2007).

After the first birthday, children begin to use speech to communicate and often initi- ate conversations with adults (Bloom et al., 1996). Toddlers’ first conversations are about themselves, but their conversational scope expands rapidly to include objects in the envi- ronment (e.g., toys, food). Later, conversations begin to include more abstract notions, such as hypothetical objects and past or future events (Foster, 1986).

Of course, young children are not always skilled conversational partners. At times, their communications are confusing, leaving a listener to wonder “What was that all about?” Every message—whether an informal conversation or a formal lecture—should have a clear meaning. But saying something clearly is often difficult because clarity can only be judged by considering the listener’s age, experience, and knowledge of the topic, along with the context of the conversation. For example, think about the simple request, “Please hand me the Phillips screwdriver.” This message may be clear to older listeners who are familiar with variants of screwdrivers, but it is vague to younger lis- teners, to whom all screwdrivers are alike. Of course, if the toolbox is filled with Phil- lips screwdrivers of assorted sizes, the message is ambiguous even to a knowledgeable listener.

Consistently constructing clear messages is a fine art, which we would hardly expect young children to have mastered. However, by the preschool years youngsters attempt to calibrate messages, adjusting them to match the listener and the context. For example, preschool children give more elaborate messages to listeners who lack access to critical information than to listeners who have this information (Nadig & Sedivy, 2002; O’Neill, 1996). A child describing where to find a toy will give more detailed directions to a listener whose eyes were covered when the toy was hidden. And if a listener wants to complete a task without help, 5-year-olds sometimes provide helpful information but conceal their intent to help so that the listener thinks she has completed the task without assistance (Grosse, Scott-Phillips, & Tomasello, 2013).

Listening Well To listen well, a person must continuously decide whether a speaker’s remarks make sense. If they do, then a listener needs to reply appropriately, typically by extending the conver- sation with another remark that’s on the topic. Otherwise, the listener needs to provide feedback that the speaker was confusing (e.g., “I don’t get what you mean”).

Few toddlers master these fundamental conversation skills. Their replies are more likely to be unrelated to the topic than related to it (Bloom, Rocissano, & Hood, 1976). Asked “Where’s the sock?” a 1½-year-old may say something like “I’m hungry!” By 3 years, children are more adept at continuing conversations by making remarks that relate to the topic being discussed.

By 4 years of age, children sometimes realize that a message is vague or confus- ing (Nilsen & Graham, 2012), but they often don’t ask speakers to clarify their intent. Instead, young listeners often assume that they know which toy the speaker had in mind (Beal & Belgrad, 1990). Because young children’s remarks are often ambiguous and because, as listeners, they often do not detect ambiguities, young children often miscommunicate. Throughout the elementary school years, youngsters gradually mas- ter the many skills involved in determining whether a message is consistent and clear (Ackerman, 1993).

Improvement in communication skill is yet another astonishing accomplishment in language during the first five years of life; changes are summarized in ◗ Table 4.2. By the time children are ready to enter kindergarten, they use language with remarkable profi- ciency and are able to communicate with growing skill.

Think About It Compare Piaget’s theory, Vygotsky’s theory, and the information-processing approach in their emphasis on the role of language in cognitive development.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

recall 1. Infants’ mastery of language sounds is fostered by

_______________ , in which adults speak slowly and exagger- ate changes in pitch and loudness.

2. Older infants’ babbling often includes _______________ , a pat- tern of rising and falling pitch that distinguishes statements from questions.

3. Youngsters with a(n) _______________ style have early vocab- ularies dominated by words that are names and use lan- guage primarily as an intellectual tool.

4. In _______________ , a young child’s meaning of a word is broader than an adult’s meaning.

5. Answers to the question “How do children acquire grammar?” include linguistic, cognitive, and _______________ influences.

6. When talking to listeners who lack critical information, pre- schoolers _______________ .

Interpret ●● How do the various explanations of grammatical develop-

ment differ in their view of the child’s role in mastering grammar?

Apply ●● According to Piaget’s theory, preschoolers are egocentric.

How should this egocentrism influence their ability to commu- nicate? Are the findings we have described on children’s com- munication skills consistent with Piaget’s view?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 4.4

Table 4.2

Major Milestones of Language Development Age milestones Birth to 1 year Babies hear language sounds from birth. They begin to coo between 2 and 4 months, then begin to babble

at about 6 months.

About the first birthday Babies begin to talk and to gesture, showing they have begun to use symbols.

1–3 years Vocabulary expands rapidly (due to fast mapping), particularly at about 18 months. Two-word sentences emerge in telegraphic speech at about 18 months, and more complex sentences are evident by 3 years. Turn-taking is evident in communication by 2 years.

3–5 years Vocabulary continues to expand; grammatical morphemes are added; and children begin to adjust their speech to listeners, but as listeners, they often ignore problems in messages they receive.

SUMMARY

4.1 The Onset of Thinking: Piaget’s Account

According to Piaget, how do schemes, assimilation, and accommodation provide the foundation for cognitive devel- opment throughout the life span?

●● In Piaget’s view, children construct their own understand- ing of the world by creating schemes, categories of related events, objects, and knowledge. Infants’ schemes are based on actions, but older children’s and adolescents’ schemes are based on functional, conceptual, and abstract properties.

●● Schemes change constantly. In assimilation, experiences are readily incorporated into existing schemes. In accommoda- tion, experiences cause schemes to be modified.

●● When accommodation becomes much more common than assimilation, this signals that schemes are inadequate; so

children reorganize them. This reorganization produces four different phases of mental development from infancy through adulthood.

how does thinking become more advanced as infants prog- ress through the sensorimotor stage?

●● The first two years of life constitute Piaget’s sensorimotor period. Over these two years, infants begin to adapt to and explore their environment, understand objects, and learn to use symbols.

What are the distinguishing characteristics of thinking during the preoperational stage?

●● From 2 to 7 years of age, children are in Piaget’s preopera- tional period. Although now capable of using symbols, their

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 151

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

152 PA rT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

thinking is limited by egocentrism—the inability to see the world from another’s point of view. Preoperational children are also centered in their thinking and sometimes confuse appearance with reality.

What are the strengths and weaknesses of Piaget’s theory?

●● One important contribution of Piaget’s theory is the view that children actively try to understand their world. Another contribution is specifying conditions that foster cognitive development.

●● However, the theory has been criticized because it under- estimates infants’ and preschoolers’ competence, is vague regarding processes of change, does not account for vari- ability in performance, and undervalues the influence of the sociocultural environment.

how have contemporary researchers extended Piaget’s theory?

●● In contrast to Piaget’s idea that children create a com- prehensive theory that integrates all their knowledge, the modern view is that children are specialists who generate naïve theories in particular domains, including physics and biology. Infants understand many properties of objects; they know how objects move, what happens when objects collide, and that objects fall when not supported.

●● Infants understand the difference between animate and inan- imate objects. As preschoolers, children know that, unlike inanimate objects, animate objects move themselves, grow, become ill, and repair through healing. Children’s thinking of living things includes teleological explanations (living things exist for a purpose) and essentialism (living things have unseen properties that give them their identities).

4.2 In formation Processing During Infancy and Early Childhood

What is the basis of the information-processing approach?

●● According to the information-processing view, cognitive development involves changes in mental hardware and in mental software.

how well do young children pay attention?

●● Infants use habituation to filter unimportant stimuli. Com- pared with older children, preschoolers are less able to pay attention to task-relevant information. Their attention can be improved by, for example, encouraging their pretend play.

What kinds of learning take place during infancy?

●● Infants are capable of many forms of learning, including classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and imitation.

Do infants and preschool children remember?

●● Infants can remember and can be reminded of events they seem to have forgotten. Memory improves during infancy, reflecting growth of the brain. Autobiographical memory

emerges in the preschool years, reflecting children’s grow- ing language skills and their sense of self.

●● Preschoolers sometimes testify in cases of child abuse. When questioned repeatedly, they often have difficulty distinguish- ing what they experienced from what others may suggest they experienced. Inaccuracies of this sort can be minimized by following guidelines when interviewing children, such as interviewing them as soon as possible after the event.

What do infants know about numbers?

●● Infants are able to distinguish small quantities, such as “twoness” from “threeness.” By 3 years of age, children can count small sets of objects and in so doing adhere to the one-to-one, stable-order, and cardinality principles.

4.3 Mind and Culture: Vygotsky’s Theory

What is the zone of proximal development? how does it help explain how children accomplish more when they collaborate with others?

●● Vygotsky believed that cognition develops first in a social setting and only gradually comes under the child’s indepen- dent control. The difference between what children can do with assistance and what they can do alone constitutes the zone of proximal development.

Why is scaffolding a particularly effective way of teaching youngsters new concepts and skills?

●● Control of cognitive skills is most readily transferred to the child through scaffolding, a teaching style in which teachers let children take on more and more of a task as they master its different components. Scaffolding is common world- wide, but the specific techniques for scaffolding children’s learning vary from one culture to the next.

When and why do children talk to themselves as they solve problems?

●● Children often talk to themselves, particularly when the task is difficult or when they have made a mistake. Such private speech helps children regulate their behavior, and it represents an intermediate step in the transfer of control of thinking from others to the self.

4.4 Language

When do infants first hear and make speech sounds?

●● Infants can hear language sounds soon after birth. They can even hear language sounds that are not used in their native language, but this ability diminishes as infants near the first birthday.

●● Infant-directed speech is adults’ speech to infants that is slower and has greater variation in pitch and loudness. Infants prefer infant-directed speech, perhaps because it gives them additional language clues.

●● Newborns’ communication is limited to crying, but babies coo at about 3 months of age. Babbling soon follows,

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T E r 4 : T H E E M E R G E N C E O F T H O U G H T A N D L A N G U A G E 153

consisting of a single syllable; over several months, infants’ babbling comes to include more syllables as well as intonation.

When do children start to talk? how do they learn word meanings?

●● After a brief period in which children appear to understand others’ speech but do not speak themselves, most infants begin to speak around the first birthday. The first use of words is triggered by the realization that words are sym- bols. Soon after, the child’s vocabulary expands rapidly.

●● Most children learn the meanings of words much too rap- idly for them to consider all plausible meanings systemati- cally. Instead, children use certain rules to determine the probable meanings of new words. The rules do not always yield the correct meaning. An underextension is a child’s meaning that is narrower than an adult’s meaning; an over- extension is a child’s meaning that is broader.

●● Individual children differ in vocabulary size, differences that are due to phonological memory and the quality of the child’s language environment. Bilingual children learn language readily and better understand the arbitrary nature of words. Some youngsters use a referential word-learning style that emphasizes words as names and that views lan- guage as an intellectual tool. Other children use an expres- sive style that emphasizes phrases and views language as a social tool.

●● Children’s vocabulary is stimulated by experience. Parents can foster the growth of vocabulary by speaking with chil- dren and reading to them. Video helps preschoolers learn new words but is ineffective with infants.

how do young children learn grammar?

●● Soon after children begin to speak, they create two-word sentences that are derived from their own experiences. Moving from two-word to more complex sentences involves adding grammatical morphemes. Children first master grammatical morphemes that express simple rela- tions and later those that denote complex relations. Mas- tery of grammatical morphemes involves learning not only rules but also exceptions to the rules.

●● Behaviorists proposed that children acquire grammar through imitation, but that explanation is incorrect. Today’s explanations come from three perspectives: The linguistic perspective emphasizes inborn mechanisms that allow chil- dren to infer the grammatical rules of their native language, the cognitive perspective emphasizes cognitive processes that allow children to find recurring patterns in the speech they hear, and the social-interaction perspective empha- sizes social interactions with adults in which both parties want improved communication.

how well do young children communicate?

●● Parents encourage turn-taking even before infants begin to talk, and later, they demonstrate both the speaker and listener roles for their children. By 3 years of age, children spontaneously take turns and prompt one another to take their turn.

●● Preschool children adjust their speech in a rudimentary fashion to fit the listener’s needs. However, preschoolers are unlikely to identify ambiguities in another person’s speech; instead, they are likely to assume that they know what the speaker meant.

4.1 1. accommodation 2. schemes 3. “out of sight, out of mind” 4. to use symbols 5. egocentric 6. animism 7. infants and young children 8. grow 4.2 1. pretend 2. let her view a moving mobile 3. monitoring the sources of their memories 4. cardinality 4.3 1. zone of proximal development 2. scaffolding 3. private speech 4.4 1. infant-directed speech 2. intonation 3. referential 4. overextension 5. social-interaction 6. provide more elaborate messages

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

schemes (118) assimilation (119) accommodation (119) equilibration (119) sensorimotor period (120) object permanence (121) egocentrism (122) animism (123) centration (123) core knowledge hypothesis (126) teleological explanations (128) essentialism (128) mental hardware (130) mental software (130)

attention (131) orienting response (131) habituation (131) classical conditioning (131) operant conditioning (132) autobiographical memory (133) one-to-one principle (135) stable-order principle (135) cardinality principle (135) intersubjectivity (136) guided participation (136) zone of proximal development (137) scaffolding (137) private speech (138)

infant-directed speech (141) cooing (141) babbling (141) fast mapping (143) underextension (144) overextension (144) phonological memory (145) referential style (145) expressive style (145) telegraphic speech (147) grammatical morphemes (147) overregularization (147)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Bl en

d Im

ag es

/A rie

l S ke

lle y/

G et

ty Im

ag es

R F

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

155

5Entering the Social WorldSocioemotional Development in Infancy and Early Childhood

Humans enjoy one another’s company. Social relationships of all sorts—friends, lovers, spouses, parents and children, coworkers, and team-

mates—make our lives both interesting and satisfying.

In this chapter, we trace the origins of these social

relationships. We begin with the first social relation-

ship—between an infant and a parent. You will see

how this relationship emerges over the first year and

how it is affected by the separation that comes when

parents work full-time. Interactions with parents

and others are often full of emotions—happiness,

satisfaction, anger, and guilt, to name just a few. In

the second section, you’ll see how children express

different emotions and how they recognize others’

emotions.

In the third section, you’ll learn how children’s

social horizons expand beyond parents to include peers. Then you’ll discover

how children play and how they help others in distress.

As children’s interactions with others become more wide-ranging, they

begin to learn about the social roles they are expected to play. Among the

first social roles children learn are those associated with gender—how society

expects boys and girls to behave. We’ll explore children’s awareness of gender

roles in the last section of the chapter.

5.1 Beginnings: Trust and Attachment 156 Erikson’s Stages of Early Psychosocial

Development 156 The Growth of Attachment 157 What Determines Quality of Attachment? 160 Attachment, Work, and Alternative Caregiving 161 REAL PEOPLE APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT:

Lois, Bill, and Sarah 162

5.2 Emerging Emotions 163 The Function of Emotions 163 Experiencing and Expressing Emotions 164 Recognizing and Using Others’ Emotions 166 Regulating Emotions 167

5.3 Interacting with Others 168 The Joys of Play 169 Helping Others 172

5.4 Gender Roles and Gender Identity 176 Images of Men and Women: Facts and Fantasy 177 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Reasoning About Gender-

Related Properties 178 Gender Typing 180 Evolving Gender Roles 184

Summary 185

Key Terms 186

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

156 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Kendra’s son Roosevelt is a happy, affectionate 18-month-old. Kendra so enjoys spending time with him that she is avoiding an important decision. She wants to return to her job as a loan officer at the local bank. Kendra knows a woman in the neighborhood who has cared for some of her friends’ children, and they all think she is a fantastic babysitter. But Kendra still has a nagging feeling that going back to work isn’t a “motherly” thing to do—that being away during the day may hamper Roosevelt’s development.

The socioemotional relationship that develops between an infant and a parent (usually, but not necessarily, the mother) is special. This is a baby’s first relationship, and scientists and parents believe that it should be satisfying and trouble-free to set the stage for later relationships. In this section, we’ll look at the steps involved in creating the baby’s first emotional relationship. Along the way, you’ll see how this relationship is affected by the separation that sometimes comes when a parent like Kendra works full-time.

Erikson’s Stages of Early Psychosocial Development Some of our keenest insights into psychosocial development come from a theory proposed by Erik Erikson (1982). We first encountered Erikson’s theory in Chapter 1; he describes development as a series of eight stages, each with a unique crisis for psychosocial growth. When a crisis is resolved successfully, an area of psychosocial strength is established. When the crisis is not resolved, that aspect of psychosocial development is stunted, often limit- ing the individual’s ability to resolve future crises.

In Erikson’s theory, infancy and the preschool years are represented by three stages, shown in ◗ Table 5.1. Let’s take a closer look at each stage.

Basic Trust Versus Mistrust Erikson argues that trust in oneself and others is the foundation of human development. Newborns leave the warmth and security of the uterus for an unfamiliar world. When parents respond to their infant’s needs consistently, the infant comes to trust and feel secure in the world. Of course, the world is not always pleasant and is sometimes danger- ous. Parents may not always reach a falling baby in time, or they may accidentally feed an infant food that is too hot. Erikson sees value in these experiences because infants learn mistrust. With a proper balance of trust and mistrust, infants can acquire hope, an open- ness to new experience tempered by wariness that discomfort or danger may arise.

hope according to Erikson, openness to new experience tempered by wariness that occurs when trust and mistrust are in balance.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are Erikson’s first three stages of psychosocial

development?

●● How do infants become emotionally attached to mother, father, and other significant people in their lives?

●● What are the different kinds of attachment relationships, how do they arise, and what are their consequences?

●● Is attachment jeopardized when parents of infants and young children are employed outside the home?

5.1 Beginnings: Trust and Attachment

Table 5.1

Erikson’s First Three Stages Age Crisis strength Infancy Basic trust vs. mistrust Hope

1–3 years Autonomy vs. shame and doubt Will

3–5 years Initiative vs. guilt Purpose

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 157

Autonomy Versus Shame and Doubt Between 1 and 3 years of age, children gradually learn that they can control their own actions. With this understanding, children strive for autonomy, for independence from others. However, autonomy is counteracted by doubt that the child can handle demanding situations and by shame that may result from failure. A blend of autonomy, shame, and doubt gives rise to will, the knowledge that within limits, youngsters can act on their world intentionally.

Initiative Versus Guilt Most 3- and 4-year-olds take some responsibility for themselves (e.g., by dressing them- selves). Youngsters also begin to identify with adults and their parents; they begin to under- stand the opportunities that are available in their culture. Play begins to have purpose as children explore adult roles such as mother, father, teacher, athlete, or writer. Youngsters start to explore the environment on their own, ask numerous questions about the world, and imagine possibilities for themselves.

This initiative is moderated by guilt as children realize that their initiative may place them in conflict with others; they cannot pursue their ambitions with abandon. purpose is achieved with a balance between individual initiative and a willingness to cooperate with others.

Erikson’s theory is valuable in tying together psychosocial developments across the entire life span. We will return to the remaining stages in later chapters. For now, let’s con- centrate on the first of Erikson’s crises—the establishment of trust in the world—and see how infants form an emotional bond with their parents.

The Growth of Attachment In explaining the essential ingredients of these early social relationships, most modern accounts take an evolutionary perspective. According to evolutionary psychology, many human behaviors represent successful adaptation to the environment. That is, over human history, some behaviors have made it more likely that people will pass on their genes to following generations. For example, we take it for granted that most people enjoy being with other people. But evolutionary psychologists argue that our “social nature” is a prod- uct of evolution: For early humans, being part of a group offered protection from preda- tors and made it easier to locate food. Thus, early humans who were social were more likely than their asocial peers to live long enough to reproduce, passing on their social orientation to their offspring (Kurzban, Burton-Chellew, & West, 2015). Over many, many generations, “being social” had such a survival advantage that nearly all people are socially oriented (although in various amounts, as we know from the research on temperament discussed in Chapter 3).

Applied to child development, evolutionary psychology highlights the adaptive value of children’s behavior at different points in development (Bjorklund & Jordan, 2013). For example, think about the time and energy parents invest in child rearing. Without such effort, infants and young children would die before they were sexually mature, which means that a parent’s genes could not be passed along to grandchildren (Geary, 2002). Here, too, parenting just seems “natural,” but it really represents an adaptation to the problem of guaranteeing that one’s helpless offspring can survive until they’re sexually mature.

Steps Toward Attachment An evolutionary perspective of early human relationships comes from John Bowlby (1969, 1991). According to Bowlby, children who form an attachment to an adult—that is, an enduring socioemotional relationship—are more likely to survive. This person is usually the mother but need not be; the key is a strong emotional relationship with a responsive,

will according to Erikson, a young child’s understanding that he or she can act on the world intentionally; this occurs when autonomy, shame, and doubt are in balance.

purpose according to Erikson, balance between individual initiative and the willingness to cooperate with others.

evolutionary psychology theoretical view that many human behav- iors represent successful adaptations to the environment.

attachment Enduring socioemotional relationship between infants and their caregivers.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

158 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

caring person. Attachments can form with fathers, grandparents, or someone else. Bowlby described four phases in the growth of attachment:

●● Preattachment (birth to 6–8 weeks). During prenatal development and soon after birth, infants rapidly learn to recognize their mothers by smell and sound, which sets the stage for forging an attachment relationship (Hofer, 2006). What’s more, evolution has endowed infants with many behaviors that elicit caregiving from adults. When babies cry, smile, or gaze intently at a par- ent, the parent usually smiles back or holds the baby. The infant’s behaviors and the responses they evoke in adults create an interactive system that is the first step in forming attachment relationships.

●● Attachment in the making (6–8 weeks to 6–8 months). During these months, babies begin to behave differently in the presence of familiar caregivers and unfamiliar adults. Babies smile and laugh more often with the primary care- giver. And when babies are upset, they’re more easily consoled by the primary caregiver. Babies are gradually identifying the primary caregiver as the person they can depend on when they’re anxious or distressed.

●● True attachment (6–8 months to 18 months). By approximately 7 or 8 months, most infants have singled out the attachment figure—usually the mother—as a special individual. The attachment figure is now the infant’s stable socioemotional base. For example, a 7-month-old will explore a novel environment but periodically look toward his mother, as if asking her to reas- sure that all is well. The behavior suggests that the infant trusts his mother and indicates that the attachment relationship is established. In addition, this behavior reflects important cognitive growth: It means that the infant has a mental representation of the mother, an understanding that she will be there to meet the infant’s needs (Lewis, 1997).

●● Reciprocal relationships (18 months on). Infants’ growing cognitive and language skills and their accumulated experience with their primary caregivers make infants better able to act as partners in the attachment relationship. They often initiate interactions and negotiate with parents (“Please read me another story!”). They begin to understand parents’ feelings and goals and sometimes use this knowledge to guide their own behav- ior (e.g., social referencing, which we describe on page 167). In addition, they cope with separation more effectively because they can anticipate that parents will return.

Forms of Attachment Thanks to biology, virtually all infants behave in ways that elicit caregiving from adults, and because of this behavior, attachment usually develops between infant and caregiver by 8 or 9 months of age. However, attachment takes different forms, and environmental factors help determine the quality of attachment between infants and caregivers. Mary Ainsworth (1978, 1993) pioneered the study of attachment relationships using a proce- dure known as the Strange Situation. You can see in ◗ Table 5.2 that the Strange Situa- tion involves a series of brief episodes. The mother and infant enter an unfamiliar room filled with interesting toys. The mother leaves briefly, then mother and baby are reunited. Meanwhile, the experimenter observes the baby and records his or her response to both separation and reunion.

Based on how the infant reacts to separation from—and reunion with—the mother, Ainsworth and other researchers have discovered four primary types of attachment rela- tionships (Ainsworth, 1993; Thompson, 2006). One is a secure attachment, and three are different types of insecure attachment (avoidant, resistant, and disorganized).

●● Secure attachment: The baby may or may not cry when the mother leaves, but when she returns, the baby wants to be with her, and if the baby is crying, he or she stops. Babies in this group seem to be saying, “I missed you terribly; I’m delighted to see you; but now that everything is okay, I’ll get back to what I was doing.” Approximately 60% to 65% of American babies have secure attachment relationships.

secure attachment relationship in which infants have come to trust and depend on their mothers.

Evolutionary psychology emphasizes the adaptive value of parents nurturing their offspring.

Se an

S pr

ag ue

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

Think About It Based on Piaget’s description of infancy (pages 120–122), what cognitive skills might be important prerequisites for the formation of an attachment relationship?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 159

●● Avoidant attachment: The baby is not upset when the mother leaves and, when she returns, may ignore her by looking or turning away. Infants with an avoid- ant attachment look as if they’re saying, “You left me again. I always have to take care of myself!” About 20% of American infants have avoidant attachment relationships, which is one of the three forms of insecure attachment.

●● Resistant attachment: The baby is upset when the mother leaves, and the baby remains upset or even angry when she returns and is difficult to console. These babies seem to be telling the mother, “Why do you do this? I need you desper- ately, and yet you just leave me without warning. I get so angry when you’re like this.” About 10% to 15% of American babies have this resistant attachment relationship, which is another form of insecure attachment.

●● Disorganized (disoriented) attachment: The baby seems confused when the mother leaves and when she returns, seems not to understand what’s happening. The baby often behaves in contradictory ways, such as nearing the mother when she returns but not looking at her, as if wondering, “What’s happening? I want you to be here, but you left and now you’re back. I don’t get what’s going on!” About 5% to 10% of American babies have this disorganized attachment relationship, the last of the three kinds of insecure attachment.

Quality of attachment during infancy predicts parent–child relations during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. Infants with secure attachment relationships tend to report, as adolescents and young adults, that they depend on their parents for care and support. In contrast, infants with insecure attachment relationships often report, as adolescents and young adults, being angry with their parents or deny being close to them (Bretherton, 2010). However, consistency is far from perfect. Stressful life events—the death of a parent, divorce, a life-threatening ill- ness, poverty—help to determine stability and change in attachment. Stressful life events are associated with insecure attachments during adolescence and young adulthood. Con- sequently, when infants with insecure attachments experience stressful life events, their attachment tends to remain insecure; when infants with secure attachment experience these same events, their attachment often becomes insecure, perhaps because stress makes parents less available and less responsive to their children (Lamb, 2012).

Men and women are equally capable of parenting and babies typically become attached to mothers and fathers at about the same time (Lamb, 2012). And the quality of attachment is often the same; for example, the most common outcome is for infants to have secure attachment relationships with mothers and fathers (Bretherton, 2010). However, because mothers spend more time in caregiving, they often become more

avoidant attachment relationship in which infants turn away from their mothers when they are reunited following a brief separation.

resistant attachment relationship in which, after a brief separa- tion, infants want to be held but are difficult to console.

disorganized (disoriented) attachment relationship in which infants don’t seem to understand what’s happening when they are separated and later reunited with their mothers.

Fathers and mothers differ in how they play with children. Fathers are more likely to engage in vigorous physical play.

D or

ot hy

L itt

el l G

re co

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

Table 5.2

Sequence of Events in the Strange Situation

1. An observer shows the experimental room to the mother and infant, then leaves the room.

2. The infant is allowed to explore the playroom for 3 minutes; the mother watches but does not participate.

3. A stranger enters the room and remains silent for one minute, talks to the baby for a minute, and then approaches the baby. The mother leaves unobtrusively.

4. The stranger does not play with the baby, but attempts to comfort the baby if necessary.

5. After 3 minutes, the mother returns, greets the baby, and consoles the baby.

6. When the baby has returned to play, the mother leaves again, this time saying “bye-bye” as she leaves.

7. The stranger attempts to calm and play with the baby.

8. After 3 minutes, the mother returns and the stranger leaves.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

160 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

skillful at parenting than fathers (Lamb, 2012). Fathers spend more time playing with their babies than taking care of them and they prefer physical play whereas mothers spend more time reading and talking to babies, showing them toys, and playing games such as patty-cake. These differences between mothers’ and fathers’ behaviors have become smaller as men and women have come to share responsibilities for child care and breadwinning (Lamb & Lewis, 2010).

Consequences of Attachment Erikson and other theorists (e.g., Waters & Cummings, 2000) believe that infant–parent attachment, the first social relationship, lays the foundation for all of the infant’s later social relationships. In this

view, infants who experience the trust and compassion of a secure attachment should develop into preschool children who interact confidently and successfully with their peers. In contrast, infants who do not experience a successful, satisfying first rela- tionship should be more prone to problems in their social interactions as preschool- ers. In fact, meta-analyses reveal that children with secure attachment relationships are more competent socially, are less prone to externalizing disorders such as bully- ing  and fighting and less prone to internalizing disorders such as depression (Groh et al., 2017.)

Two factors contribute to the benefits for children of a secure attachment relation- ship. First, secure attachment evidently leads infants to see the world positively and to trust other humans, characteristics that lead to more skilled social interactions later in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (Dykas & Cassidy, 2011). Second, parents who establish secure attachments with infants tend to provide warm, supportive, and skilled parenting throughout their child’s development (McElwain, Booth-LaForce, & Wu, 2011; Thompson, 2006). Thus, continuous exposure to high-quality parenting promotes secure attachment in infancy and positive social relationships in child- hood and adolescence. These accounts are not mutually exclusive: A successful first relationship and continued warm parenting likely work together to foster children’s development.

Of course, attachment is only the first of many steps along the long road of social development. Infants with insecure attachments are not doomed, but this initial misstep can interfere with their social development. Consequently, we need to look at the condi- tions that determine the quality of attachment.

What Determines Quality of Attachment? Because secure attachment is so important to a child’s later development, researchers have tried to identify the factors involved. The most important is the interaction between parents and their babies. A secure attachment is most likely when parents respond to infants predictably and appropriately (Boldt et al., 2016; De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997). For example, when a mother responds promptly to her baby’s cry- ing and reassures the baby, the mother’s behavior evidently conveys that social interac- tions are predictable and satisfying. This behavior seems to instill in infants the trust and confidence that are the hallmarks of secure attachment.

Why does predictable and responsive parenting promote secure attachment rela- tionships? To answer this question, think about your own friendships and romantic relationships. These are usually most satisfying when we believe we can trust the other people and depend on them in times of need. The same formula seems to hold for infants. Infants develop an internal working model, a set of expectations about par- ents’ availability and responsiveness, generally and in times of stress. When parents are dependable and caring, babies come to trust them, knowing they can be relied on for comfort. In other words, babies develop an internal working model in which they believe their parents are concerned about their needs and will try to meet them

internal working model Infant’s understanding of how responsive and dependable the mother is; thought to influence close relationships throughout the child’s life.

When infants have an attachment relationship with the mother, they use her as a secure base from which to explore the environment.

M ar

y Ka

te D

en ny

/P ho

to Ed

it

When infants who have a resistant attach- ment relationship are reunited with the mother, they’re typically tearful, angry, and difficult to console.

Ja n

M ik

a/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 161

(Huth-Bocks et al., 2004; Thompson, 2000). In contrast, when parents respond slowly, intermittently, or angrily, infants come to see social relationships as incon- sistent and often frustrating

In a clever demonstration of infants’ working models of attachment ( Johnson et al., 2010), infants were shown animated videos depicting a large ellipse (mother) paired with a small ellipse (child). The video began with the mother and child ellipses together; then the mother moved away from the child, who began to cry. On some trials, the mother ellipse returned to the child ellipse; on other trials, she continued to move away. Securely attached infants looked longer at the trials depicting an unresponsive mother, but inse- curely attached infants looked longer at the trials when the mother returned. Evidently, each group had a working model of how parents respond—securely attached infants expect parents to respond, but insecurely attached infants do not—and they looked longer at the trials that violate their expectations of maternal behavior.

Fortunately, training can help mothers respond more effectively to their baby’s needs (Dozier, Zeanah, & Bernard, 2013; Mountain, Cahill, & Thorpe, 2017). Mothers can be taught how to interact more sensitively, affection- ately, and responsively, paving the way for secure attachment and the lifelong benefits associated with a positive internal working model of interpersonal relationships.

The formation of attachment illustrates well the combined influence of the different components of the biopsychosocial framework. Many infant behaviors that elicit caregiving in adults—smiling and crying, for example—are biological in origin. When the caregiver is responsive to the infant (a sociocultural force), a secure attachment forms in which the infant trusts caregivers and knows that they can be relied on in stressful situations (a psychological force).

Attachment, Work, and Alternative Caregiving Since the 1970s, more women in the workforce and more single-parent households have made arranging for child care a reality of parenting for many American families. Today, millions of infants and toddlers are cared for by some- one other than their mother. Some are cared for in their home by their father, a grandparent, or another relative. Others receive care in the provider’s home; the provider is often but not always a relative. Still others attend day care or pre- school programs.

Parents and policy makers alike have been concerned about the impact of such care on children generally and, specifically, its impact on attachment. In fact, research shows that, overall, child care experiences are not linked to mother–infant attachment: a secure mother–infant attachment is just as likely regardless of the quality of child care, the time the child spent in care, the age when the child began care, how frequently the parents changed child care arrangements, or the type of child care (e.g., at a child care center or in the home with a nonrelative). However, insecure attachments are more common when infants and toddlers of less sensitive mothers experience low-quality or large amounts of child care (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2001; Sagi et al., 2002).

One other reasonably consistent finding from research on child care is that children who experience many hours of nonparental child care are more often overly aggressive, have more conflicts with teachers, and have less self- control. Part of the problem here is that children who spend long hours in child care  are more likely to experience low-quality care, which is typically associated with children being less skilled socially. On a reassuring note, few children in  extensive child care experience problem behaviors that would be

Perhaps the most important ingredient in fostering a secure attachment relationship is responding predictably and appropriately to the infant’s needs.

Pr of

im ed

ia .C

Z a.

s/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o St

ev e

Sk jo

ld /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Millions of American infants and preschool- ers attend day care or nursery school programs.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

162 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

clinically significant and most of the effects disappear after first grade (Huston, Bobbitt, & Bentley, 2015).

These results provide clear guidelines for parents like Kendra, the mother in the vignette. They can enroll their infants and toddlers in high-quality day care programs with little fear of lifelong harmful consequences. In trying to find high-quality care for children, parents should look for programs that have a relatively small number of chil- dren per caregiver (e.g., three infants or toddlers per caregiver) and the caregivers are well-trained, are responsive, provide age-appropriate stimulating activities, and com- municate well with parents (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2011).

Fortunately, today many employers know that convenient, high-quality child care helps attract and retain talented employees. With effort, organization, and help from the com- munity and business, full-time employment and high-quality caregiving can be compatible. We will return to this issue in Chapter 12 from the perspective of the parents. For now, the Real People feature provides one example of a father who stays home to care for his daugh- ter while her mother works full-time.

Responding to the needs of working fami- lies, many employers now provide child care on site.

M ar

k Ri

ch ar

ds /P

ho to

Ed it

Lois, 46, and Bill, 61, had been married nearly four years when Lois gave birth to Sarah. Lois, a kindergarten teacher, returned to work full-time four months after Sarah was born. Bill, who had been halfheartedly pursuing a PhD in education, became a full- time househusband. Bill does the cooking and takes care of Sarah during the day. Lois comes home from school at noon so that the family can eat lunch together, and she is

home from work by 4 in the afternoon. Once a week, Bill takes Sarah to a parent–infant play program. The other parents, all mothers in their twenties and thirties, first assumed that Bill was Sarah’s grandfather and had trouble relating to him as an older father. However, he quickly became an accepted member of the group. On weekends, Lois’s and Bill’s grown children from previous marriages often visit and enjoy caring for and playing with Sarah.

By all accounts, Sarah looks to be a healthy, happy, outgoing 9-month-old. Is this arrange- ment nontraditional? Clearly. Is it effective for Sarah, Lois, and Bill? Definitely. Sarah receives the nurturing care she needs, Lois goes to work assured that Sarah is in Bill’s knowing and caring hands, and Bill relishes being the primary caregiver.

Real People Applying Human Development

Lois, Bill, and Sarah

Think About It Imagine that your best friend is the mother of a 3-month-old. Your friend is about to return to her job as a social worker, but she’s afraid she’ll harm her baby by going back to work. What can you say to reassure her?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 163

Nicole is ecstatic that she is finally going to see her 7-month-old nephew, Claude. She rushes into the house, sees Claude playing on the floor with blocks, and sweeps him up in a big hug. After a brief, puzzled look, Claude bursts into angry tears and begins thrashing his arms and legs, as if saying to Nicole, “Who are you? What do you want? Put me down! Now!” Nicole quickly hands Claude to his mother, who is surprised by her baby’s outburst and even more surprised that he continues to sob while she rocks him.

This vignette illustrates three common emotions. Nicole’s initial joy, Claude’s anger, and his mother’s surprise are familiar to all of us. In this section, we look at when children first express emotions, how children come to understand emotions in others, and how children regulate their emotions. As we do, we’ll learn why Claude reacted to Nicole as he did and how Nicole could have prevented Claude’s outburst.

The Function of Emotions Why do people feel emotions? According to the functional approach, emotions are useful because they help people adapt to their environment (Boiger & Mesquita, 2012; Shariff & Tracy, 2011). Consider fear as an example. Most of us would rather not be afraid, but some- times feeling fearful is adaptive. Imagine you are walking alone late at night in a poorly lit section of campus. You become frightened and, as a consequence, are particularly atten- tive to sounds that might signal threat and you walk quickly to a safer location. Thus, fear is adaptive because it organizes your behavior around an important goal—avoiding danger (Tooby & Cosmides, 2008).

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● At what ages do children begin to express basic

emotions?

●● What are complex emotions, and when do they develop?

●● When do children begin to understand other people’s emotions? How do they use this information to guide their own behavior?

5.2 Emerging Emotions

Recall 1. proposed that maturational and social fac-

tors come together to pose eight unique challenges for psy- chosocial growth during the life span.

2. Infants must balance trust and mistrust to achieve , an openness to new experience that is

coupled with awareness of possible danger.

3. By approximately months of age, most infants have identified a special individual—usually but not always the mother—as the attachment figure.

4. Joan, a 12-month-old, was separated from her mother for about 15 minutes. When they were reunited, Joan would not let her mother pick her up. When her mother approached, Joan would look the other way or toddle to another part of the room. This behavior suggests that Joan has a(n)

attachment relationship.

5. The single most important factor in fostering a secure attachment relationship is .

6. Children who have a attachment relationship with their parents tend to be more competent socially and are less prone to externalizing disorders.

7. An insecure attachment relationship is likely when an infant receives poor-quality child care and .

Interpret ●● Compare the infant’s contributions to the formation of mother–

infant attachment with the mother’s contributions.

Apply ●● Based on what you know about the normal developmental

timetable for the formation of mother–infant attachment, what would seem to be the optimal age range for children to be adopted?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 5.1

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

164 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Similarly, other emotions are adaptive. For example, happiness is adaptive in con- tributing to stronger interpersonal relationships: When people are happy with another person, they smile, which often causes the other person to feel happy, too, strengthening their relationship (Conway et al., 2013). Disgust is adaptive in keeping people away from substances that might make them ill: When we discover that the milk in a glass is sour, we experience disgust and push the glass away (Oaten, Stevenson, & Case, 2009). Thus, according to the functional approach to human emotion, most emotions developed over the course of human history to meet unique life challenges and help humans to survive.

Experiencing and Expressing Emotions The three emotions from the vignette—joy, anger, and fear—are considered “basic emo- tions,” as are interest, disgust, and sadness (Lewis, 2016). Basic emotions are experienced by people worldwide, and each consists of three elements: a subjective feeling, a physiological change, and an overt behavior (Izard, 2007). For example, suppose you wake to the sound of a thunderstorm and then discover your roommate has left for class with your umbrella. Subjectively, you might feel ready to explode with anger; physiologically, your heart would beat faster; and behaviorally, you would probably be scowling.

Development of Basic Emotions Using facial expressions and other behaviors, scientists have traced the growth of basic emotions in infants. Many scientists believe that young babies experience just broad posi- tive and broad negative emotional states. These broad emotional categories differentiate rapidly, and by approximately 8 to 9 months of age, infants are thought to experience all basic emotions (Lewis, 2016). For example, the onset of happiness is evident in a baby’s smiles. In the first month, infants smile while asleep or when touched softly. The meaning of these smiles isn’t clear; they may just represent a reflexive response to bodily states. However, an important change occurs at about 2 to 3 months of age. At this age, social  smiles first appear; infants smile when they see another human face. Sometimes social smiling is accompanied by cooing, the early form of vocalization described in Chap- ter 4 (Lavelli & Fogel, 2013). Smiling and cooing seem to be an infant’s way of expressing pleasure at interacting with another person.

Anger is one of the first negative emotions to emerge from generalized distress, typi- cally between 4 and 6 months. Infants will become angry, for example, if a favorite food or toy is taken away (Sullivan & Lewis, 2003). Reflecting their growing understanding of goal-directed behavior (see Section 4.1), older infants become increasingly angry when their attempts to achieve a goal are frustrated (Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund, & Kar- rass, 2010). For example, when a parent restrains an infant trying to pick up a toy, the guaranteed result is an angry baby.

Like anger, fear emerges later in the first year. At about 6 months, infants become wary in the presence of an unfa- miliar adult, a reaction known as stranger wariness. When a stranger approaches, a 6-month-old typically looks away and begins to fuss (Stevenson-Hinde & Shoul- dice, 2009). If a grandmother picks up her grandchild without giving the infant a chance to warm up to her, the outcome is as predictable as it was with Claude, the baby boy in the vignette who was frightened by his aunt: He cries, looks frightened, and reaches with arms outstretched in the direction of someone familiar.

How wary an infant feels around strangers depends on several factors (Brooker et al., 2013; Thompson & Limber, 1991). First, infants tend to be less fearful of strangers when the environment is familiar and more fearful when it is not. Many parents know this firsthand from traveling with their

basic emotions Emotions experienced by humankind that consist of three elements: a subjective feel- ing, a physiological change, and an overt behavior.

social smiles Smile that infants produce when they see a human face.

stranger wariness first distinct signs of fear that emerge around 6 months of age when infants become wary in the presence of unfamiliar adults.

Social smiles emerge at about 2 to 3 months, when infants smile in response to a human face.

Co ur

te sy

o f R

ob er

t V . K

ai l

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 165

infants: When they enter a friend’s house for the first time, their baby clings tightly to his or her mother. Second, a baby’s anxiety depends on the stranger’s behavior. Instead of rushing to greet or pick up the baby, as Nicole did in the vignette, a stranger should talk with other adults and, in a while, perhaps offer the baby a toy (Mangelsdorf, 1992). Handled this way, many infants will soon be curious about the stranger instead of afraid.

Wariness of strangers is adaptive because it emerges at the same time children begin to master creeping and crawling (described on pages 96–97). Like Curious George, the monkey in a famous series of children’s books, babies are inquisitive and want to use their new locomotor skills to explore their world. Being wary of strangers provides a natu- ral restraint against the tendency to wander away from familiar caregivers. However, as youngsters learn to interpret facial expressions and recognize when a person is friendly, their wariness of strangers declines.

Of the negative emotions, we know the least about disgust. Preschool children may respond with disgust at the odor of feces or at being asked to touch a maggot or being asked to eat a piece of candy that’s resting on the bottom of a brand-new potty seat (Widen & Russell, 2013). Parents likely play an important role in helping children to iden- tify disgusting stimuli: Mothers respond quite vigorously to disgust-eliciting stimuli when in the presence of their children. They might say “That’s revolting!” while moving away from the stimulus (Stevenson et al., 2010). A child’s early sensitivity to disgust is useful because many of the cues that elicit disgust are also signals of potential illness: Disgusting stimuli such as feces, vomit, and maggots can all transmit disease.

Emergence of Complex Emotions In addition to basic emotions such as joy and anger, people feel complex emotions such as pride, guilt, and embarrassment. Most scientists (e.g., Lewis, 2016) believe that complex emotions don’t surface until 18 to 24 months of age because they depend on the child having some understanding of the self, which typically occurs between 15 and 18 months. For example, children feel guilty or embarrassed when they’ve done something they know they shouldn’t have done (Kochanska et al., 2002): A child who breaks a toy is thinking, You told me to be careful. But I wasn’t! Similarly, children feel pride when they accomplish a challenging task for the first time. Thus, children’s growing understanding of themselves enables them to experience complex emotions like pride and guilt (Lewis, 2016).

The features of basic and self-conscious emotions are summarized in ◗ Table 5.3.

Later Developments As children grow, their catalog of emotions continues to expand. For example, some 5- and 6-year-olds experience regret and relief, and by 9 years of age, most children experience both emotions appropriately (Van Duijvenvoorde, Huizenga, & Jansen, 2014). In addi- tion, cognitive growth means that elementary school children experience shame and guilt in situations they would not have when they were younger (Reimer, 1996). For example, unlike preschool children, many school-age children would be ashamed if they neglected to defend a classmate who had been wrongly accused of a theft.

Young babies are often wary of strangers; consequently, they’re unhappy when strang- ers hold them before giving them a chance to “warm up.”

Co ur

te sy

o f R

ob er

t V . K

ai l

Think About It How might an infant’s ability to express emotions relate to the formation of attachment? To the temperamental char- acteristics described on pages 85–86?

Think About It Explain how the different forces in the biopsychosocial framework contribute to the development of basic and complex emotions.

Table 5.3

Infants’ Expression of Emotions DefineD emerge exAmples

Basic Experienced by people worldwide and include a subjective feeling, a physiological response, and an overt behavior

Birth to 9 months Happiness, anger, fear

Self-conscious Responses to meeting or failing to meet expectations or standards

18 to 24 months Pride, guilt, embarrassment

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

166 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Fear is another emotion that can be elicited in different ways depending on a child’s age. Many preschool children are afraid of the dark and of imaginary creatures. These fears typically diminish during the elementary school years as children grow cognitively and better understand the difference between appearance and reality. Replacing these fears are concerns about school, health, and personal harm (Silverman, La Greca, & Wasserstein, 1995). Such worries are common and not a cause for concern in most children. In some youngsters, however, they become so extreme that they overwhelm the child. For example, some children worry so much about school that they refuse to go (Kearney & Spear, 2013).

Cultural Differences in Emotional Expression Children worldwide express many of the same basic and complex emotions. However, cul- tures differ in the extent to which emotional expression is encouraged Outward displays of emotion are encouraged and common in countries like those in North America and Europe that see people as independent; in contrast, emotional restraint is encouraged and common in countries like those in Asia that see people as interdependent with other group members (Camras & Shuster, 2013). Consistent with this view, U.S. children are typically more expressive emotionally than are Chinese children, both in overall expres- sivity and in terms of specific expressions (e.g., smiling more at funny pictures, more often expressing disgust when smelling vinegar).

Cultures also differ in the events that trigger emotions, particularly complex emo- tions. Situations that evoke pride in one culture may evoke embarrassment or shame in another. For example, American elementary school children often show pride at personal achievement, such as getting the highest grade on a test or coming in first place in a county fair. In contrast, Asian elementary school children are embarrassed by a public display of individual achievement but show great pride when their entire class is honored for an achievement (Furukawa, Tangney, & Higashibara, 2012; Lewis et al., 2010).

Expression of anger also varies around the world. Suppose a child has just completed a detailed drawing when a classmate spills a drink, ruining the drawing. Most American children would respond with anger. In contrast, children growing up in East Asian coun- tries that practice Buddhism (e.g., Mongolia, Thailand, Nepal) rarely respond with anger because this goes against the Buddhist tenet to be kind to all people, even those whose actions hurt others. Instead, they would probably remain quiet and experience shame that they had left the drawing in a vulnerable position (Cole, Tamang, & Shrestha, 2006).

Thus, culture can influence when and how much children express emotion. Of course, expressing emotion is only part of the developmental story. Children must also learn to recognize others’ emotions, which is our next topic.

Recognizing and Using Others’ Emotions Imagine you are short of cash and hope to borrow $20 from your roommate when she returns from class. But when she storms into your apartment, slams the door, and throws her backpack on the floor, you change your mind, realizing that now is a bad time to ask for a loan. This example reminds us that we often need to recognize others’ emotions and sometimes change our behavior as a consequence.

When can infants first identify emotions in others? Between 4 and 6 months, infants begin to distinguish facial expressions associated with different emotions. For exam- ple, they can distinguish a happy, smiling face from a sad, frowning face (Bornstein & Arterberry, 2003; Montague & Walker-Andrews, 2001), and when they hear happy- sounding voices, they tend to look at happy faces, not ones that appear angry (Bhatt et al., 2016). What’s more, like adults, infants are biased toward negative emotions (Vaish, Woodward, & Grossmann, 2008). They attend more rapidly to faces depicting negative emotions (e.g., anger, fear) and pay attention to them longer than they do to emotionless or happy faces (Heck et al., 2016; LoBue & DeLoache, 2010).

Also like adults, infants use others’ emotions to direct their behavio. Infants in an unfa- miliar or ambiguous environment often look at a parent as if searching for cues to help them

American children are often quite proud of personal achievement, but Asian children would be embarrassed by such a public dis- play of individual accomplishments.

To og

a/ St

on e/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 167

interpret the situation, a phenomenon known as social referencing. If a parent looks afraid when shown a novel object, 12-month-olds are less likely to play with the toy than if a par- ent looks happy (Repacholi, 1998). Infants’ use of parents’ cues is precise (see ◗ Figure 5.1). If two unfamiliar toys are shown to a parent who expresses disgust at one toy but not the other, 12-month-olds will avoid the toy that elicited the disgust but not the other toy (Moses et al., 2001). And if 12-month-olds encounter an unfamiliar toy in a laboratory setting where one adult seems familiar with the toy but another adult does not, infants will look at the knowledgeable adult’s expression to decide whether to play with the toy (Stenberg, 2013).

By 18 months, they’re more sophisticated: If one adult demonstrates an unfamiliar toy and a second adult comments in an angry tone, “That’s really annoying! That’s so irri- tating!” then 18-month-olds play less with the toy than if the second adult makes neutral remarks in a mild manner. Toddlers apparently decide that they shouldn’t play with the toy if it will upset the second adult again (Repacholi & Meltzoff, 2007; Repacholi, Meltzoff, & Olsen, 2008). Thus, social referencing shows that infants are remarkably skilled in using the emotions of adults to help them direct their own behavior.

Although infants and toddlers are remarkably adept at recognizing others’ emotions, these skills continue to develop. For example, older children and adolescents are more skilled in recognizing the subtle signals of an emotion (Booker & Dunsmore, 2017). Simi- larly, compared with younger children, older children more accurately recognize emotions by sound alone (Chronaki et al., 2015). Thus, expressions of emotion are recognized with steadily increasing skill throughout childhood and into adolescence.

What experiences contribute to children’s understanding of emotions? Parents and chil- dren frequently talk about past emotions and why people felt as they did, particularly for nega- tive emotions such as fear and anger (Lagattuta & Wellman, 2002). Not surprisingly, children learn about emotions when parents talk about feelings, explaining how they differ and the situations that elicit them (Brown & Dunn, 1992; Hughes, White, & Ensor, 2014). Also, a posi- tive and rewarding relationship with parents and siblings is related to children’s understand- ing of emotions (Thompson, Laible, & Ontai, 2003). The nature of this connection is still a mystery. One possibility is that within positive parent–child and sibling relationships, people express a fuller range of emotions (and do so more often) and are more willing to talk about why they feel as they do, providing children with more opportunities to learn about emotions.

Regulating Emotions People often regulate emotions; for example, we routinely try to suppress fear (because we know there’s no real need to be afraid of the dark), anger (because we don’t want to let a friend know how upset we are), and joy (because we don’t want to seem like we’re gloating over our good fortune). Sometimes people regulate their emotions behaviorally: we close our eyes to shut out a disturbing scene in a movie, move closer to a loved one when we’re frightened, or bite our inner checks to avoid laughing.

These behavioral strategies are used throughout the lifespan (Levine, Kaplan, & Davis, 2013). By 4 to 6 months, infants use simple strategies to regulate their emotions (Buss & Goldsmith, 1998; Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). When something frightens or confuses an

social referencing behavior in which infants in unfamiliar or ambiguous environments look at an adult for cues to help them interpret the situation.

●◗ Figure 5.1 If parents seem frightened by an unfamiliar object, then babies are also wary or even afraid of it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

168 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

infant—for example, a menacing stranger—babies often look away. Frightened infants also move closer to a parent, another effective way of helping to control their fear (Parritz, 1996). And by 24 months, a distressed toddler’s face typically expresses sadness instead of fear or anger; apparently by this age, toddlers have learned that a sad facial expression is the best way to get a mother’s attention and support (Buss & Kiel, 2004).

People also regulate emotions cognitively, often by reappraising the meaning of an event (or of feelings or thoughts) so that it provokes less emotion (John & Gross, 2007). For exam- ple, a soccer player nervous about taking a penalty kick can reinterpret her state of physiologi- cal arousal as being “pumped up” instead of being “scared to death.” Because such strategies depend on cognitive growth, they are more common in school-age children and adolescents (Levine et al., 2013; Silvers, Buhle, & Ochsner, 2014). For example, a child might reduce his disappointment at not receiving a much-anticipated and hoped-for gift by telling himself that he didn’t really want the gift in the first place. Or a child nervous about seeing a dentist to have a cavity filled may try to think of the benefits of the visit (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, 2011).

Unfortunately, not all children regulate their emotions well, and those who don’t tend to have problems adjusting and interacting with peers (Olson et al., 2011; Zalewski et al., 2011). When children can’t control their anger, worry, or sadness, they often have difficulty resolv- ing the conflicts that inevitably surface in peer relationships (Fabes et al., 1999). For example, when children are faced with a dispute over who gets to play with a toy, their unregulated anger can interfere with finding a mutually satisfying solution. Thus, ineffective regulation of emotions leads to more frequent conflicts with peers and, as a result, less satisfying peer relationships and less adaptive adjustment to school (Levine et al., 2013; Olson et al., 2005).

Recall 1. Basic emotions include a subjective feeling, a physiological

change, and .

2. The first detectable form of fear is , which emerges at about 6 months.

3. Wariness of strangers is adaptive because it emerges at about the same time .

4. Complex emotions such as guilt and shame emerge later than basic emotions because .

5. In social referencing, infants use a parent’s facial expression .

6. Infants often control fear by looking away from a frightening event or by .

Interpret ●● Distinguish basic emotions from complex emotions.

Apply ●● Cite similarities between developmental change in infants’

expression and regulation of emotion and developmental change in infants’ comprehension and expression of speech (described on pages 139–142 of Chapter 4).

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 5.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● When do youngsters first begin to play with each other? How

does play change during infancy and the preschool years? ●● What determines whether children help one another?

5.3 Interacting with Others

Six-year-old Santiago and his two siblings had been told not to touch their mother’s iPad. Consequently, when Santiago dropped the tablet and the screen shattered, he cried and cried. His 3-year-old brother, Alejandro, and his 2-year-old sister, Isabella, watched but did not help. Later, when their mother had soothed Santiago and assured him that the iPad was old and needed to be replaced anyway, she wondered about her younger children’s reactions. In the face of their brother’s obvious distress, why did Alejandro and Isabella do nothing?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 169

Infants’ initial interactions are with parents, but soon they begin to interact with other people, notably their peers. In this section, we’ll trace the development of these interactions and learn why children like Santiago and Isabella don’t always help others.

The Joys of Play Peer interactions begin surprisingly early in infancy. By 6 months, babies look, smile, vocalize, and point at each other. These behaviors become more common over the rest of the first year and babies become more likely to respond in kind to these behaviors— smiling or vocalizing in return (Rubin et al., 2015).

Soon after the first birthday, children begin parallel play, in which each youngster plays alone but maintains a keen interest in what another child is doing. Two toddlers may each have his or her own toys, but each will watch the other’s play, too. Exchanges between youngsters also become more common. Parallel play represents a transition from playing alone to playing with others (Howes, Unger, & Seidner, 1990).

Beginning at roughly 15 to 18 months, toddlers no longer simply watch one another at play. Instead, they engage in similar activities and talk or smile at one another, illus- trating simple social play. Play has now become interactive (Howes & Matheson, 1992). An example of simple social play is two 20-month-olds pushing toy cars along the floor, making “car sounds” and periodically trading cars.

Toward the second birthday, cooperative play emerges: Now a distinct theme organizes children’s play, and they take on special roles based on the theme. They may play hide-and-seek and alternate the roles of hider and seeker, or they may have a tea party and take turns being the host and the guest (Parten, 1932). By the time children are 3½ to 4  years old, parallel play is much less common and cooperative play is the norm (Howes & Matheson, 1992).

Make-Believe During the preschool years, cooperative play often takes the form of make-believe. Pre- schoolers have telephone conversations with imaginary partners or pretend to drink imaginary juice. In the early phases of make-believe, children rely on realistic props to support their play. While pretending to drink, younger preschoolers use a real cup; while pretending to drive a car, they use a toy steering wheel. In the later phases of make-believe, children no longer need realistic props; instead, they can imagine that a block is the cup or that a paper plate is the steering wheel.

This gradual movement toward more complex make-believe reflects growth of language and cognitive skills as well as interactions with parents (Karniol, 2016). Parents initiate pretend play with their infants and toddlers. A mother feeding her 15-month- old with a spoon may laugh and say, “Here’s the plane flying into Hassan’s mouth!” By  24  months, parents often initiate make-believe by saying “Let’s pretend,” and by age 3, children often engage in pretend play with peers. They usually tell play partners that they want to pretend (“Let’s pretend”), then describe those aspects of reality that are being changed (“I’ll be the pilot, and this is my plane,” referring to the couch). It’s as if children mutually agree to enter a parallel universe that’s governed by its own set of rules (Wyman, 2014).

As you might suspect, culture influences the development of make-believe. In many cultures (e.g., India, Peru), parents do not routinely engage in pretend play with their children. Without such parental support, children don’t begin pretend play until they’re older (Callaghan et al., 2011). In addition, the contents of pretend play reflect the val- ues important in a child’s culture (Gosso, Morais, & Otta, 2007). For example, adven- ture and fantasy are favorite themes for European American youngsters, but family roles  and everyday activities are favorites of Korean American children. Thus, cul- tural values  influence both the emergence and the content of make-believe (Farver & Shin, 1997).

parallel play when children play alone but are aware of and interested in what another child is doing.

simple social play play that begins at about 15 to 18 months; toddlers engage in similar activities as well as talk and smile at each other.

cooperative play play that is organized around a theme, with each child taking on a different role; begins at about 2 years of age.

In parallel play, children play independently but actively watch what other children are doing.

Va ne

ss a

D av

ie s/

D or

lin g

Ki nd

er sl

ey /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Think About It How might Jean Piaget have explained the emergence of make-believe dur- ing the preschool years? How would Erik Erikson have explained it?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

170 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Many teachers and parents believe that make-believe play not only is entertaining for children but also promotes their development. However, the proposed benefits of pretend play are not supported by research. In fact, most of the evidence indicates that pretend play does not enhance (or, for that mat- ter, harm) children’s creativity, their cognitive development, or their emotion regulation; pretend play may enhance chil- dren’s social skills but the findings are inconsistent (Lillard et al., 2013). In short, the main argument for pretend play is that children enjoy it, not that they benefit from it.

For many preschool children, make-believe play involves imaginary companions. Imaginary companions were once thought to be fairly rare, but many preschoolers, particularly girls as well as firstborn and only children, report imaginary companions (Taylor et al., 2004, 2013). Children can usually describe what their imaginary playmates look and sound like (Tahiroglu, Mannering, & Taylor, 2011). Having an imaginary companion is associated with many positive social charac- teristics (Davis, Meins, & Fernyhough, 2011; Giménez-Dasi, Pons, & Bender, 2016; Roby & Kidd, 2008). Compared with preschool children who lack imaginary friends, preschoolers with imaginary friends tend to be more sociable, have more real friends, and have a more advanced theory of mind. And

among older children who are at risk for developing behavior problems, an imaginary companion promotes better adjustment during adolescence (Taylor, Hulette,  & Dish- ion, 2010).

Many children also play with nonhuman animals, particularly family pets. Most families in the United States and Europe have pets, and some children become strongly attached to their pets (Endenburg, van Lith, & Kirpensteijn, 2014). Children without younger siblings are particularly likely to play with pets. Such play sometimes resembles cooperative play, particularly when dogs are involved. For example, “fetch” has the roles of “thrower” and “retriever” that are understood by child and dog. In other common forms of child-animal play, children sometimes pretend to be an animal like the pet (e.g., barking at the pet) or they pretend that the pet is human and hold conversations in which they speak for it and themselves (Melson, 2003, 2010).

Solitary Play At times throughout the preschool years, many children prefer to play alone. Should par- ents be worried? Not necessarily. Some children are simply not particularly sociable—they enjoy solitary activities (e.g., coloring, solving puzzles, or assembling Legos), although they are capable of interacting effectively with peers and often do so. However, other chil- dren are socially avoidant—they play alone not because they particularly like solitary play but because they’re uncomfortable interacting with peers. These children are often anx- ious and lonely, and are more likely to be excluded by peers. In other words, solitary play is no cause for concern when children enjoy playing alone but is worrisome when children play alone to escape interacting with peers. It’s best for these youngsters to see a profes- sional who can help them overcome their reticence in these situations (Coplan, Ooi, & Nocita, 2015).

Gender Differences in Play Between 2 and 3 years of age, children begin to prefer playing with same-sex peers (Halim et al., 2013). Little boys play together with cars, and little girls play together with dolls. Segregation of playmates by sex occurs spontaneously, and children often resist playing

Br an

d X

Pi ct

ur es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Make-believe becomes more sophisticated as children develop.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 171

with members of the other sex, even in gender-neutral activities such as playing tag or coloring (Maccoby, 1990, 1998).

This preference increases during childhood, reaching a peak in preadolescence. By age 10 or 11, the vast majority of peer activity is with same-sex children, and most of this involves sex-typed play: Boys are playing sports or playing with cars or action figures; girls are doing artwork or playing with pets or dolls (McHale et al., 2004). Then the tide begins to turn, but even in adulthood, time spent at work and at leisure is commonly segregated by gender (Hartup, 1983).

Why do boys and girls seem so attracted to same-sex play partners? The first reason is self-selection by sex. Boys and girls want to play with others like themselves, and after they know their sex, they pick others on that basis (Martin et al., 2013). Second, boys and girls differ in their styles of play. Boys prefer rough-and-tumble play and generally are more competitive and dominating in their interactions; in contrast, girls’ play is more coopera- tive, prosocial, and conversation-oriented. Generally, boys don’t enjoy the way girls play and girls are averse to boys’ style of play (Leaper, 2015; Maccoby, 1990, 1998).

Third, when girls and boys play together, girls do not readily influence boys. Girls’ interactions with one another are typically enabling—their actions and remarks tend to support others and sustain the interaction. When drawing together, one girl might say to another, “Cool picture” or “What do you want to do now?” In contrast, boy’s interactions are often constricting—one partner tries to emerge as the victor by threatening or contra- dicting the other, by exaggerating, and so on. In the same drawing task, one boy might say to another, “My picture’s better” or “Drawing is stupid—let’s watch TV.” When these styles are brought together, girls find that their enabling style is ineffective with boys. The same subtle overtures that work with other girls have no impact on boys. Boys ignore girls’ polite suggestions about what to do and ignore girls’ efforts to resolve conflicts with dis- cussion (Rose & Rudolph, 2006).

Early segregation of playmates by style of play means that boys learn primarily from boys and girls from girls. Over time, such social segregation by sex reinforces gender dif- ferences in play. When young boys spend most of their time playing with other boys, their play becomes more active and more aggressive. In contrast, when young girls spend most of their time playing with other girls, their play becomes less active and less aggressive (Leaper, 2015).

enabling actions Individuals’ actions and remarks that tend to support others and sustain the interaction.

constricting actions Interaction in which one partner tries to emerge as the victor by threatening or con- tradicting the other.

At about 2 or 3 years of age, boys and girls start to prefer playing with members of their own sex.

A le

na O

ze ro

va /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

172 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

prosocial behavior any behavior that benefits another person.

Parental Influence Parents become involved in their preschool children’s play in several ways (Parke & O’Neill, 2000):

●● Playmate. Many parents enjoy the role of playmate (and many parents deserve an Oscar for their performances). They use the opportunity to scaffold their children’s play (see Chapter 4, page 137), often raising it to more sophisticated levels (Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1996). For example, if a toddler is stacking toy plates, a parent might help the child stack the plates (play at the same level) or might pretend to wash each plate (play at a more advanced level). When par- ents demonstrate the reciprocal, cooperative nature of play, their children’s play with peers is more successful (Lindsey, Cremeens, & Caldera, 2010).

●● Social director. It takes two to interact, and young children rely on parents to create opportunities for social interactions. Many parents of young children arrange visits with peers, enroll children in activities (e.g., preschool programs), and take children to settings that attract young children (e.g., parks, swimming pools). All this effort is worth it: Children whose parents provide them with frequent opportunities for peer interaction tend to get along better with their peers (Ladd & Pettit, 2002).

●● Coach. Successful interactions require a host of skills, including how to initiate an interaction, make joint decisions, and resolve conflicts. When parents help their children acquire these skills, children tend to be more competent socially and more accepted by their peers (Grusec, 2011; Mounts, 2011). For example, when mothers emphasize how targets of relational aggression feel, their children are less likely to resort to relational aggression (Werner et al., 2014). But there’s a catch: The coaching must be constructive for children to benefit. Parent coaches sometimes make sugges- tions that are misguided. Bad coaching is worse than none at all, as it harms children’s peer relations (Russell & Finnie, 1990).

●● Mediator. When young children play, they often disagree, argue, and sometimes fight. However, children play more cooperatively and longer when parents are present to help iron out conflicts (Mize, Pettit, & Brown, 1995). When young children can’t agree on what to play, a parent can negotiate a mutually acceptable activity. When both youngsters want to play with the same toy, a parent can arrange for them to share. Here, too, parents scaffold their preschoolers’ play, smoothing the interaction by pro- viding some of the social skills that preschoolers lack.

In addition to these direct influences on children’s play, parents influence children’s play indirectly via the quality of the parent–child attachment relationship. Recall that peer relationships in childhood and adolescence are most successful when, as infants, children had a secure attachment relationship with their mother (Groh et al., 2017). A child’s rela- tionship with his or her parents is the internal working model for all future social relation- ships. When the parent–child relationship is of high quality and is emotionally satisfying, children are encouraged to form relationships with other people. Another possibility is that an infant’s secure attachment relationship with his or her mother makes the infant feel more confident about exploring the environment, which in turn provides more oppor- tunities to interact with peers. These two views are not mutually exclusive; both may con- tribute to the relative ease with which securely attached children interact with their peers (Hartup, 1992).

Helping Others prosocial behavior is any behavior that benefits another person. Cooperation—that is, working together toward a common goal—is one form of prosocial behavior. Of course, cooperation often “works” because individuals gain more than they would by not

Think About It Suppose friends ask you how their pre- school daughter could get along well with peers. What advice would you give them?

Parents influence their children’s play in many ways, perhaps none more important than by mediating the disputes that arise when preschoolers play.

A m

y Et

ra /P

ho to

Ed it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 173

cooperating. In contrast, altruism is behavior that is driven by feelings of responsibility toward other people, such as helping and sharing, in which individuals do not benefit directly from their actions. If two youngsters pool their funds to buy a candy bar to share, this is cooperative behavior. If one youngster gives half of her lunch to a peer who forgot his own, this is altruism.

Many scientists believe that humans are biologically predisposed to be helpful, to share, to cooperate, and to be concerned for others (Davidov et al., 2016). Why has pro- social behavior evolved over time? The best explanation has nothing to do with lofty moral principles; it’s more pragmatic: People who frequently help others are more likely to receive help themselves, which increases the odds that they’ll pass along their genes to future generations. In fact, basic acts of altruism can be seen by 18 months of age. When toddlers and preschoolers see other people who are obviously hurt or upset, they appear concerned, like the child in the photo at the bottom of the page. They try to comfort the person by hugging him or patting him (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992). And if an adult obvi- ously needs help, most 18-month-olds help spontaneously. For example, when a teacher accidentally drops markers on a floor, most 18-month-olds help the teacher pick them up (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Knafo-Noam, 2015).

Prosocial behavior typically increases with development: adolescents are more likely to help than children, who are more likely to help than preschoolers. These changes reflect children’s growing understanding of others’ needs and of appropriate altruistic responses (Eisenberg et al., 2016). Let’s look at some specific skills that set the stage for altruistic behaviors.

Skills Underlying Altruistic Behavior Remember from Chapter 4 that preschool children are often egocentric, so they may not see the need for altruistic behavior. For example, young children might not share candy with a younger sibling because they cannot imagine how unhappy the sibling is without the candy. In contrast, school-age children, who can more easily take another person’s per- spective, would perceive the unhappiness and be more inclined to share. In fact, research consistently indicates that altruistic behavior is related to perspective-taking skill. Young- sters who understand others’ thoughts and feelings share better with others and help them more often (Imuta et al., 2016; Vaish, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2009).

Related to perspective taking is empathy, which is the actual experiencing of another’s feelings. Children who deeply feel another individual’s fear, disappointment, sorrow, or loneliness are more inclined to help that person than are children who do not feel those emotions (Eisenberg et al., 2015). In other words, youngsters who are obviously distressed by what they are seeing are most likely to help if they can.

Of course, perspective taking and empathy do not guarantee that children always act altruistically. Even though children have the skills needed to act altruistically, they may not because of the particular situation, as we’ll see next.

Situational Influences Kind children occasionally disappoint us by being cruel, and children who are usually stingy sometimes surprise us by their generosity. Why? The setting helps determine whether children act altruistically.

●● Feelings of responsibility. Children act altruistically when they feel responsible for the person in need. For example, children may help siblings and friends more often than strangers simply because they feel a direct responsibility for people they know well (Costin & Jones, 1992). And they’re more likely to help when prompted with photos showing two people who look to be friends (Over & Carpenter, 2009). In other words, a simple reminder of the impor- tance of friendship (or affiliation with others) can be enough to elicit helping.

altruism prosocial behavior such as helping and sharing in which the individual does not benefit directly from his or her behavior.

empathy act of experiencing another person’s feelings.

By 18 months of age, toddlers try to com- fort others who are hurt or upset.

D av

id Y

ou ng

-W ol

ff /P

ho to

Ed it

When children can empathize with others who are sad or upset, they’re more likely to offer to help.

D en

is e

H ag

er /C

at ch

lig ht

V is

ua l S

er vi

ce s/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

174 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● Feelings of competence. Children act altruistically when they believe that they have the skills to help the person in need. Suppose, for example, that a preschooler is growing more and more upset because she can’t figure out how to work a computer game; a peer who knows little about computer games is not likely to come to the young girl’s aid because the peer doesn’t know what to do to help. If the peer tries to help, he or she can end up looking foolish (Peterson, 1983).

●● Mood. Children act altruistically when they are happy or when they are feeling suc- cessful but not when they are feeling sad or feeling as if they have failed (Wentzel, Filisetti, & Looney, 2007). A preschool child who just spent an exciting morning as the “leader” in nursery school is more inclined to share treats with siblings than is a pre- schooler who was punished by the teacher (Eisenberg, 2000).

●● Costs of altruism. Children act altruistically when such actions entail few or mod- est sacrifices. A preschool child who was given a snack that she doesn’t like is more inclined to share it with others than a child who was given her favorite food ( Eisenberg & Shell, 1986).

So when are children most likely to help? They help when they feel responsible for the person in need, have the needed skills, are happy, and believe they will give up little by helping. When are children least likely to help? They are least likely to help when they feel neither responsible nor capable of helping, are in a bad mood, and believe that helping will entail a large personal sacrifice.

With these guidelines in mind, can you explain why Alejandro and Isabella, the chil- dren in the opening vignette, watched idly as their older brother cried? The last two fac- tors—mood and costs—are not likely relevant. However, the first two factors may explain the failure of Alejandro and Isabella to help their older brother. Our explanation appears on page 175, just before Test Yourself.

So far, we’ve seen that altruistic behavior is determined by children’s skills (such as perspective taking) and by characteristics of situations (such as whether children feel com- petent to help in a particular situation). As we’ll see in the next few pages, whether chil- dren are altruistic is also determined by genetics and by socialization.

The Contribution of Heredity As mentioned on page 173, many scientists believe that prosocial behavior represents an evolutionary adaptation: People who help others are more likely to be helped themselves and thus are more likely to survive and have offspring. According to this argument, we should expect to find evidence for heritability of prosocial behavior, and in fact, that’s the case: Twin studies consistently find that identical twins are more similar in their prosocial behavior than are fraternal twins (Knafo-Noam et al., 2015).

One likely path of genetic influence involves oxytocin, a hormone that influences many social behaviors (e.g., nurturance, empathy, affiliation, and cooperation) and that’s been linked to a few specific genes. In this account, some children may inherit oxytocin- promoting genes that facilitate their prosocial behavior (Carter, 2014). For example, oxy- tocin may enhance perspective-taking and empathy, leading to greater prosocial behavior (Christ, Carlo, & Stoltenberg, 2016).

Genes probably also affect prosocial behavior indirectly by their influence on tem- perament. For example, children who are temperamentally less able to regulate their emo- tions (in part due to heredity) may help less often because they’re so upset by another’s distress that taking action is impossible (Eisenberg et al., 2007). Another temperamental influence may be via inhibition (shyness). Children who are temperamentally shy are often reluctant to help others, particularly people they don’t know well (Young, Fox, & Zahn- Waxler, 1999). Even though shy children realize that others need help and are upset by another person’s apparent distress, their reticence prevents them from translating these feelings into action. Thus, in both cases, children are aware that others need help. But in the first instance, they’re too upset themselves to figure out how to help, and in the second instance, they know how to help but are too inhibited to follow through.

Think About It Suppose some kindergarten children want to raise money for a gift for one of their classmates who is ill. Based on the information presented here, what advice can you give the children as they plan their fundraising?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 175

Socialization of Altruism Dr. Martin Luther King, Jr., said that his pursuit of civil rights for African Americans was particularly influenced by three people: Henry David Thoreau (a 19th-century American philosopher); Mohandas Gandhi (leader of the Indian movement for independence from England); and his father, Dr. Martin Luther King, Sr. As is true of many humanitarians, Dr. King’s prosocial behavior started in childhood at home. But how do parents foster altru- ism in their children? Several factors contribute.

●● Modeling. When children see adults helping and caring for others, they often imitate such prosocial behavior (Eisenberg et al., 2015). Of course, parents are the models to whom children are most continuously exposed, so they exert a powerful influence. When parents are helpful and responsive, their children often imitate them by being cooperative, helpful, sharing, and less critical of others. And when parents do volun- teer work for charitable organizations, their adolescent children are more likely to vol- unteer, too (McGinley et al., 2010).

●● Disciplinary practices. Children behave prosocially more often when their parents are warm and supportive, set guidelines, and provide feedback; in contrast, proso- cial behavior is less common when parenting is harsh, is threatening, and includes frequent physical punishment (Newton, Thompson, & Goodman, 2016). Particularly important is parents’ use of reasoning as a disciplinary tactic with the goal of helping children see how their actions affect others (Farrant et al., 2012). For example, after 4-year-old Rashan grabbed some crayons from a playmate, his mother told Rashan, “You shouldn’t grab things from people. It makes them angry and unhappy. Ask first, and if they say ‘no,’ then you mustn’t take them.”

●● Opportunities to behave prosocially. You need to practice to improve skills, and prosocial behaviors are no exception—children and adolescents are more likely to act prosocially when they’re routinely given the opportunity to help and cooper- ate with others. At home, children can help with household tasks such as cleaning and setting the table. Adolescents can be encouraged to participate in community service, such as working at a food bank or tutoring younger children. Experiences like these help sensitize children and adolescents to the needs of others and allow them to enjoy the satisfaction of helping (Carlo et al., 2007). Such experiences help to explain why Mexican American youth are often more prosocial than are their European American peers: Many Mexican American mothers emphasize the importance of families and expect their children to help with household chores such as caring for siblings, experiences that lead children to be attentive to others’ needs (Knight & Carlo, 2012).

Thus, parents can foster altruism in their youngsters by behaving altruistically them- selves, using reasoning to discipline their children, and encouraging their children to help at home and elsewhere. Situational factors also play a role, and altruism requires perspective taking and empathy. Combining these ingredients, we can give a general account of children’s altruistic behavior. As children get older, their perspective-taking and empathic skills develop, which enables them to see and feel another’s needs. None- theless, children are never invariably altruistic (or, fortunately, invariably nonaltruistic) because particular contexts affect altruistic behavior, too. These factors are summarized in ◗ Table 5.4.

Postscript: Why Didn’t Alejandro and Isabella Help? Here are our explanations. First, neither Alejandro nor Isabella may have felt sufficiently responsible to help because (a) with two children who could help, each child’s feeling of individual responsibility is reduced and (b) younger children are less likely to feel responsible for an older sibling. Second, neither was allowed to play with the iPad, so they wouldn’t feel competent to help: In other words, they had no idea how to fix the damaged the iPad.

Think About It Paula worries that her son Elliot is too selfish and wishes that he were more caring and compassionate. As a parent, what can Paula do to encourage Elliot to be more concerned about the welfare of others?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

176 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Meda and Perry want their 6-year-old daughter, Hope, to pick activities, friends, and ultimately a career based on her interests and abilities rather than on her gender. They have done their best to encourage gender-neutral values and behavior. Therefore, both are astonished that Hope seems to be indistinguishable from other 6-year-olds reared by conventional parents. Hope’s close friends are all girls. When Hope is with her friends, they play house or play with dolls. What seems to be going wrong with Meda and Perry’s plans for a gender-neutral girl?

Family and well-wishers are always eager to know the sex of a newborn. Why are people so interested in a baby’s sex? The answer is that being a “boy” or a “girl” is not simply a

Table 5.4

Factors Contributing to Children’s Prosocial Behavior generAl CAtegory

types of influenCe ChilDren Are more likely to help When . . .

Skills Perspective taking Empathy

they can take another person’s point of view. they feel another person’s emotions.

Situational influences

Feelings of responsibility Feelings of competence Mood Cost of altruism

they feel responsible to the person in need. they feel competent to help. they’re in a good mood. the cost of prosocial behavior is small.

Heredity Temperament they’re not shy and can control their emotions.

Parents’ influence

Modeling Discipline Opportunities

parents behave prosocially themselves. parents reason with them. they practice at home and elsewhere.

Recall 1. Toddlers who are 12 to 15 months old often engage in

play, in which they play separately but look at one another and sometimes communicate verbally.

2. Children with imaginary companions often are more sociable, have more real friends, and have more advanced

.

3. When girls interact, conflicts are typically resolved through ; boys more often resort to intimidation.

4. is the ability to understand and feel another person’s emotions.

5. Contextual influences on prosocial behavior include feelings of responsibility, feelings of competence, , and the costs associated with behaving prosocially.

6. Parents can foster altruism in their youngsters by behaving altruistically themselves, using reasoning to discipline their children, and .

Interpret ●● Why must a full account of children’s prosocial behavior

include an emphasis on skills (e.g., empathy) as well as situa- tions (e.g., whether a child feels responsible)?

Apply ●● How might children’s temperament, which we discussed in

Chapter 3, influence the development of their play with peers?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 5.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are our stereotypes about males and females? How

well do they correspond to actual differences between boys and girls?

●● How do young children learn gender roles?

●● How are gender roles changing? What further changes might the future hold?

5.4 Gender Roles and Gender Identity

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 177

biological distinction. Instead, these terms are associated with dis- tinct social roles. Like a role in a play, a social role is a set of cul- tural guidelines as to how a person should behave, particularly with other people. The roles associated with gender are among the first that children learn, starting in infancy (Shutts, 2015). Youngsters rapidly learn about the behaviors assigned to males and females in their culture. At the same time, they begin to identify with one of these groups. As they do, they take on an identity as a boy or a girl.

In this section, you’ll learn about the “female role” and the “male role” in North America today, and you’ll also discover why Meda and Perry are having so much trouble rearing a gender-neutral girl.

Images of Men and Women: Facts and Fantasy All cultures have gender stereotypes—beliefs and images about males and females that may or may not be true. For example, many men and women believe that males are ratio- nal, active, independent, competitive, and aggressive. At the same time, many men and women claim that females are emotional, passive, dependent, sensitive, and gentle (Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, 2006).

Based on gender stereotypes, we expect males and females to act and feel in particu- lar ways, and we respond to their behavior differently depending on their gender (Smith, Mackie, & Claypool, 2015). For example, if you saw a toddler playing with a doll, you would probably assume that she is a girl, based on her taste in toys. What’s more, your assump- tion would lead you to believe that she plays more quietly and is more readily frightened than a boy (Karraker, Vogel, & Lake, 1995). Once we assume that the child is a girl, our gender stereotypes lead to a host of inferences about behavior and personality.

Learning Gender Stereotypes Children don’t live in a gender-neutral world for long. Although 12-month-old boys and girls look equally at gender-stereotyped toys, 18-month-olds do not: Girls look longer at pictures of dolls than at pictures of trucks, but boys look longer at pictures of trucks (Leaper, 2015). By 4 years of age, children know much about gender-stereotyped activi- ties: They believe that girls play hopscotch but boys play football; girls help bake cookies but boys take out the trash; and women feed babies but men chop wood (Gelman, Taylor, & Nguyen, 2004). They’ve also begun to learn about behaviors and traits that are stereo- typically masculine or feminine. Preschoolers believe that boys are more often aggressive physically but that girls tend to be aggressive verbally (Giles & Heyman, 2005).

During the elementary school years, children expand their knowledge of gender-ste- reotyped traits and behaviors. They learn stereotypes about personality traits—boys are tough and girls are gentle—and about academic subjects—math is for boys and reading is for girls (Cvencek, Meltzoff, & Greenwald, 2011; Kurtz-Costes et al., 2014). By the time they enter middle school, their ideas of gender stereotypes are virtually as well formed as those of adults (Heyman & Legare, 2004).

Beyond the preschool years, children learn that occupations associated with males tend to earn more money and have greater power than those associated with females (Weisgram, Bigler, & Liben, 2010). At some point, children apparently internalize a gen- eral belief about male versus female occupations—something like “Jobs for men are more prestigious than jobs for women.”

As children develop, they also begin to understand that gender stereotypes do not always apply; older children are more willing than younger children to ignore stereotypes when judging other children. For example, told about a boy who likes to play with girls and pretend to iron, preschoolers think he would still want to play with masculine toys. However, by the middle elementary school years, children realize that this boy’s interests

social role Set of cultural guidelines about how one should behave, especially with other people.

gender stereotypes beliefs and images about males and females that are not necessarily true.

Assuming that the child is a girl leads to a host of other inferences about her personal- ity and behavior.

So m

w ay

a/ Fo

to lia

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

178 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

are not stereotypic, and he would rather play with stereotypically feminine toys (Blake- more, 2003). In other words, older children see gender stereotypes as general guidelines for behavior that are not necessarily binding for all boys and girls (Conry-Murray & Turiel, 2012). This developmental trend toward greater flexibility is evident in the study described in the Spotlight on Research feature.

On ResearchSpotlight

Reasoning About Gender-Related Properties

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Do children believe that physical and behavioral proper- ties of boys and girls are inherent and stable? Do they believe, for example, that boys necessarily like to build things and grow up to have a beard? Do they believe that girls necessarily like to play with dolls and grow up to have breasts? Marianne Taylor, Marjo- rie Rhodes, and Susan Gelman (2009) con- ducted a study to answer these questions.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Taylor and colleagues told participants about a baby girl who, immediately after birth, went to live on an island inhabited only by men, including her uncle. She had no contact with females. Participants were then shown a photo of the baby as a “big kid” and were asked several questions about her physi- cal properties (e.g., “Will she grow up to be a mommy or a daddy?”) and some questions about her behavioral properties (e.g., “Will she like to play with a tea set or with trucks?”). They were also told about a baby boy who lives on an island with his aunt and other women and then were asked the same questions.

Who were the participants in the study? The study included 68 5-year-olds, 64 10-year-olds, and 32 college students. At each age, half the children were girls.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental because Taylor and colleagues were interested in the impact of the domain—physical versus behavioral—on participants’ judgments. The study was cross-sectional because it included 5-year- olds, 10-year-olds, and college students, each tested once.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No, the children enjoyed hearing sto- ries about babies growing up on the island.

What were the results? The investigators recorded the percentage of responses that were consistent with the baby’s biological sex (e.g., predicting that the baby girl would like a tea set and want to be a nurse and that she’ll grow up to be a mom and have breasts). The

results are shown in ◗ Figure 5.2, separately for physical and behavioral properties.

Let’s start with the physical properties. There’s little developmental change in these judgments: At all ages, participants expected the baby to acquire physical properties associated with its biological sex. However, the pattern is quite different for behavioral properties, where there is a steady downward developmental trend. Five-year-olds tend to believe that boys and girls will engage in gender-stereotypic behaviors, although they see these as slightly more flex- ible than physical properties. In contrast, adults believe that a boy brought up by women will behave in a feminine stereotypic manner and that a girl brought up by men will behave in a mas- culine stereotypic manner. A final result to note is that participants at all ages claimed greater flexibility in behavioral properties for girls than for boys; they thought that girls were more likely to be influenced in a masculine direction than boys were to be influenced in a feminine direction.

What did the investigators conclude? Taylor and colleagues concluded that “young children treat the concepts of ‘boy’ and ‘girl’ as equivalent to species, in the extent to which features are inborn, inflexible, and intrinsically linked to category membership… . [By] adulthood, participants viewed both male and female behavior as more open to the environment and flexible …” (2009, p. 475).

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Most of the children in this sample were European American, from university towns in the U.S. Midwest. It would be important to see whether children from different backgrounds responded in a similar fashion. In addition, it would be valuable to extend the list of proper- ties to, for example, psychological properties (e.g., personality) to determine whether they’re considered more like physical proper- ties or more like behavioral properties in terms of flexibility.

●◗ Figure 5.2 As children develop, they gradually see behavior and traits as more open to the influence of the environment.

4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20P ro

p or

ti on

o f

ju d

gm en

ts m

at ch

in g

b io

lo gi

ca l

se x 1.0

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0.0

Baby boy: Physical properties

Baby girl: Physical properties

Baby boy: Behavioral properties

Baby girl: Behavioral properties

Age (in years)

With development,

behavioral properties

are seen as more

flexible

Data from Taylor et al. (2009).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 179

Gender-Related Differences So far, we’ve only considered people’s beliefs about differences between males and females, and many of them are false. Research reveals that males and females often do not differ in the ways specified by cultural stereotypes. What are the bona fide differences between males and females? Of course, in addition to the obvious anatomical differences, males are typically larger and stronger than females throughout most of the life span. As infants, boys are more active than girls, and this difference increases during childhood (Alexander & Wilcox, 2012; Saudino, 2009). In contrast, girls are less susceptible to stress and disease and have a lower death rate (Zaslow & Hayes, 1986).

With regard to social roles, activities for males tend to be more strenuous, involve more cooperation with others, and often require travel. Activities for females are usually less demanding physically, are more solitary, and take place closer to home. This division of roles is much the same worldwide (Whiting & Edwards, 1988).

The extent of gender differences in the intellectual and psychosocial arenas remains uncertain. Research suggests differences between males and females in the following areas:

●● Verbal ability. Girls have larger vocabularies than boys and are more talkative (Feld- man et al., 2000; Leaper & Smith, 2004). During elementary school and high school, girls read better than boys, and this difference is found in virtually all industrialized countries (Miller & Halpern, 2014). Finally, more boys are diagnosed with language- related problems such as reading disability (Halpern, 2012).

●● Mathematics. During the elementary school years, girls are usually more advanced than boys in arithmetic and mastery of basic math concepts, a difference that may be a by- product of girls’ greater language skill (Wei et al., 2012). During high school and college, boys used to get higher scores than girls on standardized math tests, but that difference has diminished substantially over the past 25 years; now boys have a negligible advan- tage (Lindberg et al., 2010). This change apparently reflects efforts to encourage girls to pursue mathematics generally and to take more math courses specifically. Around the world, gender differences in math are negligible in countries where, compared with males, females have similar access to education, occupations, and political power, but gender differences remain where females are limited to traditionally feminine-stereo- typed occupations that do not require math skills (Else-Quest, Hyde, & Linn, 2010).

●● Spatial ability. On problems like those in ◗ Figure 5.3, which measure the ability to manipulate visual information mentally, you must decide which figures are rotated variants of the standard shown at the left. Beginning in infancy, boys tend to have better mental-rotation skill than girls (Levine et al., 2016). However, on other spatial tasks, sex differences are smaller or nonexistent (Miller & Halperin, 2014).

●● Memory. Compared with boys and men, girls and women often remember the identity of objects as well as their location more accurately (Miller & Halpern, 2014; Voyer et al., 2007). For example, if shown photos of faces, girls remember those faces more accurately than boys do (Herlitz & Lovén, 2013). In addition, when describing past events (e.g., a trip to a museum, a special visitor at school), girls tend to provide more elaborate and more emotion-filled descriptions (Grysman & Hudson, 2013).

●● Social influence. Girls are more likely than boys to comply with the directions of adults (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Girls and women are also more readily influenced by others in a variety of situations, particularly when they are under group pressure (Becker, 1986; Eagly, Karau, & Makhijani, 1995). However, these gender differences may simply reflect that females value group harmony more than males do and thus seem to give in to others (Miller, Danaher, & Forbes, 1986; Strough & Berg, 2000). For instance, at a meeting to plan a school function, girls are just as likely as boys to recognize the flaws in a bad idea, but girls are more willing to go along simply because they don’t want the group to start arguing.

●● Aggression. In virtually all cultures that have been studied, boys are more physically aggressive than girls, and this is true by 2 years of age (Hay, 2017). This difference con- tinues throughout the life span (Sanson et al., 1993). In contrast, girls are more likely to resort to relational aggression in which they try to hurt others by damaging their rela- tionships with peers. They may call children names, make fun of them, spread rumors about them, or pointedly ignore them (Ostrov & Godleski, 2010).

relational aggression aggression used to hurt others by under- mining their social relationships.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

180 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● Emotional sensitivity and expression. Throughout infancy, childhood, and adolescence, girls identify facial expressions (e.g., happy face versus a sad face) more accurately than boys do (Alexander & Wilcox, 2012; Thompson & Voyer, 2014). In addition, girls are more likely to express happiness and sadness, but boys are more likely to express anger (Chaplin & Aldao, 2013). Finally, for the complex (self-conscious) emotions described on page 165, adolescent girls report experiencing shame and guilt more often than boys do (Else-Quest et al., 2012).

●● Effortful control. During story time in a preschool classroom, many children sit quietly, listening to the teacher read. But if a child is fidgeting or pestering a nearby child, it’s probably a boy. Consistent with this example, girls are more skilled at effortful control: Compared with boys, they are better able to regulate their behavior, to inhibit inappro- priate responding, and to focus their attention (Else-Quest et al., 2006; Gagne, Miller, & Goldsmith, 2013). In addition, boys are far more likely to be diagnosed with atten- tional disorders such as ADHD (Hyde, 2014).

In most other intellectual and social domains, boys and girls are similar. When thinking about areas in which sex differences have been found, keep in mind that gender differences often depend on a person’s experiences (Levine et al., 2016). Also, gender differences may fluctuate over time, reflecting historical change in the contexts of childhood for boys and girls. Finally, each result just described refers to a difference in the average performance of boys and girls. These differences tend to be small, which means that they do not apply to all boys and girls (Hyde, 2014). For example, many girls have greater spatial ability than some boys, and many boys are more susceptible to social influence than are some girls.

Gender Typing Folklore holds that parents and other adults—teachers and television characters, for exam- ple—directly shape children’s behavior toward the roles associated with their sex. Boys are rewarded for boyish behavior and punished for girlish behavior. The folklore even has a

●◗ Figure 5.3 On spatial ability tasks, which involve visualizing information in different orientations, males tend to respond more rapidly and more accurately than females.

A B C ED

From “Process Analysis of Spatial Aptitude,” by J. W. Pellegrino and R. V. Kail, (1982) in R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the Psychology of Human Intel- ligence, Vol. 1, p. 316.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 181

theoretical basis. According to social cognitive theorists Albert Bandura (1977, 1986; Ban- dura & Bussey, 2004) and Walter Mischel (1970), children learn gender roles in much the same way they learn other social behaviors: by watching the world around them and learn- ing the outcomes of different actions. Thus, parents and others shape appropriate gender roles in children, and children learn what their culture considers appropriate behavior for males and females by simply watching how adults and peers act.

How well does research support social learning theory? Overall, parents often treat sons and daughters similarly. Parents interact equally with sons and daughters, are equally warm to both, encourage both sons and daughters to be independent, and discipline sons and daughters similarly (Hallers-Haalboom et al., 2016; Lytton & Romney, 1991). And, although mothers are more engaged than fathers in their children’s education, mothers’ and fathers’ involvement has comparable impact on boys’ and girls’ success in school (Kim & Hill, 2015). However, in behavior related to gender roles, parents respond differently to sons and daughters (Lytton & Romney, 1991). Activities such as playing with dolls, dress- ing up, or helping an adult are encouraged more often in daughters than in sons; rough- and-tumble play and playing with blocks are encouraged more in sons than in daughters. Parents tolerate mild aggression in sons to a greater degree than in daughters (Martin & Ross, 2005), and following the birth of a child, especially a firstborn, they become more traditional in gender-related attitudes (Katz-Wise, Priess, & Hyde, 2010).

Fathers are more likely than mothers to treat sons and daughters differently. Fathers often encourage gender-related play and they are more likely to accept dependence in their daughters (Snow, Jacklin, & Maccoby, 1983). A father, for example, may urge his frightened young son to jump off the diving board (“Be a man!”) but not insist that his daughter do so (“That’s okay, honey”). Apparently mothers are more likely to respond based on their knowledge of the needs of individual children, but fathers respond based on gender ste- reotypes. A mother responds to her son, knowing that he’s smart but unsure of himself; a father may respond based on what he thinks boys generally should be like.

Of course, adults differ in their views on the relative rights and roles of males and females. Some have very traditional views and believe that men should be hired pref- erentially for some jobs and that it’s more important for sons than daughters to attend college; others have more gender-neutral views and believe that women should have the same business and professional opportunities as men and that daughters should have the same educational opportunities as sons. Not surprisingly, parents convey these attitudes to their children (Crouter et al., 2007). Meta-analyses show that chil- dren’s gender-related interests, attitudes, and self-concepts are more traditional when their parents have traditional views and are more gender-neutral when their parents have nontraditional views (Degner & Dalege, 2013; Tenenbaum & Leaper, 2002).

Peers are also influential. By 3 years, most children’s play shows the impact of gender ste- reotypes—boys prefer blocks and trucks, whereas girls prefer tea sets and dolls—and youngsters are critical of peers who engage in gender-inappropri- ate play (Hines, 2015). This is particularly true of boys who like feminine toys or who choose femi- nine activities. Boys who play with dolls and girls (like the one in the photo on page 182) who play with trucks will be ignored, teased, or ridiculed by their peers, but a boy will receive harsher treatment than a girl (Levy, Taylor, & Gelman, 1995). Once children learn rules about gender-typical play, they often harshly punish peers who violate those rules.

Peers influence gender roles in another way, too. We’ve seen that by 2 or 3 years of age, chil- dren most often play with same-sex peers (Leaper, 2015). This early segregation of playmates based on a child’s sex means that boys learn primarily from

Think About It The women’s liberation movement became a powerful social force in North America during the 1960s. Describe how you might do research to determine whether the movement has changed the gender roles that children learn.

Fathers are more likely than mothers to encourage their children’s gender-related play.

Im ag

e So

ur ce

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

182 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

gender-schema theory theory that states that children want to learn more about an activity only after first deciding whether it is masculine or feminine.

boys and girls from girls. This helps solidify a youngster’s emerging sense of mem- bership in a particular gender group and sharpens the contrast between their own gender and the other gender. Thus, through encouraging words, critical looks, and other forms of praise and punishment, other people influence boys and girls to behave differently.

Gender Identity If you were to listen to a typical conversation between two preschoolers, you might hear something like this:

maria: When I grow up, I’m going to be a singer. juanita: When I grow up, I’m going to be a papa. maria: No, you can’t be a papa—you’ll be a mama. juanita: No, I wanna be a papa. maria: You can’t be a papa. Only boys can be papas, and you’re a girl!

Obviously, Maria’s understanding of gender is more developed than Juanita’s. How can we explain these differences? According to Lawrence Kohlberg (1966; Kohl- berg & Ullian, 1974), full understanding of gender is said to develop gradually in three steps. Toddlers know that they are either boys or girls and label themselves accord- ingly. During the preschool years, children begin to understand that gender is stable—

boys become men, and girls become women. Yet at this age, they believe that a girl who wears her hair like a boy will become a boy and that a boy who plays with dolls will become a girl. Not until about 5 or 6 years do children come to understand that maleness and femaleness do not change over situations or according to personal wishes. They under- stand that a child’s sex is unaffected by the clothing that a child wears or the toys that a child likes. At this point, children begin to identify with one group and to develop a gender identity—a sense of the self as a male or a female.

As soon as children identify themselves as a boy or girl, they begin learning about gender-typical behavior. Explaining how this learning takes places is the aim of a theory proposed by Carol Martin (Martin et al., 1999; Martin & Ruble, 2004) (see ◗ Figure 5.4). In gender-schema theory, children first decide if an object, an activity, or a behavior is associated with females or males; then they use this information to decide whether they should learn more about the object, activity, or behavior. That is, once children know their gender, they pay attention primarily to experiences and events that are gender-appropriate (Martin & Halverson, 1987). According to gender-schema theory, a preschool boy watch- ing a group of girls playing in sand will decide that playing in sand is for girls and that because he is a boy, playing in sand is not for him. Seeing a group of older boys playing

gender identity Sense of oneself as male or a female.

Preschoolers discourage peers from cross- gender play.

Ku tt

ig -P

eo pl

e- 2/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

●◗ Figure 5.4 According to gender-schema theory, children first decide if an object, an activity, or a behavior is for females or males and then learn more about the objects, activities, or behaviors that are appropriate for their own gender.

Who for ?

Avoid

Approach and

learn

For boys

I am a girl

For girls

Not for me

For me

Doll

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 183

football, he will decide that football is for boys and that because he is a boy, football is acceptable and he should learn more about it.

According to gender-schema theory, after children acquire a gender identity, they seem to view the world through special glasses that allow only gender-typical activities to be in focus (Liben & Bigler, 2002). For example, children who have acquired a gender iden- tity much prefer toys that are liked by others of their sex (Shutts, Banaji, & Spelke, 2010). As Martin and Ruble (2004) put it: “Children are gender detectives who search for cues about gender—who should or should not engage in a particular activity, who can play with whom, and why girls and boys are different” (p. 67).

This account also helps to explain why many 3- to 5-year-old girls adore wearing exceedingly feminine clothing such as pink frilly dresses, yet by the elementary school years they become tomboys, wearing pants, playing sports, and avoiding feminine-stereo- typed toys and activities (Bailey, Bechtold, & Berenbaum, 2002; Halim, 2016). A young girl’s love of frilly dresses likely reflects the pursuit of salient symbols that match her newly acquired gender identity. However, by school age, children know that gender roles are flex- ible (e.g., wearing pants doesn’t make a girl into boy, wearing nail polish doesn’t make a boy into a girl) and that masculine roles often have more status. Consequently, elementary school girls realize that by being a tomboy, they can have some of the status associated with being a boy without jeopardizing their identity as a girl (Halim, 2016).

This selective viewing of the world explains a great deal about children’s learning of gender roles, but one final important element needs to be considered: biology.

Biological Influences Most child development researchers agree that biology contributes to gender roles and gender identity. Evolutionary developmental psychology, for example, reminds us that men and women performed vastly different roles for much of human history. Women were more invested in child rearing, and men were more invested in providing impor- tant resources (e.g., food and protection) for their offspring (Geary, 2002). In adapting to these roles, different traits and behaviors evolved for men and women. For example, men became more aggressive because that was adaptive in helping them ward off predators.

If gender roles are based in part on our evolutionary heritage, then behavior genetic research should show the impact of heredity on gender-role learning. Indeed, twin studies show a substantial hereditary impact on gender-role learning (Iervolino et al., 2005). For identical twins, if one strongly prefers sex-typical toys and activities, the other one usu- ally does, too. Fraternal twins are also similar in their preference for sex-typical toys and activities, but not to the extent of identical twins.

Twin studies point to a biological basis for gender-role learning, but the studies don’t tell us what factors are responsible. Some scientists believe that the sex hormones are  key players; consistent with this idea, for both boys and girls, exposure to testos- terone during prenatal development leads to greater interest in masculine sex-typed activities during the elementary school years (Hines, 2015). This link is particularly vivid in studies of children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), a genetic disorder in which, beginning in prenatal development, the adrenal glands secrete large amounts of androgen. During childhood and adolescence, girls with CAH prefer masculine activi- ties (such as playing with cars instead of dolls) and male playmates to a much greater extent than girls not exposed to these amounts of androgen, despite strong encourage- ment from parents to play with feminine toys (Hines, 2015). Apparently the androgen affects the prenatal development of brain regions critical for masculine and feminine gender-role behavior.

Perhaps the most accurate conclusion to draw is that biology, the socializing influence of people, and the child’s own efforts to understand gender-typical behavior all contribute to gender roles and differences. Recognizing the interactive nature of these influences on gender learning also enables us to better understand how gender roles are changing today, which we consider next.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

184 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

Evolving Gender Roles Gender roles are not etched in stone; they change with the times. In the United States, the range of acceptable roles for girls and boys and women and men has never been greater than today (Liben, 2016). For example, some fathers stay home to be primary caregivers for children, and some mothers work full-time as sole support for the family. What is the impact of these changes on gender roles? Some insights come from the results of the Family Lifestyles Project (Weisner, Garnier, & Loucky, 1994; Weisner & Wilson-Mitchell, 1990). This research has examined families in which the adults were members of the coun- terculture of the 1960s and 1970s. Some of the families are deeply committed to living their own lives and to rearing their children without traditional gender stereotypes. In these families, men and women share the household, financial, and child care tasks.

The results of this project show that parents like Meda and Perry from the opening vignette can influence some aspects of gender stereotyping more readily than others. On the one hand, children in these families tend to have same-sex friends and to like sex-typed activities: The boys enjoy physical play, and the girls enjoy drawing and reading. On the other hand, the children have few stereotypes concerning occupations: They agree that girls can be president of the United States and drive trucks and that boys can be nurses and secretaries. They also have fewer sex-typed attitudes about the use of objects. They claim that boys and girls are equally likely to use an iron, a shovel, a hammer and nails, and a needle and thread.

Apparently some features of gender roles and identities are more readily influenced by experience than others. This is as it should be. For most of our history as a species, Homo sapiens have existed in small groups of families, hunting animals and gathering vegetation. Women have given birth to the chil- dren and cared for them. Over the course of human history, it has been adap- tive for women to be caring and nurturing because this increases the odds of a secure attachment and, ultimately, the survival of the infant. Men’s responsibil- ities have included protecting the family unit from predators and hunting with other males, roles for which physical strength and aggressiveness are crucial.

Circumstances of life in the 21st century are, of course, substantially dif- ferent: Often both men and women are employed outside the home and both men and women care for children (Eagly & Wood, 2013). Nevertheless, the cultural changes of the past few decades cannot erase hundreds of thousands of years of evolutionary history. We should not be surprised that boys and girls play differently, that girls tend to be more supportive in their interactions with others, and that boys are usually more aggressive physically.

Gender roles continue to evolve, and the range of acceptable roles for men and women continues to expand.

Recall 1. are beliefs and images about males and

females that may or may not be true.

2. Research on intellectual functioning and social behavior has revealed sex differences in verbal ability, , memory, social influence, aggression, emotional sensitivity and effortful control.

3. may be particularly influential in teach- ing gender roles because they more often treat sons and daughters differently.

4. According to Martin’s theory, children focus on gender-typi- cal activities after they .

5. Children studied in the Family Lifestyles Project, whose par- ents were members of the counterculture of the 1960s and

1970s, had traditional gender-related views toward friends and .

Interpret ●● How do the different forces in the biopsychosocial framework

contribute to the development of gender roles?

Apply ●● What advice would you give to a mother who wants her

daughter to grow to be gender-free in her attitudes, beliefs, and aspirations?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 5.4

D av

id R

. F ra

zi er

P ho

to lib

ra ry

, I nc

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 5 : E N T E R I N G T H E S O C I A L W O R L D 185

5.1 Beginnings: Trust and Attachment

What are Erikson’s first three stages of psychosocial development?

●● In Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development, individu- als face certain psychosocial crises at different phases in development. The crisis of infancy is to establish a bal- ance between trust and mistrust of the world, producing hope; between 1 and 3 years of age, youngsters must blend autonomy and shame to produce will; and between 3 and 5 years, initiative and guilt must be balanced to achieve purpose.

how do infants become emotionally attached to mother, father, and other significant people in their lives?

●● Attachment is an enduring socioemotional relationship between infant and parent. For both adults and infants, many of the behaviors that contribute to the formation of attachment are biologically programmed. Bowlby’s theory of attachment is rooted in evolutionary psychology and describes four stages in the development of attachment: preattachment, attachment in the making, true attachment, and reciprocal relationships.

What are the different varieties of attachment relationships, how do they arise, and what are their consequences?

●● Research with the Strange Situation, in which infant and mother are separated briefly, reveals four primary forms of attachment. Most common is a secure attachment in which infants have complete trust in the mother. Less com- mon are three types of attachment relationships in which this trust is lacking. In avoidant relationships, infants deal with the lack of trust by ignoring their mother; in resistant relationships, infants often seem angry with her; in disor- ganized relationships, infants do not appear to understand their mother’s absence.

●● Children who have had secure attachment relationships during infancy tend to be more skilled socially and are at less risk for externalizing disorders. Secure attachment is most likely to occur when mothers respond sensitively and consistently to their infants’ needs.

●● Responsive caregiving results in infants developing an internal working model in which they expect parents will try to meet their needs.

Is attachment jeopardized when parents of young children are employed outside the home?

●● Many U.S. children are cared for at home by a father or another relative, in a day care provider’s home, or in a day care center. Infants and young children are not harmed by

such care as long as it is of high quality and parents remain responsive to their children.

5.2 Emerging Emotions

At what age do children begin to express basic emotions?

●● Basic emotions—which include joy, anger, and fear— emerge in the first year. Fear first appears in infancy as stranger wariness.

What are complex emotions, and when do they develop?

●● Complex emotions have an evaluative component and include guilt, embarrassment, and pride. They appear between 18 and 24 months and require more sophisticated cognitive skills than basic emotions. Cultures differ in the rules for expressing emotions and in the situations that elicit particular emotions.

When do children begin to understand other people’s emotions? how do they use this information to guide their own behavior?

●● By 6 months, infants have begun to recognize the emotions associated with different facial expressions. They use this information to help them evaluate unfamiliar situations. Beyond infancy, children learn more about the causes of different emotions.

●● Infants use simple strategies to regulate emotions such as fear. As children grow, they become better skilled at regu- lating their emotions. Children who do not regulate emo- tions well tend to have problems interacting with others.

5.3 Interacting with Others

When do youngsters first begin to play with each other? how does play change during infancy and the preschool years?

●● Even infants notice and respond to one another, but the first real interactions (at about 12 to 15 months) take the form of parallel play in which toddlers play alone while watching each other. A few months later, simple social play emerges in which toddlers engage in similar activities and interact with one another. At about 2 years of age, coop- erative play organized around a theme becomes common. Make-believe play is also common and becomes more sophisticated as children develop. Most forms of solitary play are harmless.

What determines whether children help one another?

●● Prosocial behaviors such as helping and sharing are more common in children who understand (by perspective tak- ing) and experience (by empathy) another’s feelings.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

186 pA RT O N E : P R E N ATA L D E V E L O P M E N T, I N FA N C Y, A N D E A R LY C H I L D H O O D

●● Prosocial behavior is more likely when children feel respon- sible for the person in distress. Also, children help more often when they believe that they have the skills needed, when they are feeling happy or successful, and when they perceive that the costs of helping are small.

●● Twin studies show that prosocial behavior is influenced by heredity, probably through its impact on oxytocin, behav- ioral control (inhibition), and emotion regulation.

●● Parents can foster altruism in their youngsters by behaving altruistically themselves, using reasoning to discipline their children, and encouraging their children to help at home and elsewhere.

5.4 Gender Roles and Gender Identity

What are stereotypes about males and females? how well do they correspond to actual differences between boys and girls?

●● Gender stereotypes are beliefs about males and females that are often used to make inferences about a person that are based solely on his or her gender; by 4 years of age, children know these stereotypes well.

●● Studies of gender differences reveal that girls have greater verbal skill and memory but that boys have greater spatial skill. Differences in math are negligible when females have access to education and occupations where math is valu- able. Girls are better able to interpret emotions, are more prone to social influence, and have better effortful control,

but boys are more aggressive. These differences vary based on a number of factors, including the historical period.

how do young children learn gender roles?

●● Parents treat sons and daughters similarly, except in sex- typed activities. Fathers may be particularly important in sex typing because they are more likely to treat sons and daughters differently.

●● Children gradually learn that gender is stable over time and cannot be changed according to personal wishes. After children understand gender, they begin to learn gender- typical behavior. According to gender-schema theory, chil- dren learn about gender by paying attention to behaviors of members of their own sex and ignoring behaviors of mem- bers of the other sex.

●● Evolutionary developmental psychology reminds us that different roles for males and females caused different traits and behaviors to evolve for men and women. The idea that biology influences some aspects of gender roles is also sup- ported by research on females exposed to male hormones during prenatal development.

how are gender roles changing?

●● Gender roles have changed considerably in the past 50 years. However, studies of nontraditional families indicate that some components of gender stereotypes are more readily changed than others.

5.1 1. Erik Erikson 2. hope 3. 6 or 7 4. avoidant insecure 5. responding consistently and appropriately 6. secure 7. insensitive, unre- sponsive mothering 5.2 1. an overt behavior 2. wariness of strangers 3. infants master creeping and crawling 4. complex emotions require more advanced cognitive skills 5. to direct their own behavior (e.g., deciding if an unfamiliar situation is safe or frightening) 6. moving closer to a parent 5.3 1. parallel 2. theory of mind 3. discussion and compromise 4. Empathy 5. mood 6. providing children with opportunities to practice being altruistic 5.4 1. Gender stereotypes 2. spatial ability 3. Fathers 4. understand gender 5. preferred activities

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

hope (156) will (157) purpose (157) evolutionary psychology (157) attachment (157) secure attachment (158) avoidant attachment (159) resistant attachment (159) disorganized (disoriented)

attachment (159)

internal working model (160) basic emotions (164) social smiles (164) stranger wariness (164) social referencing (167) parallel play (169) simple social play (169) cooperative play (169) enabling actions (171) constricting actions (171)

prosocial behavior (172) altruism (173) empathy (173) social role (177) gender stereotypes (177) relational aggression (179) gender identity (182) gender-schema theory (182)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G ag

lia rd

iIm ag

es /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

189

Every fall, American 5- and 6-year-olds trot off to kindergarten, starting an educational journey that lasts 13 or more years. As the journey begins,

many children can read only a few words and know

little math; by the end, most can read complete

books and many have learned algebra and geometry.

This mastery of complex academic skills is possible

because of profound changes in children’s thinking,

changes described in the first section of this chapter.

For most American school children, intelligence

and aptitude tests are a common part of their educa-

tional travels. In the second section of this chapter,

you’ll see what tests measure and why some children

get lower scores on tests. In the third section, you’ll

discover how tests are often used to identify school

children with atypical or special needs.

Next, we look at the way students learn to read,

write, and do math. In this section, you’ll discover

some of the educational practices that seem to fos-

ter students’ learning.

Finally, children’s growing cognitive skills, when

coupled with improved motor coordination, enable them to participate in

sports. In the last section, we’ll look at such participation and the physical

changes that make it possible.

Off to School Cognitive and Physical Development in Middle Childhood

6.1 Cognitive Development 190 More Sophisticated Thinking: Piaget’s Version 190

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Combinatorial Reasoning Goes to the Races 191

Information-Processing Strategies for Learning and Remembering 192

6.2 Aptitudes for School 194 Theories of Intelligence 195 Binet and the Development of Intelligence Testing 198 Do Tests Work? 199 Hereditary and Environmental Factors 200 The Impact of Ethnicity and Socioeconomic Status 201

6.3 Special Children, Special Needs 204 Gifted Children 204 Children with Disability 204

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Improving Children’s Knowledge of the Structure of Words Enhances Their Reading Comprehension 206

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder 208

6.4 Academic Skills 209 Reading 209 Writing 212 Math Skills 213

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Shin-Ying Loves School 214

Effective Schools, Effective Teachers 216

6.5 Physical Development 218 Growth 218 Development of Motor Skills 219 Physical Fitness 220 Participating in Sports 220

    Summary 222

    Key Terms 224

6

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

190 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Adrian, a sixth grader in middle school, just took his first social studies test—and failed. He is shocked because he’d always received As and Bs in elementary school. Adrian realizes that glancing through the textbook chapter once before a test is probably not going to work in middle school, but he’s not sure what else he should be doing.

Adrian’s cognitive skills far surpass those of the infants and toddlers we examined in Chapter 4. Yet the vignette shows that his skills aren’t flawless. In this section, we’ll learn more about cognitive growth in childhood, first by considering the perspective of Piaget’s theory and then by considering the information-processing account.

More Sophisticated Thinking: Piaget’s Version You probably remember Piaget’s stages of thinking that we described in Chapters 1 and 4. The first two stages, sensorimotor and preoperational thinking, characterize infancy and the preschool years. In the next few pages, we describe the remaining two stages, the con- crete-operational and formal-operational stages, which apply to school-age children and adolescents.

The Concrete-Operational Period Let’s start by reviewing three important limits of preoperational thinking described in Chapter 4.

●● Preschoolers are egocentric, believing that others see the world as they do. ●● Preschoolers sometimes confuse appearances with reality. ●● Preschoolers are unable to reverse their thinking.

None of these limits applies to children in the concrete-operational stage, which extends from approximately 7 to 11 years. Egocentrism wanes gradually as youngsters have more experiences with other people who assert their own perspectives on the world. Understanding that events can be interpreted in different ways leads to the realization that appearances can be deceiving. Also, thought can be reversed because school-age children have acquired mental operations, actions that can be performed on objects or ideas and that consistently yield a result. Recall from Chapter 4 that on the conservation task, concrete-operational children realize that the amount of liquid is the same after it has been poured into a different beaker—pointing out that the pouring can always be reversed.

As the name implies, concrete-operational thinking is limited to the tangible and real, to the here and now. The concrete-operational youngster takes “an earthbound, concrete, practical-minded sort of problem-solving approach” (Flavell, 1985, p. 98). Thinking abstractly and hypothetically is beyond the ability of concrete-operational children; these skills are acquired in the formal-operational period, as you’ll see in the next section.

The Formal-Operational Period With the onset of the formal-operational period, which extends from roughly age 11 into adulthood, children and adolescents apply psychological operations to abstract entities, too; they are able to think hypothetically and reason abstractly (Siegler & Alibali, 2005).

mental operations Cognitive actions that can be performed on objects or ideas.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the distinguishing characteristics of thought dur-

ing Piaget’s concrete-operational and formal-operational stages?

●● How do children use strategies and monitoring to improve learning and remembering?

6.1 Cognitive Development

Think About It Piaget and Erikson propose unique stages for ages 7 to 11 years. How similar are the stages they propose? How do they differ?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 191

To illustrate these differences, let’s look at problem-solving, where formal-operational adolescents often take a very different approach from concrete-operational children. In one of Piaget’s experiments (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958), children and adolescents were presented with several flasks, each containing what appeared to be the same clear liquid. They were told that one combination of the clear liquids would produce a blue liquid, and they were asked to determine the necessary combination. A typical concrete-operational youngster plunged right in, mixing different liquids haphazardly. In contrast, formal- operational adolescents understood that setting up the problem in abstract terms is the key. The problem is not really about pouring liquids, but about testing all possible combinations. So, formal-operational teens mixed liquid from the first flask with liquids from each of the other flasks. When these combinations failed to produce a blue liquid, the teen- agers concluded that the liquid in the first flask is not essential and mixed the liquid in the second flask with each of the remaining liquids. Formal-operational thinkers continued in this manner until they found the critical pair that produces the blue liquid. For adolescents, the problem does not involve the concrete acts of pouring and mixing. Instead, they under- stand that it involves identifying possible combinations and then evaluating each one. This sort of adolescent combinatorial reasoning is illustrated in the Real People feature.

Adolescents’ more sophisticated thinking is also shown in their ability to make appro- priate conclusions from facts, which is known as deductive reasoning. Suppose we tell a person the following:

If you hit a glass with a hammer, the glass will break.

If you then tell the person “You hit the glass with a hammer,” he or she would conclude, of course, that “the glass will break,” a conclusion that formal-operational adolescents do reach.

Concrete-operational youngsters sometimes reach this conclusion too—but based on their experience, not because the conclusion is logically necessary. To see the difference, imagine that the statement now is:

If you hit a glass with a feather, the glass will break.

Told “You hit the glass with a feather,” the conclusion “the glass will break” follows just as logically as it did in the first example. In this instance, however, the conclusion goes against what experience tells us is true. Concrete-operational 10-year-olds resist reaching conclusions that are contrary to known facts, whereas formal-operational 15-year-olds often reach such conclusions (De Neys & Everaerts, 2008). Formal-operational teenagers

deductive reasoning Drawing conclusions from facts; character- istic of formal-operational thought.

Concrete-operational thinkers often solve problems haphazardly, but formal-operational thinkers more often set up problems in abstract terms.

A bl

ei m

ag es

/J up

ite r I

m ag

es

As a 15-year-old, Robert Kail delivered the Indianapolis Star. In the spring of 1965, the newspaper announced a contest for all newspaper carriers. The carrier who created the most words from the letters contained in the words SAFE RACE would win two tickets to the Indianapolis 500 auto race.

Kail realized that this was a problem in combinatorial reasoning. He needed to create

all possible combinations of letters, then look them up in a dictionary. Following this proce- dure, he had to win (or, at worst, tie). So, he created exhaustive lists of possible words, beginning with each of the letters individually, then all possible combinations of two letters, and working his way up to all possible com- binations of all eight letters (e.g., SCAREEFA, SCAREEAF). This was monotonous enough,

but no more so than the next step: looking up all those possible words in a dictionary. (Remember, this was in the days before com- puterized spell-checkers.) Weeks later, he had generated 126 words. As predicted, a few months later, he learned that he had won the contest. Combinatorial reasoning has its payoffs!

Real People Applying Human Development

Combinatorial Reasoning Goes to the Races

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

192 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

understand that these problems are about abstractions that need not correspond to real- world relations. In contrast, concrete-operational youngsters reach conclusions based on their knowledge of the world.

Comments on Piaget’s View We mentioned in Chapter 4 that although Piaget’s theory provides our single most compre- hensive theory of cognitive development, it has some shortcomings. Specifically, it overes- timates adolescents’ cognitive competence, is vague concerning processes of change, does not account for variability in children’s performance, and undervalues the influence of the sociocultural environment. Because of these limits to Piaget’s theory, we need to look at other approaches to understand mental development during childhood and adolescence. In the next few pages, we’ll focus on the information-processing approach that we first examined in Chapter 4.

Information-Processing Strategies for Learning and Remembering You’ll remember from Chapter 4 that information-processing psychologists believe that cognitive development proceeds by increases in the efficiency with which children pro- cess information. In other words, just as personal computers have become progressively more sophisticated in their hardware and software, information-processing psychologists believe that cognitive development reflects change in mental computing power, including mental hardware and mental software.

One of the key issues in this approach concerns the means by which children store information in permanent memory and retrieve it when needed later. According to information-processing psychologists, most human thought takes place in working memory, where a relatively small number of thoughts and ideas can be stored briefly. As you read these sentences, for example, the information is stored in working memory. However, as you read additional sentences, they displace the contents of sentences you read earlier. For you to learn this information, it must be transferred to long-term memory, a permanent storehouse of knowledge that has unlimited capacity. If information you read is not trans- ferred to long-term memory, it is lost.

Memory Strategies How do you try to learn the information in this book or your other textbooks? If you’re like many college students, you probably use some combination of highlighting key sentences, outlining chapters, taking notes, writing summaries, and testing yourself. These are all effective learning strategies that make it easier for you to store text information in long- term memory.

Children begin to use simple strategies fairly early. For example, 7- or 8-year-olds use rehearsal, a strategy of repetitively naming information that is to be remembered. As chil-

dren grow older, they learn other memory strategies. One memory strategy is organization—structuring information to be remembered so that related information is placed together. For example, a sixth grader trying to remember major battles of the American Civil War could organize them geographically (e.g., Shiloh and Fort Donelson in Tennessee, Antietam and Monocacy in Maryland).

Another memory strategy is elaboration—embellishing information to be remem- bered to make it more memorable. To see elaboration in action, imagine a child who can never remember if the second syllable of rehearsal is spelled her (as it sounds) or hear. The child could remember the correct spelling by reminding herself that rehearsal is like re-hear-ing. Thus, thinking about the derivation of rehearsal makes it easier to remember how to spell it. Finally, as children grow, they’re also more likely to use external aids to memory, such as making notes and writing down information on calendars so they won’t forget future events (Eskritt & McLeod, 2008).

working memory type of memory in which a small number of items can be stored briefly.

long-term memory Permanent storehouse for memories that has unlimited capacity.

organization as applied to children’s memory, a strategy in which information to be remembered is structured so that related information is placed together.

elaboration Memory strategy in which information is embellished to make it more memorable.

As children grow, they make more use of memory aids, such as taking notes.

JP C-

PR O

D /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 193

Metacognition Just as a well-stocked toolbox isn’t valuable if you don’t know how to use the tools, memory strategies aren’t much good unless children know when to use them. For example, rehearsal is a great strategy for remembering phone numbers but lousy for remembering amendments to the U.S. Constitution or the plot of Hamlet. During the elementary school years and ado- lescence, children gradually learn to identify different kinds of memory problems and  the memory strategies most appropriate to each. For example, when reading a textbook or watching a television newscast, outlining or writing a summary are good strategies because they identify the main points and organize them. Children gradually become more skilled at selecting appropriate strategies, but even high school students do not always use effective learning strategies when they should (Grammer et al., 2011; Schneider, 2015).

After children choose a memory strategy, they need to monitor its effectiveness. That is, they need to decide if the strategy is working. If it’s not, they need to begin anew, reana- lyzing the memory task to select a better approach. If the strategy is working, they should determine the portion of the material they have not yet mastered and concentrate their efforts there. Monitoring improves gradually with age. Even preschool children can distin- guish what they know from what they don’t (Ghetti, Hembacher, & Coughlin, 2013), but older children and adolescents do so more accurately (Bjorklund, 2005).

Diagnosing memory problems accurately and monitoring the effectiveness of memory strategies are two important elements of metamemory, which refers to a child’s intuitive under- standing of memory. That is, as children develop, they learn more about how memory oper- ates and devise naïve theories of memory that represent an extension of the theory of mind described on pages 111–112 (Schneider, 2015). For example, children learn that memory is fallible (i.e., they sometimes forget!) and that some types of memory tasks are easier than oth- ers (e.g., remembering the main idea of the Gettysburg address is simpler than remembering it word for word). This growing knowledge of memory helps children use memory strategies more effectively, just as an experienced carpenter’s accumulated knowledge of wood tells her when to use nails, screws, or glue to join two boards (Ghetti & Lee, 2011).

Of course, children’s growing understanding of memory is paralleled by their increased understanding of all cognitive processes. Such knowledge and awareness of cognitive pro- cesses is called metacognitive knowledge. Metacognitive knowledge grows rapidly during the elementary school years: Children come to know much about perception, attention, intentions, knowledge, and thinking (Flavell, 2000; McCormick, 2003). For example, school-age children know that sometimes they deliberately direct their attention—as in searching for a parent’s face in a crowd—but that sometimes events capture attention—as with an unexpected clap of thunder (Parault & Schwanenflugel, 2000).

One of the most important features of children’s metacognitive knowledge is their understanding of the connections among goals, strategies, monitoring, and outcomes, shown in ◗ Figure 6.1. Children come to realize that for a broad spectrum of tasks— ranging from learning words in a spelling list to learning to spike a volleyball to learning to get along with an overly talkative classmate seated nearby—they need to regulate their learning by understanding the goal and selecting a means to achieve that goal. Then they determine whether the chosen method is working. Effective cognitive self-regulation— that is, skill at identifying goals, selecting effective strategies, and monitoring accurately—is a characteristic of successful students (Usher & Pajares, 2009; Zimmerman, 2001). A stu- dent may decide that writing each spelling word twice before the test is a good way to get all the words right. When the student gets only 70% correct on the first test, he or she switches to a new strategy (e.g., writing each word four times and writing its definition), showing the adaptive nature of cognitive processes in self-regulated learners.

Some students do not master these learning strategies spontaneously, but they may acquire them when teachers emphasize them in class (Grammer, Coffman, & Ornstein, 2013; Ornstein et al., 2010). In addition, several programs teach students strategies for studying more effectively (Pressley, 2002). For example, teachers demonstrate several strategies that promote greater reading comprehension, including selecting a goal for reading, making a mental picture of what’s going on in the text, periodically predicting what will happen next, and summarizing aloud what’s happened so far. Children practice

metamemory Person’s informal understanding of memory; includes the ability to diagnose memory problems accurately and to monitor the effectiveness of memory strategies.

metacognitive knowledge a person’s knowledge and awareness of cognitive processes.

cognitive self-regulation skill at identifying goals, selecting effective strategies, and monitoring accurately; a characteristic of successful students.

Think About It Which elements of the biopsychosocial framework are emphasized in the infor- mation-processing approach to cognitive development?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

194 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

these strategies separately and as part of a reading “tool kit.” Empowered with reading strategies such as these, students’ understanding of text is deeper and they typically obtain greater scores on standardized tests of reading comprehension (Pressley & Hilden, 2006).

●◗ Figure 6.1 Effective learning involves understand- ing the goals of the task, selecting an appropriate strategy, and monitor- ing the effectiveness of the chosen strategy.

Determine goal

Select strategy

Focus on unlearned material

Use strategy

Monitor strategy

Ineffective Effective

recall 1. During Piaget’s _______________ stage, children are first able

to represent objects mentally in different ways and to per- form mental operations.

2. Hypothetical and deductive reasoning are characteristic of children in Piaget’s _______________ stage.

3. Children and adolescents often select a memory strategy after they have _______________.

4. The term _______________ refers to periodic evaluation of a strategy to determine whether it is working.

Interpret ●● Do developmental improvements in memory strategies and

metacognition emphasize nature, nurture, or both? How?

Apply ●● Formal-operational adolescents are able to reason abstractly.

How might this ability help them use the study skills shown in Figure 6.1 more effectively?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 6.1

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is the nature of intelligence?

●● Why were intelligence tests first developed? What are their features?

●● How well do intelligence tests work?

●● How do heredity and environment influence intelligence?

●● How and why do test scores vary for different racial and ethnic groups?

6.2 Aptitudes for School

Diana is an eager fourth-grade teacher who loves history. Consequently, every year she’s frustrated when she teaches a unit on the American Civil War. Although she’s passionate about the subject, her enthusiasm is not contagious. Instead, her students’ eyes glaze over and she can see young minds drifting off. And, of course, they never seem to grasp the war’s historical significance. Diana wishes she could teach this unit differently, in a way that would engage her students more effectively.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 195

Before you read further, how would you define intelligence? If you’re typical of most Americans, you would mention reasoning logically, connecting ideas, and solving real problems. You might mention verbal ability, meaning the ability to speak clearly and artic- ulately. You might also include social competence: for example, an interest in the world at large and an ability to admit when you make a mistake (Sternberg & Kaufman, 1998).

As you’ll see in this section, many of these ideas about intelligence are included in psy- chological theories of intelligence. We’ll begin by considering the theories of intelligence, where we’ll get some insight into ways that Diana could make the Civil War come alive for her class. Next, you’ll see how intelligence tests were devised initially to assess individual differences in intellectual ability. Then we’ll look at a simple question: How well do mod- ern tests work? Finally, we’ll examine how race, ethnicity, social class, gender, environ- ment, and heredity influence intelligence.

Theories of Intelligence Psychometricians are psychologists who specialize in measuring psychological characteris- tics such as intelligence and personality. When psychometricians want to research a par- ticular question, they usually begin by administering many tests to many individuals. Then they look for patterns in performance across the tests. The basic logic underlying this technique resembles the logic a jungle hunter uses to decide whether some dark blobs in a river are three separate rotting logs or a single alligator (Cattell, 1965). If the blobs move together, the hunter decides they are part of the same structure, an alligator. If they do not move together, they are three different structures—three logs. Similarly, if changes in per- formance on one test are accompanied by changes in performance on a second test—that is, if they move together—then the tests are measuring the same attribute or factor.

The Hierarchical View of Intelligence Research analyzing performance on tests led psychometricians to propose hierarchical theories in which intelligence includes both general and specific components. John Car- roll (1993, 1996), for example, proposed the hierarchical theory with three levels shown in ◗ Figure 6.2. At the top of the hierarchy is general intelligence (often referred to as g). Underneath general intelligence are eight broad categories of intellectual skill, ranging

psychometricians Psychologists who specialize in measuring psychological traits such as intelligence and personality.

●◗ Figure 6.2 Hierarchical theories of intelligence have different levels that range from general intelligence (g) to very specific skills.

Fluid intelligence

General intelligence (g)

Sequential reasoning

Inductive reasoning

Quantitative reasoning

Crystallized intelligence

General memory

and learning

Printed language

Language comprehension

Vocabulary knowledge

Memory span

Associative memory

Visualization

Spatial relations

Closure speed

Speech sound

discrimination

General sound

discrimination

Creativity

Ideational fluency

Naming facility

Rate of test taking

Numerical facility

Perceptual speed

Simple reaction time

Choice reaction time

Semantic processing

speed

Broad visual

perception

Broad auditory

perception

Broad retrieval ability

Broad cognitive

speediness

Processing speed

Fr om

C ar

ro ll,

1 99

3.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

196 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

from fluid intelligence to processing speed. Each of the abilities in the second level is fur- ther divided into the skills listed in the third and most specific level. Crystallized intel- ligence, for example, includes understanding printed language, comprehending language, and knowing vocabulary.

Hierarchical models of intelligence are valuable because they integrate findings from decades’ of research. Nevertheless, some critics find them unsatisfactory because they ignore research and theory on cognitive development. The critics believe we need to look beyond the psychometric approach to understand intelligence. In the remainder of this section then, we’ll look at two theories that have done this.

Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences Other psychologists have viewed intelligence from the perspective of Piaget’s theory and information-processing psychology. Among the most ambitious is Howard Gardner’s (2011) theory of multiple intelligences. Rather than using test scores as the basis for his theory, Gardner draws on research in child development, studies of brain-damaged per- sons, and studies of exceptionally talented people. Using these criteria, Gardner identi- fied seven distinct intelligences when he first proposed the theory in 1983. In subsequent work, Gardner (1999, 2002) identified two additional intelligences; the complete list is shown in ◗ Table 6.1.

The first three intelligences in the list—linguistic intelligence, logical-mathematical intelligence, and spatial intelligence—are included in traditional theories of intelligence. The last six intelligences are not: Musical, bodily-kinesthetic, interpersonal, intraper- sonal, naturalistic, and existential intelligences are unique to Gardner’s theory. According to Gardner, Ariana Grande’s beautiful singing, Simone Biles’s remarkable routines on the vault, and Oprah Winfrey’s grace and charm in dealing with people are all features of intel- ligence that are ignored in traditional theories.

How did Gardner arrive at these nine distinct intelligences? First, each has a unique developmental history. Linguistic intelligence, for example, develops much earlier than the others. Second, each intelligence is regulated by distinct regions of the brain. For example, spatial intelligence is regulated by particular regions in the right hemisphere of the brain. Third, each intelligence has special cases of talented individuals. There are well-known instances of musically intelligent people, for example, who exhibit incredible talent at an

Table 6.1

Nine Intelligences in Gardner’s Theory of Multiple Intelligences Type of InTellIgence DefInITIon Linguistic Knowing the meanings of words, having the ability to use words to understand new ideas, and

using language to convey ideas to others

Logical-mathematical Understanding relations that exist among objects, actions, and ideas as well as the logical or mathematical operations that can be performed on them

Spatial Perceiving objects accurately and imagining in the mind’s eye the appearance of an object before and after it has been transformed

Musical Comprehending and producing sounds varying in pitch, rhythm, and emotional tone

Bodily-kinesthetic Using one’s body in highly differentiated ways as dancers, craftspeople, and athletes do

Interpersonal Identifying different feelings, moods, motivations, and intentions in others

Intrapersonal Understanding one’s emotions and knowing one’s strengths and weaknesses

Naturalistic Understanding the natural world, distinguishing natural objects from artifacts, and grouping and labeling natural phenomena

Existential Considering “ultimate” issues, such as the purpose of life and the nature of death

SOURCE: Gardner, 1983, 1999, 2002.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 197

early age. Claudio Arrau, one of the 20th-century’s greatest pianists, read musical notes before he read words. And Yo-Yo Ma, the famed cellist, performed in concert at 7 years of age for President John F. Kennedy.

Prompted by Gardner’s theory, researchers have begun to look at other nontraditional aspects of intelligence. Probably the best known is emotional intelligence, which is the ability to use one’s own and others’ emotions effectively for solving problems and living hap- pily. Emotional intelligence includes several distinct facets: perceiving emotions accurately (e.g., recognizing a happy face), understanding emotions (e.g., distinguishing happiness from ecstasy), and regulating one’s emotions (e.g., hiding one’s disappointment). People who are emotionally intelligent tend to have more satisfying interpersonal relationships, have greater subjective well-being, and to be more effective in the workplace (Joseph & Newman, 2010; Sánchez-Álvarez, Extremera, & Fernández-Berrocal, 2016).

The theory of multiple intelligences has important implications for education. Gardner (1993, 1995) believes that schools should foster all intelligences, not just the traditional linguistic and logical-mathematical intelligences. Teachers should capitalize on the stron- gest intelligences of individual children. That is, teachers need to know a child’s profile of intelligence—the child’s strengths and weaknesses—and gear instruction to the strengths (Chen & Gardner, 2005). For example, Diana, the fourth-grade teacher in the  opening vignette, could help some of her students understand the Civil War by studying music of that period (musical intelligence). Other students might benefit by emphasizing maps that show the movement of armies in battle (spatial intelligence). Still others might profit from focusing on the experiences of African Americans living in the North and the South (interpersonal intelligence).

These guidelines do not mean that teachers should gear instruction solely to a child’s strongest intelligence, pigeonholing youngsters as “numerical learners” or “spatial learn- ers.” Instead, whether the topic is the signing of the Declaration of Independence or Shakespeare’s Hamlet, instruction should try to engage as many different intelligences as possible (Gardner, 1999, 2002). The typical result is a much richer understanding of the topic by all students.

Some American schools have enthusiastically embraced Gardner’s ideas (Gardner, 1993). Are these schools better than those that have not? Educators in schools using the theory think so; they cite evidence that their students benefit in many ways (Kornhaber, Fierros, & Veenema, 2004), although some critics are not yet convinced (Waterhouse, 2006). In fact, a general criticism is that the theory has relatively little empirical support (Kaufman, Kaufman, & Plucker, 2013). Nevertheless, Gardner’s work has helped liberate researchers from psychometric-based views of intelligence.

Sternberg’s Theory of Successful Intelligence In Robert Sternberg’s theory, intelligence is defined as using one’s abilities skillfully to achieve one’s personal goals (Sternberg, 2015). Goals can be short-term—such as getting an A on a test, making a snack in the microwave, or winning the 100-meter hurdles—or longer-term, such as having a successful career and a happy family life. Achieving these goals by using one’s skills defines successful intelligence.

In achieving personal goals, people use three different kinds of abilities. Analytic ability involves analyzing problems and generating different solutions. Suppose a teenager wants to download songs to her iPhone, but something isn’t working. Analytic intelligence is shown when she considers different causes of the problem—maybe the iPhone is broken or maybe the software to download songs wasn’t installed correctly.

Creative ability involves dealing adaptively with novel situations and problems. Sup- pose our 12-year-old discovers that her iPhone is broken just as she’s ready to leave with her family on a daylong car trip. Lacking the time (and money) to buy a new phone, cre- ative intelligence is shown in dealing successfully with a novel goal: finding an enjoyable activity to pass the time on a long drive.

Finally, practical ability involves knowing what solution or plan will actually work. Problems can be solved in different ways in principle, but in reality, only one solution may

emotional intelligence ability to use one’s own and others’ emo- tions effectively for solving problems and living happily.

analytic ability in sternberg’s theory of intelligence, the ability to analyze problems and generate different solutions.

creative ability in sternberg’s theory of intelligence, the ability to deal adaptively with novel situations and problems.

practical ability in sternberg’s theory of intelligence, the ability to know which problem solutions are likely to work.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

198 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

be practical. Our teenager may realize that surfing the Net for a way to fix the phone’s software is the only real choice because her parents wouldn’t approve of many of the songs and she doesn’t want her sibling to know that she’s down- loading them anyway.

Like Gardner, Sternberg (1999) argues that instruction is most effective when it is geared to a child’s strength. A child with strong analytic ability, for example, may find algebra simpler when the course emphasizes analysis and evaluation; a child with strong practical ability may be at his or her best when the material is organized around practical applications. Thus, the theory of successful intelligence shows how instruction can be matched to students’ strongest abilities, enhancing students’ prospects for mastering the material (Grigorenko, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2002).

Sternberg emphasizes that successful intelligence is revealed in people’s pursuit of goals. Of course, these goals vary from one person to the next and, just as important, often vary even more in different cultural or ethnic groups. That is, intelligence is partly defined by the demands of an environ- ment or cultural context. What is intelligent for children growing up in cit- ies in North America may not be intelligent for children growing up in the Sahara desert, in the Australian outback, or on a remote island in the Pacific Ocean. For example, in Brazil, many school-age boys sell candy and fruit to bus passengers and pedestrians. These children often cannot identify the numbers on paper money, yet they know how to purchase their goods from wholesale stores, make change for customers, and keep track of their sales (Saxe, 1988).

If the Brazilian vendors were given the tests that measure intelligence in American students, they would fare poorly. Does this mean they are less intelli- gent than American children? Of course not. The skills important to American conceptions of intelligence and that are assessed on our intelligence tests are less valued in these other cultures and so are not cultivated in the young. Each

culture defines what it means to be intelligent, and the vendors with their specialized com- puting skills are just as intelligent in their cultural settings as Americans with their verbal skills are in their culture (Sternberg & Kaufman, 1998).

As with Gardner’s theory, researchers are still evaluating Sternberg’s theory and are still debating the question of what intelligence is. However it is defined, the facts are that individuals differ substantially in intellectual ability and that numerous tests have been devised to measure these differences. We’ll examine the construction, properties, and lim- its of these tests in the next section.

Binet and the Development of Intelligence Testing In the United States, school enrollment nearly doubled between 1890 and 1915 because of an influx of immigrants and because reforms restricted child labor and emphasized edu- cation (Giordano, 2005). With the increased enrollment, teachers were confronted with increasing numbers of students who did not learn as readily as the “select few” who had populated their classes previously. How to deal with “feebleminded” children was one of the pressing issues of the day for U.S. educators.

These problems were not unique to the United States. In 1904, the Minister of Public Instruction in France asked two noted psychologists of the day, Alfred Binet and Theo- phile Simon, to formulate a way of recognizing children who would be unable to learn in school without special instruction. Binet and Simon’s approach was to select simple tasks that French children of different ages ought to be able to do, such as naming colors, count- ing backward, and remembering numbers in order. Based on preliminary testing, Binet and Simon identified problems that typical 3-year-olds could solve, that typical 4-year- olds could solve, and so on. Children’s mental age (MA) referred to the difficulty of the problems they could solve correctly. For example, a child who solved problems that the average 7-year-old could solve would have an MA of 7.

mental age (MA) in intelligence testing, a measure of children’s performance corresponding to the chronological age of those whose performance equals the child’s.

For street vendors in Brazil, successful intelligence involves sophisticated arithmetic operations for buying products, making change, and keeping track of sales.

D av

id R

. F ra

zi er

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 199

Binet and Simon used mental age to distinguish “bright” from “dull” children. A bright child would have the MA of an older child—for example, a 6-year-old with an MA of 9. A dull child would have the MA of a younger child—for example, a 6-year-old with an MA of 4. Binet and Simon confirmed that bright children identified using their test did better in school than did dull children. Voilà—the first objective measure of intelligence!

The Stanford-Binet Lewis Terman, of Stanford University, revised Binet and Simon’s test substantially and published a version known as the Stanford-Binet in 1916. Terman described performance as an intelligence quotient (IQ), which was the ratio of mental age to chronological age (CA) multiplied by 100:

5 3IQ MA / CA 100

At any age, children who are perfectly average have an IQ of 100 because their mental age equals their chronological age. Furthermore, roughly two-thirds of children taking a test will have IQ scores between 85 and 115. The IQ score can also be used to compare intel- ligence in children of different ages. A 4-year-old girl with an MA of 5 has an IQ of 125 (5/4 × 100), just like that of an 8-year-old boy with an MA of 10 (10/8 × 100).

IQ scores are no longer computed as the ratio of MA to CA. Instead, children’s IQ scores are determined by comparing their test performance to the average IQ score of oth- ers their age. When children perform at the average for their age, their IQ is 100. Children who perform above the average have IQs greater than 100; children who perform below the average have IQs less than 100. Nevertheless, the concept of IQ as the ratio of MA to CA helped to popularize the Stanford-Binet test.

By the 1920s, the Stanford-Binet had been joined by many other intelligence tests. Educators greeted these new devices enthusiastically because they seemed to offer an effi- cient and objective way to assess a student’s chances of succeeding in school (Chapman, 1988). Today, nearly 100 years later, the Stanford-Binet remains a popular test; the latest version was revised in 2003. Like the earlier versions, the modern Stanford-Binet consists of various cognitive and motor tasks ranging from the extremely easy to the extremely dif- ficult. The Stanford-Binet, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-V (WISC-V), and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children-II are the primary individualized tests of intelligence in use today.

Do Tests Work? If tests work, they should predict important outcomes in children’s lives: Children who receive higher IQ scores should be more successful in school and after they leave school. In fact, IQ scores are remarkably powerful predictors of developmental outcomes. One expert argued that “IQ is the most important predictor of an individual’s ultimate position within American society” (Brody, 1992). Of course, because IQ tests were devised to pre- dict school success, it’s not surprising that they do this quite well. IQ scores predict school grades, scores on achievement tests, and number of years of education with correlations that are usually between 0.5 and 0.7 (Brody, 1992; Geary, 2005).

These correlations are far from perfect, which reminds us that some youngsters with high test scores do not excel in school and others with low test scores manage to get good grades. In fact, some researchers find that self-discipline predicts grades in school even better than IQ scores do (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005). In general, however, tests predict school success.

Intelligence scores not only predict success in school but also predict occupational success (Deary, 2012). Individuals with higher IQ scores are more likely to hold high- paying, high-prestige positions in medicine, law, and engineering (Oswald & Hough, 2012). Some of the link between IQ and occupational success occurs because these profes- sions require more education, and IQ scores predict educational success. But even within

intelligence quotient (IQ) Mathematical representation of how a per- son scores on an intelligence test in relation to how other people of the same age score.

Think About It If Jean Piaget were to create an intelligence test, how would it differ from the type of test Binet created?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

200 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

a profession—where all individuals have the same amount of education—IQ scores pre- dict job performance and earnings, particularly for more complex jobs (Henderson, 2010; Schmidt & Hunter, 2004). And among scientists with equal education, those with higher IQ scores have more patents and more articles published in scientific journals (Park, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2008).

Finally, IQ scores predict outcomes that don’t have obvious connections to school success. For example, people with higher IQ scores are more likely to be chosen as leaders of groups, and once chosen, their groups tend to be more productive and they tend to be rated as better leaders and are more popular with group members (Strenze, 2015). Intelli- gence scores even predict longevity: Individuals with greater IQ scores tend to live longer, in part because they are less likely to smoke, they drink less alcohol, they stay active physi- cally, and they eat more healthfully (Deary, 2012).

Hereditary and Environmental Factors In a typical elementary school, some first graders will have IQs greater than 120 and oth- ers will have IQ scores in the low 80s. Some of this 40-point difference reflects heredity (Bouchard, 2009). For example, identical twins’ IQs are more similar than fraternal twins’ IQs (Plomin & Deary, 2015). And adopted children’s IQ is usually more similar to their biological parents’ IQ than to their adoptive parents’ IQ (Rhea et al., 2013). What’s more, the impact of heredity on intelligence increases across childhood and adolescence. For example, identical twins’ IQs become more alike across childhood and adolescence (Plo- min & Deary, 2015).

Do these results mean that heredity is the sole determiner of intelligence? No. Three areas of research show the importance of environment on intelligence. The first is research on characteristics of families and homes. If intelligence were solely due to heredity, envi- ronment should have little or no impact on children’s intelligence. In fact, children have greater IQ scores when the family environment is intellectually stimulating—when par- ents talk more frequently to the children; when they provide their children with cogni- tive challenging materials such as puzzles and books; and when they expose children to stimulating experiences outside the home, such as visits to museums (Nisbett et al., 2012).

The impact of the environment on intelligence is also implicated by a dramatic rise in IQ test scores during the 20th century (Flynn & Weiss, 2007; Weber, Dekhtyar,  & Herlitz, 2017). For example, scores on the WISC increased by nearly 10 points over a 25-year period (Flynn, 1999). The change may reflect industrialization, which requires a more intelligent workforce and brings about better schools, smaller families, improved health (including better nutrition and hygiene) and more stimulating leisure-time activi- ties ( Nisbett et al., 2012). Regardless of the exact causes of greater IQ scores, the increase

shows the impact of changing environmental condi- tions on intelligence.

The importance of a stimulating environment for intelligence is also demonstrated by interven- tion programs that prepare economically disadvan- taged children for school. Children who grow up in poverty often struggle in school because they lack the skills needed for success. However, when such children participate in intervention programs that teach school readiness skills and social skills, they’re more successful in school, more likely to work full time, and tend to be healthier. These programs more than pay for themselves in the form of greater earn- ings (and tax revenues) for participating children and reduced costs associated with the criminal justice system (Englund et al., 2014). And in the process, they show that intelligence is fostered by a stimulat- ing and responsive environment.

Following adoption, children’s scores on intelligence tests tend to resemble the scores of their biological parents, not their adoptive parents, which shows the impact of heredity on intelligence.

D en

is e

H ag

er , C

at ch

lig ht

V is

ua l S

er vi

ce s/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 201

The Impact of Ethnicity and Socioeconomic Status On many intelligence tests, ethnic groups differ in their average scores: Asian Americans tend to have the highest scores, followed by European Americans, Latino Americans, and African Americans (Hunt & Carlson, 2007). The gaps have become smaller since the 1960s and reflect, in part, group differences in socioeconomic status (Nisbett et al., 2012; Rindermann & Thompson, 2013). Children from economically advantaged homes tend to have higher test scores than children from economically disadvantaged homes, and  European American and Asian American families are more likely to be economi- cally advantaged, whereas Latino American and African American families are more likely to be economically disadvantaged. Nevertheless, when children from comparable socioeconomic status are compared, group differences in IQ test scores are reduced but not eliminated (Magnuson & Duncan, 2006). Let’s look at four explanations for these differences.

A Role for Genetics? On page 200, you learned that heredity helps determine a child’s intelligence: Smart par- ents tend to beget smart children. Does this also mean that group differences in IQ scores reflect genetic differences between groups? No. Most researchers agree that there is no evidence that some ethnic groups have more “smart genes” than others. Instead, they believe that the environment is largely responsible for these differences (Nisbett et al., 2012).

A popular analogy (Lewontin, 1976) demonstrates the thinking here. Imagine two kinds of corn: Each kind produces both short and tall plants, and height is known to be due to heredity. If one kind of corn grows in a good soil—plenty of water and nutrients—the mature plants will reach their genetically determined heights—some short, some tall. If the other kind of corn grows in poor soil, few plants will reach their full height and overall the plants of this kind will be much shorter. Even though height is heritable for each type of corn, the difference in height between the two groups is due solely to the quality of the environment. Similarly, although IQ scores may be herita- ble for different groups, less exposure to stimulating environments may mean that one group ends up with lower IQ scores overall (just as the group of plants growing in poor soil do not reach their full height). In other words, differences within ethnic groups are partly due to heredity, but differences between groups apparently reflect environmental influences.

High-quality preschool programs provide stimulating environments that can increase children’s scores on intelligence tests and improve their school performance.

A P

Im ag

es /M

ik e

D er

er

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

202 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Experience with Test Contents Some critics contend that differences in test scores reflect bias in the tests themselves. They argue that test items reflect the cultural heritage of the test creators—typically eco- nomically advantaged European Americans—so tests are biased against children from other groups (Champion, 2003). Such critics point to test items such as this one:

A conductor is to an orchestra as a teacher is to what? book school class eraser

Children whose background includes exposure to orchestras are more likely to answer this question correctly than children who lack this exposure.

The problem of bias led to the development of culture-fair intelligence tests, which include test items based on experiences common to many cultures. An example is Raven’s Progressive Matrices, which consists items such as the one shown in ◗ Figure 6.3. Examinees are asked to select the piece that would complete the design correctly (piece 6, in this case). Although items such as this are thought to reduce the impact of specific experience on test performance, ethnic group differences remain on so-called culture-fair intelligence tests (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994). Apparently, familiarity with test-related items is not the key factor responsible for group differences.

Test-Taking Skills The impact of experience and cultural values can extend beyond particular items to a child’s familiarity with the entire testing situation. Tests underestimate a child’s intel- ligence if a child’s culture encourages children to solve problems by collaborating with others and discourages them from excelling as individuals. Moreover, because they are wary of questions posed by unfamiliar adults, many economically disadvantaged children often answer test questions by saying, “I don’t know.” Obviously, this strategy guarantees an artificially low test score. When these children are given extra time to feel at ease with the examiner, they respond less often with “I don’t know” and their test scores improve considerably (Zigler & Finn-Stevenson, 1992).

culture-fair intelligence tests intelligence tests devised using items com- mon to many cultures.

●◗ Figure 6.3 Culture-fair intelligence tests are designed to minimize the impact of experiences that are unique to some cultures or to some children within a culture.

A5

1 2 3

4 5 6

Based on Raven’s Progressive Matrices.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 203

Stereotype Threat When people know that they belong to a group that is said to lack skill in a domain, they become anxious when performing in that domain for fear of confirming the stereotype; as a result, they often do poorly. This self-fulfilling prophecy, in which knowledge of stereotypes leads to anxiety and reduced performance consistent with the original stereotype, is called stereotype threat. Applied to intelligence, the argument is that African American children experience stereotype threat when they take intelligence tests, and this contributes to their lower scores (Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995). For example, imagine two 10-year-olds taking an intelligence test for admission to a special program for gifted children. The Euro- pean American child worries that if he fails the test, he won’t be admitted to the program. The African American child has the same fears but also worries that if she does poorly, it will confirm the stereotype that African American children don’t get good scores on IQ tests (Suzuki & Aronson, 2005). Consistent with this idea, stereotype threat is reduced (and performance improves) when African American students experience self-affirmation—they remind themselves of values that are important to them and why (Sherman et al., 2013).

Interpreting Test Scores If all tests reflect cultural influences to at least some degree, how should we interpret test scores? Remember that tests assess successful adaptation to a particular cultural context. Most intelligence tests predict success in a school environment, which usually espouses middle-class values. Regardless of ethnic group, a child with a high test score has the intel- lectual skills needed for academic work based on middle-class values (Hunt & Carlson, 2007). A child with a low test score apparently lacks those skills. Does a low score mean that a child is destined to fail in school? No. It simply means that based on the child’s current skills, he or she is unlikely to do well. We know from intervention projects that improving children’s skills improves their school performance.

By focusing on groups of people, it’s easy to overlook the fact that individuals within these groups differ in intelligence. The average difference in IQ scores between various ethnic groups is relatively small compared with the entire range of scores for these groups (Sternberg, Grigorenko, & Kidd, 2005). You can easily find youngsters with high IQ scores from all ethnic groups, just as you can find youngsters with low IQ scores from all groups. In the next section, we’ll look at children at the extremes of ability.

stereotype threat an evoked fear of being judged in accor- dance with a negative stereotype about a group to which you belong.

recall 1. According to _______________theories, intelligence includes

both general intelligence and more specific abilities, such as verbal and spatial skill.

2. Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences includes linguis- tic, logical-mathematical, and spatial intelligences, which are included in psychometric theories, as well as musical, _______________, interpersonal, and intrapersonal intelli- gences, which are ignored in psychometric theories.

3. Based on Gardner’s view of intelligence, teachers should _______________.

4. According to Sternberg, successful intelligence depends on _______________, creative, and practical abilities.

5. As adopted children get older, their IQ scores increasingly resemble the IQ scores of their _______________parents.

6. Evidence for the impact of environment on children’s intelli- gence comes from studies of children’s homes, from histori- cal change in IQ scores, and from _______________.

7. The problem of cultural bias on intelligence tests led to the development of _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast the major perspectives on intelli-

gence in terms of the extent to which they make connections between different aspects of development. That is, to what extent does each perspective emphasize cognitive processes versus integrating physical, cognitive, social, and emotional development?

Apply ●● Suppose that a local government official proposes to end all

funding for preschool programs for disadvantaged children. Write a letter to this official in which you describe the value of these programs.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 6.2

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

204 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Sanjit, a second grader, has taken two intelligence tests, and both times he had above- average scores. His parents took him to an ophthalmologist, who determined that his vision is 20/30—nothing wrong with his eyes. Nevertheless, Sanjit cannot read. Letters and words are as mysterious to him as Lady Gaga’s music would be to Mozart. What is wrong?

Throughout history, societies have recognized children with unusual abilities and talents. Today, we know much about the extremes of human skill. Let’s begin with a glimpse at gifted children.

Gifted Children Traditionally, giftedness was defined by scores on intelligence tests: A score of 130 or greater was the criterion for being gifted. But modern definitions of giftedness are broader and include exceptional talent in an assortment of areas, including art, music, creative writing, dance, and sports (Olszewski-Kubilius, Subotnik, & Worrell, 2017; Winner, 2000).

Whether the field is music or math, though, exceptionally talented children have sev- eral characteristics in common (Subotnik, Olszewski-Kubilius, & Worrell, 2011). First, their ability is substantially above average; being smart is necessary but not sufficient for being gifted. Second, gifted children are passionate about their subject and have a power- ful desire to master it. Third, gifted children are creative in their thinking, coming up with novel thoughts and actions. Creativity is associated with divergent thinking, where the aim is not a single correct answer (often there isn’t one), but fresh and unusual lines of thought (Callahan, 2000). For example, creativity is shown when children respond in dif- ferent innovative ways to a common stimulus, as shown in ◗ Figure 6.4.

Fourth, exceptional talent must be nurtured. Without encouragement and support from parents and stimulating and challenging mentors, a youngster’s talents will wither. Tal- ented children need a curriculum that is challenging and complex, they need teachers who know how to foster talent, and they need like-minded peers who stimulate their interests (Subotnik et al., 2011). With this support, gifted children’s achievement can be remarkable. In a 25-year longitudinal study, gifted teens were, as adults, extraordinarily successful in school and in their careers. For example, about 10% had been awarded patents before they turned 40; many were leaders in the corporate sector, law, and medicine (Makel et al., 2016).

Finally, the stereotype is that gifted children are often troubled emotionally and unable to get along with their peers. In reality, gifted children and adults tend to be more mature than their peers and have fewer emotional problems (Simonton & Song, 2009; Subotnik et al., 2011), and as adults, they report being highly satisfied with their careers, relationships with others, and life in general (Lubinski et al., 2006).

Gifted children represent one extreme of human ability. At the other extreme are youngsters with disability, the topic of the next section.

Children with Disability “Little David,” so named because his father was also named David, was the oldest of four children. He learned to sit only days before his first birthday, he began to walk at 2, and he said his first words as a 3-year-old. By age 5, David was far behind his age-mates developmentally. David had Down syndrome, a disorder described in Section 2.1 that is caused by an extra 21st chromosome.

divergent thinking thinking in novel and unusual directions.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the characteristics of gifted children?

●● What are different forms of disability?

●● What are the distinguishing features of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder?

6.3 Special Children, Special Needs

●◗ Figure 6.4 One way to measure creativity is to determine how many original responses children can make to a specific stimulus.

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 205

Children with Intellectual Disability Down syndrome is an example of a condition that leads to intellectual disability, which refers to substantial limitations in intellectual ability as well as problems adapting to an environment, with both emerging before 18 years of age. Limited intellectual skill is often defined as a score of 70 or less on an intelligence test such as the Stanford-Binet. Adaptive behavior includes conceptual skills important for successful adjustment (e.g., literacy, an understanding of money and time), social skills (e.g., interpersonal skill), and practical skills (e.g., personal grooming, occupational skills). It is usually evaluated from interviews with a parent or another caregiver. Only individuals who are under 18, have problems adapting in these areas, and have IQ scores of 70 or less are considered to have an intel- lectual disability (AAIDD Ad Hoc Committee on Terminology and Classification, 2010).1

Modern explanations pinpoint four factors that place individuals at risk for intellec- tual disability:

●● Biomedical factors, including chromosomal disorders, malnutrition, traumatic brain injury ●● Social factors, such as poverty and impaired parent–child interactions ●● Behavioral factors, such as child neglect or domestic violence ●● Educational factors, including impaired parenting and inadequate special education

services

No individual factor in this list necessarily leads to intellectual disability. Instead, the risk for intellectual disability grows as more of these factors are present (AAIDD Ad Hoc Com- mittee on Terminology and Classification, 2010). For example, the risk is great for a child with Down syndrome whose parents live in poverty and cannot take advantage of special education services.

As you can imagine, the many factors that can lead to intellectual disability means that the term encompasses an enormous variety of individuals. Some people with severe intel- lectual disability need constant support for activities of daily living, such skills as dressing, feeding, and toileting. At the other extreme are individuals who go to school and master many academic skills, but not as quickly as a typical child. They often work and many marry. In short, although people with intellectual disability face unique challenges, with support and resources from families and communities, their lives can be active, fulfilling, and productive (Iarocci et al., 2008).

Children with Learning Disability In contrast to people with intellectual disability, children with learning disability have nor- mal intelligence. That is, children with learning disability (1) have difficulty mastering an academic subject, (2) have normal intelligence, and (3) are not suffering from other condi- tions that could explain poor performance, such as sensory impairment or inadequate instruction.

In the United States, about 5% of school-age children are classified as learning dis- abled, which translates into nearly 2.5 million youngsters (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). Three kinds of learning disability are particularly common: difficulties in reading individ- ual words, sometimes known as developmental dyslexia; difficulties understanding words that have been read successfully, which is called impaired reading comprehension; and, finally, difficulties in mathematics, which is termed mathematical learning disability or developmental dyscalculia (Snowling & Hulme, 2015).

Understanding learning disabilities is complicated because each type has its own causes and thus requires its own treatment. For example, developmental dyslexia is the most common type of learning disability. (It’s so common that sometimes it’s just referred

1 What we now call intellectual disability was long known as mental retardation, and much federal and state law in the United States still uses the latter term. However, intellectual disability is the preferred term because it better reflects the condition not as a deficit in the person, but as a poor “fit between the person’s capacities and the context in which the person is to function” (AAIDD Ad Hoc Committee on Terminology and Classification, 2010, p. 13).

intellectual disability substantially below-average intelligence and problems adapting to an environment that emerge before the age of 18.

learning disability when a child with normal intelligence has difficulty mastering at least one academic subject.

Think About It How might our definitions of giftedness and intellectual disability differ if they were based on Gardner’s theory of mul- tiple intelligences?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

206 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

to as reading disability.) Many children with this disorder have problems in distinguishing sounds in written and oral language. For children with devel- opmental dyslexia—like Sanjit (in the opening vignette) or the boy in the photograph—distinguishing bis from bep or bis from dis is very difficult; appar- ently the words all sound very similar (Hulme & Snowling, 2016).

Children with developmental dyslexia typically benefit from two kinds of instruction: training in phonological awareness—experiences that help  them to identify subtle but important differences in language sounds—along with explicit instruction on the connections between letters and their sounds. With intensive instruction of this sort, youngsters with developmental dyslexia can read more effectively (Hulme & Snowling, 2016).

Children with impaired reading comprehension, another common learn- ing disability, have no trouble reading individual words. But they understand

far less of what they read. Asked to read sentences such as The man rode the bus to go to work or The dog chased the cat through the woods, they do so easily but find it difficult to answer questions about what they’ve read (e.g., What did the man ride? Where did the man go?). These problems seem to reflect a limited spoken vocabulary (they simply know fewer words) as well as problems linking words in a sentence to create coherent meaning (Clarke et al., 2014; Hulme & Snowling, 2011). Told to select the picture showing children sitting on a table, they may point to a picture of children sitting on a rug or to a picture of children playing a game on a table, but not sitting on it (Nation et al., 2004). In other words, for these youngsters, impaired reading comprehension seems to be a by-product of impaired oral (spoken) language.

If impaired oral language drives impaired reading comprehension, then extensive instruction in vocabulary and language skills should reduce impaired comprehension. It does (Melby-Lervåg & Lervåg, 2014), as shown in the study that’s the focus of the Spotlight on Research feature.

Children with reading disabilities often have trouble associating sounds with letters.

Cs ab

a Pe

te rd

i/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

On ResearchSpotlight

Improving Children’s Knowledge of the Structure of Words Enhances Their Reading Comprehension

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? One effec- tive way to help children learn new words is to teach them about the structure of words and how words are formed. For example, understanding prefixes helps children master new words. When students who know that un means not or the opposite of first read unpack and unclean, they immediately grasp the meaning of these words. And since a larger vocabulary promotes better reading comprehension, Solveig-Alma Halaas Lyster and her colleagues, Arne Lervåg and Charles Hulme (2016), wondered whether teaching children about word structure would improve their reading comprehension.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? As preschoolers, some

children were taught how to recognize pre- fixes (unhappy), suffixes (walked), and com- pound words (tooth-brush). Training included 30-minute sessions over 17 weeks. Children in the control group received no training. As sixth graders, children’s reading com- prehension was measured with three tasks: one involved reading a long text (about four pages). Two others involved shorter text, one with an accompanying table and one with an accompanying map. After reading, children answered multiple-choice questions about the text.

Who were the participants in the study? The study included 163 Norwegian preschool children; 93 were tested again as sixth graders.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental. The independent variable was the presence or absence of training in word structure; the dependent vari- able was the number of questions answered

correctly on the three tests. The study was longitudinal because children were tested as preschoolers and sixth graders.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. Parents provided consent for their children to participate. The training was valu- able for preschoolers and the tasks adminis- tered in sixth grade were similar to those used to assess students’ reading achievement.

What were the results? ◗ Figure 6.5 shows the percentage of questions that were answered correctly on the three read- ing tasks. On each task, children who had received training on word structure as pre- schoolers answered more accurately as sixth graders.

What did the investigators conclude? Lyster and her colleagues concluded that “By giving preschoolers and young school- children knowledge of the . . . structure of words and helping them to integrate mean- ing with the spoken and (when they begin

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 207

A third common form of learning disability is mathematical disability. Roughly 5% to 10% of young children struggle with arithmetic instruction from the very beginning. These youngsters progress slowly in their efforts to learn to count, to add, and to subtract; many are also diagnosed with reading disability. As they move into second and third grade (and beyond), these children often use inefficient methods for computing solutions—for example, still using their fingers as third graders to solve problems such as 9 + 7 (Hulme & Snowling, 2015).

We know far less about mathematical learning disability, largely because mathematics engages a broader set of skills than reading does. Scientists have proposed several fac- tors that may contribute to mathematical learning disability (Hulme & Snowling, 2016. One idea is that the approximate number system (described on page 134) provides less precise estimates of quantities for children with mathematical learning disability. Another possibility is that youngsters with mathematical disability are impaired in counting and retrieving arithmetic facts from memory. Yet another idea is that mathematical disability reflects problems in the basic cognitive processes that are used in doing arithmetic, such as working memory and executive function.

Because mathematical disability is not well understood, effective interventions have just begun. For example, children at risk for mathematical learning disability benefit from intensive practice designed to increase their understanding of numbers as well as their counting and calculation skills (Hulme & Melby-Lervåg, 2015). As we learn more about the core problems that define mathematical disability, researchers and educators should be able to fine-tune instruction for these children. When that happens, children with mathematical disability, like children with developmental dys- lexia and impaired reading comprehension, will be able to develop their full intellectual potential.

to read and write) written forms of words, we may . . . enhance their oral and written language development” (Lyster et al., 2016, pp. 1285–1286). They also concluded that enhanced language leads to enhanced reading comprehension.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? This study was conducted with children learning to read Norwegian, in which words are pronounced more consistently than in English. It would be valuable to see if the results would also hold for students learning to read in English. (Training might be more complicated because English words are more variable in their pro- nunciation, but the benefits might be greater.) Also, the researchers inferred that training in word structure increased children’s reading comprehension skills by increasing children’s vocabulary; it would be valuable to measure children’s vocabulary and then link changes in vocabulary directly to training and to comprehension.

●◗ Figure 6.5 Children who had, as preschoolers, been taught about the structure of words were more likely, as sixth graders, to comprehend what they read.

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

100

Long text Text with table Text with map

P er

ce n

ta ge

o f

q u

es ti

o n

s an

sw er

ed c

o rr

ec tl

y

Training group Control group

On all three tasks, children in the training group were more accurate than those in the control group.

Reading task

D at

a fr

om L

ys te

r et

a l.

(2 01

6) .

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

208 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Many school classrooms include children who are chronically rest- less, impulsive, and distractible; often these children are unpopular because their peers find them annoying. Many of these children suffer from attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which has three defining symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2004):

●● Hyperactivity. Children with ADHD are unusually energetic, fidg- ety, and unable to keep still—especially in situations like the one in the photo where they need to limit their activity.

●● Inattention. Youngsters with ADHD do not pay attention in class and seem unable to concentrate on schoolwork; instead, they skip from one task to another.

●● Impulsivity. Children with ADHD often act before thinking; they may run into a street before looking for traffic or interrupt others who are already speaking.

In the United States, about 6% of all school-age children are diag- nosed with ADHD; boys outnumber girls by a 3:1 ratio. The diagnosis is most often made in middle childhood when it becomes obvious that a child can’t work alone in a classroom, struggles to make friends, and is disruptive at home. But markers of ADHD can be found in the pre- school years: children are more likely to be diagnosed with ADHD if, as preschoolers, they were extremely overactive or had bursts of tem- per. During adolescence, hyperactivity often fades but inattention and

impulsivity remain (Sonuga-Barke & Taylor, 2015). As young adults, few individuals with ADHD complete college and many have work-related problems (Hechtman et al., 2016).

Scientists believe that genes put some children at risk for ADHD by affecting the alert- ing and executive networks of attention and the brain structures that support those net- works (Gizer & Waldman, 2012). But environmental factors also contribute. For example, prenatal exposure to alcohol and other drugs can place children at risk for ADHD as can inadequate diet or exposure to lead (Sonuga-Barke & Taylor, 2015).

ADHD is typically treated in two ways. First, stimulant drugs such as Ritalin (meth- ylphenidate) are effective. It may seem odd that stimulants are given to children who are already overactive, but these drugs stimulate the parts of the brain that normally inhibit hyperactive and impulsive behavior. Thus, stimulants may calm many youngsters with ADHD, allowing them to focus their attention (Barkley, 2004). Second, behavioral treat- ments, typically involving parents, are beneficial. These are designed to improve children’s self-regulation, their organizational skills, and their social relationships (Sonuga-Barke & Taylor, 2015).

Unfortunately, we still know little about the best ways to implement drug and behav- ioral treatments, individually and in combination. In one study, the most effective way to reduce the number of times that children with ADHD broke classroom rules was to begin with behavioral treatment (for parents and children) and add medication if necessary ( Pelham et al., 2016). But the ideal dosages of both kinds of treatments remain unclear. Finally, long-term follow-up studies show that the impact of treatment often vanishes over time (Molina et al., 2009). This result suggests that ADHD is a chronic condition, like diabetes or asthma, one that requires ongoing monitoring and treatment (Hazell, 2009).

Tragically, many children who need these treatments do not receive them. African American and Hispanic American children are far less likely than European American youngsters to be diagnosed with and treated for ADHD, even when they have the same symptoms (Miller, Nigg, & Miller, 2009; Morgan et al., 2013). Why? Income plays a role. African American and Hispanic American families are more often economically disadvan- taged and consequently are less able to pay for diagnosis and treatment. Racial bias also contributes: Parents and professionals often attribute the symptoms of ADHD in Euro- pean American children to a biological problem that can be treated medically, but they

Children with ADHD are typically hyperactive as well as inattentive and impulsive.

Ca th

er in

e Le

dn er

/S to

ne /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 209

more often attribute these symptoms in African American or Hispanic American children to poor parenting, life stresses, or other sources that can’t be treated (Kendall & Hatton, 2002).

Obviously, all children with ADHD deserve appropriate treatment. Teachers and other professionals dealing with children need to be sure that poverty and racial bias do not prevent children from receiving the care they need.

recall 1. Giftedness was once defined solely in terms of IQ score but

modern definitions are _______________.

2. Creativity is associated with _______________thinking, in which the goal is to think in novel and unusual directions.

3. Intellectual disability involves substantial limits in intellec- tual ability and _______________that emerge before 18 years of age.

4. Biomedical, social, _______________, and educational factors place some children at risk for intellectual disability.

5. In developmental dyslexia, children have difficulty with _______________.

6. Key symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are overactivity, _______________, and impulsivity.

7. ADHD is typically treated with stimulant drugs and _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast traditional and modern definitions of

giftedness.

Apply ●● How might Jean Piaget have explained differences in intel-

lectual functioning between children with intellectual disability and children without intellectual disability? How might an infor- mation-processing psychologist explain these differences?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 6.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the components of skilled reading?

●● As children develop, how does their writing improve?

●● How do arithmetic skills change during the elementary school years? How do U.S. students compare with students from other countries?

●● What are the hallmarks of effective schools and effective teachers?

6.4 Academic Skills

Angelique is a fifth grader who loves to read. As a preschooler, Angelique’s parents read Dr.  Seuss stories to her, and now she has progressed to the point where she can read (and  understand!) 400-page novels intended for teens. Her parents marvel at this accomplishment and wish they better understood the skills that were involved so they could help Angelique’s younger brother learn to read as well as his sister does.

Reading is complex and reading well is a wonderful accomplishment. Much the same can be said for writing and math. We’ll examine each of these academic skills in this section. As we do, you’ll learn about the skills that underlie Angelique’s mastery of reading. We’ll end the section by looking at characteristics that make some schools and some teachers better than others.

Reading Try reading the following sentence:

Sumisu-san wa nawa o naifu de kirimashita.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

210 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Unless you know Japanese, you probably didn’t make much headway, did you? Now try this one:

Snore secretary green plastic sleep trucks.

These are English words, and you probably read them quite easily, but did you get anything more out of this sentence than the one in Japanese?

These examples show two important processes involved in skilled reading. word recognition is the process of identifying a unique pattern of letters. Unless you know Japanese, your word recognition was unsuccessful in the first sentence. You did not know that nawa means rope or that kirimashita is the past tense of the English verb cut. What’s more, because you could not recognize individual words, you had no idea of the meaning of the sentence.

Comprehension is the process of extracting meaning from a sequence of words. In the second sentence, your word recognition was perfect, but comprehension was impossible because the words were presented randomly. These examples remind us just how difficult learning to read can be.

In the next few pages, we’ll look at some of the skills children must acquire if they are to learn to read and to read well. We’ll start with the skills that children must have if they are to learn to read, then move to word recognition and comprehension.

Foundations of Reading Skill English words are made up of individual letters, so children need to know their letters before they can learn to read. Children learn more about letters and word forms when they’re fre- quently involved in literacy-related activities such as reading with an adult, playing with magnetic letters, or trying to print simple words (Robins, Treiman, & Rosales, 2014). And not surprisingly, children who know more about letters and word forms learn to read more easily compared with peers who know less (Levy et al., 2006; Treiman & Kessler, 2003).

A second essential skill is sensitivity to language sounds. The ability to distinguish the sounds in spoken words is known as phonological awareness. English words consist of syllables, and a syllable is made up of a vowel that’s usually but not always accompanied by consonants. For example, dust is a one-syllable word that includes the initial consonant d, the vowel u, and the final consonant cluster st. Phonological awareness is shown when children can decompose words in this manner by, for example, correctly answering “What’s the first sound in dust?” or “Dust without the d sounds like what?” Phonological awareness is strongly related to success in learning to read: Children who can readily iden- tify different sounds in spoken words learn to read more readily than children who do not (Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012).

Learning to read in English is particularly challenging because letters in English are not pronounced consistently (e.g., compare the sound of “a” in bat, far, rake, and was) and sounds are not spelled consistently (e.g., the long “e” sound is the same in each of these spellings: team, feet, piece, lady, receive, magazine). In contrast, many other languages— Greek, Finnish, German, Italian, Spanish, and Dutch—are far more consistent, which sim- plifies mapping sounds to letters. For example, in Italian most letters are pronounced the same way; reading a word such as domani (tomorrow) is simple because beginning readers just move from left to right converting each letter to sound and using simple rules: d, m, and n are pronounced as in English, o as in cold, a as in car, and i as in see (Barca, Ellis, & Burani, 2007). In fact, children learn to read more rapidly in languages where letter-sound rules are more consistent, but phonological awareness remains the single best predictor of reading success in many languages (Caravolas et al., 2012; Song et al., 2016).

If phonological skills are so essential, how can we help children master them? Reading to children is one approach that’s fun for children and parents alike. When parents read stories, their children learn many language-related skills that prepare them for reading (Justice, Pullen & Pence, 2008; Raikes et al., 2006). And the benefits are not limited to the first steps in learning to read, but persist into the middle elementary school years and are just as useful for children learning to read other languages, such as Chinese (Chow et al., 2008; Sénéchal, 2012).

word recognition the process of identifying a unique pattern of letters.

comprehension the process of extracting meaning from a sequence of words.

phonological awareness the ability to hear the distinctive sounds of letters.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 211

Recognizing Words At the very beginning of reading, children sometimes learn to read a few words “by sight,” but they have no understanding of the links between printed letters and the word’s sound. However, the first step in true reading is learning to decode printed words by sound- ing out the letters in them: Beginning readers like the boy in the photo often say the sounds associated with each letter and then blend the sounds to produce a recognizable word. After a word has been sounded out a few times, it becomes a known word that can be read by retrieving it directly from long-term memory. That is, children decode words by recognizing familiar patterns of letters and syllables (Nunes, Bryant, & Barros, 2012).

Thus, from their very first efforts to read, most children use retrieval for some words. From that point on, the general strategy is to try retrieval first and, if that fails, to sound out the word or ask a more skilled reader for help. With more experience, the child sounds out fewer words and retrieves more (Siegler, 1986). That is, by sounding out novel words, children increase their store of information about words in long-term memory that is required for direct retrieval (Cunningham et al., 2002; Share, 2008).

Comprehension As we saw on page 206, decoding words accurately does not guarantee that children will understand what they’ve read. This phenomenon is captured in the Simple View of Read- ing model in which reading comprehension is viewed as the product of two general pro- cesses: word decoding and language comprehension (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). Children can’t comprehend what they read when either a word can’t be decoded or it’s decoded but not recognized as a familiar word. Thus, skilled reading depends on decoding accurately coupled with understanding the meaning of the decoded word.

As children gain more reading experience, they better comprehend what they read. Several factors contribute to this improved comprehension (Siegler & Alibali, 2005).

●● Children’s language skills improve, which allows them to understand words they’ve decoded. As children’s vocabulary expands, they are more likely to recognize words they’ve decoded. For example, a first grader with good decoding skills might be able to decode prosper but not understand it; by fifth or sixth grade, children’s larger vocabu- lary means that they could decode and understand prosper. In addition, older children know more about grammatical structure of sentences, knowledge that helps them com- prehend the meaning of an entire sentence (Muter et al., 2004; Oakhill & Cain, 2012).

●● Children become more skilled at recognizing words, allowing more working memory capacity to be devoted to comprehension (Zinar, 2000). When children struggle to recognize individual words, they often cannot link them to derive the meaning of a passage. In contrast, when children recognize words effortlessly, they can focus their efforts on deriving meaning from the whole sentence.

●● Working memory capacity increases. Older and better readers can store more of a sentence in memory as they try to identify the propositions it contains (De Beni & Palladino, 2000; Nation et al., 1999). This extra capacity is handy when readers move from sentences such as “Kevin hit the ball” to “In the bottom of the ninth, with the bases loaded and the Cardinals down 7–4, Kevin put a line drive into the left-field bleachers, his fourth home run of the series.”

●● Children acquire more knowledge of their physical, social, and psychological worlds. This allows them to understand more of what they read (Ferreol-Barbey, Piolat, & Roussey, 2000). For example, even if a 6-year-old could recognize all of the words in the longer sentence about Kevin’s home run, the child would not fully comprehend the meaning of the passage because he or she lacks the necessary knowledge of baseball.

●● With experience, children use more appropriate reading strategies. The goal of reading and the nature of the text dictate how you read. For example, when reading a novel,

Beginning readers usually rely heavily on “sounding out” a word.

Ju ria

h M

os in

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

212 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

do you often skip sentences or paragraphs to get to “the good parts”? This approach makes sense for pleasure reading but not for reading textbooks or recipes or how-to manuals. Reading a textbook requires attention to both the overall organization and the relation of details to that organization. Children who know more about effective strategies for reading tend to understand more of what they read (Cain, 1999).

●● With experience, children better monitor their comprehension. When readers don’t grasp the meaning of a passage because it is difficult or confusing, they read it again (Baker, 1994). Try this sentence (adapted from Carpenter & Daneman, 1981): “The Midwest State Fishing Contest would draw fishermen from all around the region, including some of the best bass guitarists in Michigan.” When you first encountered bass guitarists,” you probably interpreted bass as a fish. This didn’t make much sense, so you reread the phrase to determine that bass refers to a type of guitar. Older readers are better able to realize that their understanding is not complete and take corrective action (Oakhill & Cain, 2012).

Thus, several factors contribute to improved comprehension as children get older.

Writing Although few of us end up being a Maya Angelou, a Sandra Cisneros, or a J. K. Rowling, most adults do write, both at home and at work. The basics of good writing are remarkably straightforward, but writing skill develops only gradually during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood.

Skills Contributing to Improved Writing Several factors contribute to improved writing as children develop (Adams Treiman, & Pressley, 1998; Siegler & Alibali, 2005).

Knowledge About Topics. Writing is about telling “something” to others. With age, chil- dren have more to tell as they gain more knowledge about the world and incorporate this knowledge into their writing (Benton et al., 1995; Olinghouse, Graham, & Gillespie, 2015). For example, asked to write about a mayoral election, 8-year-olds are apt to describe it as a popularity contest but 12-year-olds more often describe it in terms of complex political issues. Of course, students are sometimes asked to write about topics that are unfamiliar to them. In this case, older children’s and adolescents’ writing is usually better because they are more adept at finding useful reference material and incorporating it into their writing.

Organizing Writing. One difficult aspect of writing is arranging all the information in a manner that readers find clear and interesting. In fact, children and young adolescents organize their writing differently than do older adolescents and adults (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987). Young writers often use a knowledge-telling strategy, writing down information on the topic as they retrieve it from memory. For example, asked to write about the day’s events at school, a second grader wrote:

It is a rainy day. We hope the sun will shine. We got new spelling books. We had our pictures taken. We sang Happy Birthday to Barbara. (Waters, 1980, p. 155)

The story has no obvious structure. The first two sentences are about the weather, but the last three deal with completely independent topics. Apparently the writer simply described each event as it came to mind.

During the elementary school years, children begin to use a knowledge-transforming strategy, deciding what information to include and how best to organize it for the point they want to convey to the reader. This approach involves considering the purpose of writ- ing (e.g., to inform, to persuade, to entertain) and the information needed to achieve that purpose. It also involves considering the needs, interests, and knowledge of the antici- pated audience. For example, an adolescent’s essay written to entertain peers about humor- ous events at school would differ from one written to convince parents about problems in schoolwork (Midgette, Haria, & MacArthur, 2008).

knowledge-telling strategy writing down information as it is retrieved from memory, a common practice for young writers.

knowledge-transforming strategy Deciding what information to include and how best to organize it to convey a point.

Think About It Reading and speaking are both impor- tant elements of literacy. How is learning to read like learning to speak? How do they differ?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 213

The Mechanical Requirements of Writing. Compared with speaking, writing is more diffi- cult because it involves spelling, punctuation, and actually forming the letters. These many mechanical aspects of writing can be a burden for all writers, but particularly for young writers. For example, when youngsters such as the child in the photo are absorbed by the task of printing letters correctly, the quality of their writing usually suffers (Medwell  & Wray, 2014; Olinghouse, 2008). As children master printed and cursive letters, they can pay more attention to other aspects of writing. Similarly, correct spelling and good sen- tence structure are particularly hard for younger writers; as they learn to spell and to gen- erate clear sentences, they write more easily and more effectively (Graham et al., 1997; McCutchen et al., 1994).

Skill in Revising. Skilled writers revise and revise, then revise some more. Unfortunately, young writers often don’t revise at all—the first draft is usually the final draft. To make matters worse, when young writers revise, the changes do not necessarily improve their writing (Fitzgerald, 1987). As children develop, they’re better able to find problems with their writing and to know how to correct them, (MacArthur, 2015).

Teaching Children to Write More Effectively Writing well is challenging because many complex skills are involved. Word processing software makes writing easier by handling some of these skills (e.g., checking spelling, simplifying revising), and research indicates that writing improves when people use word processing software (Clements, 1995; Rogers & Graham, 2008).

Fortunately, students can be taught to write better. When instruction focuses on the building blocks of effective writing— strategies for planning, drafting, and revising text—students’ writing improves substantially (Graham & Perin, 2007; Tracy, Reid, & Graham, 2009). For example, one successful program for teaching writing—the Self-Regulated Strategy Development in Writing program—tells students that POW + TREE is a trick that good writers use. As you can see in ◗Figure 6.6, POW pro- vides young writers with a general plan for writing; TREE tells them how to organize their writing in a nicely structured para- graph (Harris & Graham, 2017).

Of course, mastering writing skill often spans all of childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Much the same can be said for mas- tering math skills, as we’ll see in the next section.

Math Skills By kindergarten, children have mastered counting and use this skill as the starting point for learning to add. For instance, sup- pose you ask a child to solve the following problem: “John had four oranges. Then Mary gave him two more oranges. How many oranges does John have now?” Many 6-year-old children count to solve the problem. They first count out four fingers on one hand, then count out two more on the other. Finally, they count all six fingers on both hands. To subtract, they do the same procedure in reverse (Siegler & Jenkins, 1989; Siegler & Shrager, 1984). After children begin to receive formal arithmetic instruction in first grade, addition problems are less often solved by counting aloud or by counting fingers ( Jordan et al., 2008). Instead, children add and subtract by counting mentally. That is, children act as if they are counting silently, beginning with the larger number and then adding on. By age 8 or 9, children

Writing can be particularly hard for young children who are still learning how to print or write cursive letters.

ZU M

A P

re ss

, I nc

./ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

●◗ Figure 6.6 The POW + TREE strategy for good writing provides young writers with a general plan for writing (POW) and a structure for a paragraph (TREE).

POW P Pick my idea O Organize my notes W Write and say more

TREE

T TOPIC SENTENCE Tell what you believe!

R REASONS - 3 or More Why do I believe this? Will my readers believe this?

E EXPLAIN Reasons Say more about each reason.

E ENDING Wrap it up right!

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

214 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

have learned the addition tables so well that sums of the single-digit integers (from 0  to 9) are facts that can be simply retrieved from memory ( Ashcraft, 1982).

As children learn arithmetic and math in school, they master mathematical concepts—abstract, general principles—as well as math- ematical procedures—actions that solve problems. For example, chil- dren learn the concept that fractions can be ordered from smallest to largest (e.g., ¼ < ½ < ¾) and they learn a procedure for adding fractions that have different denominators. Understanding concepts helps chil- dren to master procedures, and mastering procedures helps children to understand concepts (Rittle-Johnson, 2017).

Several techniques help children master math concepts and math procedures. One is asking students to show students two solutions to a problem and ask students to compare them. Another is to ask stu- dents to explain new information, such as a solution to a problem. Both of these methods increase children’s knowledge of math concepts and procedures, apparently by requiring students to pay attention to the underlying mathematical principles, not just the surface features of a problem (Rittle-Johnson & Schneider, 2015). These methods are valu- able in helping elementary school students master arithmetic and in helping high school students master algebra, geometry, trigonometry, and calculus.

Comparing U.S. Students with Students in Other Countries When compared with students worldwide in terms of math skills, U.S. students don’t fare well. For example, ◗Figure 6.7 shows the math results from a major international comparison (Organisa- tion for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2016). U.S. high

school students have substantially lower scores than do high school students in several nations. Phrased another way, the very best U.S. students only perform at the level of average students in many Asian countries. What’s more, these differences in math achievement have been found for at least 25 years and for achievement in elementary school, middle school, and high school (Stevenson & Lee, 1990).

Why do American students rate so poorly? The Real People feature has some answers.

Think About It What information-processing skills may contribute to growth in children’s arithmetic skills?

Young children often solve addition problems by counting, either on their fingers or in their head.

JD C/

Co rb

is /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Shin-ying is an 11-year-old attending school in Taipei, the largest city in Taiwan. Like most fifth graders, Shin-ying is in school from 8 a.m. until 4 p.m. daily. Most evenings, she spends two to three hours doing homework. This academic routine is grueling by U.S. standards, where fifth graders typically spend six to seven hours in school each day and less than an hour doing homework. We asked Shin-ying what she thought of school and schoolwork. Her answers surprised us.

US: Why do you go to school? SHIN-YING: I like what we study. US: Any other reasons? SHIN-YING: The things that I learn in school

are useful. US: What about homework? Why do you

do it? SHIN-YING: My teacher and my parents think

it’s important. And I like doing it. US: Do you think you would do nearly as well

in school if you didn’t work so hard?

SHIN-YING: Oh no. The best students are always the ones who work the hardest.

Schoolwork is the focal point of Shin-ying’s life. Although many American school chil- dren are unhappy when schoolwork intrudes on time for play and television, Shin-ying is enthusiastic about school and school-related activities.

Real People Applying Human Development

Shin-Ying Loves School

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 215

●◗ Figure 6.7 Compared with students in other developed countries, U.S. students fare poorly on tests of math skills.

Average math score

0 100 200 300 400 500 600

Mexico Chile

Turkey Greece Israel

Hungary Sweden

United States Slovak Republic

Spain Italy

Portugal Norway

Luxembourg Iceland

United Kingdom France

Czech Republic New Zealand

Denmark Slovenia

Ireland Australia

Austria Germany Belgium

Poland Canada Finland Estonia

Netherlands Switzerland

Japan Korea

Data from Kelly et al. (2013).

Many of Shin-ying’s comments are typical of elementary school students in Asia. Com- pared with students in the United States, students in Asian countries often spend more time on homework and are more enthusiastic about homework. Asian parents often set higher stan- dards for their students than American parents do; and Asian parents often focus on their chil- dren’s mistakes, while American parents point to their children’s successes. Finally, students in Asia often spend more time in school and more of this time is devoted to activities than in the United States (Ni, Chiu, & Cheng, 2010; Pomerantz et al., 2014; Stevenson & Lee, 1990).

What can Americans learn from Japanese and Taiwanese educational systems? Experts (Stevenson & Stigler, 1992; Tucker, 2011) suggest several ways American schools could be improved:

●● Improve teachers’ training by allowing them to work closely with older, more experienced teachers and give them more free time to prepare lessons and correct students’ work.

●● Organize instruction around sound principles of learning, such as providing multiple exam- ples of concepts and giving students adequate opportunities to practice newly acquired skills.

●● Create curricula that emphasize problem-solving and critical thinking. ●● Set higher standards for children who need to spend more time and effort in school-

related activities in order to achieve those standards. Changing teaching practices and attitudes toward achievement would begin to reduce the gap between American students and students in other industrialized countries, par- ticularly Asian countries. Ignoring the problem will mean an increasingly undereducated workforce and citizenry in a complex world.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

216 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Effective Schools, Effective Teachers Because education is run locally in the United States, American education is a smorgas- bord. Schools differ along many dimensions, including their emphasis on academic goals and the involvement of parents. Teachers, too, differ in many ways, such as how they run their classrooms and how they teach. These and other variables do affect student achieve- ment, as you’ll see in the next few pages. Let’s begin with school-based influences.

School-Based Influences on Student Achievement Roosevelt High School, in the center of Detroit, has an enrollment of 3,500 students in grades 9 through 12. Opened in 1936, the building shows its age. The rooms are drafty, the desks are decorated with generations of graffiti, and new technology means an overhead projector. Nevertheless, attendance at Roosevelt is good. Most students graduate, and many continue their education at community colleges and state universities. Southport High School, in Newark, has about the same enrollment as Roosevelt High, and the build- ing is about the same age. Yet truancy is commonplace at Southport, where fewer than half the students graduate and almost none go to college.

Although these schools are hypothetical, they accurately depict a common outcome in the United States. Some schools are more successful than others, whether success is defined in terms of the percentage of students who are literate, who graduate, or who go to college. Why? Researchers (El Nokali, Bachman, & Votruba-Drzal, 2010; Good & Brophy, 2008; Preston et al., 2017) have identified a number of characteristics of schools where students typically succeed.

●● Staff and students alike understand that academic excellence is the primary goal and schools provide high-quality instruction to allow students to achieve that goal. The school day emphasizes instruction—not nonacademic activities—and students are recognized publicly for their academic accomplishments. Teachers engage students through dialogue, feedback, and responsiveness.

●● The school climate is safe and nurturing. Students know that they can devote their energy to learning (instead of worrying about being harmed in school) and that the staff truly cares that they succeed.

●● Parents and communities are involved. Parents may participate through parent–teacher organizations or informally (e.g., helping a teacher to prepare a lesson or to grade papers). Social agencies may provide mentors who guide students at risk for dropping out; busi- nesses partner with schools to provide internships or vocational training. Such involve- ment signals that a network of concerned adults is committed to students’ success.

●● Progress of students, teachers, and programs is monitored. The only way to know whether schools are succeeding is by measuring performance. Students, teachers, and programs are evaluated regularly, using objective measures that reflect academic goals.

In schools where these guidelines are followed regularly, students usually succeed. In schools where the guidelines are ignored, students more often fail.

Of course, on a daily basis, individual teachers have the most potential for impact. Let’s see how teachers can influence their students’ achievement.

Teacher-Based Influences on Student Achievement In most schools, some teachers are highly sought after because their classes are very successful: Students learn and the classroom climate is usually positive. What are the keys to the success of these master teachers? Research reveals that several factors are critical for students’ achievement (Allen et al., 2013; Good & Brophy, 2008; Walberg, 1995). Students tend to learn the most when teachers:

●● Manage the classroom effectively so that they can devote most of their time to instruction. When teachers spend much time disciplining stu- dents or when students do not move smoothly from one class activity to the next, instructional time is wasted and students are apt to learn less.

In successful schools, the child’s parents are involved—often as tutors.

M ic

ha el

J . D

oo lit

tle /T

he Im

ag e

W or

ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 217

●● Believe that they are responsible for their students’ learning and that their students will learn when taught well. When students don’t understand a new topic, these teachers may repeat the original instruction (in case the student missed something) or create new instruction (in case the student heard everything but just didn’t “get it”). These teachers keep plugging away because they feel at fault if students don’t learn.

●● Emphasize mastery of topics. Teachers introduce a topic, then give students many opportunities to understand, practice, and apply the topic. Just as you’d find it hard to go directly from driver’s education to race car driving, students more often achieve when they grasp a new topic thoroughly, then gradually move to other more advanced topics.

●● Teach actively. Effective teachers don’t just talk or give students an endless stream of worksheets. Instead, they demonstrate topics concretely or provide hands-on demonstrations for students. They also have students participate in class activ- ities and encourage students to interact, generating ideas and solving problems together.

●● Pay careful attention to pacing. Teachers present material slowly enough so that stu- dents can understand a new concept but not so slowly that students get bored.

●● Value tutoring. Teachers work with students individually or in small groups so they can gear their instruction to each student’s level and check each student’s understand- ing. They also encourage peer tutoring, in which more capable students tutor less capable students. Children who are tutored by peers do learn, and so do the tutors because teaching helps tutors organize their knowledge.

●● Teach students techniques for monitoring and managing their own learning. Students are more likely to achieve when they are taught how to recognize the aims of school tasks as well as effective strategies for achieving those aims.

Thus, what makes for effective schools and teachers? No single element is crucial. Instead, many factors contribute to making some schools and teachers remarkably effective. Some of the essential ingredients include parents who are involved; teachers who care deeply about their students’ learning and manage classrooms well; and a school that is safe, is nurturing, and emphasizes achievement.

Peer tutoring can be very effective; both the tutored student and the tutor usually learn.

H ol

bo x/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Think About It Would some ways to promote students’ learning be more appropriate for students in Piaget’s concrete-operational stage? Would some be better for students in the formal-operational stage?

recall 1. Important prereading skills include knowing letters and

_______________.

2. Beginning readers typically recognize words by sound- ing them out; with greater experience, readers often _______________.

3. Older and more experienced readers understand more of what they read because the capacity of working memory increases, they have more general knowledge of the world, _______________, and they are more likely to use appropriate reading strategies.

4. Children typically use a(n) _______________to organize their writing.

5. Children write best when _______________.

6. The simplest way to solve addition problems is to _______________; the most advanced way is to retrieve sums from long-term memory.

7. Compared with students in U.S. elementary schools, students in Asia spend more time in school, and a greater proportion of that time is _______________.

8. In schools where students usually succeed, academic excel- lence is a priority, the school is safe and nurturant, progress of students and teachers is monitored, and _______________.

9. Effective teachers manage classrooms well, believe they are responsible for their students’ learning, _______________, teach actively, pay attention to pacing, value tutoring, and show children how to monitor their own learning.

Interpret ●● Review the research on pages 211–212 regarding factors

associated with skilled reading comprehension. Which of these factors—if any—might also contribute to skilled writing?

Apply ●● Imagine two children, both entering first grade. One has mas-

tered prereading skills, can sound out many words, and recog- nizes a rapidly growing set of words. The second child knows most of the letters of the alphabet but knows only a handful of letter–sound correspondences. How are these differences in reading skills likely to lead to different experiences in first grade?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 6.4

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

218 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How much do school-age children grow?

●● How do motor skills improve during the elementary school years?

●● Are American children physically fit?

●● What are the consequences of participating in sports?

6.5 Physical Development

Miguel and Dan are 9-year-olds playing organized baseball for the first time. Miguel’s coach is always upbeat. He constantly emphasizes the positive. When Miguel’s team lost a game 12 to 2, the coach complimented all the players on their play in the field and at bat. In contrast, Dan’s coach was livid when the team lost, and he was extremely critical of three players who made errors that contributed to the loss. Miguel thinks that baseball is great, but Dan can hardly wait for the season to be over.

During the elementary school years, children grow steadily and their motor skills continue to improve. We’ll trace these changes in the first two parts of this section. Then we’ll see whether U.S. children are physically fit. We’ll end the section by examining children’s par- ticipation in sports and see how coaches like those in the vignette influence children in organized sports.

Growth Physical growth during the elementary school years continues at the steady pace estab- lished during the preschool years. From ◗ Figure 6.8, you can see that a typical 6-year-old weighs about 45 pounds and is 45 inches tall, but grows to about 90 pounds and 60 inches by age 12. In other words, most children gain about 8 pounds and 2 to 3 inches per year. Many parents notice that their elementary school children outgrow shoes and pants more rapidly than they outgrow sweaters, shirts, or jackets; this is because most of the increase in height comes from the legs, not the trunk.

●◗ Figure 6.8 Height and weight increase steadily during the elementary school years.

64

62

60

58

56

54

52

50

48

46

44

42

40

H ei

gh t

(i n

i n

ch es

)

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Age (in years)

140

130

120

110

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

W ei

gh t

(i n

p ou

n d

s)

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Age (in years)

95th percentile

50th percentile

5th percentile 95th percentile

50th percentile

5th percentile ©

2 01

9 C

en ga

ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 219

Boys and girls are about the same size for most of these years (which is why they are combined in the figure), but girls are more likely than boys to enter puberty toward the end of the elementary school years. Once girls enter puberty, they grow rapidly and become much bigger than the boys their age. (We have more to say about this in Chapter 8.) Thus, at ages 11 and 12, the average girl is about half an inch taller than the average boy.

To support this growth and to provide energy for their busy lives, school-age children need to eat more. Although preschool children need only consume about 1,500 to 1,700 calories per day, the average 7- to 10-year-old needs about 2,400 calories each day. Of course, the exact figure depends on the child’s age and size and can range anywhere from roughly 1,700 to 3,300 calories daily.

As was true for preschool children, elementary school children need a well-balanced diet. They should eat regularly from each of the major food groups: grains, vegetables, fruits, milk, meat, and beans. Too often children consume “empty” calories from sweets that have very little nutritional value.

It’s also important that school-age children eat breakfast. At this age, many children skip breakfast—often because they’re too rushed in the morning. In fact, breakfast should provide about one-fourth of a child’s daily calories. When children are well fed, they’re better able to pay attention in class and this translates into better grades and higher scores on achievement tests—for children who are well nourished overall as well as for children who are undernourished (Adolphus, Lawton, & Dye, 2013; Littlecott et al., 2016). There- fore, parents should plan mornings so children have enough time for breakfast.

Development of Motor Skills Elementary school children’s greater size and strength contribute to improved motor skills. During these years, children steadily run faster and jump farther. For example, ◗ Figure 6.9 shows how far a typical boy and girl can throw a ball and how far they can jump (in the standing long jump). By the time children are 11 years old, they can throw a ball three times farther than they could at age 6 and can jump nearly twice as far.

●◗ Figure 6.9 Between 6 and 11 years, children’s motor skills improve considerably.

Girls throw a ball Boys throw a ball

Feet Feet

Standing long jump

Inches

Age 6 Age 8 Age 11

10 20 4030 50 60

12 24 36 48 60 72

2010 30 40 50 60 70 9080 100

Inches 12 24 36 48 60 72

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

220 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Fine motor skills also improve as children move through the elementary school years. Children’s greater dexterity is evident in a host of activities ranging from typing, writing, and drawing to working on puzzles, playing the piano, and building model cars. Children gain much greater control over their fingers and hands, making them more nimble. This greater fine motor coordination is obvious in children’s handwriting.

Gender Differences in Motor Skills In both gross and fine motor skills, there are gender differences in performance levels. For example, girls tend to excel in fine motor skills; their handwriting tends to be better than that of boys. Girls also excel in gross motor skills that require flexibility and balance, such as tumbling. On gross motor skills that emphasize strength, boys usually have the advan- tage. Figure 6.9 shows that boys throw and jump farther than girls.

Some of the gender differences in gross motor skills that require strength reflect the fact that as children approach and enter puberty, girls’ bodies have proportionately more fat and less muscle than do boys’ bodies. This difference explains why, for example, boys can hang by their hands or arms from a bar much longer than girls can. However, for other gross motor skills such as running, throwing, and catching, body composition is less important (Duff, Ericsson, & Baluch, 2007; Smoll & Schutz, 1990). In these cases, children’s experience is crucial. During recess, elementary school girls are more often found playing on a swing set, jumping rope, or perhaps talking quietly in a group; in contrast, boys are playing football or shooting baskets. Many girls and their parents believe that sports and physical fitness are less valuable for girls than boys. Consequently, girls spend less time in these sports and fitness-related activities than boys, depriving them of the opportunities to practice that are essential for developing motor skills (Fredricks & Eccles, 2005).

Physical Fitness Exercise has many benefits: Children who are active physically are healthier, have better self-esteem, and greater achievement in school (Booth et al., 2014; Lubans et al., 2016; Rangul et al., 2012). Because of these benefits, the President’s Council on Physical Fit- ness, Sports and Nutrition has long recommended that children and adolescents spend 60 minutes daily in physical activity. Unfortunately, few youth—about half of elementary school children and one-third of high school students meet this goal (President’s Council on Physical Fitness, Sports and Nutrition, 2012).

Experts recommend many ways to increase children’s and adolescent’s fitness. At school, physical education classes can be more frequent, longer, and spend more time in vigorous activity; in addition, classroom time can include regular stints of physical activ- ity. Children and adolescents can be encouraged to walk or ride a bicycle to work, and communities can support these activities by building sidewalks and bike lanes (President’s Council on Physical Fitness, Sports and Nutrition, 2012). Families can encourage fitness, too. Instead of spending an afternoon watching TV, parents and children can go biking or play Pokémon Go. Or families can play exergames, which are digital games such as Dance Dance Revolution that combine video gaming with exercise; playing these games regularly enhances physical fitness (Best, 2013).

Participating in Sports Children’s greater motor skill means they are able to participate in many team sports, includ- ing baseball, softball, basketball, and soccer. Obviously, when children play  sports, they get exercise and improve their motor skills. But there are other benefits as well. Sports can enhance participants’ self-esteem and can help them to learn initiative (Bowker, 2006; Eime at al., 2013). Sports can provide children with a chance to learn important social skills, such as how to work effectively (often in complementary roles) as part of a group (Brenner, 2016). And playing sports allows children to use their emerging cognitive skills as they devise new playing strategies or modify the rules of a game.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 221

These benefits of participating in sports are balanced by potential hazards. Special- izing in one sport early—before early or mid-adolescence—can lead to injuries because of overuse and can limit children’s peer relationships to others participating in the same sport (Brenner, 2016). Also, several studies have linked adolescents’ participation in sports to delinquent and antisocial behavior (e.g., Gardner, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). However, outcomes are usually positive when sports participation is combined with participation in activities that involve adults, such as school, religious, or youth groups (Linver, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Zarrett et al., 2009). But these potential benefits hinge on the adults who are involved. When adult coaches like the one in the opening vignette encourage their players and emphasize skill development, children usually enjoy playing, often improve their skills, and increase their self-esteem. In contrast, when coaches emphasize winning over skill development and criticize or punish players for bad plays, children lose interest and stop playing (Curran et al., 2015). And when adolescents find sports too stressful, they often get “burned out”—they lose interest and quit (Lu et al., 2016).

To encourage youth to participate, adults (and parents) need to have realistic expecta- tions for children and coach positively, praising children instead of criticizing them. And they need to remember that children play games for recreation, which means they should have fun!

Think About It What skills of concrete-operational thinking make it possible for children to participate in organized sports?

recall 1. Boys and girls grow at about the same rate during ele-

mentary school years, but at the end of this period, girls _______________.

2. When children skip breakfast, _______________.

3. Boys typically have the advantage of gross motor skills that emphasize strength, but girls tend to have the advantage of _______________.

4. Children may lose interest in sports and quit playing if coaches _______________, and criticize or punish players for mistakes.

Interpret ●● What are the pros and cons of children and adolescents par-

ticipating in organized sports?

Apply ●● Describe how participation in sports illustrates connections

between motor, cognitive, and social development.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 6.5

When adult coaches encourage their team instead of criticizing mistakes, children are likely to enjoy playing sports.

iS to

ck .c

om /k

al i9

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

222 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

6.1 Cognitive Development

what are the distinguishing characteristics of thought during Piaget’s concrete-operational and formal-operational stages?

●● In progressing to Piaget’s stage of concrete operations, chil- dren become less egocentric, rarely confuse appearances with reality, and are able to reverse their thinking. They now solve perspective-taking and conservation problems correctly. Thinking at this stage is limited to the concrete and the real.

●● With the onset of formal-operational thinking, adolescents can think hypothetically and reason abstractly. In deductive reasoning, they understand that conclusions are based on logic, not on experience.

how do children use strategies and monitoring to improve learning and remembering?

●● Rehearsal and other memory strategies are used to trans- fer information from working memory, a temporary store of information, to long-term memory, a permanent store of knowledge. Children begin to rehearse at about age 7 or 8 and take up other strategies as they grow older.

●● Effective use of strategies for learning and remembering begins with an analysis of the goals of a learning task. It also includes monitoring one’s performance to determine whether the strategy is working. Teachers can help students master these skills.

6.2 Aptitudes for School

what is the nature of intelligence?

●● Traditional approaches to intelligence led to hierarchical theories that include both general intelligence and specific skills, such as verbal and spatial ability.

●● Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences proposes nine distinct intelligences. Three are found in psycho- metric theories (linguistic, logical-mathematical, and spatial intelligence), but six are new (musical, bodily- kinesthetic, interpersonal, intrapersonal, naturalistic, and existential intelligence). Gardner’s theory has stimulated research on nontraditional forms of intelligence, such as emotional intelligence. The theory also has implications for education—suggesting, for example, that schools should adjust teaching to each child’s unique intellectual strengths.

●● According to Robert Sternberg, intelligence is defined as using abilities to achieve short- and long-term goals and

depends on three abilities: analytic ability to analyze a prob- lem and generate a solution, creative ability to deal adap- tively with novel situations, and practical ability to know what solutions will work.

why were intelligence tests first developed? what are their features?

●● Binet created the first intelligence test to identify stu- dents who would have difficulty in school. Using this work, Terman created the Stanford-Binet in 1916; it remains an important intelligence test. The Stanford-Binet introduced the concept of the intelligence quotient (IQ): MA/CA × 100.

how well do intelligence tests work?

●● Intelligence tests are reasonably valid measures of achieve- ment in school. They also predict people’s performance in the workplace and their longevity.

how do heredity and environment influence intelligence?

●● Evidence for the impact of heredity on IQ comes from the findings that (1) siblings’ IQ scores become more alike as siblings become more similar genetically and (2) adopted children’s IQ scores are more like their biological parents’ test scores than their adoptive parents’ scores. Evidence for the influence of the environment comes from the impact on IQ scores of home environments, historical change, and intervention programs.

how and why do test scores vary for different racial and ethnic groups?

●● There are substantial differences between ethnic groups in their average scores on IQ tests. This difference is attrib- uted to the greater likelihood of Latino American and Afri- can American youth being economically disadvantaged as well as stereotype threat and test-taking skills. IQ scores remain valid predictors of school success because middle- class experience is often a prerequisite for school success.

6.3 Special Children, Special Needs

what are the characteristics of gifted children?

●● Traditionally, gifted children have high scores on IQ tests. Modern definitions of giftedness have been broadened to include exceptional talent in the arts. However defined, giftedness must be nurtured by parents and teachers alike. Contrary to folklore, gifted children usually are socially mature and emotionally stable.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e r 6 : O F F T O S C H O O L 223

what are the different forms of disability?

●● Individuals with intellectual disability have IQ scores of 70 or lower and deficits in adaptive behavior. Biomedical, social, behavioral, and educational factors place children at risk for intellectual disability. Children with learning disabil- ity have normal intelligence but have difficulty mastering specific academic subjects. The most common is develop- mental dyslexia, which involves difficulty reading individual words because children haven’t mastered language sounds.

what are the distinguishing features of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder?

●● Children with ADHD are distinguished by being hyperac- tive, inattentive, and impulsive. Treating ADHD with medi- cation and psychosocial treatment is effective in the short run but does not “cure” children of the disorder.

6.4 Academic Skills

what are the components of skilled reading?

●● Reading includes a number of component skills. Preread- ing skills include knowing letters and the sounds associated with them. Word recognition is the process of identifying a word. Beginning readers more often accomplish this by sounding out words; advanced readers more often retrieve a word from long-term memory. Comprehension, the act of extracting meaning from text, improves with age as a result of several factors: Vocabulary and working memory capac- ity increase, readers gain more world knowledge, and read- ers are better able to monitor what they read and to match their reading strategies to the goals of the reading task.

As children develop, how does their writing improve?

●● As children develop, their writing improves, which reflects several factors: They know more about the world and so have more to say; they use more effective ways of organiz- ing their writing; they master the mechanics of writing (e.g., handwriting, spelling); and they become more skilled at revising their writing.

how do arithmetic skills change during the elementary school years? how do U.S. students compare with students in other countries?

●● Children first add and subtract by counting, but soon they use more effective strategies such as retrieving addition facts directly from memory. In mathematics, U.S. students lag behind students in most other industrialized countries, chiefly because of cultural differences in time spent on schoolwork and homework and in parents’ attitudes toward school, effort, and ability.

what are the hallmarks of effective schools and effective teachers?

●● Schools influence students’ achievement in many ways. Students are most likely to achieve when their school emphasizes academic excellence, has a safe and nurturing environment, monitors pupils’ and teachers’ progress, and encourages parents to be involved.

●● Students achieve at higher levels when their teachers man- age classrooms effectively, take responsibility for their students’ learning, teach mastery of material, pace material well, value tutoring, and show children how to monitor their own learning.

6.5 Physical Development

how much do school-age children grow?

●● Elementary school children grow at a steady pace, but more so in their legs than in the trunk. Boys and girls tend to be about the same size for most of these years.

●● School-age children need approximately 2,400 calo- ries daily, preferably drawn from each of the basic food groups. Children need to eat breakfast, a meal that should provide approximately one-fourth of their calories. Without breakfast, children often have trouble concen- trating in school.

how do motor skills develop during the elementary school years?

●● Fine and gross motor skills improve substantially over the elementary school years, reflecting children’s greater size and strength. Girls tend to excel in fine motor skills that emphasize dexterity as well as in gross motor skills that require flexibility and balance; boys tend to excel in gross motor skills that emphasize strength. Although some of these differences reflect differences in body makeup, they also reflect different cultural expectations regarding motor skills for boys and girls.

Are American children physically fit?

●● Many American school children don’t meet today’s stan- dards for being physically fit, and childhood obesity is a growing concern.

what are the consequences of participating in sports?

●● Many school-age children participate in team sports. Bene- fits of participation include exercise, enhanced self-esteem, and improved social skills. But participation sometimes leads to antisocial behavior, and when adults are involved, they sometimes overemphasize competition, which can turn “play” into “work.”

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

224 PA rT T w o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Test Yourself Recall Answers 6.1 1. concrete-operational 2. formal-operational 3. determined the goal of the memory task 4. monitoring 6.2 1. hierarchical 2. bodily- kinesthetic 3. teach in a manner that engages as many different intelligences as possible 4. analytic 5. biological 6. intervention stud- ies 7. culture-fair intelligence tests 6.3 1. are broader and include exceptional talent in many areas 2. divergent 3. problems adapting to the environment 4. behavioral 5. phonological awareness (distinguishing language sounds) 6. inattentiveness 7. psychosocial treatment that improves children’s cognitive and social skills 6.4 1. sounds associated with each letter 2. retrieve words from long-term memory 3.  they monitor their comprehension more effectively 4. knowledge-telling strategy 5. the topic is familiar to them 6. count on one’s fingers 7. devoted to academic activities 8. parents are involved 9. emphasize mastery of topics 6.5 1. are more likely to enter puberty and grow rapidly 2. they often have difficulty paying attention and remembering in school 3. fine motor skills that emphasize dexterity 4. emphasize winning over skill development

Key Terms mental operations (190) deductive reasoning (191) working memory (192) long-term memory (192) organization (192) elaboration (192) metamemory (193) metacognitive knowledge (193) cognitive self-regulation (193)

psychometricians (195) emotional intelligence (197) analytic ability (197) creative ability (197) practical ability (197) mental age (MA) (198) intelligence quotient (IQ) (199) culture-fair intelligence tests (202) stereotype threat (203)

divergent thinking (204) intellectual disability (205) learning disability (205) word recognition (210) comprehension (210) phonological awareness (210) knowledge-telling strategy (212) knowledge-transforming strategy (212)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

H er

o Im

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

227

Although you’ve never had a course called “ Culture 101,” your knowledge of your culture is deep. Like all human beings, you have been

learning since birth to live in your culture. Teaching

children the values, roles, and behaviors of their culture—

socialization—is a major goal of all people. In most cul-

tures, the task of socialization falls first to parents. In

the first section of this chapter, we see how parents

set and try to enforce standards of behavior for their

children.

Soon other powerful forces contribute to socializa-

tion. In the second section, you’ll discover how peers

become influential through both individual friendships

and social groups. Next, you’ll learn how the media—

particularly television—contribute to socialization

as well.

As children become socialized, they begin to

understand more about other people. We’ll examine this growing understand-

ing in the last section of the chapter.

Expanding Social Horizons Socioemotional Development in Middle Childhood

7.1 Family Relationships 228 The Family as a System 228 Dimensions and Styles of Parenting 229 Siblings 235 Divorce and Remarriage 238 Parent-Child Relationships Gone Awry:

Child Maltreatment 239

7.2 Peers 243 Friendships 243 Groups 245 Popularity and Rejection 246 Aggressive Children and Their Victims 248

7.3 Electronic Media 249 Television Programs 250 Video Games 251

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Playing a Video Game Improves Children’s Visual-Spatial Skill 251

Social Media 252

7.4 Understanding Others 253 Describing Others 253

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Tell Me About a Girl You Like a Lot 254

Understanding What Others Think 254 Prejudice 256

    Summary 258

    Key Terms 260

7

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

228 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

socialization teaching children the values, roles, and behaviors of their culture.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is a systems approach to parenting?

●● What are the primary dimensions of parenting? How do they affect children’s development?

●● What determines how siblings get along? How do first- born, later-born, and only children differ?

●● How do divorce and remarriage affect children?

●● What factors lead children to be maltreated?

7.1 Family Relationships

Tanya and Sheila, both sixth graders, wanted to go to a Selena Gomez concert with two boys from their school. When Tanya asked if she could go, her mom said, “No way!” Tanya replied defiantly, “Why not?” Her mother blew up: “Because I say so. That’s why. Stop bug- ging me.” Sheila wasn’t allowed to go either. When she asked why, her mom said, “I think that you’re still too young to be dating. I don’t mind your going to the concert. If you want to go just with Tanya, that would be fine. What do you think of that?”

The vignette illustrates what we all know from personal experience—parents go about child rearing in many ways. We study these different approaches in this chapter and learn how Tanya and Sheila are likely to be affected by their mothers’ styles of parenting. But we’ll start by considering parents as key components of a broader family system.

The Family as a System Families are rare in the animal kingdom. Only human beings and a handful of other spe- cies form family-like units. Why? Compared with the young in other species, children develop slowly. And because children are unable to care for themselves for many years, the family structure evolved as a way to protect and nurture young children during their development (Bjorklund, Yunger, & Pellegrini, 2002). Of course, modern families serve many other functions as well—they’re economic units, and they provide emotional sup- port—but child rearing is probably the most important family function.

Today, most theorists view families from a contextual perspective (described in Chapter 1) in which families form a system of interacting elements: Parents and children influence one another (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Schermerhorn & Cummings, 2008), and families are part of a much larger system that includes extended family, friends, and teachers as well as institutions that influence development (e.g., schools).

In the systems view, parents influence their children both directly (e.g., by encourag- ing them to study hard) and indirectly (e.g., by being generous and kind to others). And children influence their parents. By their behaviors, attitudes, and interests, children affect how their parents behave toward them. For example, when children resist discipline, par- ents may become less willing to reason and more inclined to use force (Ritchie, 1999).

The systems view reveals other, more subtle influences. For example, fathers’ behav- iors can affect mother—child relationships; a demanding husband may leave his wife with little time, energy, or interest in helping her daughter with her homework. Or when sib- lings argue constantly, parents may become preoccupied with avoiding problems rather than encouraging their children’s development.

The family itself is embedded in other social systems, such as neighborhoods and reli- gious institutions (Parke & Buriel, 1998). These other institutions can affect family dynam- ics. Sometimes they simplify child rearing, as when neighbors are trusted friends and can help care for each others’ children. However, sometimes they complicate child rearing, as when nearby relatives create friction within the family. At times, the impact of the larger systems is indirect, as when work schedules cause a parent to be away from home or when schools eliminate programs that benefit children.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 229

◗ Figure 7.1 summarizes the many interactive influences that exist in a systems view of families. In the remainder of this section, we’ll describe parents’ influences on children and then discuss how children affect their parents’ behavior.

Dimensions and Styles of Parenting Parenting can be described in terms of general dimensions that are like person- ality traits in representing stable aspects of parental behavior that hold across different situations (Holden & Miller, 1999). When viewed this way, two general dimensions of parental behavior emerge. One is the degree of warmth and responsiveness that parents show their children. Some parents are openly warm and affectionate with their children. They are involved with them, respond to their emotional needs, and spend much time with them. Other parents are relatively uninvolved with their children and sometimes even hostile toward them. These parents often seem more focused on their own needs and interests than those of their children. Warm parents enjoy hearing their children describe the day’s activities; uninvolved or hostile parents aren’t interested, considering it a waste of their time. Warm parents see when their children are upset and try to comfort them; uninvolved or hostile parents pay lit- tle attention to their children’s emotional states and invest little effort in comforting them when they’re upset. As you might expect, children benefit from warm and responsive parenting (Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, 1997; Zhou et al., 2002).

A second general dimension of parental behavior involves control, which comes in two forms (Grusec, 2011). Psycholog- ical  control refers to parents’ efforts to manipulate their children’s emotional states by, for example, withdrawing their love or making children feel guilty. Behavioral control refers to parents’ efforts to set rules for their children and to impose limits on what children can and cannot do. Some parents are dictatorial: They try to regulate every facet of their children’s lives, like a puppeteer controlling a marionette. At the other extreme are parents who exert little or no control over their children: These children do whatever they want without asking their parents first or worrying about their parents’ response.

What’s best for children is minimal psychological control combined with an intermedi- ate amount of behavioral control. Children typically fare best when parents set reasonable standards for children’s behavior, expect their children to meet those standards, and know where their children are, what they’re doing, and with whom (Racz & McMahon, 2011).

Parenting Styles Combining the dimensions of warmth and control produces four prototypic styles of par- enting, as shown in ◗ Figure 7.2 (Baumrind, 1975, 1991).

●● authoritarian parenting combines high control with little warmth. These parents lay down the rules and expect them to be followed without discussion. These parents emphasize respect and obedience. There is little give-and-take between parent and child because authoritarian parents do not consider children’s needs or wishes. This style is illustrated by Tanya’s mother in the opening vignette. She feels no obligation to explain her decision.

●● authoritative parenting combines a fair degree of parental control with being warm and responsive to children. Authoritative parents explain rules and encourage discus- sion. This style is exemplified by Sheila’s mother in the opening vignette. She explained why she did not want the girls going to the concert with the boys and encouraged her daughter to discuss the issue with her.

authoritarian parenting Style of parenting in which parents show high levels of control and low levels of warmth toward their children.

authoritative parenting Style of parenting in which parents use a moderate amount of control and are warm and responsive to their children.

●◗ Figure 7.1 In a systems view of families, parents and children influence each other; this interacting family unit is also influenced by other forces outside the family.

MotherFather Family

Culture

Children

Neighborhood W ork

Rel igi

ou s

org ani

zat ion

s

E xt

en de

d fa

m ily

School

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

230 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

●● Permissive parenting offers warmth and caring but little parental control. These parents generally accept their children’s behavior and punish them infrequently. An indulgent-permissive parent would readily agree to Tanya’s or Sheila’s request to go to the concert simply because it is something the child wants to do.

●● Uninvolved parenting provides neither warmth nor control. Indifferent-uninvolved parents provide for their children’s basic physical and emotional needs but little else. They try to minimize the time they spend with their children and avoid becoming emotionally involved with them. If Tanya had parents with this style, she might have gone to the concert without asking, knowing that her parents wouldn’t care and would rather not be bothered.

Authoritative parenting is best for most children most of the time. Children and adolescents with authoritative parents tend to have higher grades, they are responsi- ble, self-reliant, and friendly, and they’re less likely to drink alcohol (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Aunola, Stattin, & Nurmi, 2000; Merianos et al., 2015). In contrast, children with authoritarian parents are often unhappy, have low self-esteem, and frequently are overly aggressive (e.g., Braza et al., 2015; Silk et al., 2003; Zhou et al., 2008). Finally, children with permissive parents are often impulsive and have little self-control, whereas chil-

dren with uninvolved parents often do poorly in school and are aggressive (Aunola et al., 2000; Barber & Olsen, 1997; Driscoll, Russell, & Crockett, 2008). Thus, children typically thrive on a parental style that combines control, warmth, and affection.

Variations Associated with Culture and Socioeconomic Status The general aim of child rearing—helping children become contributing members of their culture—is much the same worldwide (Lansford et al., 2016), and warmth and control are universal aspects of parents’ behavior. But views about the “proper” amount of warmth and the “proper” amount of control vary with particular cultures. Euro- pean Americans want their children to be happy and self-reliant individuals, and they believe these goals are best achieved when parents are warm and exert moderate con- trol (Goodnow, 1992). However, in many countries around the world, individualism is less important than cooperation and collaboration. In China, for example, emotional restraint and obedience are seen as the keys to family harmony (Chao, 2001). Con- sequently, parents in China often rely on an authoritarian style in which they are less often affectionate and expect their children to obey them without question (Lin & Fu, 1990; Zhou et al., 2008).

Another common pattern worldwide is for parents to be warm and controlling (Deater-Deckard et al., 2011). For example, Latino culture typically places greater empha- sis on having strong family ties and respecting the roles of all family members, particularly adults; these values lead parents to be more protective of their children and to set more rules for them (Halgunseth, Ispa, & Rudy, 2006). Thus, cultural values help specify appro- priate ways for parents to interact with their offspring.

Parental styles vary not only across cultures but also within cultures, depending on parents’ socioeconomic status. Within the United States, parents of lower socioeconomic status tend to be more controlling and more punitive—characteristics associated with the authoritarian parenting style—than are parents of higher socioeconomic status (Pace et al., 2017). This difference may reflect educational differences that help to define socioeconomic status. Parents of higher socioeconomic status are, by definition, more edu- cated and consequently often see development as a more complex process requiring the more nuanced and child-friendly approach that marks authoritative parenting (Skinner, 1985). Parents who are relatively uneducated often find themselves employed in positions where they’re used to taking orders from others; when they’re at home, these parents reverse roles and order their children around (Greenberger, O’Neil, & Nagel, 1994).

permissive parenting Style of parenting in which parents offer warmth and caring but little control over their children.

uninvolved parenting Style of parenting in which parents provide neither warmth nor control and minimize the time they spend with their children.

●◗ Figure 7.2 Combining the two dimensions of parental behavior (warmth and con- trol) creates four prototypic styles of parenting.

Parental control

Pa re

n ta

l in

vo lv

em en

t

High

High

Low

Low

Authoritative

Authoritarian

Permissive

Uninvolved

© 2019 Cengage

Authoritative parents are warm and responsive with children and encourage discussion.

Ro b

M ar

m io

n/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 231

Another contributing factor derives from another variable that defines socioeconomic status: income (Melby et al., 2008). Because of their limited financial resources, parents of lower socioeconomic status often lead more stressful lives (e.g., they wonder whether they’ll have enough money at the end of the month for groceries) and are far more likely to live in neighborhoods where violence, drugs, and crime are commonplace. Thus, parents of lower socioeconomic status may be too stressed to invest the energy needed for the warmth and moderate control that define authoritative parenting (Simons et al., 2016).

Genetic Influences on Parenting Families and parenting are adaptations that evolved to provide for children until they mature. In other words, genes linked to behaviors that make for effective parenting (e.g., being nurturing) were more likely to be passed on because they helped children to reach maturity. Consistent with this view, research on twins as parents shows the impact of genetics on parental style. For example, as parents, identical twins are more similar than fraternal twins in the amount of warmth that they express to their children ( McAdams et  al., 2017); apparently heredity makes it easier for some people to be warm parents. Behavior genetic studies also reveal several environmental influences on parental style, including the quality of the parents’ marital relationship and the children themselves (Klahr & Burt, 2014); we’ll examine these later in this module.

Parental Behavior Dimensions and styles are useful as general characterizations of parents, but they tell us little about how parents behave in specific situations and how these parental behaviors influence children’s development. Researchers have identified three specific parental behaviors that influence children: direct instruction, modeling, and feedback.

Direct Instruction and Coaching. Parents often tell their children what to do. But ordering children around—“Clean your room!” “Turn off the TV!”—is not very effective. A better approach is direct instruction, which involves telling a child what to do, when, and why. Instead of just shouting, “Share your candy with your brother!” a parent should explain when and why it’s important to share with a sibling.

In addition, just as coaches help athletes master sports skills, parents can help their youngsters master social and emotional skills. Parents can explain links between emotions and behavior: “Catlin is sad because you broke her crayon” (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996). They can also teach children how to deal with difficult social situations: “When you ask Lindsey if she can sleep over, do it privately so you don’t hurt Ali’s or Hannah’s feel- ings” (Mize & Pettit, 1997). In general, children who get this sort of parental “coaching” tend to be more socially skilled and, not surprisingly, get along better with their peers.

Modeling. Children learn a great deal from parents simply by watching them. The parents’ modeling and the youngsters’ observational learning leads to imitation, so children’s behavior resembles the behavior they observe. Observational learning can also produce counterimitation, learning what should not be done. If an older sibling kicks a friend and parents punish the older sibling, the younger child may learn not to kick others.

Observational learning likely contributes to intergenerational continuity of parenting behavior. Parental behavior is often consistent from one generation to the next. When, for example, parents often use harsh physical punishment to discipline their children, these children will, when they are parents, follow suit (Bailey et al., 2009).

Feedback. By giving feedback to their children, parents indicate whether a behavior is appropriate and should continue or is inappropriate and should stop. Feedback comes in two general forms. Reinforcement is any action that increases the likelihood of the response that it follows. Parents may use praise to reinforce a child’s studying or give a reward for completing household chores. Punishment is any action that discourages the recurrence of the response that it follows. Parents may forbid children to watch television when they get poor grades in school or make children go to bed early for neglecting household chores.

direct instruction telling a child what to do, when, and why.

counterimitation learning what should not be done by observing the behavior.

reinforcement Consequence that increases the likelihood that a behavior will be repeated in the future.

punishment application of an aversive stimulus (e.g., a spanking) or removal of an attractive stimu- lus (e.g., tV viewing).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

232 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Of course, parents have been rewarding and punishing their chil- dren for centuries, so what do psychologists know that parents don’t know already? In fact, researchers have made some surprising dis- coveries concerning the nature of reward and punishment. Parents often unwittingly reinforce the very behaviors they want to discourage, a situation called the negative reinforcement trap (Patterson, 1980). The negative reinforcement trap occurs in three steps, most often between a mother and her son. In the first step, the mother tells her son to do something he doesn’t want to do. She might tell him to clean up his room, to come inside when he’s outdoors playing with friends, or to study instead of watching television. In the next step, the son responds with some behavior that most parents find intoler- able: He argues, complains, or whines for an extended period of time. In the last step, the mother gives in—saying that the son needn’t do as she told him initially—simply to get the son to stop the behavior that is so intolerable. The feedback to the son is that arguing (or com- plaining or whining) works; the mother rewards that behavior by withdrawing the request that the son did not like.

As for punishment, research shows that it works best when

●● administered directly after the undesired behavior occurs, not hours later.

●● an undesired behavior consistently leads to punishment. ●● accompanied by an explanation of why the child was punished

and how punishment can be avoided in the future. ●● the child has a warm, affectionate relationship with the person

administering the punishment.

At the same time, research reveals some serious drawbacks to punishment. One is that punishment is primarily suppressive: Pun- ished responses are stopped, but only temporarily if children do not learn new behaviors to replace those that were punished. For exam-

ple, denying TV to brothers who are fighting stops the undesirable behavior, but fighting is likely to recur unless the boys learn new ways of solving their disputes.

A second drawback is that punishment can have undesirable side effects. Children become upset when they are being punished, which means they often miss the feedback that punishment is meant to convey. A child denied TV for misbehaving may become angry over the punishment itself and ignore why he’s being punished.

Spanking illustrates the problems with punishment. Although used by many parents in the United States and around the world, it is ineffective in getting children to comply with parents and often leads children to aggressive behavior (Altschul, Lee, & Gershoff, 2016). And harsher forms of physical punishment are associated with a range of negative outcomes, including mental health problems, impaired parent–child relationships, and delayed cognitive development (Gershoff, 2013). Because physical punishment is so harm- ful to children, many countries around the world (e.g., Costa Rica, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Spain) have banned it altogether (Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punish- ment of Children, 2011).

One method combines the best features of punishment while avoiding its shortcom- ings. In time-out, a child who misbehaves must briefly sit alone in a quiet, unstimulating location. Some parents have children sit alone in a bathroom; other parents have children sit in a corner of a room. Time-out is punishing because it interrupts the child’s ongoing activity and isolates the child from other family members, toys, books, and generally all forms of rewarding stimulation.

A time-out period usually lasts just a few minutes, which helps parents use the method consistently. During time-out, both parent and child typically calm down. When time-out is over, a parent explains to the child why the punished behavior is objectionable and what the child should do instead. “Reasoning” like this—even with preschool children—is effective

negative reinforcement trap unwittingly reinforcing a behavior you want to discourage.

time-out punishment that involves removing chil- dren who are misbehaving from a situation to a quiet, unstimulating environment.

Parents can use reinforcement to encourage their children to complete tasks they don’t enjoy, such as household chores.

Think About It When 10-year-old Dylan’s family got a puppy, he agreed to walk it every day after school. But when his mom reminds him to do this, he gets angry because he’d rather watch TV. After they argue for about 15 minutes, Dylan’s mom gives up and walks the dog herself. And Dylan goes back to watching TV. Analyze this situation. What could Dylan’s mom do to prevent these regular arguments?

Si m

on W

in na

ll/ Ta

xi /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 233

because it emphasizes why a parent punished initially and how punishment can be avoided in the future.

These techniques can be taught to parents. Many meta-analyses document the effectiveness of programs that teach parenting skills (e.g., using positive rein- forcement and using nonphysical punishment con- sistently), that promote good communication within families, and that make parents feel confident in their ability to be good parents. These kinds of interven- tion programs make parents feel more satisfied with their parenting, lead them to parent more effectively, and reduce children’s behavior problems (Sanders, 2014; Sandler et al., 2015). Thus, although research on genetics shows that the path to good parenting may be easier for some people, most adults can master the skills that foster children’s development.

Influences of the Marital System When Derek returned from 7-Eleven with a six-pack of beer and chips instead of diapers and baby food, Anita exploded. “How could you! I used the last diaper an hour ago!” Hud- dled in the corner of the kitchen, their son Randy watched yet another episode in the daily soap opera that featured Derek and Anita.

Although Derek and Anita aren’t arguing about Randy—in fact, they’re so wrapped up in their conflict that they forget he’s in the room—it’s easy to imagine that such chronic parental conflict is harmful for children. When parents are constantly in conflict, children and adolescents often become anxious, withdrawn, and aggressive and are more prone to chronic diseases and to be abusive in their own romantic relationships (Miller & Chen, 2010; Narayan, Englund, & Egeland, 2013; Rhoades, 2008).

Parental conflict affects children’s development through three distinct mechanisms. First, seeing parents fight jeopardizes a child’s feeling that the family is stable and secure, making a child feel anxious, frightened, and sad (Cummings et al., 2012; Davies et al., 2016). Second, chronic conflict between parents often spills over into the parent–child relationship. When a wife is unhappy with her husband, this often leads her to be unhappy with her children as well (Kouros et al., 2014). Third, when parents invest time and energy fighting with each other, they’re often too tired or too preoccupied to invest themselves in high-quality parenting (Katz & Woodin, 2002).

Of course, all long-term relationships experience conflict at some point. Does this mean that all children bear at least some scars? Not necessarily. Many parents resolve con- flicts in a manner that’s constructive instead of destructive. For example, suppose one par- ent believes that his child should attend a summer camp but the other parent believes it’s too expensive and not worthwhile because the child attended the previous summer. Instead of shouting and name calling (e.g., “You’re always so cheap!”), some parents seek mutually acceptable solutions: The child could attend the camp if she earns money to cover part of the cost, or the child could attend a different, less expensive camp. When disagreements are routinely resolved this way, children respond positively to conflict, apparently because they believe their family is cohesive and can withstand problems (Bergman, Cummings, & Warmuth, 2016). And youth exposed to this sort of constructive approach rely on it when solving conflicts in their peer and romantic relationships (Miga, Gdula, & Allen, 2012).

The extent and resolution of conflict is an obvious way in which the parental system affects children, but it’s not the only way. Another influence concerns parents’ effective- ness as a parenting team. Parenting is far less effective when each parent tries to “go it alone” instead of working with the other to achieve shared goals using methods they both accept. When parents don’t work together, when they compete, or when they limit each other’s access to their children, problems can result; for example, children can become withdrawn (McHale et al., 2002; Scrimgeour et al., 2013).

An effective form of punishment is time-out, in which children sit alone briefly.

St oc

k Co

nn ec

tio n

Bl ue

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

234 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

So far, we’ve seen that to understand parents’ impact on children’s development, we need to consider the nature of the marital relationship as well as parenting style and spe- cific parenting behaviors (e.g., use of feedback). In addition, Figure 7.1 reminds us that forces outside the family can influence parenting and children’s development. To illus- trate, let’s consider work-related influences. One such influence is a parent’s job security: When children believe that their parents may lose their jobs, children’s well-being suf- fers—they’re more likely to be sad or anxious and they’re less likely to believe they can suc- ceed—and their grades in school often go down. Children and adolescents lose self-esteem and find it difficult to concentrate in school when their parents become unemployed or, for that matter, when they worry that their parents may become unemployed (Mauno, Cheng, & Lim, 2017).

Another well-known factor is work-related stress. Not surprisingly, when men and women lead stressful lives at work, they parent less effectively. Some frazzled parents withdraw from family interactions; they seem detached and uninterested, which makes children anxious and upset. Others are less warm and less consistent in their parenting (Cooklin, et al., 2016; Crouter & Bumpus, 2001).

Thus, a person’s work life can profoundly affect children and adolescents by changing the parenting they experience. For now, another way to view family systems in action is by switching perspectives to see how children affect parenting behavior.

Children’s Contributions: Reciprocal Influence At the beginning of this chapter, we emphasized that the family is a dynamic, interactive system with parents and children influencing each other. In fact, children begin at birth to influence the way their parents treat them. Let’s look at two characteristics of children that contribute to this influence.

Age. Parenting changes as children grow. The same parenting that works well with infants and toddlers is inappropriate for adolescents. These age-related changes in parenting are evident in the two basic dimensions of parental behavior: warmth and control. Warmth is beneficial throughout development because toddlers and teens alike enjoy knowing that others care about them. But the manifestation of parental affection changes, becoming more reserved as children develop. The enthusiastic hugging and kissing that delights tod- dlers embarrasses adolescents.

Parental control also changes as children develop. As children develop cognitively and are better able to make their own decisions, parents gradually relinquish control and expect children to be responsible for themselves. As children enter adolescence, they believe that parents have less authority to make decisions for them, especially in the personal domain (Darling, Cumsille, & Martínez, 2008). Parents do relinquish control—although sometimes not as rapidly as adolescents want them to—and increases in decision- making autonomy are associated with greater adolescent well-being (Eagleton, Williams, & Merten, 2016; Wray-Lake, Crouter, & McHale, 2010).

Temperament and Behavior. A child’s temperament can have a powerful effect on parental behavior. To illustrate the reciprocal influence of parents and children, imagine two chil- dren with different temperaments as they respond to a parent’s authoritative style. The first child has an “easy” temperament; she readily complies with parental requests and responds well to family discussions about parental expectations. But suppose the second child has a “difficult” temperament and complies reluctantly or sometimes not at all. Over time, the parent becomes more controlling and less affectionate. The child in turn com- plies even less in the future, leading the parent to adopt an authoritarian parenting style (Brody et al., 2017).

As this example illustrates, parenting behaviors and styles often evolve as a conse- quence of the child’s behavior. With a moderately active young child who is eager to please adults, a parent may discover that a modest amount of control is adequate. But for a very active child who is not as eager to please, a parent may need to be more controlling and directive. Influence is reciprocal: Children’s behavior helps determine how parents treat

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 235

them, and the resulting parental behavior influences children’s behav- ior, which in turn causes parents to change their behavior again (Choe, Olson, & Sameroff, 2013; Schermerhorn, Chow, & Cummings, 2010).

As time goes by, these reciprocal influences lead many families to adopt routine ways of interacting with each other. Some families end up functioning smoothly: Parents and children cooperate, antici- pate each other’s needs, and are generally happy. Unfortunately, other families end up troubled: Disagreements are common, parents spend much time trying unsuccessfully to control their defiant children, and everyone is often angry and upset. Still others are characterized by disengagement: Parents withdraw from each other and are not avail- able to their children ( Sturge-Apple, Davies, & Cummings, 2010). These troubled families do not fare well in the long, but they can be helped with therapy that improves a family’s communication and problem-solving skills (Liddle, 2016).

Although parent–child relationships are central to human development, other rela- tionships within the family are also influential. For many children, relationships with sib- lings are very important, as we’ll see in the next few pages.

Siblings Most children—about 85% in the United States—grow up with brothers and sisters. Sibling relationships are complicated from the beginning. Expectant parents typically are excited by the prospect of another child, and their enthusiasm is contagious: Their children, too, eagerly await the arrival of the newest family member. However, the baby’s arrival prompts varied responses: Some children are distressed, sad, and less responsive to parents, responses that are more common with younger children (Volling, 2012). Parents can minimize their older children’s distress by remaining attentive to their needs (Howe & Ross, 1990).

Many older siblings enjoy helping their parents take care of newborns. Older chil- dren play with, console, or feed the baby or change the baby’s diapers. In middle-class Western families, such caregiving often occurs in the context of play, with parents nearby. But in many developing nations, children—particularly girls—play an important role in providing care for their younger siblings (Zukow-Goldring, 2002). As the infant grows, interactions between siblings become more frequent and more complicated. For example, toddlers tend to talk more to parents than to older siblings. But by the time the younger sibling is 4 years old, the situation is reversed: Now young siblings talk more to older sib- lings than to their mother (Brown & Dunn, 1996). Older siblings become a source of care and comfort for younger siblings when they are distressed or upset (Gass, Jenkins, & Dunn, 2007; Kim et al., 2007).

Older siblings also serve as teachers for their younger siblings, teaching them to play games or to cook simple foods (Maynard, 2002). Finally, when older children do well in school and are popular with peers, younger siblings often follow suit (Brody et al., 2003).

As time goes by, some siblings grow close, becoming best friends in ways that nonsiblings can never be. Other siblings constantly argue, compete, and, overall, do not get along with each other. The basic pattern of sibling interaction seems to be established early in development and remains fairly stable (Kramer, 2010). In gen- eral, siblings who get along as preschoolers continue to get along as young adoles- cents, and siblings who quarrel as preschoolers often quarrel as young adolescents (Dunn, Slomkowski, & Beardsall, 1994).

Getting along with siblings makes growing up easier, for children and par- ents alike. But there are also longer-term effects of the quality of sibling relation- ship on children’s development (McHale, Updegraff, & Whiteman, 2012). When siblings have high-quality relationships, they have better relationships with peers and better well-being overall. Unfortunately, the converse is also true: when siblings have low-quality relationships, they often behave aggressively, bully their peers, and are prone to anxiety and depression (Dirks et al., 2015). Apparently children take

When children respond defiantly to disci- pline, parents often resort to harsher disci- pline in the future.

In many cultures, older siblings regularly provide care for younger siblings.

VG st

oc ks

tu di

o/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Ch ar

lie E

dw ar

d/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

236 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

the social-interaction skills developed with siblings and apply them with peers, a strategy that’s productive with the effective social skills learned in high-quality sibling relationships but not with the antagonistic, coercive skills learned in low-quality relationships.

Why are some sibling relationships so filled with love and respect while others are dominated by jealousy and resentment? First, children’s sex and temperament matter. Sib- ling relations are more likely to be warm and harmonious when siblings are the same sex (Dunn & Kendrick, 1981) and when neither sibling is too emotional (Brody, Stoneman, & McCoy, 1994). Age is also important: Sibling relationships generally improve as the younger child approaches adolescence because siblings begin to perceive one another as equals (Kim et al., 2006; McHale et al., 2013).

Parents contribute to the quality of sibling relationships, both directly and indirectly (Brody, 1998). The direct influence stems from parents’ treatment. Siblings more often get along when they believe that parents have no “favorites,” but treat all siblings fairly (McGuire & Shanahan, 2010). When parents lavishly praise one child’s accomplishments while ignoring another’s, children notice the difference, and their sibling relationship suf- fers (Updegraff, Thayer, et al., 2005).

This doesn’t mean that parents must treat all their children the same. Children under- stand that parents should treat their kids differently—based on their age or personal needs. Only when differential treatment is not justified do sibling relationships deteriorate (Kowal & Kramer, 1997). In fact, during adolescence, siblings get along better when each has a unique, well-defined relationship with parents (Feinberg et al., 2003).

The indirect influence of parents on sibling relationships stems from the quality of the parents’ relationship with each other: A warm, harmonious relationship between parents fosters positive sibling relationships. Conflict between parents is associated with conflict between siblings, although intense marital conflict sometimes leads siblings to become closer, as they support each other emotionally (McHale, Updegraff, & Whiteman, 2013).

Many of the features associated with high-quality sibling relationships, such as the sex of the siblings, are common across different ethnic groups. But some unique features also emerge. For example, in a study of African American families, sibling relations were more positive when children had a stronger ethnic identity (McHale et al., 2007). And a study of Mexican American families found that siblings feel closer and spend more time together when siblings have a strong commitment to their family—that is, they felt obligated to their family and viewed it as an important source of support (Updegraff, McHale, et al., 2005).

In the pursuit of family harmony (what many parents call “peace and quiet”), parents can influence some of the factors affecting sibling relationships but not others. Parents can help reduce friction between siblings by being equally affectionate, responsive, and caring to all of their children and by caring for one another. At the same time, parents must real- ize that some dissension is natural in families, especially those with young boys and girls. Children’s different interests lead to conflicts that youngsters cannot resolve because their social skills are limited.

Adopted Children The U.S. government doesn’t keep official statistics on the number of adopted children, but the best estimate is that 2% to 4% of U.S. children are adopted. The most common form of adoption is for a foster parent or relative to adopt children who are in foster care because their birth families mistreated them. About 50,000 children are adopted annually in this manner, typically at about 6 or 7 years of age. Also common is for children to be adopted through pri- vate agencies, most often because young parents believe they cannot provide adequately for the child. Roughly 14,000 children are adopted annually in this way, usually as infants. Finally, about 10,000 children are adopted annually from other countries, such as China and Ethiopia. These adoptions involve infants and preschool children (Grotevant & McDermott, 2014).

Adopted children often experience adversity before being adopted. For example, children adopted from foster care have often experienced maltreatment that led them to be placed in foster care; many children adopted internationally were abandoned and lived in institu- tions prior to adoption. These circumstances aren’t optimal for children’s development, so

Think About It Calvin, age 8, and his younger sister, Hope, argue over just about everything and constantly compete for their parents’ attention. Teenage sisters Melissa and Caroline love doing everything together and enjoy sharing clothes and secrets about their teen romances. Why might Calvin and Hope get along so poorly while Melissa and Caroline get along so well?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 237

it’s not surprising that adopted children are at risk for many problems, including antiso- cial and aggressive behavior, depression and anxiety, and learning problems (Grotevant & McDermott, 2014). However, outcomes are varied. Most adopted children develop within the typical range. Problems are most likely when children are adopted after infancy and when their care before adoption was poor (e.g., they were institutionalized or lived in a series of foster homes). For example, the fall of the Ceaușescu regime in Romania in 1989 revealed hundreds of thousands of children living in orphanages under incredibly primitive conditions. Beginning in the 1990s, many of these children were adopted internationally. Some have shown remarkable catch-up growth, but many show multiple impairments, such as delayed cognitive development and disordered attachment (Kreppner et al., 2007).

Thus, although adoption per se is not a fundamental developmental challenge for most children, quality of life before adoption certainly places some adopted children at risk. And these children often fare well when they receive excellent care after adoption (Grotevant & McDermott, 2014).

In recent years, adopted children (and their adoptive families) are more likely to com- municate with the children’s birth family, an arrangement known as an open adoption. Open adoptions come in any forms. Contact can be face-to-face or via social media and can be occasional or frequent. Research on the impact of open adoption suggests that adoptees who are satisfied with the amount of contact with their birth families are at less risk for externalizing disorders. In addition, contact with birth families can lead adoptees and their adoptive families to discuss adoption, and these discussions help adopted teens to construct an identity as an adoptee (Grotevant et al., 2013).

Impact of Birth Order First-born children are often “guinea pigs” for most parents, who have much enthusiasm but little practical experience rearing children. Parents typically have high expectations for their first-borns and are both more affectionate and more punitive toward them. As more chil- dren arrive, most parents become more adept at their roles, having learned “the tricks of the trade” from earlier children. With later-born children, parents are more relaxed in their dis- cipline and less likely to punish children for not doing well in school (Hotz & Pantano, 2013).

The different approaches that parents use with their first- and later-born children help explain differences that are commonly observed between these children. First-born children generally have higher scores on intelligence tests, are more likely to go to college,

open adoption an adoption in which adopted children (and their adoptive families) communicate with the children’s birth family.

Contrary to folklore, only children are often smarter and more mature than children with siblings.

Bl en

d Im

ag es

–H ill

S tr

ee t S

tu di

os /B

ra nd

X P

ic tu

re s/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

238 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

and are more conscientious. They are also more willing to conform to parents’ and adults’ requests. In contrast, perhaps because later-born children are less concerned about pleas- ing parents and adults, they are more outgoing, more popular with their peers, more inno- vative, and more likely to task risks (Beck, Burnet, & Vosper, 2006; Bjerkedal et al., 2007; Sulloway & Zweigenhaft, 2010).

What about only children? According to conventional wisdom, parents dote on “onlies,” who therefore become selfish and egotistical. Is the folklore correct? No. Only children are more likely to succeed in school than are other children and to have higher levels of intelligence and self-esteem but don’t differ in popularity, adjustment, and per- sonality (Falbo, 2012; Falbo & Polit, 1986).

The possibility that only children are selfish has been a concern in China, where only children are common because of governmental efforts to limit population growth. How- ever, many studies have compared only and non-only children in China and most find no differences. One of the few differences is that Chinese only children, like Western only chil- dren, are more successful in school (Falbo, 2012; Liu, Lin, & Chen, 2010). Thus, contrary to the popular stereotype, only children are much like children who grow up with siblings.

Whether U.S. children grow up with siblings or as “onlies,” they are more likely than children in other countries to have their family relationships disrupted by divorce. What is the impact of divorce on children and adolescents?

Divorce and Remarriage Today, many North American children experience their parents’ divorce. These children often fare poorly in school achievement, conduct, adjustment, self-concept, and parent– child relations when compared with children from intact families. As adults, children of divorce are more likely to experience conflict in their own marriages, to become divorced themselves, and to become depressed (Lansford, 2009). These findings don’t mean that children of divorce are destined to be unhappy and unsuccessful during childhood and to have unhappy, conflict-ridden marriages that inevitably lead to divorce, but children of divorce are at greater risk for these outcomes.

The first year following a divorce is often rocky for parents and children alike. But begin- ning in the second year, most children begin to adjust to their new circumstances, especially when their parents get along (Sweeper, 2012). In joint custody, both parents retain legal custody of the children. Children benefit from joint custody—they adjust better behaviorally and socially, especially when they spend approximately equal time with both parents (Baude, Pearson, & Drapeau, 2016). When joint custody is not an option, mothers have traditionally been awarded custody; when this happens, children benefit when fathers remain involved in parenting, particularly when they get along with their ex-wives (Modecki et al., 2015).

Which Children Are Most Affected by Divorce? One striking finding of research on the impact of divorce is that although many children suf- fer in the short term, only a few children suffer serious harm in the longer term. Researchers have identified several features that predict how well children adjust in the longer term. Many features concern parents. Children fare best when custodial and noncustodial parents don’t experience psychological problems themselves, when they get along with each other, and when they stay involved with their children and parent effectively (Amato, 2010; Tabor, 2016). In addition, children are less likely to suffer harm from divorce when they are tem- peramentally easy, are smart, don’t consistently interpret life events negatively, and actively cope with problems brought on by divorce (Lansford, 2009; Weaver & Schofield, 2015).

Parents can reduce divorce-related stress and help children adjust to their new life circumstances. Together, parents should explain to their children what divorce means and what their children can expect to happen. They should reassure children that they will always love them and always be their parents; parents must back up these words with actions by remaining involved in their children’s lives. Finally, parents must expect that their children will sometimes be angry or sad about the divorce, and they should encour- age children to discuss these feelings with them.

joint custody Custody agreement in which both parents retain legal custody of their children follow- ing divorce.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 239

To help children deal with divorce, parents should not compete with each other for their children’s love and attention. Parents should neither take out their anger with each other on their children nor criticize their ex-spouse in front of them. Finally, parents should not ask children to mediate disputes; parents should work out problems without putting children in the middle.

Following all these rules consistently is difficult. After all, divorce is stressful and pain- ful for adults, too. Fortunately, effective programs are available to help parents and chil- dren adjust to life following divorce (Vélez, Wolchik, & Sandler, 2014). These programs aim to improve children’s behavioral and social skills and allow children to express feelings related to divorce; parents learn how to improve relationships with their children and how to discipline them.

Blended Families Following divorce, most children live in a single-parent household for about five years. However, most men and women eventually remarry (Sweeney, 2010). The resulting unit, consisting of a biological parent, a stepparent, and children, is known as a blended family. (Other terms for this family configuration are “remarried family” and “reconstituted fam- ily.”) The most common form of blended family is a mother, her children, and a stepfather. Children can thrive in a blended family: Those who report being close to their mother and their stepfather are well-adjusted (Amato, King, & Thorsen, 2016).

Adjusting to life in a blended family is more difficult when a stepfather brings his own biological children to the mix. In such families, parents sometimes favor their biological children over their stepchildren—they’re more involved with and warmer toward their biological children. Such preferential treatment usually leads to conflict and unhappiness (Dunn & Davies, 2001; Sweeney, 2010).

The best strategy for stepfathers is to be interested in their new stepchildren but to avoid encroaching on established relationships. Newly remarried mothers must be careful that their enthusiasm for their new spouse does not come at the expense of spending time with and showing affection for their children. Both parents and children need to have real- istic expectations. The blended family can be successful, but it takes effort because of the complicated relationships, conflicting loyalties, and jealousies that usually exist (Sweeney, 2010; White & Gilbreth, 2001).

Unfortunately, second marriages are slightly more likely than first marriages to end in divorce, particularly when stepchildren are involved (Teachman, 2008). This means that many children relive the trauma of divorce. Fortunately, programs are available to help members of blended families adjust to their new roles (Bullard et al., 2010). Such programs emphasize effective parenting (described on page 231) and, in particular, ways of deal- ing with behavior problems that children often display with stepparents. These programs result in fewer behavior problems and greater marital satisfaction.

Parent–Child Relationships Gone Awry: Child Maltreatment The first time that 7-year-old Max came to school with bruises on his face, he explained to his teacher that he had fallen down the basement steps. When Max had similar bruises a few weeks later, his teacher spoke with the school principal, who contacted local authorities. It turned out that Max’s mother thrashed him with a paddle for even minor misconduct; for serious transgressions, she beat Max and made him sleep alone in a dark, unheated basement.

Unfortunately, cases like Max’s occur far too often in modern America. Maltreatment comes in many forms. The two that often come to mind first are physical abuse involving assault that leads to injuries and sexual abuse involving fondling, intercourse, or other sexual behaviors. Another form of maltreatment is neglect—not

blended family family consisting of a biological parent, a stepparent, and children.

Following divorce, most men and women remarry, creating a blended family.

Ka yt

e D

ei om

a/ Ph

ot oE

di t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

240 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

giving children adequate food, clothing, or medical care. And children can also be harmed by psychological abuse—ridicule, rejection, and humiliation (Wicks-Nelson & Israel, 2015).

The frequency of these various forms of child maltreatment is difficult to estimate because so many cases go unreported. According to the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2016), nearly 700,000 children annually suffer maltreatment or neglect. About 75% are neglected, about 17% are abused physically, about 8% are abused sexually, and 6% are maltreated psychologically. (The percentages sum to more than 100 because some children experience more than one form of abuse.)

Who Are the Abusing Parents? Why would a parent abuse a child? Most abusing parents do not suffer from any specific mental or psychological disorder. Instead, a host of factors put some children at risk for abuse and protect others; the number and combination of factors determine whether the child is a likely target for abuse (Cicchetti & Toth, 2006). Let’s look at three of the most important factors: those associated with the cultural context, those associated with par- ents, and those associated with children themselves.

The most general category of contributing factors is that dealing with cultural values and the social conditions in which parents rear their children. Many countries in Europe and Asia have strong cultural prohibitions against physical punishment. In many countries, including Austria, Croatia, Germany, Israel, and Sweden, spanking is against the law and would be viewed in much the same way we would view American parents who punished their children by not feeding them for a few days. Nevertheless, in the United States, physical punishment is com- mon. Condoning physical punishment in this manner opens the door for child maltreatment.

What social conditions seem to foster maltreatment? Poverty is one. Maltreatment is more common among children living in poverty, in part because lack of money increases

the stress of daily life, and this means that parents are more likely to punish their children physically instead of making the extra effort to reason with them (Bywaters et al., 2016). Social isolation is a sec- ond force. Abuse is more likely when families are socially isolated from other relatives or neighbors because living in isolation deprives children of adults who could protect them and deprives parents of social support that would help them better deal with life’s stresses (Coulton et al., 2007).

Cultural factors clearly contribute to child abuse, but they are only part of the puzzle. Although maltreatment is more common among families living in poverty, it does not occur in a majority of these families and it occurs in middle- and upper-class families, too. Consequently, we need to look for additional factors to explain why abuse occurs in some families but not in others.

Child development researchers have identified several character- istics of parents that lead them to abuse their children. First, parents who maltreat their children often were maltreated themselves, which may lead them to believe that abuse is a normal part of childhood. This does not mean that abused children inevitably become abusing parents—only about one-third do. But a history of child abuse places adults at risk for mistreating their own children (Widom, Czaja, & DuMont, 2015). Second, parents who mistreat their children often use ineffective parenting techniques (e.g., inconsistent discipline), have such unrealistic expectations that their children can never meet them, and often believe they are powerless to control their children. For example, when abusive parents do not get along with their chil- dren, they often chalk this up to factors out of their control, such as children having a difficult temperament or being tired that day; they’re less likely to think their own behavior contributed to unpleasant inter- actions. Third, in families where abuse occurs, the couple’s interac- tions are often unpredictable, unsupportive, and unsatisfying for both

Child abuse is more common in societies that condone physical punishment such as spanking.

Ci nd

y Ch

ar le

s/ Ph

ot oE

di t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 241

husbands and wives. This marital discord makes life more stressful and makes it more dif- ficult for parents to invest effort in child rearing.

To place the last few pieces in the puzzle, we must look at the abused children them- selves. Children may inadvertently, through their behavior, contribute to their own abuse. In fact, infants and preschoolers are more often abused than older children are, probably because they are less able to regulate aversive behaviors that may elicit abuse (Sidebotham, Heron, & the ALSPAC Study Team, 2003). You’ve probably heard stories about a parent who shakes a baby to death because the baby wouldn’t stop crying. Because younger chil- dren are more likely to cry or whine excessively—behaviors that irritate all parents sooner or later—they are more likely to be the targets of abuse.

For much the same reason, children who are frequently ill or who are disabled are more often abused (Jones et al., 2012). When children are sick or disabled, they often need extra attention and special care (which means additional expense), conditions that increase stress in a family and put children at risk for maltreatment. And, just as Cinderella’s stepmother doted on her biological children but abused Cinderella, stepchildren are more prone to abuse and neglect than biological children (Archer, 2013). Adults are less invested emotionally in their stepchildren, and this lack of emotional investment leaves stepchildren more vulnerable.

Thus, cultural, parental, and child factors all contribute to child maltreatment. Any single factor usually will not result in abuse. Maltreatment becomes more likely when cul- tures condone physical punishment, parents lack effective skills for dealing with children, and a child’s behavior is frequently aversive.

Effects of Abuse on Children The prognosis for youngsters like Max is not very good. Some, of course, suffer permanent physical damage. Even when there is no lasting physical damage, children’s social and emo- tional development is often disrupted. They tend to have poor relationships with peers, either because they withdraw from interactions or because they are too aggressive (Cicchetti, 2016). Their cognitive development and academic performance are also disturbed. Abused young- sters often don’t do well in school, they don’t regulate their emotions well, they don’t recog- nize others’ emotions accurately, and they are at risk for depression (Cyr & Alink, 2017). As adults, children who were abused are at risk for psychiatric disorders (Rehan et al., 2017). In short, when children are maltreated, the effects are usually widespread and long-lasting.

Resilience Although the overall picture is bleak, some children are remarkably resilient to the impact of abuse. One factor that protects children is their ego resilience, which denotes children’s ability to respond adaptively and resourcefully to new situations. The effects of abuse tend to be smaller when children are flexible in responding to novel and challenging social situ- ations (Cicchetti, 2013). Another protective factor is being engaged in school: When mal- treated children are cognitively engaged in school—they pay attention, complete tasks, and are well organized—they are less prone to antisocial and aggressive behavior (Pears et al., 2013). A final protective factor is a positive mother–child relationship: When children have a positive representation of their mother—they describe her as “kind” and “loving,” for example—they suffer relatively few symptoms of maltreatment (Cicchetti, 2013).

Preventing Abuse and Maltreatment The complexity of child abuse dashes any hopes for a simple solution (Kelly, 2011). Because maltreatment is more apt to occur when several contributing factors are present, eradicat- ing child maltreatment requires many different approaches.

American attitudes toward “acceptable” levels of punishment and poverty would have to change. American children will be abused as long as physical punishment is considered acceptable and effective and as long as poverty-stricken families live in chronic stress from trying to provide food and shelter. Parents also need counseling and training in parent- ing skills. Abuse will continue as long as parents remain ignorant of effective methods of parenting and discipline.

ego resilience person’s ability to respond adaptively and resourcefully to new situations.

Think About It Kevin has never physically abused his 10-year-old son, Alex, but he constantly torments Alex emotionally. For example, when Alex got an F on a spelling test, Kevin screamed, “I skipped Monday Night Football just to help you, but you still flunked. You’re such a dummy.” When Alex began to cry, Kevin taunted, “Look at Alex, crying like a baby.” These interactions occur nearly every day. What are the likely effects of such repeated episodes of emotional abuse?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

242 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

It would be naïve to expect all these changes to occur overnight. However, by focusing on some of the more manageable factors, the risk of maltreatment can be reduced. Social supports help. When parents know that they can turn to other helpful adults for advice and reassurance, they better manage the stresses of child rearing that might otherwise lead to abuse. Families can also be taught more effective ways of coping with situations that might otherwise trigger abuse (Wicks-Nelson & Israel, 2015). Through role-playing ses- sions, parents can learn the benefits of authoritative parenting and effective ways of using feedback and modeling to regulate children’s behavior.

Providing social supports and teaching effective parenting skills are typically done when maltreatment and abuse have already occurred. Of course, preventing maltreat- ment in the first place is more desirable and more cost-effective. For prevention, one useful tool is familiar: early childhood intervention programs. Maltreatment and abuse can be reduced when children participate in preschool programs like Head Start (Zhai, Waldfogel,  & Brooks-Gunn, 2013). When children attend these programs, their parents become more committed to their children’s education. This leads their children to be more successful in school, reducing a source of stress and enhancing parents’ confidence in their child-rearing skills, thereby reducing the risks of maltreatment.

Another successful approach focuses specifically on parenting skills in families where children are at risk for maltreatment. In this approach, parents learn how to build warm and positive relationships with their children, to have reasonable expectations for their children, and to use more effective disciplinary practices. When parents of at-risk children participate in programs such as these, they report less stress, their behavior with their chil- dren becomes more positive (more praise and fewer commands), and, critically, suspected abuse is reduced (Thomas & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2011).

Finally, we need to remember that most parents who have mistreated their children need our help. Although we must not tolerate child maltreatment, most of these parents and children are attached to each other; maltreatment is typically a consequence of igno- rance and burden, not malice.

Recall 1. According to the systems approach, the family consists of

interacting elements that influence each other, and the fam- ily itself is _______________.

2. A(n) _______________.parental style combines high control with low involvement.

3. Most children seem to benefit when parents rely on a(n) _______________ style.

4. Parental behaviors that influence children include direct instruction, modeling (learning through observation), and _______________.

5. Some parents do not make a good team: they don’t work together, they compete with each other for their children’s attention, and _______________.

6. Compared with first-born children, later-born children are more outgoing, popular, innovative, and are more likely _______________.

7. Following divorce, children often adjust better behaviorally and socially when they spend _______________.

8. Children are harmed less from divorce when they are tem- peramentally easy, are smart, don’t interpret life events negatively, and _______________.

9. Children are more likely to be abused when they are younger, when they are stepchildren, and when they are _______________.

Interpret ●● How can child abuse be explained in terms of the biological,

psychological, and sociocultural forces in the biopsychosocial framework?

apply ●● Imagine a family in which Mom and Dad both work full-time

outside the home. Mom’s employer wants her to take a new position in a distant small town. Mom is tempted because the position represents a promotion with more responsibil- ity and higher pay. However, because the town is so small, Dad wouldn’t be able to get a job comparable to the one he has now, which he likes. Based on the family systems theory shown in Figure 7.1, how might the move affect the couple’s 10-year-old daughter and 4-year-old son?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 7.1

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 243

Only 36 hours had passed since the campers arrived at Crab Orchard Summer Camp. Nevertheless, groups had already formed spontaneously based on the campers’ main inter- ests: arts and crafts, hiking, and swimming. Within each group, leaders and followers had already emerged. This happens every year, but the staff is always astonished at how quickly a “social network” emerges at camp.

The groups that form at summer camps—as well as in schools and neighborhoods— represent one of the more complex forms of peer relationships: Many children are involved, and there are multiple relationships. We’ll examine these kinds of interactions later in this section. Let’s start by looking at a simpler social relationship, friendship.

Friendships Over time, children develop special relationships with some peers. Friendship is a volun- tary relationship between two people involving mutual liking. By age 4 or 5, most children claim to have a “best friend.” If you ask them how they can tell a child is their best friend, their response will probably resemble 5-year-old Katelyn’s:

Interviewer: Why is Heidi your best friend? Katelyn: Because she plays with me. And she’s nice to me. Interviewer: Are there any other reasons? Katelyn: Yeah, Heidi lets me play with her dolls.

Thus, the key elements of friendship for younger children are that children like each other and enjoy playing together.

As children develop, their friendships become more complex. For older elementary school children (ages 8 to 11), mutual liking and shared activities are joined by features that are more psychological in nature: trust and assistance. At this age, children expect that they can depend on their friends—their friends will be nice to them, will keep their promises, and won’t say mean things about them to others. Children also expect friends to step forward in times of need: A friend should willingly help with homework or willingly share a snack.

Adolescence adds another layer of complexity to friendships. Mutual liking, common interests, and trust remain. New to adolescence is intimacy—friends now confide in one another, sharing personal thoughts and feelings. Teenagers reveal their excitement over a new romance or disappointment at not being cast in a school musical. Intimacy is more common in friendships among girls, who are more likely than boys to have one exclusive “best friend” (Markovits, Benenson, & Dolenszky, 2001). Because intimacy is at the core of their friendships, girls are also more likely to be concerned about the faithfulness of their friends and worry about being rejected (Rose & Asher, 2017).

The emergence of intimacy in adolescent friendships means that friends also come to be seen as sources of social and emotional support. Elementary school children gener- ally rely on close family members—parents, siblings, and grandparents—when they need help or are upset. Adolescents sometimes rely on adults, but they also turn to friends for support (van Rijsweijk et al., 2016).

friendship Voluntary relationship between two people involving mutual liking.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the benefits of friendship?

●● What are the important features of groups of children and adolescents? How do these groups influence individuals?

●● Why are some children more popular than others? What are the causes and consequences of being rejected?

●● What are some effects of childhood aggression? Why are some children chronic victims of aggression?

7.2 Peers

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

244 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Hand in hand with the emphasis on intimacy is loyalty. Having confided in friends, adolescents expect friends to stick with them through good and bad times. If a friend is disloyal, adolescents are afraid that they may be humiliated because their intimate thoughts and feelings will become known to a much broader circle of people (Berndt & Perry, 1990).

Who Are Friends? Most friends are alike in age, gender, and race (Hamm, 2000; Mehta & Strough, 2009). Because friends are supposed to treat each other as equals, friendships are rare between an older, more experienced child and a younger, less experi-

enced child. Because children typically play with same-sex peers, boys and girls rarely become friends.

Friendships are more common between children and adolescents from the same race or ethnic group than between those from different groups, reflecting segregation in American society. Friendships among children of different groups are more com- mon in schools when a child’s school is ethnically diverse. Such friendships are valuable: Children from majority groups typically form more positive attitudes toward a minority group following a friendship with a youth from that group (Turner & Cameron, 2016). And children in cross-group friendships are less often targets of relational aggression (Kawabata & Crick, 2011).

Besides being alike in age, sex, and race, friends also tend to be alike in popular- ity: Highly popular youth befriend popular peers and avoid friendships with less popu- lar peers (Dijkstra, Cillessen, & Borch, 2013). In addition, friends have similar attitudes toward school, recreation, and problem behaviors like breaking rules and using drugs. Children and adolescents befriend others who are similar to them and, as time passes, friends become more similar in their attitudes and values (Franken et al., 2016; Shin & Ryan, 2014). Nevertheless, friends are not photocopies of each other; friends are less simi- lar, for example, than spouses or dizygotic twins (Rushton & Bons, 2005).

Although children’s friendships are overwhelmingly with members of their own sex, some children have friendships with opposite-sex children. Children with same- and opposite-sex friendships tend to be very well-adjusted, whereas children with only opposite-sex friendships tend to be unpopular, to be less competent academically and socially, and to have lower self-esteem. Children with both same- and opposite-sex friends are so popular that both boys and girls are eager to be their friends. In contrast, children with only opposite-sex friendships are socially unskilled, unpopular youngsters who are rejected by their same-sex peers and form friendships with opposite-sex children as a last resort (Bukowski, Sippola, & Hoza, 1999).

Quality and Consequences of Friendships You probably remember some childhood friendships that were long-lasting and satisfy- ing as well as others that rapidly wore thin and soon dissolved. What accounts for these differences in the quality and longevity of friendships? Sometimes friendships are brief because children have the skills to create friendships—they know funny stories, they kid around, they know good gossip—but lack the skills to sustain those friendships— they can’t keep secrets, are too bossy, or are too emotional (Blair et al., 2014; Jiao, 1999; Parker  & Seal, 1996). Sometimes friendships end because when conflicts arise, children are more concerned about their own interests and are unwilling to compromise or nego- tiate (Glick  & Rose, 2011; Rose & Asher, 1999). And sometimes friendships end when children discover that their needs and interests aren’t as similar as they initially thought (Gavin & Furman, 1996).

Long-lasting friendships are to be treasured. In fact, children benefit from having good friends (Berndt & Murphy, 2002). Compared with children who lack friends, chil- dren with good friends have higher self-esteem, are less likely to be lonely and depressed, and more often share and cooperate (Burk & Laursen, 2005; Hartup & Stevens, 1999).

Friends tend to be alike in age, race, and sex.

M uh

am m

ad S

ub ky

L at

ef A

yo b/

12 3R

F

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 245

Children with good friends cope better with stressful experiences, such as doing poorly on an exam or being rejected by peers (Adams, Santo, & Bukowski, 2011; McDonald et al., 2010). And they’re less likely to be victimized by peers (Schwartz et al., 2000). The benefits of friendship are long-lasting as well: Children who have friends are, as adults, at less risk for depression, anxiety, and aggressive behavior (Sakyi et al., 2015).

Although children and adolescents benefit from their friends’ support, there can be costs as well. Sometimes friends spend much of their time together discussing each other’s personal problems, which is known as co-rumination. Girls do this more than boys (consistent with the fact that intimacy is more important to girls’ friendships). Such co-rumination strengthens girls’ friendships but also puts them at greater risk for depression and anxiety. In other words, when Avanti and Fahima spend day after day talking about their problems with parents and schoolwork, they grow closer but also more troubled (Brendgen et al., 2010; Schwartz-Mette & Rose, 2012).

There are other ways in which friendships can be hazardous. For example, when aggressive children are friends, they often encour- age each other’s aggressive behavior (Dishion, Poulin, & Burraston, 2001; Piehler & Dishion, 2007). Similarly, when teens engage in risky behavior (e.g., when they drink, smoke, or have sex), they often rein- force each other’s risky behavior (Huang et al., 2014).

Thus, friends are one important way in which peers influence children’s development. Peers also influence development through groups, the topic of the next section.

Groups At the summer camp in the vignette, new campers form groups based on common inter- ests. Groups are just as prevalent in American schools. “Jocks,” “preps,” “burnouts,” “drug- gies,” “nerds,” and “brains”—you may remember these or similar terms referring to groups of older children and adolescents. During late childhood and early adolescence, the peer group becomes the focal point of social relationships for youth. The starting point is often a clique—a small group of children or adolescents who are friends and tend to be similar in age, sex, race, and attitudes. Members of a clique spend time together and often dress, talk, and act alike. A crowd is a larger mixed-sex group of older children or adolescents who have similar values and attitudes and are known by a common label such as “jocks” or “nerds” (Rubin, Bukowski, & Bowker, 2015).

Crowds emerge in the transition from middle school to high school. Crowds have stereotyped identities—brains are smart kids who enjoy school—and younger adolescents gravitate to crowds that match their person identity. Initially, some crowds have more sta- tus than others. For example, jocks are often the most prestigious crowd, while burnouts are the least. However, later in high school, the status of crowds becomes less significant and the boundaries between crowds become fuzzy (Brown, 2014)

Group Structure Groups—be they in school, at a summer camp as in the vignette, or elsewhere—typically have a well-defined structure. Often groups have a dominance hierarchy, which is headed by a leader to whom all other members of the group defer. Other members know their posi- tion in the hierarchy. They yield to members who are above them in the hierarchy and assert themselves over members who are below them. A dominance hierarchy is useful in reducing conflict within groups because every member knows his or her place (Rubin et al., 2015).

co-rumination Conversations about one’s personal prob- lems, common among adolescent girls.

clique Small group of friends who are similar in age, sex, and race.

crowd large group including many cliques that have similar attitudes and values.

dominance hierarchy ordering of individuals within a group in which group members with lower status defer to those with greater status.

When youth have good friends, they’re bet- ter able to cope with life’s stresses.

Im ag

e So

ur ce

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

246 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

What determines where members stand in the hier- archy? In children, especially boys, physical power is often the basis for the dominance hierarchy. The leader is usu- ally the member who is the most intimidating physically (Hawley, 2016). Among girls and older boys, hierarchies are often based on individual traits that relate to the group’s main function. For example, girls chosen to be patrol leaders in Girl Scouts tend to be bright and goal- oriented and to have new ideas (Edwards, 1994). These characteristics are appropriate because the primary func- tion of patrol leaders is to help plan activities for the entire troop. Similarly, in classroom discussion groups, the leaders are often children who are outgoing and have good ideas (Li et al., 2007). Thus, leadership based on key skills is effective because it gives the greatest influ- ence to those with the skills most important to group functioning.

Peer Influence Groups establish norms—standards of behavior that apply to all group members—and may urge members to conform to these norms. The stereotype is that teenagers exert enormous, irresistible pressure on each other to behave antisocially. In reality, peer pres- sure is neither always powerful nor always evil. For example, most junior and senior high students resist peer pressure to behave in ways that are clearly antisocial, such as stealing (Cook, Buehler, & Henson, 2009), and such resistance increases from mid- to late adoles- cence (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). And peer pressure can be positive, too. Peers often urge one another to work hard in school, to participate in school activities, and to get along with their family (Brown, 2014).

Peer pressure is not all-powerful. Instead, peer influence is stronger when youth are younger and more socially anxious, peers have high status or are friends; and standards for appropriate behavior are not clear-cut, as in the case of tastes in music or clothing or stan- dards for smoking and drinking (Allen et al., 2012; Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011). Thus, when 14-year-old Aya’s best friend (who’s one of the most popular kids in school) gets her hair cut like Taylor Swift, Aya may go along because she’s young, the peer is popular and is her friend, and standards for hairstyles are vague. But when an unpopular kid that 18-year- old Kelly barely knows suggests to her that they go to the mall and shoplift some earrings, Kelly will resist because she’s older, the peer is unpopular and not a friend, and norms for shoplifting are clear.

Peers can influence behavior through other means beyond pressure. For example, a crowd provides models for how its members should look and act; members go along will- ingly because they want to identify with the group (Brown et al., 2008). And the mere presence of peers can prime the adolescent brain’s reward system, making it particularly sensitive to rewards associated with risky behavior like driving fast or experimenting with drugs (Albert, Chein, & Steinberg, 2013).

Popularity and Rejection Eileen is the most popular child in her fifth-grade class. Most of the other students like to play with her and to sit near her at lunch or on the school bus. Whenever the class must vote to pick a child for something special—to be class representative to the student council or to recite the class poem on Martin Luther King Day—Eileen invariably wins. In con- trast, Jay is the least popular child in the class. When he sits down at the lunch table, other kids move away. When he tries to join a game of four square, others quit. Students in the class detest Jay as much as they like Eileen.

Think About It Chapter 5 described important differ- ences in the ways that boys and girls interact with same-sex peers. How might these differences help explain why boys’ and girls’ dominance hierarchies differ?

Group leaders tend to be those who have skills that are valuable to the group: Girl Scout patrol leaders, for example, tend to be goal-oriented and to have good ideas.

Je ff

S in

er /K

RT /N

ew sc

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 247

Popular and rejected children like Eileen and Jay are common. In fact, studies of pop- ularity (Rubin et al., 2015) reveal that most children in elementary school classrooms can be placed, fairly consistently, in one of these five categories:

●● Popular children are liked by many classmates. ●● Rejected children are disliked by many classmates. ●● Controversial children are both liked and disliked by classmates. ●● average children are liked and disliked by some classmates, but without the intensity

found for popular, rejected, or controversial children. ●● Neglected children are ignored by classmates.

Of these categories, we know the most about popular and rejected children. Each of these categories includes two subtypes. Most popular children are skilled academically and socially. They are good students who are usually friendly, cooperative, helpful, and funny. They are more skillful at communicating and better at integrating themselves into an ongoing conversation or play session—they “fit in” instead of “barging in.” A smaller group of popular children includes physically aggressive boys who pick fights with peers and relationally aggressive girls who, like the “Plastics” in the film Mean Girls, thrive on manipulating social relationships. Although these youth are not particularly friendly, their antisocial behavior nevertheless garners respect from peers (Rubin et al., 2015).

Being well-liked seems straightforward: Be pleasant, friendly, and socially skilled, not obnoxious. These results hold for children in many areas of the world, including Canada, Europe, Israel, and China. However sometimes popular children have other characteristics unique to their cultural setting. For example, in Israel popular children are more likely to be assertive and direct than children in other countries (Krispin, Sternberg, & Lamb, 1992). In China, historically shy children were often popular because their restrained behavior was taken as a sign of social maturity. However, economic reforms in China in the past 25 years have resulted in greater emphasis on taking ini- tiative and being assertive. Consequently, shy children living in urban areas that have experienced economic reform are now rejected by peers, but they remain popular in more traditional rural areas (Chen, Wang, & Cao, 2011). Evidently, good social skills are at the core of popularity in most countries, but other features are important, reflecting culturally specific values.

As for rejected children, many are overly aggressive, hyperactive, socially unskilled, and unable to regulate their emotions. These children are usually more hostile than popu- lar aggressive children and seem to be aggressive for the sheer fun of it, which peers dis- like, instead of using aggression as a means toward other ends, which peers may not like but grudgingly respect. Other rejected children are shy, withdrawn, timid, and, not sur- prisingly, lonely (Rubin et al., 2015).

Causes and Consequences of Rejection No one enjoys being rejected. Not surprisingly, peer rejection is a major obstacle in children’s development. Over time, rejected youngsters become less involved in classroom activities; they end up feeling lonely and disliking school. Repeated peer rejection in childhood can lead youth to drop out of school, commit juve- nile  offenses, and suffer from psychopathology (Rubin et al., 2015).

Peer rejection can be traced, at least in part, to direct and indirect influences of parents. The direct influence is via disciplinary practices. Harsh discipline and incon- sistent discipline are associated with antisocial, aggres- sive behavior, paving the way for rejection (Kawabata & Crick, 2016). The indirect influence is via modeling: When parents typically respond to interpersonal conflict

popular children Children who are liked by many classmates.

rejected children as applied to children’s popularity, children who are disliked by many classmates.

controversial children as applied to children’s popularity, chil- dren who are intensely liked or disliked by classmates.

average children as applied to children’s popularity, chil- dren who are liked and disliked by differ- ent classmates, but with relatively little intensity.

neglected children as applied to children’s popularity, children who are ignored—neither liked nor dis- liked—by their classmates.

Most popular children are good students and are socially skilled: they tend to be friendly and helpful with peers.

Think About It Effective parents and popular children have many characteristics in common. What are they?

St oc

kb ro

ke r/

M BI

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

248 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

with intimidation or aggression, their children may imitate them, hampering the develop- ment of their social skills and making them less popular (Cummings, Goeke-Morey, & Papp, 2016).

Thus, the origins of rejection are clear: Socially awkward, aggressive children are often rejected because they rely on an aggressive interpersonal style, which can be traced to parenting. The implication is that by teaching youngsters (and their parents) more effective ways of interacting with others, we can make rejection less likely. With improved social skills, rejected children would not need to resort to antisocial behaviors that peers deplore. Training of this sort does work. Rejected children can learn skills that lead to peer acceptance and thereby avoid the long-term harm associated with being rejected (Loch- man et al., 2014).

Aggressive Children and Their Victims By the time toddlers are old enough to play with one another, they show aggression. For example, 1- and 2-year-olds sometimes use physical aggression to resolve their conflicts (Hay, 2017). In instrumental aggression, a child uses aggression to achieve an explicit goal. By the start of the elementary school years, another form of aggression emerges (Coie et al., 1991). hostile aggression is unprovoked and seems to have as its sole goal to intimi- date, harass, or humiliate another child. Hostile aggression is illustrated by a child who spontaneously says, “You’re stupid!” and then kicks a classmate. A third form of aggression is relational aggression, in which children try to hurt others by undermining their social relationships (see Chapter 5). Examples include telling friends to avoid a particular class- mate or spreading malicious gossip (Kawabata & Crick, 2016).

Children’s tendencies to behave aggressively are stable over time, particularly among children who are highly aggressive at a young age. For example, infants who often bite or strike other people are, as preschoolers, more likely to kick or hit peers to obtain toys (Hay, 2017). And preschool children judged by their teachers to be very aggressive are, as young adults, much more likely to commit crimes (Asendorpf, Denissen, & van Aken, 2008). Finally, aggression during childhood often leads to failure in high school (e.g., drop- ping out, failing a grade), which leaves few options for employment (Alatupa et al., 2013). Clearly, aggression is not simply a case of playful pushing and shoving that children even- tually outgrow. To the contrary, children who are highly aggressive can develop into young adults who wreak havoc.

Most school children are the targets of an occasional aggressive act—a shove or kick to gain a desired toy or a stinging insult by someone trying to save face. However, a small per- centage of children are chronic targets of bullying. In both Europe and the United States, 10% to 25% of elementary school children and adolescents are chronic victims of physi- cal attacks, name-calling, backstabbing, and similar aggressive acts (Juvonen & Graham, 2014). In recent years, some youth have been victims of electronic bullying in which they are harassed via cell phones or the Internet (Kowalski et al., 2014).

As you can imagine, being tormented daily by peers is hard on children. When children are chronic victims of aggression, they’re often lonely, anxious, and depressed; they dislike school and have low self-esteem (Rubin et al., 2015). As adults, they’re prone to poor health and unsatisfying social relationships (Wolke et al., 2013). Why do some children suffer the sad fate of being victims? Some victims are aggressive themselves. These youngsters often overreact, are restless, and are easily irritated. Their peers soon learn to insult or ridicule these children, knowing that they will probably start a fight. Other victims tend to be withdrawn and submissive. Because they are unwilling or unable to defend themselves from their peers’ aggression, they are usually referred to as passive victims. When attacked, they show obvious signs of distress and usually give in to their attackers, thereby rewarding the aggressive behavior. Thus, both aggressive and withdrawn submissive children end up as victims (Rubin et al., 2015).

instrumental aggression aggression used to achieve an explicit goal.

hostile aggression unprovoked aggression that seems to have the sole goal of intimidating, harassing, or humiliating another child.

Children are likely to become chronic vic- tims of aggression if they refuse to defend themselves.

Je nn

ie W

oo dc

oc k;

R ef

le ct

io ns

P ho

to lib

ra ry

/C or

bi s

Do cu

m en

ta ry

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 249

Other factors contribute to make children victims of bullying. Children are more likely to be bullied when they are obese, depressed, disabled, or immigrants. In essence, any feature that makes children different from their peers puts them in the sights of bul- lies (Juvonen & Graham, 2014; Strohmeier, Kärnä, & Salmivalli, 2011). Also, bullying is more likely in classrooms that have a strong status hierarchy—everyone in the class knows which children are high status and which are low status—and when the norm is that bully- ing is acceptable (Saarento & Salmivalli, 2015).

Researchers have developed effective programs to combat bullying in schools. These programs work at many levels: schools build a school climate in which bullying is not condoned and victims are supported by their peers; teachers learn how to recognize bul- lying and how to respond when they see it; children who have been victimized learn social skills for dealing with peers and strategies for regulating their emotions; and parents of victimized children learn how to help their children and how to work with school person- nel. Programs that include these elements can reduce bullying and the harmful outcomes associated with it (Bradshaw, 2015).

Recall 1. Friends are usually similar in age, sex, race, popularity, and

_______________.

2. Children with friends have higher self-esteem, are less likely to be lonely, and _______________than children without friends.

3. As a group forms, a _______________typically emerges, with the leader at the top.

4. Peer pressure is most powerful when _______________.

5. Popular children are socially skilled; they communicate more effectively and_______________.

6. Rejected youngsters are more likely to drop out of school, to commit juvenile offenses, and _______________.

7. Some children who are chronic victims of aggression over- react and are easily irritated; other chronic victims are _______________.

Interpret ●● How could developmental change in the nature of friendship

be explained in terms of Piaget’s stages of intellectual devel- opment, discussed in Chapters 4 and 6?

apply ●● On page 246, you met Jay, who is the least popular child in his

class. Jay’s mom is worried about her son’s lack of popularity and wants to know what she can do to help him. Jay’s dad thinks that Jay’s mom is upset over nothing—he argues that, like fame, popularity is fleeting and that Jay will turn out okay in the end. What advice would you give to Jay’s parents?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 7.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is the impact of watching television on children’s

attitudes, behavior, and cognitive development? ●● How does playing video games affect youth?

●● How does use of social media affect development?

7.3 Electronic Media

Every day, 7-year-old Roberto follows the same routine when he gets home from school: He watches one action-adventure cartoon after another until it’s time for dinner. Roberto’s mother is disturbed by her son’s constant TV viewing, particularly because of the amount of violence in the shows he likes. Her husband tells her to stop worrying: “Let him watch what he wants. It won’t hurt him, and besides, it keeps him out of your hair.”

In generations past, children learned their culture’s values from parents, teachers, religious leaders, and print media. These sources of cultural knowledge were joined by television in the 1950s, by the Internet in the 1990s, and by social media in the 2000s. In this section,

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

250 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

we examine three ways in which technology can influence children and adolescents: by watching television programs, by playing video games, and by communicating via social media. As we look at their influence, we’ll see if Roberto’s mother should be worried.

Television Programs The cartoon exaggerates TV’s impact on North American children, but only some- what. Typical U.S. children and adolescents spent about 1½ hours watching programs on TV and another hour watching DVDs or videos (Rideout, 2016). If 30-second TV ads are designed to influence children’s preferences in toys, cereals, and hamburg- ers, the programs themselves ought to have even more impact. And a half-century of research consistently shows that they do. For example, Roberto’s mother should be concerned: Children become more aggressive after viewing violence on television. In addition, from watching TV, children learn stereotypes about gender and race and they’re more likely to engage in risky behaviors (e.g., use drugs, have unprotected sex) after seeing such behaviors glorified in TV ads or movies (Prot et al., 2014).

Watching television can also help children learn to be more generous and coop- erative and have greater self-control. Youngsters who watch TV shows that emphasize prosocial behavior, such as Mister Rogers’ Neighborhood, are more likely to behave pro- socially (Calvert, 2015). However, because prosocial behaviors are portrayed on TV far less frequently than aggressive behaviors, opportunities to learn the former from tele- vision are limited; we are far from harnessing the power of television for prosocial uses.

The biggest positive influence of TV on children worldwide has been Sesame Street. Big Bird, Bert, Ernie, and their friends have been helping to educate preschool children for nearly 50 years. Youngsters who watch regularly improve their knowledge of numbers, letters, shapes, and colors; they learn more about the physical and social world; and they develop positive attitudes toward social outgroups (Mares & Pan, 2013). Other programs designed to teach young children about language and reading skills (Martha Speaks, Super Why!, WordGirl) and about basic science and math con-

cepts (Cyberchase, Curious George, Dinosaur Train, Sid the Science Kid) show that TV can be harnessed to help children learn important academic skills (Prot et al., 2014).

For decades, critics have argued that the medium itself—independent of the content of programs—harms viewers, particularly children (Huston & Wright, 1998). One com- mon criticism is that because TV programs consist of many brief segments presented in rapid succession, children who watch a lot of TV develop short attention spans and have difficulty concentrating in school. Another concern heard frequently is that because TV provides ready-made, simple-to-interpret images, children who watch a lot of TV become passive, lazy thinkers and become less creative.

In fact, as stated, neither of these criticisms is consistently supported by research (Huston & Wright, 1998). The first criticism—TV watching reduces attention—is easi- est to dismiss. Research repeatedly shows that increased TV viewing does not lead to reduced attention, greater impulsivity, reduced task persistence, or increased activity lev- els (Foster & Watkins, 2010). The content of TV programs can influence these dimensions of children’s behavior—children who watch impulsive models behave more impulsively themselves—but TV per se does not harm children’s ability to pay attention.

As for the criticism that TV viewing fosters lazy thinking and stifles creativity, the evidence is mixed. On the one hand, some educational programs depict people being cre- ative and encourage children to pretend; when children watch these programs frequently, they’re often more creative. On the other hand, when children often watch programs that are action-oriented, they may be less creative. The content and pacing of these shows often do not provide viewers with the time to reflect that is essential for creativity ( Calvert  & Valkenburg, 2013).

In general, then, the sheer amount of TV that children watch is not a powerful influ- ence on development. Most of the impact of TV—for good or bad—comes through the content of TV programs that children watch.

1995 Martha F. Campbell. Reprinted with permission.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 251

Video Games Adolescent boys spend nearly an hour every day playing video games but girls average less than 10 minutes daily; about 40% of boys play every day compared with less than 10% of girls (Rideout, 2016). The impact of such game playing on youth depends on a game’s con- tents. Many games, including Tetris and Star Fox, emphasize perceptual–spatial skills, such as estimating the trajectory of a moving object, responding rapidly, and shifting efficiently from one task goal to another. When children play such games frequently, they improve their spatial skills, processing speed, and executive functioning (Cardoso-Leite, & Bavelier, 2014). The Spotlight on Research feature shows the benefits of game-playing on visual-spatial skills.

On ResearchSpotlight

Playing a Video Game Improves Children’s Visual-Spatial Skill

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Visual spatial skills are part of intelligence (pages 193–194) and involve several specific sub- skills, such as the ability to imagine an object’s appearance after it has been rotated in space. Many video games require players to rotate two-dimensional objects quickly. Richard De Lisi and Jennifer Wolford (2002) wondered whether playing such games would improve children’s spatial skills.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Before training, De Lisi and Wolford assessed children’s spatial skills by having children judge whether pairs of figures like those shown in the left panel of ◗ Figure 7-3 would be identical if they appeared in the same orientation. Then, for eleven 30-minute sessions, children played one of two video games: Tetris, in which a player moves and rotates rapidly falling tiles to fit into a line, or Carmen Sandiego, in which a player is a detective chasing a thief who’s try- ing to steal famous landmarks. After the final video-game session, children again judged pairs of figures like those shown in Figure 7-3.

Who were the participants in the study? The study included 47 8- and 9-year-olds who were in third grade: 23 played Tetris and 24 played Carmen Sandiego.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental. The independent variable was the type of video game that chil- dren played (Tetris or Carmen Sandiego); the dependent variable was the number of pairs judged correctly before and after playing the video games. The study was not develop- mental because the researchers tested only 8- and 9-year-olds.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. Parents provided consent for

their children to participate and the children enjoyed playing the video games.

What were the results? The right panel of ◗ Figure 7.3 shows the percentage of pairs that children answered correctly before and after playing the video games. Before play- ing, the groups answered the same percent- age of questions accurately. After playing, children who had played Carmen Sandiego answered the same percentage of questions correctly as before playing, but children who had played Tetris answered significantly more questions correctly. In other words, play- ing Tetris improved performance but playing Carmen Sandiego did not.

What did the investigators conclude? De Lisi and Wolford (2002) concluded that “This

study provides evidence of a successful train- ing method that led to improved performance on a [visual-spatial] task in third graders” (p. 280). That is, the 5½ hours that children spent playing Tetris improved their ability to rotate objects mentally and this generalized to their performance on the mental rotation task administered at the end of the study.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? The study was conducted with 8- and 9-year-olds and assessed visual-spatial skills immediately after playing video games. It would be valuable to see if the results would hold for younger children as well as for adolescents and to see how long children’s improved visual-spatial skills persist.

●◗ Figure 7.3 Children improved their spatial skill when they played a video game that required them to rapidly rotate stimuli.

Example problems

Same

Dif ferent (mirror images)

Playing Carmen Sandiego Playing Tetris

Before playing game

P er

ce n

ta ge

o f

p ro

b le

m s

an sw

er ed

c or

re ct

ly

70

75

80

85

90

95

After playing game

Only children playing Tetris improved their performance

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

252 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

In contrast, many popular games such Manhunt and Grand Theft Auto are violent, with players killing game characters in extraordinarily gruesome ways. Just as expo- sure to televised violence can make children behave more aggressively, playing violent video games can make chil- dren more aggressive, less empathic, and desensitize them to violence (Calvert et al., 2017).

Finally, a minority—roughly 10%—of youth get “hooked” on video games (Gentile, 2009). They show many of the same symptoms associated with pathologi- cal gambling: Playing video games comes to dominate their lives, it provides a “high,” and it leads to conflict with others. Not surprisingly, extreme video-game playing is associated with less success in school, apparently because youth spend time playing games instead of studying (Weis & Cerankosky, 2010).

Social Media Children don’t spend much time on social media—less than 20 minutes daily—but ado- lescents spend nearly 1½ hours daily using social media or video chatting (Rideout, 2016). Teens use social media to stay in touch with friends. Teenage girls send and receive about 40 texts a day, boys about 20 texts a day (Lenhart, 2015). Such communication leads most to feel more connected with their friends. One explanation of this result is that adolescents are more likely to reveal intimate thoughts and feelings to friends over social media. Such self-disclosure promotes high-quality friendships—revealing something personal creates a bond with the other person and encourages that person to reveal in turn (Valkenburg & Peter, 2011).

Using social media helps adolescents form new friends face-to-face. In one study (Koutamanis et al., 2013), teens who messaged often were, a year later, more confident in their friendship skills. One idea is that social media provide a forum where adolescents can practice these friendship-related social skills, a forum where the costs of failure—someone rejects your invitations—are less painful.

There’s also a dark side to social media: cyberbullying, which refers to the use of social media to harm other people, by repeatedly insulting them, excluding them or spreading rumors about them. You may have received a nasty text—or even sent one—but that doesn’t really count as cyberbullying. Instead, cyberbullying refers to repeated, deliberate actions, actions for which a victim is basically defenseless.

Cyberbullying is a relatively common feature of modern adolescence—about 10% to 40% of adolescents report being the victim of cyberbullying, depending on how it’s defined (Kowalski et al., 2014). The consequences of cyberbullying are much the same as those for offline bullying: victims are, at the least, embarrassed, and, at the worst, frightened, angry, lonely, and depressed. Another parallel: Perpetrators and victims of cyberbullying tend to be the teens who are perpetrators and victims of offline bullying (Hong et al., 2016; Kowalski et al., 2014). This is “good” news because it means that cyberbullying can be addressed with the same preventive measures that work for offline bullying.

In many respects, social media have changed the how of adolescence but not the what. As with previous generations, youth still are particularly concerned about their peer rela- tions and some youth pick on others. Social media simply provide different means for accomplishing these tasks.

cyberbullying using social media to hurt other people by repeatedly insulting them, excluding them, or spreading rumors about them.

When children play violent video games, they often become more aggressive.

CI O

T/ BS

IP /A

G E

Fo to

st oc

k

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 253

When 12-year-old Ian agreed to babysit Kyle, his 5-year-old brother, their mother reminded Ian to keep Kyle out of the basement because Kyle’s birthday presents were there, unwrapped. But as soon as their mother left, Kyle wanted to go to the basement to ride his tricycle. When Ian told him no, Kyle burst into angry tears and shouted, “I’m gonna tell Mom that you were mean to me!” Ian wanted to explain to Kyle, but he knew that would just cause more trouble!

As children spend more time with other people, they begin to understand other people better. In this vignette, for example, Ian realizes why Kyle is angry, and he knows that if he gives in to Kyle now, his mother will be angry when she returns. Children’s growing understanding of others is the focus of this section. We begin by looking at how children describe others and then examine their understanding of how others think. Finally, we’ll see how children’s recognition of different social groups can lead to prejudices.

Describing Others As children develop, their descriptions of themselves become richer, more abstract, and more psychological. These same changes also occur in children’s descriptions of others. Children begin by describing other people in terms of concrete features, such as behavior, and progress to describing them in terms of abstract traits (Barenboim, 1981; Livesley & Bromley, 1973). The Real People feature shows this progression in one child.

Research confirms that Tamsen—the girl in the Real People feature on page 254— describes friends like most children her age do: Descriptions referring to appearances or possessions become less common as children grow older, as do descriptions giving general information such as the person’s age, gender, religion, or school. In contrast, descriptions of personality traits (e.g., “friendly” or “conceited”) increase between 8 and 14 years of age (Livesley & Bromley, 1973). Thus, children’s descriptions of others begin with the concrete and later become more conceptual.

Recall 1. When children watch a lot of TV violence, they often become

_______________.

2. Contrary to popular criticisms, frequent TV viewing is not consistently related to reduced attention or to a lack of _______________.

3. A minority of youth become addicted to video games; play- ing games dominates their lives, it provides a “high,” and it _______________.

4. Social media can improve adolescents’ friendships by pro- moting self-disclosure and by _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast the ways in which TV viewing and web

surfing might affect children’s development.

apply ●● Assume that you had the authority to write new regulations for

children’s TV programs. What shows would you encourage? What shows would you want to limit?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 7.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● As children develop, how do their descriptions of others

change?

●● How does understanding of others’ thinking change as children develop?

●● When and why do children develop prejudice toward others?

7.4 Understanding Others

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

254 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

More recent research also supports the trend to more abstract and richer psychological descriptions of others but indicates that young children’s understanding of other people is  more sophisticated than is suggested by their verbal descriptions of people they know (Heyman, 2009). Indeed, 4- and 5-year-olds have begun to think about other people in terms of psychological traits such as being smart, friendly, helpful, and shy. They can use behavioral examples to infer an underlying trait: Told about a child who won’t share cookies or won’t allow another child to play with a toy, 4- and 5-year-olds accurately describe the child as selfish. In addition, given information about a trait, they correctly predict future behavior: Told about a child who is shy, they believe that the child will not volunteer to help a puppeteer and will be quiet at a meal with many relatives (Liu, Gelman, & Wellman, 2007).

One idiosyncrasy of young children’s descriptions of others is that they see others “through rose-colored glasses”—that is, until about 10 years of age, children have a bias to look for positive, not negative, traits in others. Young children are willing to believe that someone is smart (or friendly or helpful) based on relatively little evidence (and based on inconsistent evidence), but they require more evidence (and more consistent evidence) to decide that someone is mean or stupid (Boseovski, 2010).

Understanding What Others Think One trademark of the preschooler’s thinking is difficulty in seeing the world from another person’s view. Piaget’s term for this is egocentrism, and it is a defining characteristic of his pre- operational stage of development. As children move beyond the preschool years, they realize that others see the world differently, both literally and figuratively. For example, in the vignette, Ian knows why his little brother, Kyle, is angry: Kyle thinks that Ian is being bossy and mean. Ian understands that Kyle doesn’t know that there is a good reason he can’t go to the basement.

Sophisticated understanding of how others think is achieved gradually throughout childhood and adolescence. According to a theory proposed by Robert Selman (1980, 1981), understanding other people begins with the egocentric thinking characteristic of preoperational children—they think that others think as they do. As children develop, they are able to take the perspective of other people. In Selman’s theory, this perspective- taking skill progresses through five stages, which are shown in ◗ Table 7.1.

Every few years Tamsen was asked to describe a girl she liked a lot. Her descrip- tions changed, focusing less on behavior and emphasizing psychological properties. Let’s start with the description she gave as a 7-year-old:

Vanessa is short. She has black hair and brown eyes. She uses a wheelchair because she can’t walk. She’s in my class. She has dolls just like mine. She likes to sing and read.

Tamsen’s description of Vanessa is prob- ably not too different from the way she would have described herself: The emphasis is on concrete characteristics such as Vanessa’s appearance, possessions, and preferences. Contrast this with the following description, which Tamsen gave as a 10-year-old:

Kate lives in my apartment building. She is a very good reader and is good at math and science. She’s nice to everyone in our class. And she’s very funny. Some- times her jokes make me laugh so-o-o hard! She takes piano lessons and likes to play soccer.

Tamsen’s account still includes concrete fea- tures, such as where Kate lives and what she likes to do. However, psychological traits are also evident: Tamsen describes Kate as nice and funny. By age 10, children move beyond the purely concrete and observable in describ- ing others. During adolescence, descriptions become even more complex, as you can see in the following, from Tamsen as a 16-year-old:

Jeannie is very understanding. Whenever anyone at school is upset, she’s there to

give a helping hand. Yet in private, Jean- nie can be so sarcastic. She can say some really nasty things about people. But I know she’d never say that stuff if she thought people would hear it because she wouldn’t want to hurt their feelings.

This description is more abstract: Tamsen now focuses on psychological traits like under- standing and concern for others’ feelings. It’s also more integrated: Tamsen tries to explain how Jeannie can be both understanding and sarcastic.

Each of Tamsen’s three descriptions is typi- cal. As a 7-year-old, she emphasized concrete characteristics; as a 10-year-old, she began to include psychological traits; and as a 16-year- old, she tried to integrate traits to form a cohesive account.

Real People Applying Human Development

Tell Me About a Girl You Like a Lot

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 255

To see the progression from stage to stage, imagine two boys arguing about what to do after school. One wants to go to a playground, and the other wants to watch TV. If the boys were 5-year-olds (undifferentiated stage), neither would understand why the other wants to do something different. Their reasoning is stone simple: “If I want to go to the playground, you should, too!”

During the early elementary school years (social-informational stage), each child understands that the other wants to do something different and they explain their differing views in terms of the other person lacking essential information. Their thinking is along the lines, “I know that you want to watch TV, but if you knew what I knew, you’d want to go to the playground.” By the late elementary school years (self-reflective stage), the boys would understand that each wants to do something different and they could “step into the other’s shoes” to understand why: “I know you want to go to the playground because you haven’t been there all week.”

In early adolescence (third-person stage), the boys could step even farther apart and imagine how another person (e.g., a parent or teacher) could view the disagreement. Finally, in late adolescence (societal stage), the boys (now young men, really) can remove themselves even further and appreciate, for example, that many people would think it’s silly to watch TV on a beautiful sunny day.

As predicted by Selman’s theory, children’s reasoning moves through each stage in sequence as they grow older. In addition, children at more advanced cognitive levels tend to be at more advanced stages in perspective taking (Gurucharri & Selman, 1982; Krebs & Gillmore, 1982). However, many scientists are not convinced that more sophisticated per- spective taking occurs in such a stagelike fashion; they believe that it improves steadily throughout childhood and adolescence (just as cognitive development is now seen to be more continuous than Piaget’s theory predicted).

Some investigators have linked improved perspective taking to the developing theory of mind, described in Chapter 3 (Chandler & Carpendale, 1998). The traditional false- belief task, for example, reveals children’s understanding that another person’s actions are often based on their beliefs even when those beliefs are wrong. As an illustration, suppose children hear the following story:

Lindsay and Angela are in the park and see some kids playing softball. Lindsay wants to play, so she runs home for her glove. Angela waits at the park for her, but while Lindsay’s away, the kids decide it’s too hot for softball and leave to get ice cream.

Children understand false belief if they say that Lindsay will return to the ball field ( acting on her false belief that the kids are still playing ball). But we can add a new wrinkle to the story.

Table 7.1

Selman’s Stages of Perspective Taking Stage approximate ageS DeScription Undifferentiated 3–6 years Children know that self and others can have different thoughts and feelings

but often confuse the two.

Social-informational 4–9 years Children know that perspectives differ because people have access to different information.

Self-reflective 7–12 years Children can step into another person’s shoes and view themselves as others do; they know that others can do the same.

Third-person 10–15 years Children can step outside the immediate situation to see how they and another person are viewed by a third person.

Societal 14 years to adult Adolescents realize that a third-person perspective is influenced by broader personal, social, and cultural contexts.

Think About It How do Selman’s stages of perspective taking correspond to Piaget’s and Erikson’s stages?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

256 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

As the kids are leaving the park, one of them thinks that Lindsay might like to join them for ice cream. So she phones Lindsay and tells her the plan.

Now children are asked: “Where does Angela think Lindsay thinks the kids are?” Children understand second-order belief if they say that Angela thinks that Lindsay will go to the ball field. This sort of “he thinks that she thinks . . . ” reasoning is known as recursive thinking. It emerges at about 5 or 6 years of age and improves steadily during the elemen- tary school years due to the combined effects of increased language skill and greater exec- utive functioning (Miller, 2009).

One of the benefits of a developing appreciation of others’ thoughts and viewpoints is that it allows children to get along better with their peers. That is, children who can readily take another person’s perspective and better understand other people’s thinking are typically well-liked by their peers (Slaughter et al., 2015). For example, perspective- taking skill allows children to realize that a peer wants to join in their fun and so they invite the peer.

Of course, mere understanding does not guarantee good social behavior; some- times children who understand what another child is thinking take advantage of that child. In general, however, greater understanding of others seems to promote positive interactions.

Prejudice Around the world, many adults are prejudiced against individuals based solely on their membership in a social group (e.g., a racial, ethnic, or religious group). Preferring one’s group over others is first observed in 2- to 4-year-olds, becomes stronger in 5- to 7-year- olds, and remains strong thereafter (Raabe & Beelmann, 2011). But there’s more to the story. By the preschool years, most children can distinguish males from females and can identify people from different racial groups (Nesdale, 2001). After children learn their membership in a specific group, they typically have an enhanced view of that group. That is, preschool and kindergarten children attribute many positive traits, such as being friendly and smart, and few negative traits, such as being mean, to their own group (Bigler, Jones, & Lobliner, 1997; Patterson & Bigler, 2006).

Negative views of other groups form more slowly, beginning in the elementary school years (Buttelmann & Böhm, 2014). During the elementary school years, many children come to see race as a “natural kind”—determined by birth, stable, and referring to people who are similar to each other physically and behaviorally (Rhodes, 2013). At the same time, overt prejudice declines some, in part because children learn norms that dis-

courage openly favoring their own group over others (Apfelbaum et al., 2008). But implicit bias remains— many children automatically associate their group with positive features and associate other groups with bad features (Baron, 2015).

As children move into the elementary school years, their knowledge of racial stereotypes and prejudices increases steadily; by 10 or 11 years of age, most children are aware of broadly held racial stereotypes (Pauker, Ambady, & Apfelbaum, 2010).

Some scientists believe that bias and prejudice emerge naturally out of children’s efforts to understand their social world (Bigler & Liben, 2007). Young chil- dren actively categorize animate and inanimate objects as they try to understand the world around them. As children’s social horizons expand beyond their parents to include peers, they continue to categorize and try to decide how different groups of people “go together.” They use perceptually salient features (e.g., race, gender,

recursive thinking thoughts that focus on what another per- son is thinking.

Ph ot

os ho

t

One effective way to reduce prejudice is for children from different races to work together toward a common goal, such as completing a class project.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 257

age) as well as verbal labels that adults may apply to different groups (e.g., “The girls go to lunch first, then the boys”). After children have identified the salient features that define peers in their environment, they begin to classify people they encounter along these dimensions. Jacob is now seen as a white boy; Kalika is now seen as a black girl ( Patterson & Bigler, 2006).

What can parents, teachers, and other adults do to rid children of prejudice? One way is to encourage friendly and constructive contacts between children from different groups. However, contact alone usually accomplishes little. Intergroup contact is most likely to reduce when the participating groups of children are equal in status, when the contact between groups involves pursuing common goals (not competing), when par- ents and teachers support the goal of reducing prejudice, and when children are ready for contact—they’re not anxious about interacting with children from the other group and have a positive attitude toward the interactions (Cameron et al., 2006; Turner & Cameron, 2016). For example, adults might have children from different groups work together toward common goals. In school, this might be a class project. In sports, it might be mastering a new skill. By working together, Gary starts to realize that Farès acts, thinks, and feels as he does simply because he’s Farès, not because he’s a Lebanese American.

Another useful approach is to ask children to play different roles (Davidson & Davidson, 1994; Tynes, 2007). They can be asked to imagine that—because of their race, ethnic background, or gender—they have been insulted verbally or not allowed to par- ticipate in special activities. A child might be asked to imagine that she can’t go to a pri- vate swimming club because she’s African American or that she wasn’t invited to a party because she’s Hispanic American. Afterward, children reflect on how they felt when prej- udice and discrimination was directed at them. They’re also asked to think about what would be fair: What should be done in situations like these?

A final strategy against prejudice involves education. In one study (Hughes, Bigler, & Levy, 2007), European American elementary school children learned about the rac- ism that famous African Americans experienced. For example, they learned that Jackie Robinson played for a team in the old Negro Leagues because the white people in charge of Major League Baseball wouldn’t allow African Americans to play. The study also included a control group in which the biographies omitted the experiences of racism. When chil- dren learned about racism directed at African Americans, they had more positive atti- tudes toward African Americans.

From experiences like these, children and adolescents discover for themselves that a person’s membership in a social group tells very little about that person. They learn, instead, that all children are different and that each person is a unique mix of experiences, skills, and values.

Recall 1. When adolescents describe others, they usually

_______________.

2. In the most advanced stage of Selman’s theory, adolescents _______________.

3. One view is that bias and prejudice emerge naturally out of _______________.

Interpret ●● How might an information-processing theorist describe the

stages of Selman’s perspective-taking theory?

apply ●● Based on what you’ve learned in this section, what can par-

ents and teachers do to discourage prejudice in children?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 7.4

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

258 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

7.1 Family Relationships

What is a systems approach to parenting?

●● According to the systems approach, the family consists of interacting elements; that is, parents and children influ- ence each other. The family itself is influenced by other social systems, such as neighborhoods and religious organizations.

What are the primary dimensions of parenting? how do they affect children’s development?

●● One key factor in parent–child relationships is the degree of warmth that parents express: Children clearly benefit from warm, caring parents. A second factor is control, which is complicated because neither too much nor too little control is desirable. Effective parental control involves setting appropriate standards, enforcing them, and trying to anticipate conflicts.

●● Taking into account both warmth and control, four pro- totypic parental styles emerge: Authoritarian parents are controlling but uninvolved; authoritative parents are fairly controlling but are also responsive to their children; permissive parents are loving but exert little control; and uninvolved parents are neither warm nor controlling. Authoritative parenting seems best for children in terms of both cognitive and social development, but important excep- tions are associated with culture and socioeconomic status.

●● Parents influence development by direct instruction and coaching. In addition, parents serve as models for their children, who sometimes imitate parents’ behavior directly. Sometimes children behave in ways that are similar to what they have seen and sometimes in ways that are the opposite of what they’ve seen (counterimitation).

●● Parents also use feedback to influence children’s behavior. Sometimes parents fall into the negative reinforcement trap, inadvertently reinforcing behaviors that they want to discourage.

●● Punishment is effective when it is prompt, consistent, accompanied by an explanation, and delivered by a person with whom the child has a warm relationship. Punishment has limited value because it suppresses behaviors but does not eliminate them, and it often has side effects. Time-out is one useful form of punishment.

●● Chronic conflict is harmful to children, but children benefit when their parents solve problems constructively. Parent- ing is a team sport, but not all parents play well together

because they may disagree in child-rearing goals or parent- ing methods.

●● Parenting is influenced by characteristics of children them- selves. A child’s age and temperament will influence how a parent tries to exert control over the child.

What determines how siblings get along? how do first-born, later-born, and only children differ?

●● The birth of a sibling can be stressful for children, particu- larly when they are still young and parents ignore their needs. Siblings get along better when they are of the same sex, believe that parents treat them similarly, enter adoles- cence, and have parents who get along well.

●● Although most adopted children develop within the typical range, experiencing adversity before being adopted puts some adopted children at risk for problems such as disordered attachment and delayed cognitive development. Problems are most likely when children are adopted after infancy and when their care before adoption was inadequate.

●● Parents have higher expectations for first-born children, which explains why such children are more intelligent and more likely to go to college. Later-born children are more popular and more innovative. Contradicting the folk- lore, only children are rarely worse off than children with siblings; in some respects (such as intelligence and self- esteem), they are often better off.

how do divorce and remarriage affect children?

●● Divorce can harm children in a number of areas, ranging from school achievement to adjustment. Many children suffer from divorce in the short-term; in the longer-term children fare better when parents get along and stay involved and when the children are easy, smart, and actively cope with problems.

●● Children can thrive in a blended family but this is more difficult when a stepfather brings his own children to the family.

What factors lead children to be maltreated?

●● Factors that contribute to child abuse include poverty, social isolation, and a culture’s views on violence. Parents who abuse their children were often neglected or abused themselves and tend to be unhappy, socially unskilled indi- viduals. Younger or unhealthy children are more likely to be targets of abuse. Children who are abused often lag behind in cognitive and social development.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a P T E R 7 : E x PA N D I N G S O C I A L H O R I z O N S 259

7.2 Peers

What are the benefits of friendship?

●● Friendships among preschoolers are based on mutual lik- ing. As children grow, loyalty, trust, and intimacy become more important features in their friendships. Friends are usually similar in age, sex, race, and attitudes. Children with friends are more skilled socially and are better adjusted.

What are the important features of groups of children and adolescents? how do these groups influence individuals?

●● Older children and adolescents often form cliques—small groups of like-minded individuals—that become part of a crowd. Some crowds have higher status than others.

●● Common to most groups is a dominance hierarchy, a well- defined structure with a leader at the top. Physical power often determines the dominance hierarchy, particularly among boys. However, with older children and adolescents, dominance hierarchies are more often based on skills that are important to the group.

●● Peer pressure is neither totally powerful nor totally evil. In fact, groups influence individuals primarily in areas where standards of behavior are unclear, as in tastes in music or clothing or with regard to drinking, drug use, and sex.

Why are some children more popular than others? What are the causes and consequences of being rejected?

●● Most popular children are skilled academically and socially. A far smaller number of popular children use aggression to achieve their social goals.

●● Some children are rejected by their peers because they are too aggressive. Other children are rejected for being too timid or withdrawn. Repeated peer rejection often leads to school failure and behavioral problems.

What are some effects of childhood aggression? Why are some children chronic victims of aggression?

●● Many highly aggressive children end up being violent as adults. Children who are chronic victims of aggression typically overreact or refuse to defend themselves.

7.3 Electronic Media

What is the impact of watching television on children’s atti- tudes, behavior, and cognitive development?

●● TV programs can cause children to become more aggres- sive, to adopt gender stereotypes, and to act prosocially.

Programs designed to foster children’s cognitive skills, such as Sesame Street, are effective. Many criticisms about TV as a medium (e.g., it shortens children’s attention span) are not supported by research.

how does playing video games affect youth?

●● The content of playing video games affects children and adolescents. Playing some games improves youth’s cognitive skills (e.g., visual-spatial skill); playing other games makes youth more aggressive.

how does use of social media affect development?

●● Children use social media infrequently, but adolescents use it often, primarily to communicate with friends. Such communication strengthens friendships and allows teens to develop social skills. Cyberbullying parallels offline bullying: the consequences, perpetrators, and victims are similar.

7.4 Understanding Others

as children develop, how do their descriptions of others change?

●● Children’s descriptions of others change in much the same way children’s descriptions of themselves change. During the early elementary school years, descriptions emphasize concrete characteristics. In the late elementary school years, they emphasize personality traits. In adoles- cence, they emphasize providing an integrated picture of others.

how does understanding of others’ thinking change as chil- dren develop?

●● According to Selman’s theory, children’s understanding of how others think progresses through five stages. In the first (undifferentiated) stage, children often confuse their own and another person’s view. In the last (societal) stage, ado- lescents can take a third-person perspective and know that this perspective is influenced by context.

When and why do children develop prejudice toward others?

●● Prejudice, which emerges in the preschool years and becomes stronger in the elementary school years, is a com- mon byproduct of children’s efforts to categorize social groups. Ways to reduce prejudice include exposure to indi- viduals from other social groups and by educating children about the ills of prejudice.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

260 Pa RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

socialization (228) authoritarian parenting (229) authoritative parenting (229) permissive parenting (230) uninvolved parenting (230) direct instruction (231) counterimitation (231) reinforcement (231) punishment (231) negative reinforcement trap (232)

time-out (232) open adoption (237) joint custody (238) blended family (239) ego resilience (241) friendship (243) co-rumination (245) clique (245) crowd (245) dominance hierarchy (245)

popular children (247) rejected children (247) controversial children (247) average children (247) neglected children (247) instrumental aggression (248) hostile aggression (248) cyberbullying (252) recursive thinking (256)

Key Terms

7.1 1. embedded in other social systems, such as neighborhoods 2. authoritarian 3. authoritative 4. feedback (reward and punishment) 5. they limit each other’s access to the child 6. to take risks 7. approximately equal time with both parents 8. actively cope with problems brought on by divorce 9. often ill 7.2 1. attitudes 2. more often act prosocially (sharing and cooperating) 3. dominance hierarchy 4. standards for appropriate behavior are vague 5. better integrate themselves into ongoing conversations 6. to suffer from psychopathology 7. unwilling or unable to defend themselves 7.3 1. more aggressive 2. creativity 3. leads to conflict with others 4. allowing them to practice friendship skills 7.4 1. try to provide a cohesive, integrated account 2. provide a third-person perspective on situations and recognize the influence of context on this perspective 3. children’s efforts to understand their social world

Test Yourself Recall Answers

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Co ur

te sy

o f R

ob er

t V . K

ai l

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

263

At age 13, Miley Cyrus premiered in the title role of Hannah Montana, which quickly became a top-rated TV series; at age 15, she was included in

Time magazine’s list of the 100 most influential peo-

ple in the world; and a few weeks before she turned

21, her recording of “Wrecking Ball” became her

first number-one single in the United States. Miley’s

steady march to the top of her profession over her

adolescent years is a remarkable feat. Yet in a less

dramatic and less public way, these years are times of

profound changes for all adolescents. In this chapter,

we’ll examine physical and cognitive developments in

adolescence. We’ll begin by describing the important

features of physical growth in the teenage years. Then

we’ll consider some of the necessary ingredients for

healthy growth in adolescence. Next, we’ll examine

the nature of information processing during adolescence. Finally, we’ll end the

chapter by examining how adolescents reason about moral issues.

Rites of Passage Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence

8.1 Pubertal Changes 264 Signs of Physical Maturation 264 Mechanisms of Maturation 266 Psychological Impact of Puberty 267

8.2 Health 270 Nutrition 270 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Evaluating a Program

for Preventing Eating Disorders 273 Physical Fitness 274 Threats to Adolescent Well-Being 275

8.3 Information Processing During Adolescence 276 Working Memory and Processing Speed 276 Content Knowledge, Strategies, and Metacognitive

Skill 277 Problem-Solving and Reasoning 277

8.4 Reasoning About Moral Issues 279 Kohlberg’s Theory 280 REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

Schindler’s List 282 Evaluating Kohlberg’s Theory 282 Promoting Moral Reasoning 283

Summary 285

Key Terms 286

8

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

264 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Pete just celebrated his 15th birthday, but as far as he is concerned, there is no reason to celebrate. Although most of his friends have grown about 6 inches in the past year or so, have a much larger penis and larger testicles, and have mounds of pubic hair, Pete looks just as he did when he was 10 years old. He is embarrassed by his appearance, particularly in the locker room, where he looks like a little boy among men. “Won’t I ever change?” he wonders.

The appearance of body hair, the emergence of breasts, and the enlargement of the penis and testicles are all signs that the child is gone and the adolescent has arrived. Many ado- lescents take great satisfaction in these signs of maturity. Others, like Pete, worry through their teenage years as they wait for the physical signs of adolescence.

In this section, we’ll begin by describing the normal pattern of physical changes that take place in adolescence and look at the mechanisms responsible for them. Then we’ll discover the impact of these physical changes on adolescents’ psychological functioning. As we do, we’ll learn about the possible effects of Pete’s maturing later than his peers.

Signs of Physical Maturation Puberty denotes two general types of physical changes that mark the transition from childhood to young adulthood. The first are bodily changes, including a large increase in height and weight, as well as changes in the body’s fat and muscle content. The second concern sexual maturation, including change in the reproductive organs and the appearance of secondary sexual characteristics such as facial and body hair and growth of the breasts.

Physical Growth For physical growth, the elementary school years are the calm before the adolescent storm. ◗ Figure 8.1 shows that in an aver- age year, a typical 6- to 10-year-old gains about 5 to 7 pounds and grows 2 to 3 inches. In contrast, during the peak of the adoles- cent growth spurt, a girl may gain as many as 14 to 15 pounds in a year and a boy 16 to 17 pounds (Tanner, 1970).

Figure 8.1 also shows that girls typically begin their growth spurt about two years before boys. Girls typically start the growth spurt at about age 11, reach their peak rate of growth at about 12, and achieve their mature stature at about 15. In con- trast, boys start the growth spurt at age 13, hit peak growth at 14, and reach mature stature at 17. This two-year difference in the growth spurt can lead to awkward social interactions between 11- and 12-year-old boys and girls because the girls are often taller and look more mature than the boys.

Body parts don’t mature at the same rate. Instead, the head, hands, and feet usually begin to grow first, followed by growth in the arms and legs. The trunk and shoulders are the last to

puberty Collection of physical changes that marks the onset of adolescence, including a growth spurt and the growth of breasts or testes.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What physical changes during adolescence mark the

transition to a mature young adult?

●● What factors cause the physical changes associated with puberty?

●● How do physical changes affect adolescents’ psychological development?

8.1 Pubertal Changes

During the growth spurt, girls are often much taller than boys of the same age.

Ju st

in P

um fr

ey /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 265

grow (Tanner, 1990). The result of these differing growth rates is that an adolescent’s body sometimes seems out of proportion—teens have a head and hands that are too big for the rest of their body. Fortunately, these imbalances don’t last long as the later developing parts catch up.

During the growth spurt, bones become longer (which, of course, is why adolescents grow taller) and denser. Bone growth is accompanied by several other changes that differ for boys and girls. Muscle fibers become thicker and denser during adolescence, produc- ing substantial increases in strength, particularly for boys (Smoll & Schutz, 1990). Body fat also increases during adolescence, but more rapidly in girls than in boys. Finally, heart and lung capacity increases more in adolescent boys than in adolescent girls. Together, these changes help to explain why the typical adolescent boy is stronger, is quicker, and has greater endurance than the typical adolescent girl.

Brain Growth in Adolescence At the beginning of adolescence, the brain is nearly full size—it’s about 95% of the size and weight of an adult’s brain. Nevertheless, adolescence is important for fine-tuning the brain’s functioning. Two features of brain development that begin early in life (and that we discussed in Chapter 3) are nearly complete in adolescence: myelination, which is the acquisition of fatty insulation that allows neurons to transmit information faster, and synaptic pruning, which is the weeding out of unnecessary connections between neu- rons (Jernigan & Stiles, 2017). These changes mean that different regions in the adolescent brain are well connected and that information is rapidly conveyed between them, which means that adolescents can process information more efficiently than the child, a theme that we will explore more on pages 274–275.

Another distinguishing feature of the adolescent brain is that some, but not all, brain regions reach maturity. Some brain systems that are sensitive to reward (especially to rewards from peers) may reach maturity in adolescence but the systems responsi- ble for self-control aren’t fully specialized until adulthood (Casey, 2015). As shown in ◗ Figure 8.2, this makes adolescents vulnerable: The reward- and pleasure-seeking sys- tems are more mature than the systems for controlling behavior. Consequently, even though adolescents may know that behaviors involve risk, the anticipated rewards and pleasure of risky behavior sometimes swamp the adolescent’s ability to suppress the desire to engage in such activities (Sturman & Moghaddam, 2012). As we’ll see on page  275, this makes adolescents vulnerable to high-risk activities (e.g., drinking, unprotected sex).

●◗ Figure 8.1 Children grow steadily taller and heavier until puberty, when they experience a rapid increase known as the adolescent growth spurt.

A ve

ra ge

i n

ch es

g ro

w n

p er

y ea

r

20 0

2

4

6

8

10

12

4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 Age (years)

A ve

ra ge

p ou

n d

s ga

in ed

p er

y ea

r

20 0

4

9

13

18

4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 Age (years)

Girls

Boys

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Think About It Compare and contrast the events of puberty for boys and girls.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

266 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Sexual Maturation Not only do adolescents become taller and heavier, they also become mature sexually. Sexual maturation includes change in primary sex characteristics, which refer to organs that are directly involved in reproduction. These include the ovaries, uterus, and vagina in girls and the scrotum, testes, and penis in boys. Sexual maturation also includes changes in secondary sex characteristics, which are physical signs of maturity not directly linked to the reproductive organs. These include the growth of breasts and the widening of the pelvis in girls, the appearance of facial hair and the broadening of shoulders in boys, and the appearance of body hair and changes in voice and skin in both boys and girls.

Changes in primary and secondary sexual characteristics occur in a predictable sequence for boys and for girls. For girls, puberty begins with growth of the breasts and the growth spurt, followed by the appearance of pubic hair. Menarche, the onset of men- struation, typically occurs at about age 13. Early menstrual cycles are usually irregular and without ovulation.

For boys, puberty usually commences with the growth of the testes and scrotum, fol- lowed by the appearance of pubic hair, the start of the growth spurt, and growth of the penis. At about age 13, most boys reach spermarche, the first spontaneous ejaculation of sperm-laden fluid. Initial ejaculations often contain relatively few sperm; only months or sometimes years later are there sufficient sperm to fertilize an egg (Dorn et al., 2006).

Mechanisms of Maturation What causes the many physical changes that occur during puberty? The pituitary gland is key: It helps to regulate physical development by releasing growth hormone. In addition, the pituitary regulates pubertal changes by signaling other glands to secrete hormones. During the early elementary school years—long before there are any outward signs of puberty—the pituitary signals the adrenal glands to release androgens, initiating the biochemical changes that will produce body hair. A few years later, in girls, the pituitary signals the ovaries to release estrogen, which causes the breasts to enlarge, the female genitals to mature, and fat to accumulate. In boys, the pituitary signals the testes to release the androgen hormone testosterone, which causes the male genitals to mature and muscle mass to increase.

Although estrogen is often described as a “female hormone” and androgen as a “male hormone,” estrogen and androgen are present in both boys and girls. As we’ve seen, in girls, the adrenal glands secrete androgens. The amount is tiny compared with that secreted by boys’ testes, but it is enough to influence the emergence of body hair. In boys, the testes secrete very small amounts of estrogen, which explains why some boys’ breasts temporar- ily enlarge early in adolescence.

primary sex characteristics Physical signs of maturity that are directly linked to the reproductive organs.

secondary sex characteristics Physical signs of maturity that are not directly linked to reproductive organs.

menarche Onset of menstruation.

spermarche First spontaneous ejaculation of sperm.

●◗ Figure 8.2 Adolescence is a vulnerable time because the reward- and pleasure-seeking centers of the brain (limbic system) mature more rapidly than the behavioral control systems (frontal system); the gap between the two systems is particularly great in adolescence.

F u

n ct

io n

al M

at u

ri ty

10 0

50

100

2050 15 25 30 Age (years)

Frontal system

Limbic system

75

25

From Casey et al., 2008, “The Adolescent Brain,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, Fig. 3, p. 116.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 267

The timing of pubertal events is regulated, in part, by genetics ( Cousminer et al., 2013). This is shown by the closer synchrony of puber- tal events in identical twins than in fraternal twins: If one identical twin has body hair, the odds are that the other twin will too (Mustanski et al., 2004). Genetic influence is also shown by the fact that a mother’s age at menarche is related to her daughter’s age at menarche (Belsky, Bakermans- Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007). However, these genetic forces are strongly influenced by the environment, particularly an adolescent’s nutri- tion and health. In general, puberty occurs earlier in adolescents who are well nourished and healthy than in adolescents who are not (St.  George, Williams, & Silva, 1994).

The social environment also influences the onset of puberty, at least for girls. Menarche occurs at younger ages in girls who experience chronic stress or harsh punishment, or who are depressed (James et al., 2012). When young girls experience chronic socioemotional stress, this may release hor- mones that activate the hormones that trigger menarche.

This mechanism has an evolutionary advantage: If events of a girl’s life suggest that her future reproductive success is uncertain—as indicated by chronic socioemotional stress—then it may be adaptive to reproduce as soon as possible instead of waiting until later when she would be more mature and better able to care for her offspring. That is, the evolution- ary gamble in this case might favor “lower-quality” offspring early over “higher-quality” offspring later (Cabeza de Baca & Ellis, 2017).

Psychological Impact of Puberty Of course, teenagers are well aware of the changes taking places in their bodies. Not sur- prisingly, some of these changes affect adolescents’ psychological development.

Body Image Compared with children and adults, adolescents are more concerned about their overall appearance. Many teenagers look in the mirror regularly, checking for signs of additional physical change. Generally, girls worry more than boys about appearance and are more likely to be dissatisfied with their appearance (Vander Wal & Thelen, 2000). Girls are par- ticularly likely to be unhappy with their looks when appearance is a frequent topic of con- versation with friends, leading girls to spend more time comparing their own appearance with that of their peers. Peers have relatively little influence on boys’ satisfaction with their looks; instead, boys are unhappy with their appearance when they expect to have an ideal- ized strong, muscular body but do not (Carlson Jones, 2004).

Response to Menarche and Spermarche Carrie was horror writer Stephen King’s first novel (and later a movie starring Sissy Spacek); it opens with a riveting scene in which the title character has her first menstrual period in the shower at school and, not knowing what is happening, fears that she will bleed to death. Fortunately, most adolescent girls today know about menstruation before- hand—usually from discussions with their mothers. Being prepared, their responses are usually fairly mild. Most girls are moderately pleased at this new sign of maturity but mod- erately irritated by the inconvenience and messiness of menstruation (Brooks-Gunn & Ruble, 1982). Girls usually tell their moms about menarche right away, and after two or three menstrual periods, they tell their friends, too (Brooks-Gunn & Ruble, 1982).

Menarche is usually a private occasion for adolescents living in industrialized coun- tries, but in traditional cultures, it is often celebrated. For example, the Western Apache, who live in the southwest portion of the United States, traditionally have a spectacular ceremony to celebrate a girl’s menarche (Basso, 1970). After a girl’s first menstrual period, a group of older adults decide when the ceremony will be held and select a sponsor—a

Young adolescents are often quite concerned about their appearance.

Think About It The Apache have an elaborate celebra- tion for menarche. Can you think of other similar ceremonies—perhaps not as elaborate—that take place to cel- ebrate other milestones of adolescent development?

Ia ko

v Fi

lim on

ov /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

268 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

woman of good character and wealth (she helps to pay for the ceremony) who is unrelated to the initiate. On the day before the ceremony, the sponsor serves a large feast for the girl and her family; at the end of the ceremony, the family reciprocates, symbolizing that the sponsor is now a member of their family.

The ceremony itself begins at sunrise and lasts a few hours; it includes eight distinct phases in which the initiate, dressed in ceremonial attire, dances or chants, sometimes accompanied by her sponsor or a medicine man. The intent of these actions is to transform the girl into “Changing Woman,” a heroic figure in Apache myth. With this transformation comes longevity and perpetual strength. The ceremony is a signal to all in the community that the initiate is now an adult, and it tells the initiate that her community now has adult- like expectations for her.

In contrast to menarche, much less is known about boys’ reactions to spermarche. Most boys know about spontaneous ejaculations beforehand, and they get their informa- tion by reading, not by asking parents (Gaddis & Brooks-Gunn, 1985). When boys are prepared for spermarche, they feel more positively about it. Nevertheless, boys rarely tell parents or friends about this new development (Stein & Reiser, 1994).

Moodiness Adolescents are often thought to be extraordinarily moody—quickly moving from joy to sadness to—reflecting the influx of hormones associated with puberty. In fact, ado- lescents are moodier than children or adults, but this is not due primarily to hormones (Steinberg, 1999). Scientists often find that rapid increases in hormone levels are associ- ated with greater irritability and greater impulsivity, but the correlations tend to be small and are found primarily in early adolescence (Buchanan, Eccles, & Becker, 1992).

Instead of reflecting hormones, teenagers’ moodiness reflects frequent changes in their activities and social settings. Teens are more likely to report being in a good mood when hanging out with friends or when recreating; they tend to report being in a bad mood when in adult-regulated settings such as school classrooms or at a part-time job. Because adolescents often change activities and social settings many times in a single day, they appear to be moodier than adults (Larson, Csikszentmihalyi, & Graef, 2014).

Rate of Maturation Although puberty begins at age 10 in the average girl and age 12 in the average boy, for many children, puberty begins months or even years before or after these norms. An early-maturing boy might begin puberty at age 11, whereas a late-maturing boy might

The Apache celebrate menarche with a special ceremony in which a girl is said to become a legendary hero.

A nd

er s

Ry m

an /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 269

Because children enter puberty at different ages, early-maturing teens tower over their later-maturing age-mates.

Pe te

r H vi

zd ak

/T he

Im ag

e W

or ks

start at 15 or 16. An early-maturing girl might start puberty at age 9; a late-maturing girl, at 14 or 15.

Maturing early can be harmful for girls. Girls who mature early often lack self-confidence, are less popular, are more likely to be depressed and have behavior problems, and are more likely to smoke and drink. Early maturation often leads girls to relationships with older boys, and these girls are ill-prepared to cope with the demands of these relationships (Skoog & Stattin, 2014). In turn, this can result in life-changing effects on early-maturing girls who are pressured into sex and become mothers while still teenagers: as adults, they typically have less prestigious and lower-paying jobs (Mendle, Turkheimer, & Emery, 2007). These harmful outcomes are more likely when early- maturing girls live in poverty or experience harsh punishment from parents (Skoog & Stattin, 2014). Fortunately, when early-maturing girls have warm, supportive parents, they are less likely to suffer the harm- ful consequences of early maturation (Ge et al., 2002).

Maturing early can be harmful for boys, too. Early-maturing boys are at risk for psychological disorders such as depression; they are also more prone to substance abuse and to sexual activity (Mendle & Ferrero, 2012). Being physically advanced for their age may cause early-maturing boys to have problems with their peers who have not yet matured (lead- ing to depression) and cause them to spend more time with older boys (exposing them to risky behavior). However, the effects of early matura- tion are weaker for boys than they are for girls (Graber, 2013).

For boys and girls, maturing late poses few risks. Late-maturing girls fare well; late-maturing boys are at somewhat greater risk for depression (Mendle & Ferrero, 2012). But otherwise, Pete, the late- maturing boy in the opening vignette, has nothing to worry about. When he finally matures, others will treat him like an adult, and the few extra years of being treated like a child will not be harmful (Weichold & Silbereisen, 2005).

Recall 1. Puberty refers to changes in height and weight, to changes

in the body’s fat and muscle content, and to _______________.

2. Girls tend to have their growth spurts about _______________ earlier than boys.

3. During adolescent physical growth, boys have greater mus- cle growth than girls, acquire less _______________, and have greater increases in heart and lung capacity.

4. Primary sex characteristics are organs directly related to reproduction, whereas secondary sex characteristics are _______________.

5. During puberty in girls, the ovaries secrete estrogen, which causes the breasts to enlarge, the genitals to mature, and _______________.

6. Influence of the social environment on the onset of puberty is illustrated by the findings that menarche occurs earlier for girls who experience chronic stress or _______________ or who are depressed.

7. Adolescents are moodier than children and adults primarily because _______________.

8. Early maturation tends to be harmful to girls because _______________.

Interpret ●● Compare and contrast the impact of rate of maturation—that

is, maturing early versus late—on boys and girls.

Apply ●● At first blush, the onset of puberty would seem to be due

entirely to biology. In fact, the child’s environment influences the onset of puberty. Summarize the ways in which biology and experience interact to trigger the onset of puberty.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 8.1

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

270 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Dana had just started the seventh grade and was overjoyed that he could try out for the middle school football team. He’d always excelled in sports and was usually the star football player on the playground or in gym class. But this was Dana’s first opportunity to play on an actual team—with a real helmet, jersey, and pads—and he was jazzed! Dana’s dad played football in high school and thought Dana could benefit from the experience. His mom wasn’t so sure—she was afraid that he would get hurt and have to deal with the injury for the rest of his life.

Adolescence is a time of transition with regard to health. On the one hand, teens are less affected by the minor illnesses that would have kept them home in bed as children. On the other hand, teens are at greater risk for harm because of their unhealthy and risky behav- iors. In this section, we’ll look at some of the factors essential to adolescent health and see whether Dana’s mother should be worried about sports-related injuries. We’ll start with nutrition.

Nutrition The physical growth associated with puberty means that the body has special nutri- tional needs. A typical teenage girl should consume about 2,200 calories per day; a typical boy should consume about 2,700 calories. (The exact levels depend on a num- ber of factors, including body composition, growth rate, and activity level.) Teenagers also need calcium for bone growth and iron to make extra hemoglobin, the matter in

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the elements of a healthy diet for

adolescents? Why do some adolescents suffer from eating disorders?

●● Do adolescents get enough exercise? What are the pros and cons of participating in sports in high school?

●● What are common obstacles to healthy growth in adolescence?

8.2 Health

Many American teenagers eat far too many fast-food meals, which are notoriously high in calories.

M BI

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 271

red blood cells that carries oxygen. Boys need additional hemoglobin because of their increased muscle mass; girls need hemoglobin to replace that lost during menstruation.

Unfortunately, although many U.S. teenagers consume enough calories each day, too much of their intake consists of fast food rather than well-balanced meals. The result of too many meals of burgers, french fries, and a shake is that teens may get inadequate iron or calcium and far too much sodium and fat. With inadequate iron, teens are often listless and moody; with inadequate calcium, bones may not develop fully, placing the person at risk later in life for osteoporosis.

Obesity In part because of a diet high in fast foods, many American children and adolescents are overweight. The technical definition of “overweight” is based on the body mass index (BMI), which is an adjusted ratio of weight to height. Children and adolescents who are in the upper 5% (very heavy for their height) are defined as being overweight. Childhood obesity in the United States has reached epidemic proportions. In the past 25 to 30 years, the number of overweight children has doubled and the number of overweight adoles- cents has tripled so that today, roughly one adolescent out of five is overweight (Ogden et al., 2015). This trend is not unique to the United States: it is evident in many developed nations and in developing nations as they adopt diets and lifestyles like those in Western countries (World Health Organization, 2010).

Overweight youngsters are often unpopular and at risk for depression and anxiety (Pryor et al., 2016). They are also at risk for many medical problems throughout life, including high blood pressure and diabetes, because the vast majority of overweight chil- dren and adolescents become overweight adults (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2010).

No single factor causes children to become obese. Instead, several factors contribute, including the following:

●● Heredity Obesity runs in families, showing that genes contribute perhaps by causing some peo- ple to overeat, to be sedentary, or to be less able to convert fat to fuel (Cheung & Mao, 2012).

●● Parents Some parents urge children to “clean their plates” even when the children are not hun- gry. Other parents use food to comfort children who are upset. Both practices cause children to rely on external cues to eat instead of eating only when they’re hungry (Coelho et al., 2009; Wansink & Sobal, 2007). In contrast, authorita- tive parenting seems to protect youth from obe- sity, apparently because authoritative parents are more likely to eat healthily themselves as well as monitor their children’s eating (Harrist et al., 2017; Sleddens et al., 2011).

●● Sedentary lifestyle Youth are more prone to obesity when they are physically inactive, such as watching television instead of playing outdoors (de Rezende et al., 2014).

●● Too little sleep Adolescents who do not sleep enough tend to gain weight, perhaps because they have more opportunities to eat because they are awake longer, they experience increasing feel- ings of hunger, or they are too tired to exercise (Magee & Hale, 2012).

body mass index (BMI) Adjusted ratio of weight to height; used to define “overweight”.

Childhood obesity has reached epidemic proportions in the United States.

PN C/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

272 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Individually these factors may not lead to obesity. But collectively they may put chil- dren and adolescents on the path to obesity: A youth is at greater risk if she is geneti- cally prone to overeat, is sedentary, and is chronically sleepy. To understand obesity during childhood and adolescence, we need to consider all of these risk factors, as well as the amount and quality of food that’s available to children and adolescents (Harrison et al., 2011).

Obese youth can lose weight. The most effective weight-loss programs focus on changing children’s eating habits and encouraging them to become more active. Chil- dren and adolescents set goals for eating and exercise; parents help them set realistic goals, reward them for progress, and monitor their own eating and exercising. When programs incorporate these features, obese youth do lose weight (Mitchell, Amaro,  & Steele, 2016). However, even after losing weight, many youth participating in these programs remain overweight. Consequently, it is best to avoid overweight and obesity in the first place by encouraging children and adolescents to eat healthfully and to be active physically.

Yet another food-related problem common in adolescence are two similar eating dis- orders, anorexia and bulimia.

Anorexia and Bulimia In 2010, French model and actress Isabelle Caro died of respiratory disease just months after turning 26. Near her death, she weighed less than 75 pounds. Caro suffered from anorexia nervosa, a disorder marked by a persistent refusal to eat and an irrational fear of being overweight. Individuals with anorexia nervosa have a grossly distorted image of their body and claim to be overweight despite being painfully thin. Anorexia is a serious disor- der that, left untreated, can result in death (Attia, 2017).

A related eating disorder is bulimia nervosa. Individuals with bulimia nervosa alternate between binge eating periods, when they eat uncontrollably, and purging through self-induced vomiting, laxatives, fasting, or excessive exercise. During binge eating, adolescents with bulimia consume two days’ worth of calories in two hours or less, then purge once or twice daily (Keel, 2017).

Anorexia and bulimia are alike in many respects. Both disor- ders primarily affect females and emerge in adolescence and many of the same factors put teenage girls at risk for both eating disorders. Heredity puts some girls at risk as do psychosocial factors, such as adverse life experiences (e.g., stressful or traumatic events) and low self-esteem. However, the most important risk factor for adolescents is being overly concerned about one’s body and internalizing the thin body that is often thought to be ideal in Western cultures. Teenage girls are at risk when they frequently watch TV shows that empha- size attractive, thin characters and when their friends frequently talk about weight and diet constantly to stay thin (von Ranson & Wallace, 2014).

Although eating disorders are far more common in girls, boys make up about 10% of diagnosed cases of eating disorders. Boys with eating disorders have a distorted body image but, instead of worry- ing that they’re not sufficiently thin, they worry that they’re not suf- ficiently muscular. In trying to achieve a muscular ideal, boys with eating disorders reduce their intake of calories and they exercise exces- sively (Weltzin, 2017).

Fortunately, there are programs that can help protect teens from eating disorders (Becker et al., in press). These programs work to change attitudes toward being thin and provide ways to resist social pressure to be thin. One such program is described in the Spotlight on Research feature.

anorexia nervosa Persistent refusal to eat accompanied by an irrational fear of being overweight.

bulimia nervosa Disease in which people alternate between binge eating—periods when they eat uncontrollably—and purging with self- induced vomiting, laxatives, fasting, or excessive exercise.

Adolescent girls with anorexia nervosa believe that they are overweight, and they refuse to eat.

Bu bb

le s

Ph ot

ol ib

ra ry

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 273

On ResearchSpotlight

Evaluating a Program for Preventing Eating Disorders

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? One way to prevent teenage girls from developing eating disorders is to help them see faults in the thin female body that is often ideal- ized in the media. Teens who participate in activities critiquing this ideal are less prone to eating disorders; for example, they no longer find the thin ideal as attractive, they’re less likely to diet, and they report fewer symptoms of eating disorders. How- ever, research demonstrating the effective- ness of this prevention has been conducted under highly controlled conditions (e.g., in a research center, with facilitators who are highly trained and closely monitored); Eric Stice and colleagues (2009) wanted to con- firm that the prevention would work when administered in a more realistic setting—in this case, by school personnel (e.g., nurses, counselors) in a high school.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Stice and colleagues recruited high school girls who had con- cerns about their body image. Half of the girls were assigned to a control condition in which they were given a brochure describ- ing eating disorders and suggesting ways to improve one’s body image. The other half were assigned to an intervention condition in which they attended four 1-hour ses- sions that included a variety of exercises designed to show girls the costs of the thin ideal. For example, they engaged in role play in which they tried to convince group lead- ers why girls shouldn’t pursue the thin ideal and they completed homework in which they listed pressures they experienced to be thin and ways to resist those pressures. Before the intervention started and at 1, 6, and 12 months after it ended, girls in both groups completed questionnaires measuring their adherence to the thin ideal, their body

dissatisfaction, their dieting behavior, and their symptoms of eating disorders.

Who were the participants in the study? The researchers tested 306 adolescent girls; 139 were in the intervention condition, and 167 were in the control condition.

What was the design of the study? This study was experimental because Stice and colleagues were interested in comparing the impact of the prevention program, relative to the control condition, on adolescent girls’ attitudes and behaviors related to eating dis- orders. The researchers did not investigate age differences, so the study was neither cross-sectional nor longitudinal.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. The researchers obtained informed consent from the adolescents and their parents. The measures and the pre- vention program posed no obvious risks to participants. Any girls in either condition who showed symptoms of eating disorders were referred to treatment.

What were the results? The results were similar for the different outcome measures. For simplicity, we focus on dieting behavior, which was measured with a questionnaire that included items such as “Do you try to eat less at meal times than you would like to eat?” and “Do you deliberately eat foods that are slimming?” Scores on the scale ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The results, shown in ◗●Figure 8.3, indicate that at the pretest, girls in both groups occasionally endorsed dieting-related behaviors. What’s noteworthy is that these responses were reasonably stable for girls in the control con- dition but declined for girls in the prevention group. In other words, the prevention condi- tion caused girls to be less likely to report dieting-related behaviors.

What did the investigators conclude? These findings, like the prior work, illustrate that girls change their body- and eating- related attitudes and behaviors after participat- ing in a prevention emphasizing the costs and

shortcomings of an ideal thin body. As Stice and colleagues put it, “the findings from the present trial suggest that positive intervention effects still emerge when real-world provid- ers deliver the prevention program in eco- logically valid settings with a heterogeneous population” (p. 831).

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? All four out- come measures were obtained from ques- tionnaires completed by the adolescents. An obvious way to provide converging evidence would be to measure some of these out- comes differently, such as by asking parents to describe their daughter’s dieting-related behaviors, by using experimental tasks to estimate body dissatisfaction, or by obtain- ing physiological measures that reflect eating disorders.

SOURCE: Stice, E., Rohde, P., Gau, J., & Shaw, H. (2009). An effectiveness trial of dissonance-based eating disorder prevention program for high-risk adolescent girls. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 825–834.

●◗ Figure 8.3 When teenage girls participate in pro- grams that emphasize the costs and shortcomings of an ideal thin body, they change their body- and eating-related atti- tudes and behaviors.

Testing time

Pr et

es t

Sc o

re

2.5

2.25

2

1.75

1.5

1 m on

th p

os t-t

es t

6 m

on th

p os

t-t es

t

12 m

on th

p os

t-t es

t

Prevention Control

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Programs such as the one described in the Spotlight on Research feature are effec- tive: At-risk adolescents who participate in these programs are more satisfied with their appearance and are less likely to diet or overeat (Stice, South, & Shaw, 2012). For those teens affected by eating disorders, treatment is available: Like prevention programs, treatment typically focuses on modifying key attitudes and behaviors (Puhl & Brownell, 2005).

Think About It Describe how obesity, anorexia, and buli- mia represent the different forces in the biopsychosocial network.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

274 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Think About It Many teenagers do not eat well-balanced meals, and many do not get enough exercise. What would you do to improve teenagers’ dietary and exercise habits?

From participating in sports, youth can improve their self-esteem and learn skills about how to work effectively as part of a group.

ZU M

A P

re ss

, I nc

./ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Physical Fitness Being physically active promotes mental and physical health during adolescence and throughout adulthood. Individuals who regularly engage in physical activity reduce their risk for obesity, cancer, heart disease, diabetes, and psychological disorders, including depression and anxiety. “Regular activity” typically means exercising for 30 minutes at least three times a week at a pace that keeps an adolescent’s heart rate at about 140 beats per minute (President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports, 2004). Running, vigorous walking, swimming, aerobic dancing, biking, and cross-country skiing are all examples of activities that can provide this level of intensity.

Unfortunately, all the evidence indicates that most adolescents rarely get enough exer- cise. Part of the problem is that physical education classes provide the only regular oppor- tunity for many high school students to exercise, yet a minority of high school students are enrolled in physical education and most who are enrolled do not attend daily.

Many teenagers get exercise by participating in organized sports. Today, approximately 4.5 million boys and 3.3 million girls participate in sports (National Federation of State High School Associations, 2017). Participating in sports has many benefits for youth. Sports can enhance participants’ self-esteem and can help them to learn initiative (Bowker, 2006; Eime et al., 2013). Sports can also provide adolescents a chance to learn important social skills, such as how to work effectively as part of a group, often in complementary roles.

These benefits of participating in sports are balanced by potential hazards. About 15% of high school athletes will be injured, requiring some medical treatment. Fortunately, most of these injuries are not serious (Nelson, 1996). Dana’s mom can rest easy; the odds are that he won’t be injured, and if he is, it won’t be serious. Another concern is that sev- eral studies have linked youth participation in sports to delinquent and antisocial behavior (e.g., Gardner, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). However, outcomes are usually positive when sports participation is combined with participation in activities that involve adults, such as school, religious, or youth groups (Linver, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009; Zarrett et al., 2009).

A more serious problem is the use of illegal drugs to improve performance. Some athletes use anabolic steroids—drugs that are chemically similar to the male hormone testosterone—to increase muscle size and strength and to promote more rapid recovery from injury. Approxi- mately 3% of high school students report having used anabolic steroids, with boys more likely than girls to use steroids (LaBotz & Griesemer, 2016). This is worrisome because steroid use has been linked to cardiovascular disease, cancer, and mental health problems (Bird et al., 2015).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 275

Parents, coaches, and health professionals need to make sure that high school athletes are aware of the dangers of steroids and should encourage youth to meet their athletic goals through methods that do not involve drug use (LaBotz & Griesemer, 2016).

Threats to Adolescent Well-Being Every year approximately 1 U.S. adolescent out of 2,000 dies, with boys more than twice as likely as girls to die. ◗●Figure 8.4 shows that for boys and girls, most deaths are due to accidents involving motor vehicles (for boys and girls) or firearms for boys). The next most common cause of death for boys and girls is suicide, fol- lowed by homicide for boys and cancer for girls (Heron, 2016).

Sadly, many of these deaths are preventable. Far too many adolescents are killed because they drive too fast, drive while drunk or texting, or drive without wearing a seat belt (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). And deaths due to guns are often linked to “all-too-easy” access to firearms in the home: In far too many homes, firearms are stored loaded, unlocked, or both (Johnson et al., 2006).

Adolescent deaths from accidents can be explained in part because adoles- cents take risks that adults often find unacceptable (Nell, 2002). Teens drive cars recklessly, engage in unprotected sex, and sometimes use illegal and dangerous drugs (we’ll discuss the latter behaviors more in Chapter 9). Why? Many adolescents believe they are invulnerable—only others experience the harmful consequences of risky behaviors (Hill, Duggan, & Lapsley, 2012).

In addition, adolescents find the rewards associated with risky behavior far more appealing than adults do—so much so that they’re willing to ignore the risks. For many adolescents, the pleasure, excitement, and intimacy of sex far outweigh the risks of disease and pregnancy, just as the relaxation associated with smoking outweighs the threat of lung cancer (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). And as we saw earlier in this chapter (pages 263–264), the appeal of high-risk behaviors reflects the maturity of the pleasure-seeking brain regions relative to those regions that control behavior (Casey, 2015).

●◗ Figure 8.4 Adolescent boys and girls are more likely to die from accidents involving cars or firearms than any other single cause.

Boys Girls

Accidents

Suicide

Homicide

Other

Accidents

Suicide

Cancer

Other

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2005.

Recall 1. An adolescent’s diet should contain adequate calories,

_______________, and iron.

2. Individuals with _______________ alternate between binge eating and purging.

3. During adolescence, the most important risk factor for anorexia and bulimia is _______________.

4. Regular physical activity helps to promote _______________ and physical health.

5. Some teenage athletes use anabolic steroids to increase muscular strength and to _______________.

6. More teenager die from _______________ than from any other single cause.

7. Because they place greater emphasis on the _______________ actions, adolescents make what adults think are risky decisions.

Interpret ●● Distinguish the biological factors that contribute to obesity

from the environmental factors.

Apply ●● How does adolescent risk taking illustrate the idea that indi-

viduals help to shape their own development?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 8.2

Adolescents are far more accident-prone than children or adults, in part because they believe the rewards of risky behavior far out- weigh the potential harm.

Br oo

ke W

ha tn

al l/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

276 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Amir, a 14-year-old boy, was an enigma to his mother, Faiza. On one hand, Amir’s growing reasoning skills impressed and sometimes surprised her: He not only readily grasped tech- nical discussions of her medical work but also was becoming adept at finding loopholes in her explanations of why he wasn’t allowed to do some activities with his friends. On the other hand, sometimes Amir was a real teenage “space cadet.” Simple problem-solving stumped him, or he made silly mistakes and got the wrong answer. Amir didn’t correspond to Faiza’s image of the formal-operational thinker that she remembered from her college human development class.

For information-processing theorists, adolescence does not represent a distinct, qualita- tively different stage of cognitive development. Instead, adolescence represents a transi- tion from the rapidly changing cognitive processes of childhood to the mature cognitive processes of young adulthood. Cognitive changes take place during adolescence, but they are small compared with those seen in childhood. Adolescence is a time when cognitive processes are tweaked to adult levels. We’ll describe these changes in this section, and as we do, we’ll see why adolescents such as Faiza’s son don’t always think as effectively as they might.

Working Memory and Processing Speed Working memory is the site of ongoing cognitive processing, and processing speed is the speed with which people complete basic cognitive processes. Both of these capaci- ties achieve adultlike levels during adolescence. Adolescents’ working memory has about the same capacity as adults’ working memory, which means that teenagers are better able than children to store information needed for ongoing cognitive processes. In addi- tion, processing speed becomes faster, as illustrated in ◗ Figure 8.5 and exemplified by

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do working memory and processing speed change in

adolescence?

●● How do increases in content knowledge, strategies, and metacognitive skill influence adolescent cognition?

●● What changes in problem-solving and reasoning take place in adolescence?

8.3 Information Processing During Adolescence

●◗ Figure 8.5 Response time declines steadily during childhood and reaches adultlike levels during middle adolescence.

R ea

ct io

n t

im e

(m il

li se

co n

d s)

9 0

100

200

300

400

500

1375 11 15 17 19 21 23 Age (years)

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Data from Kail (2004).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 277

performance on a simple response-time task in which individuals press a button as rapidly as possible in response to a visual stimulus. The time needed to respond drops steadily during childhood—from about one-third of a second at age 8 to one-quarter of a second at age 12—but changes little thereafter. This pattern of change is not spe- cific to simple response time; instead, it is found for a wide range of cognitive tasks: Adolescents generally process information just about as quickly as young adults (Kail, 2004). Change in working memory and processing speed means that compared with children, adolescents process information very efficiently.

These changes in efficiency reflect changes to the brain described earlier in this chapter (page 263). Increases in myelination during ado- lescence allow nerve impulses to travel more rapidly, which contrib- utes to more rapid and more efficient information processing during this period (Scantlebury et al., 2014).

Content Knowledge, Strategies, and Metacognitive Skill As children move into adolescence, they acquire adultlike levels of knowledge and understanding in many domains. For example, many parents turn to their teens for help in learning how to use fancy fea- tures on their smartphone. This increased knowledge is useful for its own sake, but it also has the indirect effect of enabling adolescents to learn, understand, and remember more of new experiences (Schneider, 2015). Imagine two middle school students—one a baseball expert, the other not—watching a baseball game. Compared with the novice, the  adolescent expert would understand many of the nuances of the game and, later, remember more features of the game.

As their content knowledge increases, adolescents also become better skilled at identifying strategies appropriate for a specific task, then monitoring the chosen strategy to verify that it is working (Schneider, 2015). For example, adolescents are more likely to outline and highlight information in a text. They are more likely to make lists of material they don’t know well and should study more, and they more often embed these activities in a master study plan (e.g., a list of assignments, quizzes, and tests for a two-week period). All these activities help adolescents learn more effectively and remem- ber more accurately (Schneider, 2015).

Problem-Solving and Reasoning When solving problems, two approaches are common (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). One is to rely on heuristics, rules of thumb that do not guarantee a solution but are useful in solving a range of problems. Heuristics tend to be fast and require little effort. Another approach is to determine an answer mathematically or logically. Compared with heuris- tics, analytic solutions are slower and require more effort.

To see the difference between heuristic and analytic solutions, think about the follow- ing problem (based on Kail, 2013):

Gabriela wants a new smartphone. A well-known tech website reports that con- sumers rate the Stratosphere as the best model, but Gabriela’s aunt has a Superba that she likes a lot. Which phone should Gabriela get, the Stratosphere or the Superba?

The heuristic solution relies on personal testimony from familiar people. In this case, that means relying on the aunt’s experience. In contrast, the analytic solution relies on the sta- tistical information from consumers’ ratings.

Adolescents often have adultlike skills in some domains, such as using an iPad, which allows them to teach adults.

Co ur

te sy

o f R

ob er

t V . K

ai l

Think About It Students typically are introduced to the study of complex topics such as phi- losophy and experimental science during adolescence. Explain how their maturing cognitive skills contribute to the study of these and other subject areas.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

278 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Heuristics are used throughout childhood, adolescence, and adulthood, but analytic solutions become more common in adolescence (Klaczynski, 2013). In other words, teens are more likely than children to rely on logic or math and would be more likely to sug- gest that Gabriela pick the phone with the higher ratings from consumers. This develop- mental change reflects adolescents’ mastery of formal operational thinking (described on pages 188–190). But it also reflects adolescents’ greater working-memory capacity (needed because analytic solutions require effort) and their greater self-regulation (needed to sup- press fast-acting heuristics).

Adolescents also are better skilled at finding weaknesses in arguments. In logical rea- soning, they can pinpoint flaws in arguments (Weinstock, Neuman, & Glassner, 2006). In scientific reasoning, adolescents recognize the hazards in making generalizations from extremely small samples. They would be wary of concluding that people from another country are particularly friendly based on meeting just two people from that country (Klaczynski & Lavallee, 2005).

Of course, adolescents may not always use their skills effectively. Sometimes they resort to heuristics because they take less effort and are “good enough” for the problem. Also, sometimes adolescents’ beliefs interfere with effective thinking: When evidence is inconsistent with adolescents’ beliefs, they may dismiss the evidence as being irrelevant or try to reinterpret the evidence to make it consistent with their beliefs. For example, an American teenager may be quick to find flaws in a study showing that Americans are self-centered but overlook similar flaws in a study showing that Americans are better lead- ers than people in other countries (Byrnes & Dunbar, 2014). Thus, adolescents use their reasoning skills selectively, raising their standards to dismiss findings that threaten their beliefs and lowering them to admit findings compatible with their beliefs.

Findings such as these tell us that Faiza, the mother in the opening vignette, should not be so perplexed by her son’s seemingly erratic thinking: Adolescents (and adults, for that matter) do not always use the most powerful levels of thinking that they possess. The information-processing account of intellectual functioning in adolescence is a description of how children and adolescents can think, not how they always or even usually think.

These changing features of information processing are summarized in ◗ Table 8.1. Change in each of these elements of information processing occurs gradually. When com- bined, they contribute to the steady progress to mature thinking that is the destination of adolescent cognitive development.

Ja ni

ne W

ie de

l/ Ph

ot ol

ib ra

ry /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

When children reach adolescence, they are more likely to use reasoning skills and solve problems analytically.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 279

Table 8.1

Information Processing During Adolescence FeaTure STaTe in adoleScence Working memory and processing speed

Adolescents have adultlike working memory capacity and processing speed, enabling them to process information efficiently.

Content knowledge Adolescents’ greater knowledge of the world facilitates understanding and memory of new experiences.

Strategies and metacognition Adolescents are better able to identify task- appropriate strategies and to monitor the effectiveness of those strategies.

Problem-solving and reasoning Adolescents often solve problems analytically by relying on mathematics or logic, and they can detect weaknesses in scientific evidence and logical arguments.

© 2019 Cengage

Recall 1. According to information-processing theorists, adoles-

cence is a time of important changes in working memory, processing speed, _______________, strategies, and metacognition.

2. Information-processing theorists view adolescence as a time of _______________.

3. When solving problems, adolescents are more likely than children to solve problems _______________.

4. When evidence is inconsistent with their beliefs, adolescents often _______________.

Interpret ●● The information-processing account of cognitive change in

adolescence emphasizes working memory, knowledge, and strategies. How might each of these factors be influenced by nature? By nurture?

Apply ●● How can the information-processing skills described here—

and, in particular, the limits in adolescent thinking—help to explain adolescent risk taking described on page 275.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 8.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do adolescents reason about moral issues?

●● What other factors influence moral reasoning?

●● What factors help promote more sophisticated reasoning about moral issues?

8.4 Reasoning About Moral Issues

Howard, the least popular boy in the eighth grade, had been wrongly accused of stealing a sixth grader’s iPod. Min-shen, another eighth grader, knew that Howard was innocent but said nothing to the school principal for fear of what his friends would say about siding with Howard. A few days later when Min-shen’s father heard about the incident, he was upset that his son apparently had so little “moral fiber.” Why hadn’t Min-shen acted in the face of an injustice?

One day the local paper had two articles about youth from the area. One article was about a 15-year-old girl who was badly burned while saving her younger brothers from a fire in their apartment. Her mother said she wasn’t surprised by her daughter’s actions

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

280 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

instrumental orientation Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 2, in which moral reasoning is based on the aim of looking out for one’s own needs.

because she had always been an extraordinarily caring person. The other article was about two 17-year-old boys who had beaten an elderly man to death. They had only planned to steal his wallet, but when he insulted them and tried to punch them, they became enraged.

Reading articles like these, you can’t help but question why some teenagers (and  adults) act in ways that earn our deepest respect and admiration, whereas others earn our utter contempt as well as our pity. And at a more mundane level, we wonder why Min-shen didn’t tell the principal the truth about the stolen iPod. In this section, we’ll start our exploration of moral reasoning with an influential theory proposed by Lawrence Kohlberg.

Kohlberg’s Theory Some of the world’s great novels are based on moral dilemmas. For example, Victor Hugo’s Les Misérables begins with the protagonist, Jean Valjean, stealing a loaf of bread to feed his sister’s starving child. You can probably give many reasons for Valjean to have stolen the bread as well as argue why he shouldn’t have stolen the bread. Lawrence Kohlberg created stories like this one in which decisions were difficult because every alternative involved some undesirable consequences. In fact, there is no “correct” answer—that’s why the sto- ries are referred to as moral “dilemmas.” Kohlberg was more interested in the reasoning used to justify a decision—Why should Jean Valjean steal the bread? Why should he not steal the bread?—than in the decision itself.

Kohlberg’s (1969) best-known moral dilemma is this story about Heinz, whose wife is dying:

In Europe, a woman was near death from cancer. One drug might save her, a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered. The druggist was charging $2,000, ten times what the drug cost him to make. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could only get together about half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said, “No.” The husband got desperate and broke into the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife. (p. 379)

Thus, Heinz and Jean Valjean both face moral dilemmas in which the various alternative courses of action have desirable and undesirable features.

Kohlberg analyzed children’s, adolescents’, and adults’ responses to a large number of dilemmas and identified three levels of moral reasoning, each divided into two stages. Across the six stages, the basis for moral reasoning shifts. In the earliest stages, moral rea- soning is based on external forces, such as the promise of reward or the threat of punish- ment. At the most advanced levels, moral reasoning is based on a personal, internal moral code and is unaffected by others’ views or society’s expectations.

Kohlberg identified three levels of moral reasoning: preconventional, conventional, and postconventional. Each level is subdivided into two substages. At the preconventional level, moral reasoning is based on external forces. For most children, many adolescents, and some adults, moral reasoning is controlled almost exclusively by rewards and punish- ments. Individuals in stage 1 moral reasoning assume an obedience orientation, which means believing that authority figures know what is right and wrong. Consequently, stage 1 individuals do what authorities say is right to avoid being punished. At this stage, one might argue that Heinz shouldn’t steal the drug because an authority figure (e.g., parent or police officer) said he shouldn’t do it. Alternatively, one might argue that Heinz should steal the drug because he would get into trouble if he let his wife die.

In stage 2 of the preconventional level, people adopt an instrumental orientation, in which they look out for their own needs. Stage 2 individuals are nice to others because they

preconventional level First level of reasoning in Kohlberg’s theory, where moral reasoning is based on external forces.

obedience orientation Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 1, in which moral reasoning is based on the belief that adults know what is right and wrong.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 281

expect the favor to be returned in the future. Someone at this stage could justify stealing the drug because Heinz’s wife might do something nice for Heinz in return. Or someone might argue that Heinz shouldn’t steal the drug because it will create more problems for him.

At the conventional level, adolescents and adults look to society’s norms for moral guidance. In other words, people’s moral reasoning is largely determined by others’ expectations of them. In stage 3, adolescents’ and adults’ moral reasoning is based on interpersonal norms. The aim is to win the approval of other people by behaving as “good boys” and “good girls” would. Stage 3 individuals might argue that Heinz shouldn’t steal the drug because he must keep his reputation as an honest man or that he should steal the drug because no one would think negatively of him for trying to save his wife’s life.

Stage 4 of the conventional level focuses on social system morality. Here, adolescents and adults believe that social roles, expectations, and laws exist to maintain order within society and to promote the good of all people. Stage 4 individuals might reason that Heinz shouldn’t steal the drug, even though his wife might die, because it is illegal and no one is above the law. Alternatively, they might claim that he should steal it to live up to his mar- riage vow of protecting his wife even though he will face negative consequences for his theft.

At the postconventional level, moral reasoning is based on a personal moral code. The emphasis is no longer on external forces such as punishment, reward, or social roles. In stage 5, people base their moral reasoning on a social contract. Adults agree that mem- bers of social groups adhere to a social contract because a common set of expectations and laws benefits all group members. However, if these expectations and laws no longer pro- mote the welfare of individuals, they become invalid. Consequently, stage 5 individuals might reason that Heinz should steal the drug because social rules about property rights no longer benefit individuals’ welfare. Alternatively, they could argue that he shouldn’t steal the drug because it would create social anarchy.

Finally, in stage 6 of the postconventional level, universal ethical principles dominate moral reasoning. Abstract principles such as justice, compassion, and equality form the basis of a personal code that may conflict with society’s expectations and laws. Stage 6 individuals might argue that Heinz should steal the drug because saving a life takes prece- dence over everything else, including the law. Or they might claim that Heinz’s wife has a right to die and that he should not force his views on her by stealing and administering the drug.

Putting the stages together, the entire sequence of moral development looks like this:

Preconventional Level: Punishment and Reward Stage 1: Obedience to authority Stage 2: Nice behavior in exchange for future favors

Conventional Level: Social Norms Stage 3: Live up to others’ expectations Stage 4: Follow rules to maintain social order

Postconventional Level: Moral Codes Stage 5: Adhere to a social contract when it is valid Stage 6: Personal moral system based on abstract principles

This developmental sequence usually unfolds over many years, but sometimes it hap- pens more dramatically, such as when individuals undergo a major transformation in their moral motivation. One noteworthy example of such a transformation was depicted in Steven Spielberg’s Oscar-winning movie Schindler’s List, as described in the Real People feature.

conventional level Second level of reasoning in Kohlberg’s theory, where moral reasoning is based on society’s norms.

interpersonal norms Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 3, in which moral reasoning is based on winning the approval of others.

social system morality Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 4, in which moral reasoning is based on mainte- nance of order in society.

postconventional level Third level of reasoning in Kohlberg’s theory, in which morality is based on a  personal moral code.

social contract Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 5, in which moral reasoning is based on the belief that laws are for the good of all mem- bers of society.

universal ethical principles Characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 6, in which moral reasoning is based on moral principles that apply to all.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

282 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Evaluating Kohlberg’s Theory Research supports many features of Kohlberg’s theory. For example, Kohlberg proposed that individuals move through the six stages only in the order listed and longitudinal stud- ies confirm this prediction: individuals progress through each stage in sequence, rarely skipping stages (Colby et al., 1983). In addition, because less advanced moral reasoning reflects the influence of external forces (e.g., rewards) but more advanced reasoning is based on a personal moral code, only individuals with advanced reasoning should be com- pelled to moral action when external forces may not favor it. Consistent with this claim, adolescents who defend their principles in difficult situations tend to be more advanced in Kohlberg’s stages (Gibbs et al., 1986). In contrast, Min-shen, the boy in the vignette, said nothing because speaking out on behalf of the unpopular student is unlikely to lead to being rewarded by his peers.

On some other features, the theory does not fare as well. For example, Kohlberg claimed that the sequence of stages is universal: However, except for the earliest stages, moral reasoning in other cultures is often not described well by Kohlberg’s theory, mainly because not all cultures and religions share the theory’s emphasis on individual rights and justice. For example, the Hindu religion emphasizes duty and responsibility to oth- ers. Consistent with this emphasis, when Hindu children and adults respond to moral dilemmas, they favor solutions that provide care for others even when individual rights or justice may suffer (Miller & Bersoff, 1992). Thus, the bases of moral reasoning are not universal as Kohlberg claimed; instead, they reflect cultural values.

Even for people living in Western cultures, research does not consistently support the theory’s emphasis on justice. Many children, adolescents, and adults think about moral issues in terms of justice and caring, depending on the nature of the moral dilemma and the context (Turiel, 2006). To illustrate, suppose a child forgot to bring her lunch to school. One classmate might refuse to share her lunch with the child and justify it in justice terms by saying, first, that the lunch is the child’s property and she has the right to eat all of it, and, second, that the lunch room rules prohibit sharing. Another classmate might be eager to share her lunch and justifying it in caring terms by saying that helping others is para- mount and requires that she share.

In 1939, Oskar Schindler was an entre-preneur who made a great deal of money working for the Germans after they conquered Poland. Motivated at first strictly by the poten- tial for personal profit, he opened—with few qualms—a factory in which he employed Jews as slave labor.

Schindler’s company was quite success- ful. But as the war continued, Jewish citizens in Poland were rounded up and shipped to concentration camps or summarily executed. This deeply disturbed Schindler, and his attitudes began to change. His employees suggested that he give the Germans a list of workers essential to the factory’s contin- ued operation. The list protected employees because the plant’s products were used in the war effort. No longer driven by profit,

Schindler went to great lengths to preserve life: He created cover stories to support his claims that certain employees were essential and went to Auschwitz to rescue employees who were sent there despite being included on his list.

Schindler’s list saved many lives. Profits were made (and helped provide the perfect cover), but he employed Jews in his factory primarily to save them from the gas cham- ber. Schindler may have begun the war at Kohlberg’s preconventional level—where he was motivated solely by personal profit—but he ultimately moved to the postconven- tional level—where he was motivated by the higher principle of saving lives. It is at the postconventional level that heroes are made.

Real People Applying Human Development

Schindler’s List

During World War II, Oskar Schindler saved the lives of many Jews by adding their names to lists of employees who were essential for his factory’s operation.

A P

Im ag

es /P

et er

H ill

eb re

ch t

Think About It Research shows that people sometimes do not reason at the most advanced levels of which they are capable; instead, they revert to simpler, less mature levels. Might this happen in the realm of moral reasoning too? What factors might cause a person’s moral reasoning to revert to a less sophisticated level?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 283

Also inconsistent with Kohlberg’s theory is work on the impact on membership in social groups on moral reasoning. The peer group is particularly salient during adoles- cence and teens’ judgments of what’s fair reflect their ideas about what’s best for their group. For example, adolescents are more likely to exclude peers if they believe that those peers will hurt a group’s identity or functioning. If a school band decides to enter a com- petition, adolescents are more likely than children to say that only the best players should be in the competitive band (Mulvey, 2016). Similarly, adolescents are more likely to favor peers whose views benefit a group, even if they believe those views are unfair. If a group traditionally shares resources equally with other groups but one group member suggests keeping more resources for the group, adolescents view the group member more favorably than children do (Rutland & Killen, 2017). Thus, adolescents are more likely than children to balance being fair to others with being loyal to one’s group, a phenomenon that’s not well integrated into Kohlberg’s theory.

Promoting Moral Reasoning Whether it is based on justice or care, most cultures and most parents want to encourage adolescents to think carefully about moral issues. What can be done to help adolescents develop more mature forms of moral reasoning? Sometimes simply being exposed to more advanced moral reasoning is sufficient to promote developmental change (Walker, 1980). Adolescents may notice, for example, that older friends do not wait to be rewarded to help others. Or a teenager may notice that respected peers take courageous positions regard- less of the social consequences. Such experiences apparently cause adolescents to reevalu- ate their reasoning on moral issues and propel them toward more sophisticated thinking.

Discussion can be particularly effective in revealing shortcomings in moral reason- ing (Berkowitz et al., 2006). When people reason about moral issues with others whose reasoning is at a higher level, the usual result is that individuals reasoning at lower levels improve. This is particularly true when the conversational partner with the more sophisti- cated reasoning tries to understand the other’s view by asking the other child to clarify or paraphrase his or her thinking (Walker, Hennig, & Krettenauer, 2000).

Adolescents’ moral reasoning (and moral behavior) is also influenced by their involve- ment in religion. Adolescents who are more involved in religion have greater concern for others and place more emphasis on helping them (Saroglou et al., 2005). An obvious expla- nation for this link is that religion provides moral beliefs and guidelines for adolescents.

Teenagers who engage in moral behavior, such as participating in protest marches, often reason at high levels in Kohlberg’s theory.

Ji m

W es

t/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

284 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

But participation in religion can promote moral reasoning in a second, less direct way. Involvement in a religious community—typically through youth groups associated with a church, synagogue, or mosque—connects teens to an extended network of caring peers and adults. From interacting with individuals in this network, earning their trust, and sharing their values, adolescents gain a sense of responsibility to and concern for others (King & Furrow, 2008).

Research findings such as these send an important message to parents: The best way for them to promote their children’s moral reasoning is through discussion (Walker & Tay- lor, 1991). Research consistently shows that mature moral reasoning comes about when adolescents are free to express their opinions on moral issues to their parents, who in turn express their own opinions and thus expose their adolescent children to more mature moral reasoning (Hoffman, 1988, 1994).

Recall 1. Kohlberg’s theory includes the preconventional, conven-

tional, and _______________ levels.

2. For children and adolescents in the preconventional level, moral reasoning is strongly influenced by _______________.

3. Supporting Kohlberg’s theory are findings that people progress through the stages in the predicted sequence, and that_______________.

4. Although Kohlberg’s theory emphasizes the role of justice in moral reasoning, research shows that _______________ also influences moral reasoning.

5. In adolescents’ moral reasoning, they often balance fairness with _______________.

6. If parents want to foster their children’s moral development, they should _______________ with them.

Interpret ●● How similar is Piaget’s stage of formal operational thought to

Kohlberg’s stage of conventional moral reasoning?

Apply ●● Imagine that you were the father of Min-shen, the boy in

the vignette who did not stand up for the boy who was wrongly accused of stealing the iPod. Based on the research described in this section, what might you do to try to advance Min-shen’s level of moral reasoning?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 8.4

When adolescents discuss moral issues together, the thinking of those at lower stages in Kohlberg’s theory is often influ- enced by those whose thinking is at the higher stages; that is, individuals who rea- son at the lower levels typically move their thinking to a more sophisticated stage.

Pa ul

C on

kl in

/P ho

to Ed

it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e R 8 : R I T E S O F PA S S A G E 285

8.1 Pubertal Changes

What physical changes during adolescence mark the transi- tion to a mature young adult?

●● Puberty includes bodily changes in height and weight as well as sexual maturation. Girls typically begin the growth spurt earlier than boys, who acquire more muscle, less fat, and greater heart and lung capacity. The brain commu- nicates more effectively, and the frontal cortex continues to mature. Sexual maturation, which includes primary and secondary sex characteristics, occurs in predictable sequences for boys and girls.

What factors cause the physical changes associated with puberty?

●● Pubertal changes take place when the pituitary gland signals the adrenal gland, ovaries, and testes to secrete hor- mones that initiate physical changes. The timing of puberty is influenced strongly by health and nutrition. Its timing is also influenced by the social environment; for example, puberty occurs earlier for girls who experience stress, harsh punishment, or depression.

How do physical changes affect adolescents’ psychological development?

●● Pubertal changes affect adolescents’ psychological func- tioning. Teens, particularly girls, become concerned about their appearance. When forewarned, adolescents respond positively to menarche and spermarche. Adolescents are moodier than children or adults primarily because their moods shift in response to frequent changes in activities and social setting. Early maturation tends to be harmful, especially for girls.

8.2 Health

What are the elements of a healthy diet for adolescents? Why do some adolescents suffer from eating disorders?

●● For proper growth, teenagers need to consume adequate calories, calcium, and iron. Unfortunately, many teen- agers do not eat properly and do not receive adequate nutrition.

●● Anorexia and bulimia, eating disorders that typically affect adolescent girls, are characterized by an irrational fear of being overweight. Adolescents are at greatest risk for eat- ing disorders when they’re overly concerned with their bodies and have internalized the thin-body ideal. Treat- ment and prevention programs emphasize changing ado- lescents’ views toward thinness and their eating-related behaviors.

Do adolescents get enough exercise? What are the pros and cons of participating in sports in high school?

●● Individuals who work out at least three times weekly often have improved physical and mental health. Unfortunately, many high school students do not get enough exercise.

●● Millions of American boys and girls participate in sports. The benefits of participating in sports include improved physical fitness, enhanced self-esteem, and an understand- ing about teamwork. The potential costs include injury and abuse of performance-enhancing drugs.

What are common obstacles to healthy growth in adolescence?

●● Accidents involving automobiles or firearms are the most common cause of death in American teenagers. Many of these deaths could be prevented if, for example, adolescents did not drive recklessly (e.g., too fast and without wearing seat belts). Adolescents often seem themselves as invulner- able to risk and often place greater value on the rewards associated with risky behavior.

8.3 Information Processing During Adolescence

How do working memory and processing speed change in adolescence?

●● Working memory increases in capacity, and processing speed becomes faster. Both achieve adultlike levels during adolescence.

How do increases in content knowledge, strategies, and metacognitive skill influence adolescent cognition?

●● Content knowledge increases to expertlike levels in some domains, and strategies and metacognitive skills become more sophisticated.

What changes in problem-solving and reasoning take place in adolescence?

●● Adolescents often solve problems analytically, using math- ematics or logic. They also acquire skill in detecting weak- nesses in scientific evidence and in logical arguments. But they don’t consistently use these skills, sometimes resorting to simpler heuristics and sometimes allowing beliefs to interfere with reasoning.

8.4 Reasoning About Moral Issues

How do adolescents reason about moral issues?

●● Kohlberg proposed that moral reasoning includes precon- ventional, conventional, and postconventional levels. Moral reasoning is first based on rewards and punishments and much later on personal moral codes.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

286 PA RT T W O : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

What other factors influence moral reasoning?

●● As predicted by Kohlberg’s theory, people progress through the stages in sequence and morally advanced reasoning is associated with more frequent moral behavior. However, Kohlberg’s theory ignores the impact of caring, which often influences moral reasoning. And adolescents often balance fairness with group loyalty.

What factors help promote more sophisticated reasoning about moral issues?

●● Many factors can promote more sophisticated moral rea- soning, including (1) observing others reasoning at more advanced levels; (2) discussing moral issues with peers, teachers, and parents; and (3) being involved in a religious community that connects adolescents to a network of car- ing peers and adults.

Test Yourself: Recall Answers 8.1 1. sexual maturation 2. two years 3. fat 4. physical signs of maturity that are not linked directly to reproductive organs, such as the appearance of body hair 5. fat to accumulate 6. harsh punishment 7. they change activities and social settings frequently and their moods track these changes 8. it leads them to associate with older adolescents, which means they may become involved in activities for which they are ill-prepared, such as drinking and sex 8.2 1. calcium 2. bulimia nervosa 3. internalizing the “thin ideal” and being overly concerned about one’s body 4. mental health 5. promote more rapid recovery from injury 6. accidents 7. rewards associated with their 8.3 1.  content knowledge 2. gradual cognitive change 3. analytically, using mathematics or logic 4. ignore or dismiss the evidence 8.4 1. postconven- tional 2. reward or punishment 3. more advanced moral reasoning is associated with moral action 4. caring for others 5. loyalty to their group 6. discuss moral issues

Key Terms puberty (264) primary sex characteristics (266) secondary sex characteristics (266) menarche (266) spermarche (266) body mass index (BMi) (271)

anorexia nervosa (272) bulimia nervosa (272) preconventional level (280) obedience orientation (280) instrumental orientation (280) conventional level (281)

interpersonal norms (281) social system morality (281) postconventional level (281) social contract (281) universal ethical principles (281)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G eo

rg ije

vi c/

E+ /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

289

You probably have vivid memories of your teenage years. Remember the exhilarating moments—high school graduation, your first pay-

check from a part-time job, and your first feelings of

love and sexuality? Of course, there were also pain-

ful times—not being able to do anything right on

your first day on the job, not knowing what to say

on a date with a person you desperately wanted to

impress, and arguing repeatedly with your parents.

Feelings of pride and accomplishment accompanied

by feelings of embarrassment and bewilderment are

common to individuals who are on the threshold of

adulthood.

Adolescence represents the transition from child-

hood to adulthood and is a time when individu-

als grapple with their identity; many have their first

experiences with love and sex, and some enter the world of work. In the first

three sections of this chapter, we investigate these challenging developmental

issues. Then we look at the special obstacles that sometimes make adolescence

difficult to handle.

Moving into the Adult Social World Socioemotional Development in Adolescence

9.1 Identity and Self-Esteem 290 The Search for Identity 290 Ethnic Identity 293 Self-Esteem in Adolescence 294 The Myth of Storm and Stress 296

9.2 Romantic Relationships and Sexuality 297 Romantic Relationships 297 Sexual Behavior 298 Sexual Minority Youth 300 Dating Violence 300

9.3 The World of Work 302 Career Development 302 REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT

“The Life of Lynne”: A Drama in Three Acts 303 Part-Time Employment 305

9.4 The Dark Side 307 Drug Use 307 Depression 308 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Does Racial Discrimination

Lead to Depression? 309 Delinquency 310 WHAT DO YOU THINK? When Juveniles Commit

Serious Crimes, Should They Be Tried as Adults? 313

    Summary 313

    Key Terms 315

9

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

290 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Dea was born in Seoul of Korean parents but was adopted by a Dutch couple in Michigan when she was 3 months old. Growing up, she considered herself a red-blooded American. In college, however, Dea realized that others saw her as an Asian American, an identity about which she had never given much thought. She began to wonder, “Who am I really? American? Dutch American? Asian American?”

Like Dea, do you sometimes wonder who you are? Self-concept refers to the attitudes, behav- iors, and values that make a person unique. In adolescence, self-concept takes on special sig- nificance as individuals struggle to achieve an identity that will allow them to participate in the adult world. Youth search for an identity to integrate the many different and sometimes conflicting elements of the self. In this section, we’ll learn more about the adolescent search for an identity. Along the way, we’ll learn more about Dea’s struggle to figure out who she is.

The Search for Identity Erik Erikson’s (1968) account of identity formation has been particularly influential in our understanding of adolescence. Erikson argued that adolescents face a crisis between iden- tity and role confusion. This crisis involves balancing the desire to try out many possible selves and the need to select a single self. Adolescents who achieve a sense of identity are well prepared to face the next developmental challenge: establishing intimate, shar- ing relationships with others. However, Erikson believed that teenagers who are confused about their identity do not experience intimacy in human relationships but remain iso- lated and respond to others stereotypically.

How do adolescents achieve an identity? They use the hypothetical reasoning skills of the formal-operational stage to experiment with different selves to learn more about pos- sible identities. Adolescents’ advanced cognitive skills enable them to imagine themselves in different roles.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do adolescents achieve an identity?

●● What are the stages and results of acquiring an ethnic identity?

●● How does self-esteem change in adolescence?

9.1 Identity and Self-Esteem

As part of their search for an identity, adolescents often try on different roles (e.g., imagining what life might be like as a rock star). Ju

ic e

Im ag

es /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 291

Much of the testing and experimentation is career-oriented. Some adolescents may envision themselves as rock stars; others may imagine being professional athletes, Peace Corps workers, or best-selling novelists. Other testing is romantically oriented. Teens may fall in love and imagine living with the loved one. Still other exploration involves religious and political beliefs (Lopez, Huynh, & Fuligni, 2011). Teens give different identities a trial run just as you might test-drive different cars before buying one. By fantasizing about the future, adolescents begin to discover who they will be.

As adolescents strive to achieve an identity, they often progress through different phases or statuses as shown in ◗ Table 9.1. Unlike Piaget’s stages, these four phases do not necessarily occur in sequence. Most young adolescents are in a state of diffusion or fore- closure. The common element in these phases is that teens are not exploring alternative identities. They are avoiding the crisis altogether or have resolved it by taking on an iden- tity suggested by parents or other adults. As individuals move beyond adolescence into young adulthood and have more opportunity to explore alternative identities, diffusion and foreclosure become less common and achievement and moratorium become more common (Meeus et al., 2010).

However, identity is not set in stone in adolescence. Periodically, adolescents and adults evaluate different features of their identity. Sometimes these evaluations reassure people of their chosen identity: When a college student who has long wanted to be an ele- mentary-school teacher enjoys tutoring a first grader, this reaffirms the student’s choice to be a teacher. But sometimes evaluations cause people to reconsider their identity: When a teenager who has long identified as a member of a religious group discovers that her values are not shared by most of the other members of the group, she may question this aspect of her identity. Reevaluations like these cause adolescents and young adults to shift between moratorium and achievement (Crocetti, 2017).

During the search for identity, adolescents reveal a number of characteristic ways of thinking. They are often very self-oriented. The self-absorption that marks the teenage search for identity is referred to as adolescent egocentrism (Elkind, 1978). Unlike pre- schoolers, adolescents know that others have different perspectives on the world. Adoles- cents are simply much more interested in their own feelings and experiences than in

adolescent egocentrism Self-absorption that is characteristic of teenagers as they search for identity.

Table 9.1

Four Identity Statuses StatuS Definition example Diffusion The individual is overwhelmed by the task of achieving

an identity and does little to accomplish the task. Larry hates the idea of deciding what to do with his future, so he spends most of his free time playing video games.

Foreclosure The individual has a status determined by adults rather than by personal exploration.

For as long as she can remember, Sakura’s parents have told her that she should be an attorney and join the family law firm. She plans to study prelaw in college, although she’s never given the matter much thought.

Moratorium The individual is examining different alternatives but has yet to find one that’s satisfactory.

Brad enjoys most of his high school classes. Some days he thinks it would be fun to be a chemist, some days he wants to be a novelist, and some days he’d like to be an elementary school teacher. He thinks it’s a little weird to change his mind so often, but he also enjoys thinking about different jobs.

Achievement The individual has explored alternatives and has deliberately chosen a specific identity.

Throughout middle school, Efrat wanted to play in the WNBA. During 9th and 10th grades, she thought it would be cool to be a physician. In 11th grade, she took a computing course and everything finally “clicked”—she’d found her niche. She knew that she wanted to study computer science in college.

Marcia (1980).

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

292 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

anyone else’s experiences. In addition, as they search for an identity, many adolescents wrongly believe that they are the focus of others’ thinking. A teen who spills food on herself may imagine that all her friends are thinking only about the stain on her blouse and how sloppy she is. Many adolescents believe that they are, in effect, actors whose performance is watched constantly by their peers, a phenome- non known as the imaginary audience.

Adolescent self-absorption is also demonstrated by the personal fable, teenagers’ belief that their experiences and feelings are unique and that no one has ever felt or thought as they do. Whether it is the excitement of first love, the despair of a broken relationship, or the confusion of planning for the future, adolescents often believe they are the first to experience these feelings and that no one else could possibly understand the power of their emotions (Elkind & Bowen, 1979). Adolescents’ belief in their uniqueness also contributes to an illusion of invulnerability: the belief that misfortune happens only to others. They think that they can have sex without becoming preg-

nant or that they can drive recklessly without being in an auto accident. These character- istics of adolescents’ thinking are summarized in ◗ Table 9.2.

As adolescents progress toward achieving an identity, adolescent egocentrism, imaginary audiences, personal fables, and the illusion of invulnerability become less common. What circumstances help adolescents achieve identity? Parents are influ- ential. When parents encourage discussion and recognize children’s autonomy, their children are more likely to reach the achievement status. Apparently these youth feel encouraged to undertake the personal experimentation that leads to identity. In con- trast, when parents set rules with little justification and enforce them without expla- nation, children are more likely to remain in the foreclosure status. These teens are discouraged from experimenting personally; instead, their parents simply tell them what identity to adopt. Overall, adolescents are most likely to establish a well-defined identity in a family atmosphere where parents encourage children to explore alterna- tives on their own but do not pressure or provide explicit direction (Crocetti, 2017; Koepke & Denissen, 2012).

imaginary audience Adolescents’ feeling that their behavior is constantly being watched by their peers.

personal fable Belief of many adolescents that their feel- ings and experiences are unique and have never been experienced by anyone else.

illusion of invulnerability Adolescents’ belief that misfortunes cannot happen to them.

Adolescents often believe that others are constantly watching them, a phenomenon known as the imaginary audience; as a result, they’re often upset or embarrassed when they make obvious mistakes or blun- ders, such as spilling food or drink.

Se an

M ur

ph y/

Th e

Im ag

e Ba

nk /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Table 9.2

Characteristics of Adolescents’ Thinking feature Definition example Adolescent egocentrism Adolescents are overly concerned with their

own thoughts and feelings. When Levi’s grandmother died unexpectedly, Levi was pre- occupied with how the funeral would affect his weekend plans and ignored how upset his mother was by her own mother’s death.

Imaginary audience Adolescents believe that others are watching them constantly.

Tom had to ride his bike to football practice because his dad wouldn’t let him have the car; he was sure that all his car- driving friends would see and make fun of him.

Personal fable Adolescents believe that their experiences and feelings are unique.

When Rosa’s boyfriend decided to date another girl, Rosa cried and cried. She couldn’t believe how sad she was, and she was sure that her mom had never felt this way.

Illusion of invulnerability Adolescents think that misfortune happens only to others.

Kumares and his girlfriend had been having sex for about six months. Although she thought it would be a good idea to use birth control, he thought it was unnecessary: There was no way his girlfriend would get pregnant.

© 2019 Cengage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 293

Beyond parents, peers are also influential. When adolescents have close friends whom they trust, they feel more secure exploring alternatives (Doumen et al., 2012). The broader social context also contributes (Bosma & Kunnen, 2001). Exploration takes time and access to resources; neither may be readily available to adolescents living in poverty (e.g., they can’t explore because they drop out of school to support themselves and their family). Finally, through their personality, adolescents themselves may affect the ease with which they achieve an identity. Individuals who are more open to experience and are more agreeable (friendly, generous, helpful) are more likely to achieve an identity (Crocetti et al., 2008; Klimstra et al., 2013).

Ethnic Identity Roughly one-third of the adolescents and young adults living in the United States are members of ethnic minority groups, including African Americans, Asian Americans, Latino Americans, and Native Americans. These individuals typically develop an ethnic  identity: They feel a part of their ethnic group and learn the special customs and traditions of their group’s culture and heritage (Phinney, 2005).

Achieving an ethnic identity seems to occur in three phases. Initially, adolescents have not examined their ethnic roots. A teenage Vietnamese American girl in this phase remarked, “Why should I learn about the Boat People? I’m not interested in what hap- pened in Vietnam before I was born. Besides, I’m an American.” For this girl, ethnic iden- tity is not yet an important personal issue.

In the second phase, adolescents begin to explore the personal impact of their ethnic heritage. The curiosity and questioning that is characteristic of this stage is captured in the comments of a teenage African American girl who said, “I want to learn more about our history—back in Africa, in slavery, and during the Civil Rights movement. Going to the Black Cultural Center is one way I can find out about myself.” Part of this phase involves learning cultural traditions; for example, many adolescents learn to prepare ethnic food.

In the third phase, individuals achieve a distinct ethnic self-concept. One Latino American adolescent explained his ethnic identification this way: “I was born in LA, but my parents grew up in Mexico and came here when they were teenagers like me. I love hearing them talk about their lives there, and I’m proud that I can speak Spanish with my cousins who live in Mexico. But I’m also proud to be an American and I like to learn about my country’s heritage.”

To see if you understand the differences between these stages of ethnic identity, reread the vignette on page 290 about Dea, the Dutch Asian American college student. Then decide which stage applies to her. Our answer appears on page 296, just before Test Yourself.

Older adolescents are more likely than younger ones to have achieved an ethnic iden- tity because they are more likely to have had opportunities to explore their cultural heri- tage (French et al., 2006). As adolescents explore their ethnic identity, they often change the way they refer to themselves. For example, a U.S. teen whose parents were born in Vietnam might refer to herself at different times as Vietnamese, Viet- namese American, or Asian American, in no particular order (Fuligni et al., 2008).

Do adolescents benefit from a strong ethnic identity? Yes. Adolescents who have achieved an ethnic identity tend to have greater self-esteem and find their interactions with fam- ily and friends more satisfying (Mandara et al., 2009; Rivas- Drake et al., 2014). They’re also happier and worry less (Kiang et al., 2006). In addition, adolescents with a strong ethnic identity are less affected by discrimination—they maintain their self-worth after experiencing racial or ethnic discrimi- nation (Neblett, Rivas-Drake, & Umaña-Taylor, 2012; Tynes et al., 2012).

ethnic identity Feeling that one belongs to a specific ethnic group.

Think About It Although Piaget’s theory of cognitive development was not concerned with identity formation, how might his theory explain why identity is a central issue in adolescence?

Part of the search for an ethnic identity involves learning cultural traditions, such as how to prepare foods associated with one’s ethnic group.

Pu re

st oc

k/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

294 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

However, we need to remember that racial and ethnic groups living in the United States are diverse. African American, Asian American, Hispanic American, and Native American cultures and heritages differ; thus, we should expect that the nature and con- sequences of a strong ethnic self-concept may differ across these and other ethnic groups (Phinney, 2005).

Even within a particular group, the nature and consequences of ethnic identity may change over successive generations. As successive generations become more assimilated into mainstream culture, they may identify less strongly with ethnic culture (Marks, Pat- ton, & García Coll, 2011). When parents maintain strong feelings of ethnic identity that their children don’t share, problems sometimes develop, as immigrant parents cling to the “old ways” but their children embrace the new culture. For example, in one study of Korean American families (Choi et al., 2017), adolescents who identified strongly with the United States were more prone to depression and antisocial behavior. Thus, children’s fail- ure to identify with their parent’s cultural roots can lead to difficulties.

Finally, let’s think about adolescents for whom an ethnic identity is a particu- lar challenge—those whose parents come from different racial or ethnic groups. For these adolescents, identity can be quite fluid (Doyle & Kao, 2007). Some biracial ado- lescents first identify themselves as monoracial and then embrace a biracial identity; others  shift  in the opposite direction, converging on a single racial identity; and still  others shift from one racial identity to another. Collectively, youth with shifting racial identities tend to have lower self-esteem than those with a consistent biracial iden- tity ( Csizmadia, Brunsma, & Cooney, 2012). And U.S. biracial adolescents who identify themselves as white have less self-esteem and are less successful in school (Burke & Kao, 2013; Csizmadia & Ispa, 2014).

Self-Esteem in Adolescence Self-esteem is normally very high in preschool children but declines gradually during the early elementary school years as children compare themselves to others. By the begin- ning of adolescence, self-esteem has usually stabilized—it neither increases nor decreases in these years (Harter, Whitesell, & Kowalski, 1992). Evidently, children learn their place in the “pecking order” of different domains and adjust their self-esteem accordingly. However, self-esteem sometimes drops when children move from elementary school to middle school or junior high (Harris et al., 2017). Apparently when students from differ- ent elementary schools enter the same middle school or junior high, they know where they stand compared with their old elementary school classmates but not compared with students from other elementary schools. Thus, peer comparisons begin anew, and self- esteem often suffers temporarily. As a new school becomes familiar and students gradu- ally adjust to the new pecking order, self-esteem again increases.

These changes in overall level of self-esteem are accompanied by another important change: Self-esteem becomes more differentiated as children enter adolescence (Boivin, Vitaro, & Gagnon, 1992). Youth are able to evaluate themselves in more domains as they develop, and their evaluations in each domain are increasingly independent. That is, children’s ratings of self-esteem are often consistent across different dimensions of self- esteem, but adolescents’ ratings more often vary from one domain to another. For exam- ple, a 9-year-old may have high self-esteem in the academic, social, and physical domains, but a 15-year-old might have high self-esteem in the academic domain, moderate self- esteem in the social domain, and low self-esteem in the physical domain.

As children progress through elementary school and enter junior high or middle school, their academic self-concepts become particularly well defined (Marsh & Martin, 2011). As students accumulate successes and failures in school, they form beliefs about their ability in different content areas (e.g., English, math, science), and these beliefs con- tribute to their overall academic self-concept. A teen who believes she is skilled at English and math but not so skilled in science will probably have a positive academic self-concept overall. But a teen who believes he is untalented in most academic areas will have a nega- tive academic self-concept.

Think About It What factors in the biopsychosocial framework are shown by adolescents who develop an ethnic identity?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 295

During adolescence, the social component of self-esteem becomes particularly well differentiated. Adolescents distinguish self-worth in many different social relationships. A teenager may, for example, feel very positive about her relationships with her parents but believe that she’s a loser in romantic relationships. Another teen may feel loved and valued by his parents but believe that the coworkers at his part-time job can’t stand him (Harter, Waters, & Whitesell, 1998).

Growth of self-worth among U.S. children and adolescents also varies depending on their ethnicity. Compared with European American children, African Americans and Hispanic Americans have lower self-esteem during most of the elementary school years. However, in adolescence, the gap narrows for Hispanic Americans and actually reverses for African American adolescents, who have greater self-esteem than their European American peers (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Herman, 2004; Twenge & Crocker, 2002). One explanation of this change is that as African American and His- panic American teens take pride in belonging to a distinct social and cultural group, their self-worth increases (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Umaña-Taylor, Diversi, & Fine, 2002).

Thus, between the late preschool years and adolescence, self-esteem becomes more complex as older children and adolescents identify distinct domains of self-worth. This growing complexity is not surprising; it reflects the older child’s and adolescent’s greater cognitive skill and the more extensive social world of older children and adolescents.

Influences on Adolescents’ Self-Esteem What factors contribute to adolescents’ self-esteem? Heredity contributes indirectly. Genes help to make some youth smarter, more sociable, more attractive, and more skilled athletically. Consequently, such youth are more likely to have greater self- worth because they are competent in so many domains. In other words, genes lead to greater competence, which fosters greater self-worth (Harter, 2012; Neiss, Sedikides, & Stevenson, 2006).

Children’s and adolescents’ self-worth is also affected by how others view them, par- ticularly other people who are important to them. Parents matter, of course—even to adolescents. Children are more likely to view themselves positively when their parents are affectionate toward them and are involved with them (Behnke et al., 2011; Ojanen & Perry, 2007). Around the world, children have greater self-esteem when families live in harmony and parents nurture their children (Scott, Scott, & McCabe, 1991). A father who routinely hugs his daughter and gladly takes her to piano lessons is saying to her, “You are important to me.” When children hear this regularly from parents, they evi- dently internalize the message and come to see themselves positively. That said, when adults use inflated praise—for example, saying to their children “You played that song amazingly well!” when the playing was mediocre—children with low self-esteem tend to shy away from challenges because they’re afraid they won’t succeed (Brummelman et al., 2014).

Parents’ discipline also is related to self-esteem. Children with high self-esteem gen- erally have parents who have reasonable expectations for their children and are willing to discuss rules and discipline with their children (Awong, Grusec, & Sorenson, 2008; Laible & Carlo, 2004). Parents who fail to set rules are implicitly telling their children they don’t care—they don’t value their children enough to go to the trouble of creating rules and enforcing them. In much the same way, parents who refuse to discuss discipline with their children are saying, “Your opinions don’t matter to me.” Not surprisingly, when chil- dren internalize these messages, the result is lower overall self-worth.

Peers’ views are important, too. Children’s and particularly adolescents’ self-worth is greater when they believe that their peers think highly of them (Harter, 2012). Lauren’s self-worth increases, for example, when she hears that Pedro, Matt, and Michael think that she’s the smartest girl in the eighth grade. Conversely, self-esteem drops when peers provide negative feedback, especially when those peers are popular themselves (Thomas et al., 2010).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

296 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

The Myth of Storm and Stress According to novelists and filmmakers, the search for identity that we just described is inherently a struggle, a time of storm and stress for adolescents. Although this view may make for best-selling novels and hit movies, in reality, the rebellious teen is vastly over- stated. Adolescents generally enjoy happy and satisfying relationships with their parents (Steinberg, 2001). Most teens love their parents and feel loved by them. And they embrace many of their parents’ values and look to them for advice.

Evidence from around the world documents that adolescence is not necessarily a time of turmoil and conflict. Teens in the U.S. Midwest reported that their parents support them—they listen, give good advice, and help make good decisions (Rueger et al., 2014). Adolescents in Turkey and Belgium rated their mothers as being very supportive, endorsing items such as “My mother supports me in dealing with prob- lems” and “My mother talks to me in a comforting way” (Güngör & Bornstein, 2010). Finally, Palestinian teens who were refugees in Jordan described their families as being very close (Ahmad, Smetana, & Klimstra, 2015). These findings undercut the myth of adolescence as necessarily being a time when adolescent storms rain on parent–child relationships.

Of course, parent–child relations do change during adolescence. As teens become more independent, their relationships with their parents become more egalitarian. Parents must adjust to their children’s growing sense of autonomy by treating them more like equals (Laursen & Collins, 1994). This growing independence means that teens spend less time with their parents; are less affectionate toward them; and argue more often with them about matters of style, taste, and freedom (Shanahan et al., 2007; Stanik, Riina, & McHale, 2013). Although adolescents do have more disagree- ments with parents, these disputes are usually relatively mild—bickering, not all- out shouting matches—and usually concern an adolescent’s personal choices (e.g., hairstyle, clothing), autonomy, and responsibilities (Chen-Gaddini, 2012; Ehrlich, Dykas,  & Cassidy, 2012). These changes are natural by-products of an evolving parent–child relationship in which the “child” is nearly a fully independent young adult ( Steinberg & Silk, 2002).

Before you think that this portrait of parent–child relationships in adolescence is too good to be true, we want to add two cautionary notes. First, conflicts between parents and their adolescent children are often very distressing for parents, who may read far more into these conflicts than their teenagers do (Steinberg, 2001). Parents sometimes fear that arguments over attire or household chores may reflect more fundamental disagreements about values: A mother may interpret her son’s refusal to clean his room as a rejection of values concerning the need for order and cleanliness, when the son simply doesn’t want to waste time cleaning a room that he knows will soon become a mess again. Second, for a minority of families (roughly 25%), parent–child conflicts in adolescence are more serious and are associated with behavior problems in adolescents (Ehrlich et al., 2012). These more harmful conflicts are more common among adolescents who don’t regulate their emotions well (Eisenberg et al., 2008), and they often predate adolescence—as chil- dren, these adolescents were prone to conflict with their parents (Steeger & Gondoli, 2013; Steinberg, 2001).

Response to question on page 293 about Dea’s ethnic identity. Dea, the Dutch Asian American college student, doesn’t know how to integrate the Korean heritage of her biological parents with the Dutch American culture in which she was reared. This would put her in the second phase of acquiring an ethnic identity. On the one hand, she is examin- ing her ethnic roots, which means she’s progressed beyond the initial stages. On the other hand, she has not yet integrated her Asian and European roots and so has not reached the third and final phase.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 297

For six months, 15-year-old Gretchen has been dating Jeff, a 17-year-old. She thinks she is truly in love for the first time, and she often imagines being married to Jeff. They have had sex a few times, each time without contraception. It sometimes crosses Gretchen’s mind that if she gets pregnant, she could move into her own apartment and begin a family.

The fires of romantic relationships have long warmed the hearts of American adolescents. Often, as with Jeff and Gretchen, romance leads to sex. In this section, we’ll explore ado- lescent dating and sexual behavior. As we do, you’ll better understand Gretchen’s reasons for having unprotected sex with Jeff.

Romantic Relationships The social landscape adds a distinctive landmark in adolescence—romantic relationships. These are uncommon during elementary school, but by high school, roughly two-thirds of U.S. adolescents have had a romantic relationship within the previous one-and-a-half years and most have been involved in a romance lasting nearly a year (Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003). However, cultural factors influence the timing of romantic relationships for teenagers in America. Traditional Latino American and Asian American parents empha- size family ties and loyalty to parents. Because romantic relationships are a sign of inde- pendence and usually result in less time spent with family, it’s not surprising that Latino American and Asian American adolescents often begin to date at an older age and date less frequently (Collins, Welsh, & Furman, 2009).

Recall 1. According to Erikson, adolescents face a crisis between

identity and _______________.

2. The _______________ status would describe an adolescent who has attained an identity based almost entirely on her parents’ advice and urging.

3. A person who has simply put off searching for an identity because it seems too confusing and too overwhelming is in the _______________ status.

4. _______________ refers to the fact that adolescents some- times believe their lives are a performance with their peers watching them constantly.

5. Adolescents are most likely to achieve an identity when par- ents encourage them _______________.

6. In the second phase of achieving an ethnic identity, adoles- cents _______________.

7. Self-esteem often drops when students enter middle school or junior high school because young adolescents _______________.

Interpret ●● How do parent–child relationships change in adolescence?

Do these changes indicate a period of storm and stress?

apply ●● The Tran family has just immigrated to the United States from

Vietnam. The mother and father want their two children to grow up appreciating their Vietnamese heritage but worry that a strong ethnic identity may not be good for their kids. What advice would you give Mr. and Mrs. Tran about the impact of ethnic identity on children’s development?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 9.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Why do teenagers date?

●● Why are some adolescents sexually active? Why do so few use contraceptives?

●● Who are sexual-minority youth?

●● What circumstances make date violence especially likely?

9.2 Romantic Relationships and Sexuality

Think About It According to the “storm and stress” view of adolescence, sexual behavior is one way for adolescents to rebel against their parents. Does research on adolescent sexuality support this prediction?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

298 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Romantic relationships build on friendships. Like friends, roman- tic partners tend to be similar in popularity and physical attractiveness. And a best friendship serves as both a prototype for and a source of support during ups and downs of close relationships (Collins et al., 2009). What’s more, romantic relationships change over time in ways that resemble changes in friendship: For younger adolescents, romantic relationships offer companionship (like that provided by a best friend) and an outlet for sexual exploration. For older adolescents like those in the photo, intimacy, trust, and support become important features of romantic relationships (Shulman & Kipnis, 2001). Finally, like friend- ships, when children have high-quality parenting, they more readily invest in romantic relationships as adults (Oriña et al., 2011).

It’s tempting to dismiss teen romances as nothing more than puppy love, but they are often developmentally significant (Collins et al.,

2009). On the one hand, adolescents involved in a romantic relationship are often more self-confident and have greater self-esteem. And high-quality adolescent romances are associated with positive relationships during adulthood. On the other hand, adolescents in romantic relationships report more emotional upheaval and conflict. In addition, early dating with many different partners is associated with a host of problems in adolescence (e.g., drug use, lower grades) and is associated with less satisfying romantic relationships in adulthood (Furman & Rose, 2015).

Sexual Behavior We’ve already seen that sexual exploration is an important feature of romantic rela- tionships for younger adolescents. In fact, by the end of high school, about two-thirds of American adolescents will have had intercourse at least once (Eaton et al., 2008). No single factor predicts adolescent sexual behavior. Instead, adolescents are more likely to be sexually active when they acquire (from parents, peers, and media) permissive atti- tudes toward sex, when their parents don’t monitor their behavior, when they are more physically mature, and when they drink alcohol regularly (Belsky et al., 2010; Collins et al., 2011; Hipwell et al., 2010; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008). What’s more, adolescents exposed to harsh environments (e.g., living in poverty) start having sex at younger ages, but adolescents with better executive functioning start at older ages (Carlson, Mendle, & Harden, 2014; Khurana et al., 2012).

Although a majority of boys and girls have sex at some point during adolescence, sex- ual activity has very different meanings for boys and girls (Brooks-Gunn & Paikoff, 1993). Girls tend to describe their first sexual partner as “someone they love,” but boys describe their first partner as a “casual date.” Girls report stronger feelings of love for their first sexual partner than for a later partner, but boys don’t. Girls have mixed feelings after their first sexual experience—fear and guilt mixed with happiness and excitement—whereas boys’ feelings are more uniformly positive. Finally, when describing their sexual experi- ences to peers, girls’ peers typically express some disapproval but boys’ peers typically do not. In short, for boys, sexual behavior is viewed as recreational and self-oriented; for girls, sexual behavior is viewed as romantic and is interpreted through their capacity to form intimate interpersonal relationships (Steinberg, 1999).

Sexually Transmitted Diseases Adolescent sexual activity is cause for concern because a number of diseases are trans- mitted from one person to another through sexual intercourse. ◗ Table 9.3 lists several of the most common types of sexually transmitted infections (STIs). Most STIs are readily cured with antibiotics. In contrast, the prognosis is bleak for individuals who contract the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), which typically leads to acquired immunode- ficiency syndrome (AIDS). In persons with AIDS, the immune system is no longer able to protect the body from infections, and they often die from one of these infections.

Adolescent romantic relationships build on friendships and offer companionship as well as an outlet for sexual exploration.

A kh

en at

on Im

ag es

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 299

Young adults—those in their 20s—account for roughly one-third of all new cases of AIDS in the United States and many contracted the disease during adolescence (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016). Many factors make adolescents especially sus- ceptible to AIDS. Teenagers and young adults are more likely than older adults to engage in unprotected sex and to use intravenous drugs, which are common pathways for the transmission of AIDS.

Teenage Pregnancy and Contraception Adolescents’ sexual behavior is also troubling because among American adolescent girls who have had intercourse, approximately one in eight becomes pregnant. The result is that nearly a quarter million babies are born to American teenagers annually. African American and Latino American adolescents are most likely to become pregnant (Kost & Maddow-Zimet, 2016).

Teen pregnancy is common because many sexually active teens do not use birth con- trol consistently or correctly (Guttmacher Institute, 2013). Some adolescents see no need because they believe they are invulnerable—that only others become pregnant. Others don’t know how to use contraception or where to obtain it (Ralph & Brindis, 2010). And some adolescent girls, like Gretchen from the opening vignette, avoid contraception because they see becoming pregnant as a way to break away from parents and gain status as an independent-living adult (Phipps et al., 2008).

Table 9.3

Features of Sexually Transmitted Infections DiSeaSe u.S. frequency SymptomS complicationS Caused by Bacteria

Chlamydia 3.3% of adolescent females and 0.7% of adolescent males

75% of women and 50% of men have no symptoms; sometimes abnormal discharge of pus from the vagina or penis or pain while urinating

Infections of the cervix and Fallopian tubes that can lead to infertility; rare in men

Gonorrhea 0.6% of adolescent females and 0.3% of adolescent males

Often no symptoms at all; pus discharged from the penis or vagina, pain associated with urination; for women, pain during intercourse; for men, swollen testicles

Pelvic inflammatory disease, a serious infection of the female reproductive tract that can lead to infertility; in men, epididymitis, an infection of the testicles that can lead to infertility

Syphilis About 4,000 cases annually among 15- to 24-year-olds

A sore, called a chancre, at the site of the infection—usually the penis, vulva, or vagina

Left untreated, can damage internal organs such as the brain, nerves, eyes, heart, bones, and joints

Caused by Virus

Genital herpes At least 45 million of age 12 and older (roughly 1 in 5 adolescents and adults)

Itching, burning, or pain in the genital or anal area; sores on the mouth, penis, or vagina

Recurrent sores; pregnant women can pass the virus (which can be fatal to the newborn) to the baby during birth

Genital human papilloma virus (HPV)

20 million Usually no symptoms; sometimes genital warts or discharge from the penis or vagina

Usually goes away; in rare cases leads to cervical cancer

Hepatitis B About 75,000 annually Jaundice, fatigue, loss of appetite, abdominal pain

Death from chronic liver disease

HIV About 40,000 diagnosed annually

Initially a flulike illness; later, enlarged lymph nodes, lack of energy, weight loss, frequent fevers

Loss of immune cells (AIDS), cancer, death

SouRCe: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2007, 2010.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

300 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

How can we reduce adolescent sexual behavior and teen pregnancy? Parents mat- ter: when they discuss sex with their teenage children, those children are more likely to use contraception (Widman et al., 2016). However, more effective are comprehensive sex education programs that teach the biological aspects of sex and emphasize sexual behavior that is responsible and safe. Teens who participate in such programs know more about the risks of unprotected sex and better understand contraception as well as have safer sexual practices, such as using contraceptives and limiting sexual partners (Denford et al., 2017).

Sexual Minority Youth For most adolescents, dating and romance involve members of the opposite sex. However, in early and mid-adolescence, some identify as gay, lesbian, bisexual, queer, intersex, or transgender; collectively, these adolescents are often described as sexual- minority youth (Martin-Storey, 2016). The paths that lead to youth to identify with a sexual minority are poorly understood. For boys who identify as gay, one idea is that genes and hormones may lead some boys to feel “different” during early adolescence; these feelings lead to an interest in gender-atypical activities and, later, attraction to other males (Diamond, 2007). For females, the path to a sexual-minority identity is more variable. During adolescence, some girls are attracted to girls and consistently identify as lesbians thereafter. For some women, attraction to other females does not emerge until later in life, often growing out of deep feelings for a particular woman. And still other women have repeated transitions in their sexual orientation and identity (Farr, Diamond, & Boker, 2014).

Scientists know even less about transgender youth—those whose gender identity does not match their biological sex. This identity often emerges early. For example, some preschool boys insist that they are girls and resist parental efforts to become more masculine but are happy when allowed to wear their hair long and wear girl’s clothing. These children were once thought to be confused about their gender identity and they received treatment designed to align their gender identity with their biologi- cal sex. An alternative, more recent view is a transgender identity is simply an uncom- mon but normal gender identity, one that’s most likely in children who insist they are members of the other sex, not just that they wish they were members of the other sex (Olson, 2016).

Although the origins of gender identity in sexual-minority youth are not yet well understood, it is clear that they face many challenges. Their behavior and appearance often differ from what’s considered appropriate for their gender, leading them to be harassed and attacked by others; in turn, this may lead sexual-minority youth to experience mental

health problems, including depression and anxiety (Martin-Storey, 2016). However, in recent years, more (and more visible) role models and more centers for sexual- minority youth are making it easier for these youth to cope; most end up as well- adjusted as heterosexual youth (Olson et al., 2016; Saewyc, 2011).

Dating Violence As adolescents begin to explore romantic relationships and sex, many teens experi- ence violence in dating, which can include physical violence (e.g., being hit or kicked), emotional violence (e.g., experiencing threats or bullying designed to harm self- worth), sexual violence (being forced to engage in sexual activity against one’s will), or stalking (being harassed or threatened in a way designed to frighten). Roughly 20% of adolescent girls and 10% of adolescent boys report these experiences, and, as you can imagine, these youth often don’t do well in school and suffer from mental health and behavioral problems (Centers for Disease Control, 2016).

Think About It Suppose you had to convince a group of 15-year-olds about the hazards of adolescent sex and teenage pregnancy. What would you say?

About 5% of adolescents identify themselves as gay or lesbian.

G re

g Ce

o/ Th

e Im

ag e

Ba nk

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 301

Several factors make teenage boys and girls more likely to perpetrate dating violence. Parents contribute: Teens are more likely to be violent during dating when they’re exposed to violence at home—either through maltreatment from their parents or from observing violence between their parents (Calvete et al., 2017). But dating violence is less likely when teens have caring parents who communicate well with them (Kast, Eisenberg, & Sieving, 2016). Peers matter as well: Dating violence is more common when teens believe that their peers condone dating violence, when they know peers who are in abusive relationships, and when their school’s climate condones dating violence (Garthe, Sullivan, & McDaniel, 2017; Giordano et al., 2015). Finally, teens are more likely to commit dating violence when they’re antisocial, aggressive, not successful in school, and use drugs (Foshee & Reyes, 2012; Johnson et al., 2017).

Because dating violence leads to many harmful consequences for victims, including depression, antisocial behavior, and substance abuse, scientists have devised programs to prevent dating violence. For example, in a program called “Families for Safe Dates,” teens and parents completed activities designed to reduce the risk of dating abuse (e.g., how to recognize data abuse). For example, one booklet used dating scenarios to teach teens and caregivers how to recognize dating abuse. Compared with teens in the control condition, teens in the treatment condition were much less accepting of dating abuse and they expe- rienced less physical abuse (Foshee et al., 2012, 2015).

Most colleges and universities offer workshops on dating abuse. The first genera- tion of programs focused on individuals, aiming to change attitudes and behaviors that put individuals at risk. However, newer programs take a community-based approach, one that emphasizes that all members of a community (e.g., a college campus) have a stake in preventing dating violence. Accordingly, bystanders learn to recognize situa- tions conducive to sexual violence and learn strategies for intervening effectively and safely. For example, One Act is a program developed at the University of North Car- olina–Chapel Hill that includes four hours of instruction—typically given to student organizations (e.g., fraternities)—in which students use role play to learn to recognize warning signs, to learn effective strategies (e.g., create a distraction), and to learn how to follow up to be sure the situation is resolved. Programs like these make students less tolerant of dating violence as well as more willing and better able to intervene (Alegría- Flores et al., 2017).

Recall 1. For younger adolescents, romantic relationships offer com-

panionship and _______________.

2. Adolescents are more likely to have sex when they acquire permissive attitudes toward sex and when they’re exposed to _______________ environments.

3. Adolescents and young adults are at particular risk for con- tracting AIDS when they_______________ and use intravenous drugs.

4. Adolescents often fail to use contraception because they believe they are invulnerable, don’t know how to use contra- ception or where to obtain it, and _______________.

5. For some boys, the first step toward a gay sexual orientation occurs in early development when they _______________.

6. Teens are more likely to perpetrate dating violence when their parents’ discipline is harsh, when they believe that their peers _______________ and when they are antisocial, aggres- sive, not successful in school, and use drugs.

Interpret ●● Some sexually active teenagers do not use contraceptives.

How do the reasons for this failure show connections between cognitive, social, and emotional development?

apply ●● Prepare a brief fact sheet for incoming college freshmen that

summarizes the factors that put women at risk for dating violence.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 9.2

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

302 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

When 15-year-old Aaron announced that he wanted an after-school job at the local super- market, his mother was delighted, believing that he would learn much from the experience. Five months later, she has her doubts. Aaron has lost interest in school, and they argue constantly about how he spends his money.

“What do you want to be when you grow up?” Children are often asked this question in fun. However, it takes on special significance in adolescence because work is such an important element of the adult life that looms on the horizon. A job—be it as a bricklayer, reporter, or child care worker—helps define who we are. In this section, we’ll see how adolescents begin to think about possible occupations. We’ll also look at adolescents’ first exposure to the world of work, which usually occurs in the form of part-time jobs after school or on weekends. As we do, we’ll see whether Aaron’s changed behavior is typical of teens who work part-time.

Career Development In most developed nations, adolescence is a time when youth face the challenge of select- ing a career. According to a theory proposed by Donald Super (1976, 1980), identity is a primary force in an adolescent’s choice of a career. At about age 13 or 14, adolescents use their emerging identity as a source of ideas about careers, a process called crystallization. Teenagers use their ideas about their own talents and interests to limit potential career prospects. A teenager who is extroverted and sociable may decide that working with peo- ple is the career for him. Decisions are provisional, and adolescents experiment with hypothetical careers, trying to envision what each is like. Discussions with parents help adolescents refine their emerging ideas; schools also help by providing job fairs and assess- ing students’ job-related interests (Diemer, 2007).

crystallization First phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which adolescents use their emerging identities to form ideas about careers.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do adolescents select an occupation? ●● What is the impact of part-time employment on adolescents?

9.3 The World of Work

In the specification stage of career development, adolescents try to learn more about different careers, sometimes by serving an apprenticeship.

M ar

k Ri

ch ar

d/ Ph

ot oE

di t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 303

At about age 18, adolescents extend the activities associated with crystallization and enter a new phase. During specification, individuals further limit their career possibilities by learning more about specific lines of work and starting to obtain the training required for a specific job. The extroverted teenager who wants to work with people may decide that a career in sales is a good match for his abilities and interests. The teen who likes math may have learned more about careers and decided that she’d like to be an accountant. Some teens may begin an apprenticeship as a way to learn a trade.

The end of the teenage years or the early 20s marks the beginning of the third phase. During implementation, individuals enter the workforce and learn firsthand about jobs. This is a time of learning about responsibility and productivity, of learning to get along with coworkers, and of altering lifestyle to accommodate work. This period is often unsta- ble; individuals may change jobs frequently as they adjust to the reality of life in the workplace.

In the Real People feature, you can see these three phases in one young woman’s career development.

specification Second phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which adolescents learn more about specific lines of work and begin training.

implementation Third phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which individuals enter the workforce.

“The Life of Lynne”: A Drama in Three Acts

Act 1: Crystallization. Throughout high school, Lynne was active in a number of organizations. She often served as the trea- surer and found it satisfying to keep the finan- cial records in order. By the end of her junior year, Lynne decided that she wanted to study business in college, a decision that fit with her good grades in English and math.

Act 2: Specification. Lynne was accepted into the business school of a large state uni- versity. She decided that accounting fit her

skills and temperament, so this became her major. During the summers, she worked as a cashier at Target. This helped pay for college and gave her experience in the world of retail sales.

Act 3: Implementation. A few months after graduation, Lynne was offered a junior accounting position with Walmart. Her job required that she work Tuesday through Fri- day, auditing Walmart stores in several nearby cities. Lynne liked the pay, the company car,

the pay, the feeling of independence, and the pay. However, having to hit the road every morning by 7:30 was a jolt to someone used to rising at 10. Also, Lynne often found it awk- ward to deal with store managers, many of whom were twice her age and often intimidat- ing. She was coming to the conclusion that there was more to a successful career as an accountant than simply having the numbers add up correctly.

Real People Applying Human Development

“The Life of Lynne” illustrates the progressive refinement that takes place in a person’s career development. An initial interest in math and finance led to a degree in business, which led to a job as an accountant. However, one other aspect of Lynne’s life sheds more light on Super’s theory. After 18 months on the job, Lynne’s accounting group was merged with another. The merger would have required Lynne to move to another state; she quit instead. After six months looking for another accounting job, Lynne gave up and began studying to become a real estate agent. The moral? Economic conditions and opportuni- ties also shape career development. Changing times can force individuals to take new, often unexpected career paths.

Personality-Type Theory Super’s (1976, 1980) work helps to explain how self-concept and career aspirations develop hand in hand, but his theory does not explain why particular individuals are attracted to one line of work rather than another. Explaining the match between people and occupa- tions has been the aim of a theory devised by John Holland (1985, 1987, 1996). According to Holland’s personality-type theory, people find work fulfilling when the important  features of a job or profession fit their personality. Holland identified six prototypic personalities that are relevant to the world of work. Each one is best suited to a specific set of occupa- tions, as indicated in the right-hand column of ◗ Table 9.4. Remember, these are merely prototypes. Most people do not match any one personality type exactly. Instead, their work-related personalities are a blend of the six.

personality-type theory View proposed by Holland that people find their work fulfilling when the important features of a job or profession fit their personality.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

304 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

When people have jobs that match their personality type, they are more productive employees in the short run and they have more stable career paths in the long run (Hol- land, 1996; Nye et al., 2012). For example, an enterprising youth is likely to be successful in business because he enjoys positions of power in which he can use his verbal skills. This model is useful in describing the career preferences of African, Asian, European, Native, and Latino American adolescents (Gupta, Tracey, & Gore, 2008).

Social Cognitive Career Theory Another approach to career choice combines elements of Super’s focus on develop- ment and Holland’s focus on the match between interests and careers. According to social cognitive career theory (Brown & Lent, 2016; Lent, 2005) progress toward a vocation rests on self-efficacy, which refers to youths’ beliefs about their ability to suc- ceed in specific domains and their outcome expectations, which refers to youths’ beliefs about the likely outcome of their behavior. These beliefs are based on youths’ history of success and failures. As children and adolescents experience success and failure on different activities and tasks, they develop beliefs about themselves and beliefs about usual outcomes. These beliefs lead to interests—youth enjoy activities in which they’re skilled and likely to succeed. And interests lead to goals—youth decide that they should pursue activities in which they’re interested. Pursuing these activities often leads to

additional success, strengthening youths’ beliefs about themselves and the outcomes they’re likely to experience.

We’ve summarized these connections in ◗Figure 9.1 and you can see them in an example. Imagine a fourth grader who is often successful on math tests. Over time, she comes to believe that she’s good at math and that she’s usually successful on math-related activities. These beliefs forge an interest in math and promote math-related goals (e.g., joining a math club), and participating in these activities would typically reinforce her beliefs about her strengths in math. As she starts to think about work, she naturally thinks about occupations that would allow her to pursue her math- related interests and goals. Similarly, success in art, sports, or litera- ture could forge beliefs, interests, and goals in those domains. Thus, social cognitive career theory helps to explain why adolescents

Table 9.4

Personality Types in Holland’s Theory perSonality type DeScription careerS Realistic Individuals enjoy doing physical labor and working with

their hands; they like to solve concrete problems. Mechanic, truck driver, construction worker

Investigative Individuals are task-oriented and enjoy thinking about abstract relations.

Scientist, technical writer

Social Individuals are skilled verbally and interpersonally; they enjoy solving problems using these skills.

Teacher, counselor, social worker

Conventional Individuals have verbal and quantitative skills that they like to apply to structured, well-defined tasks assigned to them by others.

Bank teller, payroll clerk, traffic manager

Enterprising Individuals enjoy using their verbal skills in positions of power, status, and leadership.

Business executive, television producer, real estate agent

Artistic Individuals enjoy expressing themselves through unstructured tasks.

Poet, musician, actor

© 2019 Cengage

According to Holland’s personality-type theory, people are satisfied with a job when it matches their personality; for example, adolescents with an enterprising personality type enjoy working in business because this allows them to use verbal skills in positions of leadership.

D m

itr y

Ka lin

ov sk

y/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 305

develop interests in some domains but not in others and it explains how, over time, expe- riences, beliefs, and interests gradually lead adolescents to target occupations that are well suited to them.

Of course, whether a person pursues a specific job (and is successful in it) depends on more than the match between interests, skills, and the important features of a job. Even when people are potentially well suited for a job, they may not be able to obtain the educa- tion or experience that’s necessary (e.g., the fourth grader who’s a math star may be unable to afford to go to college). Or they may get a job for which they’re well suited but not enjoy it because of stress in the workplace or frequent conflicts between work and family obligations (Hammer et al., 2005). We’ll discuss these issues in more detail in Chapter 12. Nevertheless, the theories we’ve described here describe the first steps in moving toward a vocation.

Part-Time Employment About 20% of American high-school students hold part-time jobs, typically in retail (Child Trends, 2016). Most adults praise teens for working, believing that early exposure to the workplace teaches adolescents self-discipline, self-confidence, and important job skills (Stevens, 2016). For most adolescents, however, the reality is quite different. Part-time work can actually be harmful, for several reasons.

1. School performance suffers. When students work more than approximately 15 to 20 hours per week, they become less engaged in school and are less likely to be successful in college, particularly if they are European Americans (Bachman et al., 2013; Hwang & Domina, 2017). Many high school students apparently do not have the foresight and discipline necessary to consis- tently meet the combined demands of work and school.

2. Mental health and behavioral problems. Adolescents who work long hours—more than 15 or 20 hours a week—are more likely to experi- ence anxiety and depression; in addition, their self-esteem and quality of life often suffer. Many adolescents find themselves in jobs that are repetitive and boring but stressful, and such con- ditions undermine self-esteem and breed anxiety. Extensive part-time work frequently leads to sub- stance abuse and frequent problem behavior (e.g., antisocial behavior, including theft), especially for younger teens who attend school sporadi- cally (Graves et al., 20117; Monahan, Steinberg, & Cauffman, 2013).

●◗ Figure 9.1 In social cognitive career theory, experiences lead to beliefs, which are linked to interests and goals.

Self-ef f icacy beliefs

Experiences with success and failure

Beliefs about outcomes

Interests and goals

Based on Lent (2005), Figure 7.2.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Think About It How do the different personality types in Holland’s theory relate to the different types of intelligence proposed by How- ard Gardner, described in Chapter 6?

Many American adolescents hold part-time jobs; these can be beneficial but not when adolescents work more than 15 to 20 hours a week.

Ro sa

Ire ne

Be ta

nc ou

rt 7

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

306 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

When adolescents work long hours in a part-time job, they often have trouble juggling the demands of work, school, and sleep.

D ig

ita l V

is io

n/ Ju

pi te

rim ag

es

their income for future goals, such as a college education, or use it to contribute to their family’s expenses (Bachman et al., 2014). Because parents customarily pay for many of the essential expenses associated with independent living—rent, utili- ties, and groceries, for example—working adolescents often have a much higher percentage of their income available for discretionary spending than do working adults. Thus, for many teens, the part-time work experience provides unrealistic expectations about how income can be allocated.

The message that emerges repeatedly from research on part-time employment is hardly encouraging. Like Aaron, the teenage boy in the vignette, adolescents who work long hours at part-time jobs do not benefit from the experience. To the contrary, they do worse in school, are more likely to have behavioral problems, and learn how to spend rather than manage money. Ironically, though, there is a long-term benefit: Young adults who had a stressful part-time job as an adolescent are better able to cope with stressful adult jobs (Mortimer & Staff, 2004).

Does this mean that teenagers who are still in school should never work part-time? Not necessarily. Part-time employment can be a good experience, depending on the cir- cumstances. One key is the number of hours of work. Although the exact number of hours varies from one student to the next, most students could easily work five hours a week without harm, and many could work ten hours a week. Another key is the type of job. When adolescents have jobs that allow them to use their skills (e.g., bookkeeping, com- puting, or typing) and acquire new ones, self-esteem is enhanced and they learn from their work experience (Staff, Mont’Alvao, & Mortimer, 2015). Yet another factor is the link between work and school: Teens often benefit from apprenticeships or internships that are explicitly linked to school so that work experiences complement classroom experiences (Symonds, Schwartz, & Ferguson, 2011). A final factor is how teens spend their earnings. When they save their money or use it to pay for clothes and school expenses, the relation- ship with their parents often improves (Shanahan et al., 1996).

By these criteria, who is likely to show the harmful effects of part-time work? A teen who spends 30 hours a week bagging groceries and spends most of it on food, clothes, and video games. Who is likely to benefit from part-time work? A teen who likes to create apps for smartphones and spends Saturdays working at an Apple store, setting aside some of his earnings for college.

Finally, summer jobs typically do not involve conflict between work and school. Con- sequently, many of the harmful effects associated with part-time employment during the school year do not hold for summer employment. In fact, such employment has benefits. Following a summer of full-time employment, many high school students do better in school and are less likely to be involved in criminal activity (Heller, 2014; Schwartz, Leos- Urbel, & Wiswall, 2015).

Why employment is associated with all of these problems is not clear. Perhaps employed adolescents turn to drugs to help them cope with the anxiety and depres- sion brought on by work. Arguments with parents may become more common because anxious, depressed ado- lescents are more prone to argue or because wage-earning adolescents may believe that their freedom should match their income. Whatever the exact mechanism, extensive part-time work is clearly detrimental to the mental health of most adolescents.

3. Teens learn bad habits for handling money. Adults sometimes argue that work is good for teenagers because it teaches them “the value of a dollar,” but in reality, the typical teenage pattern is to “earn and spend.” Working adolescents spend most of their earnings on themselves: to buy clothing, snack food, or cosmetics and to pay for entertainment. Few working teens set aside much of

Think About It Think back to your own high school years and those of your friends. Think of students (including yourself) who showed harmful effects from part-time work. Think of people who benefited from part- time work.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 307

Recall 1. During the _______________ phase of vocational choice, ado-

lescents learn more about specific lines of work and begin training.

2. Individuals with a(n) _______________ personality type are best suited for a career as a teacher or counselor.

3. Adolescents who work extensively at part-time jobs during the school year often get lower grades, have behavior prob- lems, and _______________.

4. Part-time employment during the school year can be benefi- cial if adolescents limit the number of hours they work and _______________.

Interpret ●● Based on the description of Lynne’s career, how would you

describe continuity of vocational development during adoles- cence and young adulthood?

apply ●● Suppose that you are a high school guidance counselor

and have been asked to prepare a set of guidelines for students who want to work part-time. What would you recommend?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 9.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Why do teenagers drink and use drugs?

●● What leads some adolescents to become depressed? How can depression be treated?

●● What are the causes of juvenile delinquency?

9.4 The Dark Side

Jaylen was an excellent student and a starter on his high school basketball team. He was looking forward to going to the senior prom with Makayla, his long-time girlfriend, and then going to the state college with her in the fall. Then, without a hint that anything was wrong in their relationship, Makayla dropped Jaylen and moved in with the singer in a hip hop band. Jaylen was stunned and miserable. Without Makayla, life meant so little. Basketball and college seemed pointless. Some days Jaylen wondered if he should just kill himself to make the pain go away.

Some young people do not adapt well to the new demands and responsibilities of adoles- cence and respond in ways that are unhealthy. In this last section of Chapter 9, we look at three problems, often interrelated, that create the “three Ds” of adolescent development: drugs, depression, and delinquency. As we look at these problems, you’ll understand why Jaylen feels so miserable without Makayla.

Drug Use Teenage Drinking Teen use of illicit drugs such as cocaine and methamphetamine often makes headlines, but in reality, most adolescents avoid drugs, with one glaring exception—alcohol. About half of U.S. high school seniors have drunk alcohol within the past year, and about one-third report that they’ve been drunk (Johnston et al., 2017).

Teens are more likely to drink when their parents drink, when their parents do not monitor their teen’s behavior, and when parents are not warm toward them (Donald- son, Handren, & Crano, 2016). Also, they’re more likely to drink when peers do (Leung, Toumbourou, & Hemphill, 2014). Finally, teens who report frequent life stresses— problems with parents or peers and, for minority teens, racial discrimination—more often drink (Goldbach et al., 2015).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

308 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Because teenage drinking has so many causes, no single approach is likely to eliminate alcohol abuse. Adolescents who drink to reduce their tension can profit from therapy designed to teach them more effective means of coping with stress. School-based pro- grams that are interactive—featuring student-led discussion—can be effective in teaching the facts about drinking and strategies for resist- ing peer pressure to drink (Agabio et al., 2017). Stopping teens from drinking before it becomes habitual is essential because adolescents who drink are at risk for becoming alcohol-dependent, depressed, or anxious as adults (Cable & Sacker, 2008; Trim et al., 2007).

Teenage Smoking Approximately 30% of American teens experiment with ciga- rette smoking at some point in their teenage years ( Johnston

et al., 2017). American teenagers who smoke typically begin sometime between sixth and ninth grade. As was true for teenage drinking, parents and peers are influential in determining whether youth smoke. When parents smoke, their teenage children are more likely to smoke, too. But the parent–child relationship also contributes: Teens are less likely to smoke when they experience the supportive parenting associated with authoritative parenting (Foster et al., 2007). Like parents, peer influences can be direct and indirect. Teenagers more often smoke when peers do (Fujimoto, Unger,  & Valente, 2012). However, a more subtle influence of peers on teen smoking comes

from informal school norms. When most students in a school think that it’s okay  to smoke—even though many of them do not themselves  smoke—teens are more likely to start smoking (Su  & Supple, 2016).

The dangers of cigarette smoking for adults are well known. Many teenage smokers believe that cigarette smoking is harmless. But they’re wrong: Smoking can interfere with the growth of the lungs, and teens who smoke often have a variety of health problems such as respiratory illnesses. What’s more, smoking is often the fateful first step on the path to abuse of more powerful substances, including alcohol, marijuana, and cocaine (Strong, Juon, & Ens- minger, 2016).

Faced with these many harmful consequences of teenage smok- ing, health care professionals and human development researchers have worked hard to create effective programs to discourage ado- lescents from smoking. In fact, just as comprehensive school-based programs can reduce teenage sex, such programs are effective in reducing teenage smoking. These programs typically provide infor- mation about short- and long-term health and social consequences of smoking and provide students with effective ways to respond to peer pressure to smoke. In addition, programs are most effective when they are implemented alongside community initiatives, such as banning smoking in public places popular with youth (Dobbins et al., 2008).

Depression The challenges of adolescence can lead some youth to become depressed (Fried, 2005). When suffering from depression, adolescents have perva- sive feelings of sadness, are irritable, have low self-esteem, sleep poorly, and are unable to concentrate. About 5% to 15% of adolescents are

Adolescents often drink because their peers encourage them.

Ku tt

ig –P

eo pl

e/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o Adolescents sometimes become depressed when they feel as if they’ve lost control of their lives.

iS to

ck .c

om /S

tH el

en a

depression Disorder characterized by pervasive feelings of sadness, irritability, and low self-esteem.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 309

depressed; adolescent girls are more often affected than boys, probably because social chal- lenges in adolescence are greater for girls than boys (Garber & Rao, 2014). Depressed ado- lescents are often unhappy, angry, and annoyed. They believe that family and peers are not friendly to them (Cole & Jordan, 1995), and they are often extremely lonely (Mahon et al., 2006). Rather than being satisfying and rewarding, life is empty and joyless for depressed adolescents.

Depression is often triggered when adolescents experience a serious loss, disap- pointment, or failure, such as when a loved one dies or when a much-anticipated date turns out to be a fiasco (Schneiders et al., 2006). Think back to Jaylen, the adolescent in the vignette at the beginning of this section. His girlfriend had been the center of his life. When she left him unexpectedly, he felt helpless to control his own destiny. Similarly, an athlete may play poorly in the championship game because of illness, or a high school senior may get a lower score on the SAT exam because of a family crisis the night before the test. In each case, the adolescent could do nothing to avoid an undesir- able result.

Of course, many adolescents experience negative events like these, but don’t become depressed. Why? Heredity plays a role, putting some adolescents at greater risk for depres- sion. Another contributing factor is temperament: Children who are less able to regulate their emotions are, as adolescents, more prone to depression. A third factor is a belief system in which adolescents see themselves in an extremely negative light. Depression- prone adolescents are, for example, more likely to blame themselves for failure (Gregory et al., 2007). Thus, after the disappointing date, a depression-prone teen is likely to think “I acted like a fool,” instead of placing blame elsewhere by thinking “Gee—he was a real jerk!” (Garber & Rao, 2014).

Parents and families can also put an adolescent at risk for depression. Not surpris- ingly, adolescents more often become depressed when their parents are critical of them, don’t respect their opinions and choices, and seem unwilling to accept them as they are. In addition, adolescents are at risk when parents are inconsistent in their discipline and when they’re frequently in conflict with one another (Yap et al., 2014).

Another potential trigger for depression—racial discrimination—is the focus of the Spotlight on Research feature.

Think About It How does depression illustrate the inter- action of biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces on development?

On ResearchSpotlight

Does Racial Discrimination Lead to Depression?

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Many African American adolescents are subject to racial discrimination, an experience that is extremely stressful. Because other stress- producing events (e.g., parental conflict) have been linked to depression, Devin English, Sharon Lambert, and Nicholas Ialongo (2014) wondered whether experiencing racial dis- crimination would make African American youth more prone to depression.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? The investigators had African American youth complete two ques- tionnaires. One assessed their perception

[Continued on next page]

of racial discrimination. For example, they were asked how often they believed they had been left out of a group activity because of discrimination. The second questionnaire assessed frequency of symptoms of depres- sion, such as feeling sad and lonely and sleeping poorly.

Who were the participants in the study? The study included 504 African American teenagers living in Baltimore, Maryland. They were tested annually in grades 7 to 10.

What was the design of the study? The study was correlational because researchers examined links between perceived racial dis- crimination and symptoms of depression. The study was longitudinal because adolescents were tested in four successive years.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? No. The questionnaires were ones used often by other researchers. The teens agreed to participate and their parents pro- vided consent.

What were the results? The left panel of ◗ Figure 9.2 shows correlations between racial discrimination and symptoms of depres- sion two years later (racial discrimination at grade 7 and depression at grade 9; racial discrimination at grade 8 and depression at grade 10). The correlations—positive and statistically significant—indicate that greater perceived racial discrimination was associ- ated with more symptoms of depression. The right panel shows the complementary correlations—predicting racial discrimination

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

310 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

Treating Depression Drugs and psychotherapy are successful in helping adolescents who are depressed. Drugs operate by allowing the brain regions that regulate mood to communicate more effec- tively. Although drug treatment reduces the symptoms of depression, many adolescents dislike this kind of treatment and often don’t take drugs that are prescribed. Consequently, psychotherapy is often the preferred approach. For example, in cognitive behavioral ther- apy, adolescents learn to replace negative thoughts (e.g., blaming themselves) with more positive thoughts and they learn how to become involved in activities that are pleasurable and rewarding. These methods work, but we still don’t know the conditions under which they are most effective (Asarnow & Miranda, 2014).

Preventing Teen Suicides Suicide is the third most frequent cause of death (after accidents and homicide) among U.S. adolescents: Nearly 10% of adolescents report having attempted suicide. Girls are nearly twice as likely as boys to attempt suicide; Native American and Latino teens are more likely to attempt suicide than other groups (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2015).

Many features are linked to suicide, including substance abuse and a family history of suicide, but the strongest predictor is depression. Between 40 and 80% of teens who attempt suicide are depressed (Cash & Bridge, 2009). Few suicides are truly spontaneous, and in most cases, there are warning signs, such as talking about death or looking for ways to kill one’s self. If someone you know shows these signs, don’t ignore them and don’t leave him or her alone. Seek help immediately: call 911 or contact a mental health professional (Rudd et al., 2006).

Delinquency Adolescents are responsible for much of the criminal activity committed in the United States. For example, they account for about 15% of all arrests for violent crimes (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2016). To understand such delinquent behavior, we need to

from depressive symptoms. In both cases, these correlations are not significant.

What did the investigators conclude? Racial discrimination, a well-established stressor, can put African American youth at risk for depres- sion. English and his colleagues note “for Afri- can American adolescents, experienced racial discrimination predicted depressive symptoms … the persistence of this predictive association across adolescence suggests that experienc- ing racial discrimination is a phenomenon that is chronic and detrimental across the life span for African Americans” (p. 1194).

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? One limitation of the study is that racial discrimination and depression were both estimated by responses from the adolescents themselves. The find- ings would be more compelling if one of the constructs were measured independently; for example, parents could be asked to judge adolescents’ depressive symptoms. In addi- tion, stress is the assumed link between racial discrimination and depression; it would be valuable to assess stress and to assess those links (racial discrimination→stress and stress→depression) directly.

●◗ Figure 9.2 African American adolescents who perceive that they are victims of racial discrimina- tion are more likely to become depressed.

0.25

Predicting depression from perceived racial discrimination

Discrimination grade 7 -

depression grade 9

Discrimination grade 8 -

depression grade 10

C or

re la

ti on

Predicting perceived racial discrimination from

depression

0.2

0.15

0.1

0.05

0

0.25

Depression grade 7 -

discrimination grade 9

Depression grade 8 -

discrimination grade 10

0.2

0.15

0.1

0.05

0

Data from English et al. (2014).

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 311

distinguish two forms of antisocial behavior (Piquero & Moffitt, 2014). The most common form is relatively mild: adolescent-limited antisocial behavior refers to relatively minor criminal acts by adolescents who aren’t consistently antisocial. These youth may become involved in petty crimes such as shoplifting or using drugs but may be careful to follow all school rules. As the name implies, their antisocial behavior is short-lived, usually vanish- ing in late adolescence or early adulthood.

A second form of delinquent behavior is far more serious but, fortunately, much less common. Life-course persistent antisocial behavior refers to antisocial behavior that emerges at an early age and continues throughout life. These individuals may start hitting at 3 years of age and progress to shoplifting at age 12 and then to stealing cars at age 16. Fewer than 5% of youth fit this pattern of antisocial behavior, but they account for most adolescent criminal activity.

Researchers have identified several forces that contribute to this type of antisocial and delinquent behavior.

1. Biological contributions. Born to Be Bad is the title of at least two movies, two songs, and three books. Implicit in this popular title is the idea that from birth, some individuals follow a developmental track that leads to destructive, violent, or criminal behavior. In other words, the claim is that biology pushes people to be aggressive long before experience can affect development.

Is there any truth to this idea? In fact, biology and heredity do contribute to aggressive and violent behavior, but not in the manner suggested by the epithet “born to be bad.” Twin studies make it clear that heredity contributes: Identi- cal twins are usually more alike in their levels of physical aggression than are fraternal twins (Brendgen et al., 2006; Lacourse et al., 2014). But these stud- ies do not tell us that aggression per se is inherited; instead, they indicate that some children inherit factors that place them at risk for aggressive or violent behavior. Temperament is one such factor: Youngsters who temperamentally have trouble regulating and processing emotions (especially negative ones like anger) are more likely to be aggressive. What’s more these children seem to be prone to sensation seeking and find that aggressive behavior is rewarding (van Goozen, 2015). Hormones represent another factor: Higher levels of the hor- mone testosterone are often associated, weakly, with greater aggression and stronger responses to provocation (Carré, McCormick, & Hariri, 2011; Tremblay et al., 1998).

These factors don’t cause a child to be aggressive. But they do make aggres- sive behavior more likely: For instance, children who are emotional and easily irritated may be disliked by their peers and be in frequent conflict with them, opening the door for aggressive responses. Thus, biological factors place children at risk for aggression; to understand which youth actually become aggressive, we need to look at interactions between inherited factors and youth’s experiences (van Goozen, 2015).

2. Cognitive processes. The cognitive skills described in Chapters 6 and 8 also play a role in antisocial behavior. One general factor is executive functioning. Children who are less skilled in inhibiting, shifting, and updating behaviors and thoughts are prone to aggressive behavior (McQuade, 2017).

Cognitive processes contribute to antisocial behavior in another way, too: Aggressive youth often respond aggressively because they are not skilled at inter- preting other people’s intentions. Without a clear interpretation in mind, they respond aggressively by default. That is, aggressive boys far too often think, “I don’t know what you’re up to, and when in doubt, attack” (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Galán et al., 2017). When others inadvertently get in their way, delinquent ado- lescents often respond without regard to the nature of the other person’s acts or intentions.

3. Family processes. Antisocial behavior has roots in parenting. Adolescents are more likely to become involved in delinquent acts when their parents use harsh discipline or don’t monitor effectively (Gershoff, 2013; Lee, Altschul, & Gershoff,

adolescent-limited antisocial behavior Behavior of youth who engage in relatively minor criminal acts but aren’t consistently antisocial.

life-course persistent antisocial behavior Antisocial behavior that emerges at an early age and continues throughout life.

Think About It A letter to the editor of your local paper claims that “juvenile delinquents should be thrown in jail because they’re born as ‘bad apples’ and will always be that way.” Write a reply that states the facts correctly.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

312 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

2013; Vieno et al., 2009). Parents may also contribute to delinquent behavior if their marital relationship is marked by constant conflict. When parents constantly argue and fight, their children are more likely to be antisocial (Cummings et al., 2006; Narayan, Englund, & Englund, 2013). Of course, children have ringside seats for many of these confronta- tions; thus, they can see firsthand how parents use verbal and physical aggression against each other. And, sadly, children come to believe that these patterns of interacting represent “natural” ways of  solving problems (Graham-Bermann & Brescoll, 2000).

4. Poverty. Aggressive and antisocial behavior is more common among children living in poverty than among children who are economically advantaged (Williams, Conger, & Blozis, 2007). As we’ve seen, living in poverty is extremely stressful for par- ents and often leads to the very parental behaviors that pro- mote aggression—harsh discipline and lax monitoring (Shaw & Shelleby, 2014). In addition, violent crime is far more common in poverty-stricken neighborhoods. Older children and ado- lescents exposed to such violence are more likely to be aggres- sive and violent themselves as they get older (Bingenheimer, Brennan, & Earls, 2005).

Obviously, many factors contribute to making some adoles- cents prone to violent behavior. As you can imagine, when risk fac- tors mount up in children’s lives, they are at ever-greater risk for aggressive behavior (Greenberg et al., 1999). What’s more, many of the factors operate in a cascading fashion, such that later risk factors build on prior factors (Vaillancourt et al., 2013): Poverty or maternal depression can lead to harsh, ineffective parenting. In

Aggressive teens see the world as a hostile place and typically respond aggressively by default.

Im ag

e So

ur ce

/A ge

F ot

os to

ck

turn, this leads children to be unprepared for school (both academically and socially), which leads to school failure and conduct problems. These difficulties cause some par- ents to become less active and less invested in parenting, which means that they mon- itor their adolescents less often,  allowing them to associate with deviant, aggressive peers (Dodge, Greenberg, & Malone, 2008).

Thus, the developmental journey that leads to a violent, aggressive, antisocial ado- lescent starts in early childhood but gains momentum along the way. Consequently, efforts to prevent children from taking this path must begin early, be maintained over childhood, and target children and their parents. An example of a successful interven- tion program is Fast Track (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2015), which is designed to teach academic and social skills to elementary school children and life and vocational skills to adolescents. In addition, parents are taught skills for effective child rearing and, later, ways to stay involved with their children and to monitor their behav- ior. As young adults—seven years after the program had ended—antisocial behavior, criminal activity, and substance abuse were reduced among those who had participated in Fast Track.

Of course, this sort of successful program comes with an expensive price tag. But costs of prevention programs are a fraction of the costs associated with the by-products of aggressive behavior: One analysis suggests that each dollar invested in such programs saves more than $15 in crime-related costs, including payments to victims, court costs, and costs of incarceration (Farrington & Koegl, 2015). Thus, programs such as Fast Track not only improve children’s lives (and the lives of people around them) but also are cost-effective.

In the What Do You Think? feature, we describe a very different approach to dealing with adolescent crime.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Recall 1. The main factors that determine whether teenagers drink

include parents, peers, and _______________.

2. Peers influence teenage smoking indirectly by _______________.

3. The factors that put adolescents at risk for depression include heredity, temperament, a belief system in which adolescents blame themselves, parents, and, for adoles- cents who are members of minority groups, _______________.

4. Treatments for depression include drugs and psychotherapy that emphasizes _______________.

5. _______________ refers to antisocial behavior that begins at an early age and continues throughout life.

6. The factors that contribute to juvenile delinquency include biology, cognitive processes, _______________, and poverty.

Interpret ●● Describe potential biological and environmental contributions

to delinquency.

apply ●● Prepare a fact sheet that middle schools could use to educate

antisocial adolescents about ways to resolve conflicts and achieve goals without relying on aggression.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 9.4

Traditionally, when adolescents under 18 commit crimes, the case is handled in the juvenile justice system. Although proce- dures vary from state to state, most adoles- cents who are arrested do not go to court; instead, law enforcement and legal authori- ties have considerable discretionary power. They may, for example, release arrested adolescents into the custody of their parents. However, when adolescents commit serious or violent crimes, there will be a hearing with a judge. This hearing is closed to the press and public; no jury is involved. Instead, the judge receives reports from police, probation

officers, school officials, medical authorities, and other interested parties. Adolescents judged guilty can be placed on probation at home, in foster care outside the home, or in a facility for youth offenders.

Because juveniles are committing more serious crimes, many law enforcement and legal authorities believe that juveniles should be tried as adults. Advocates of this posi- tion argue for lowering the minimum age for mandatory transfer of a case to adult courts, increasing the range of offenses that must be tried in adult court, and giving prosecutors more authority to file cases with juveniles in

adult criminal court. Critics argue that treat- ing juvenile offenders as adults ignores the fact that juveniles are less able than adults to understand the nature and consequences of committing a crime. Also, they argue, punish- ments appropriate for adults are inappropriate for juveniles (Monahan, Steinberg, & Piquero, 2015).

What do you think? Should we lower the age at which juveniles are tried as adults? Based on the theories of development we dis- cussed, what guidelines would you propose in deciding when a juvenile should be tried as an adult?

When Juveniles Commit Serious Crimes, Should They Be Tried as Adults?

What Do YOU Think?

9.1 Identity and Self-Esteem

how do adolescents achieve an identity?

●● The task for adolescents is to find an identity, a search that typically involves four statuses: Diffusion and foreclosure are more common in early adolescence; moratorium and achievement are more common in late adolescence and young adulthood. As they seek identity, adolescents often believe that others are constantly watching them and that no one else has felt as they do.

●● Adolescents are more likely to achieve an identity when parents encourage discussion and recognize their auton- omy; they are least likely to achieve an identity when par- ents set rules and enforce them without explanation.

What are the stages and results of acquiring an ethnic identity?

●● Adolescents from ethnic groups often progress through three phases in acquiring an ethnic identity: initial disinterest, exploration, and identity achievement. Achiev- ing an ethnic identity usually results in higher self-esteem.

SUMMARY

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 313

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

314 pa RT T W o : S C H O O L - A G E C H I L D R E N A N D A D O L E S C E N T S

how does self-esteem change in adolescence?

●● Social comparisons begin anew when children move from elementary school to middle or junior high school; con- sequently, self-esteem usually declines somewhat during this transition. In middle and late adolescence, self-esteem becomes differentiated, especially in the academic and social domains. Self-esteem is linked to adolescents’ actual competence in domains that matter to them and is linked to how parents and peers view them.

●● The parent–child relationship becomes more egalitarian during the adolescent years, reflecting adolescents’ growing independence. Contrary to myth, adolescence is not usually a period of storm and stress. Most adolescents love their parents, feel loved by them, rely on them for advice, and adopt their values.

9.2 Romantic Relationships and Sexuality

Why do teenagers date?

●● Romantic relationships emerge in mid-adolescence. For younger adolescents, dating is for both companionship and sexual exploration; for older adolescents, it is a source of trust and support. Adolescents in romantic relationships are more self-confident but also report more emotional upheaval.

Why are some adolescents sexually active? Why do so few use contraceptives?

●● By the end of adolescence, most American boys and girls have had sexual intercourse, which boys view as recre- ational but girls see as romantic. Adolescents are more likely to be sexually active if they believe that their parents and peers approve of sex. Adolescents do not use birth control consistently because they do not see the need for contraception, don’t know where to obtain contraceptives, and sometimes find pregnancy appealing. Because they use contraception infrequently, they are at risk for contracting sexually transmitted diseases and becoming pregnant.

Who are sexual-minority youth?

●● Sexual-minority youth identify as gay, lesbian, bisexual, queer, intersex, or transgender. For boys who identify as gay, the first step involves feeling different from other boys and becoming interested in gender-atypical activities. For females, the path to a sexual-minority identity is more vari- able. Sexual-minority youth face special challenges, and some suffer from mental health problems.

What circumstances make dating violence likely?

●● Many adolescents experience dating violence, particularly if they’ve experience harsh parenting, their peers are famil- iar with and accept dating violence, and they’re aggressive and antisocial. Programs to prevent dating violence provide greater awareness of dating abuse and emphasize that all com- munity members have a stake in preventing dating violence.

9.3 The World of Work

how do adolescents select an occupation?

●● In his theory of vocational choice, Super proposes three phases of vocational development during adolescence and young adulthood: crystallization, in which basic interests are identified; specification, in which jobs associated with interests are identified; and implementation, which marks entry into the workforce.

●● Holland proposes six different work-related personalities: realistic, investigative, social, conventional, enterprising, and artistic. Each is uniquely suited to certain jobs. People are more productive when their personality fits their job and less productive when it does not.

●● According to social cognitive career theory, vocational choice reflects beliefs about their abilities and beliefs about the outcome of their behavior.

What is the impact of part-time employment on adolescents?

●● Most adolescents in the United States have part-time jobs. Adolescents who are employed more than 15 to 20 hours per week during the school year typically do poorly in school, often have lowered self-esteem and increased anxiety, and have problems interacting with others. Most employed adolescents save little of their income. Instead, they spend it on clothing, food, and entertainment, which can yield misleading expectations about how to allocate income.

●● Part-time employment can be beneficial if adolescents work relatively few hours, if the work allows them to use existing skills or acquire new ones, and if teens save some of their earnings. Summer employment, which does not conflict with the demands of school, can also be beneficial.

9.4 The Dark Side

Why do teenagers drink and use drugs?

●● Many adolescents drink alcohol regularly. The primary factors that influence whether adolescents drink are encouragement from others (parents and peers) and stress. Similarly, teenage smoking is influenced by parents and peers.

What leads some adolescents to become depressed? how can depression be treated?

●● Depressed adolescents have little enthusiasm for life, believe that others are unfriendly, and want to be left alone. Depression can be triggered by a negative event; those adolescents who are most likely to be affected can’t con- trol their emotions and see themselves in a negative light. Treating depression relies on medications and on therapy designed to improve social skills and restructure adoles- cents’ interpretation of life events.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T E R 9 : M O V I N G I N T O T H E A D U LT S O C I A L W O R L D 315

What are the causes of juvenile delinquency?

●● Many young people engage in antisocial behavior briefly during adolescence. In contrast, the small percentage of adolescents who engage in life-course persistent antiso- cial behavior are involved in many of the serious crimes

committed in the United States. Life-course persistent antisocial behavior has been linked to biology, cognitive processes, family processes, and poverty. Efforts to reduce adolescent criminal activity must address all of these variables.

9.1 1. role confusion 2. foreclosure 3. diffusion 4. Imaginary audience 5. to explore alternative identities but do not pressure them or provide direction 6. start to explore the personal impact of their ethnic roots 7. no longer know where they stand among their peers, which means they must establish a new pecking order 9.2 1. an outlet for sexual exploration 2. harsh 3. engage in unprotected sex 4. see becoming pregnant as a way to become an adult 5. feel different and are interested in gender-atypical activities; 6. condone dating violence 9.3 1. specification 2. social 3. learn bad habits for handling money 4. hold jobs that allow them to use their skills and develop new ones 9.4 1. stress 2. establishing an informal school norm in which smoking is approved 3. racial discrimination 4. improving cognitive and social skills 5. Life-course persistent antisocial behavior 6. family processes

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

adolescent egocentrism (291) imaginary audience (292) personal fable (292) illusion of invulnerability (292) ethnic identity (293)

crystallization (302) specification (303) implementation (303) personality-type theory (303) depression (308)

adolescent-limited antisocial behavior (311)

life-course persistent antisocial behavior (311)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

A rie

l S ke

lle y/

Bl en

d Im

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

317

10Becoming an AdultPhysical, Cognitive, and Personality Development in Young Adulthood

There comes a time in life when we feel the urge to move beyond adolescence and aspire to be an adult. In some societies, the transition to adult-

hood is abrupt and dramatic, marked by clear rites

of passage. In Western society, it is fuzzier; the only

apparent marker may be a birthday ritual. We may

even ask “real” adults what it’s like to be one. Adult-

hood is marked in numerous ways, some of which we

explore in the first section.

Without question, young adulthood is the peak

of physical processes and health. It is also a time

when people who acquired unhealthy habits earlier

in life may decide to adopt a healthier lifestyle. Young

adulthood also marks the peak of some cognitive

abilities and the next step in the continued develop-

ment of others.

On a more personal level, young adulthood is a

time when we make plans and dream of what lies

ahead. It is a time when we think about what life as

an adult will be like. But above all, it is a time when

we lay the foundation for the developmental changes that we will experience

during the rest of our lives. We will consider these issues as we examine young

adulthood in this chapter.

10.1 Emerging Adulthood 318 Role Transitions Marking Adulthood 319 Neuroscience, Behavior, and Emerging Adulthood 320 Achieving Milestones: Education, Workforce,

and Erikson’s Intimacy 321 So When Do People Become Adults? 323

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Does Being Rich and Famous Mean You’re an Adult? 324

10.2 Physical Development and Health 325 Growth, Strength, and Physical Functioning 325 Lifestyle Factors in Health 325

WHAT DO YOU THINK? Healthcare Disparities in the United States 333

10.3 Cognitive Development 334 How Should We View Intelligence in Adults? 334 Primary and Secondary Mental Abilities 335

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: The Seattle Longitudinal Study 336

Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence 337 Neuroscience Research and Intelligence 339 Going Beyond Formal Operations: Thinking in

Adulthood 340 Integrating Emotion and Logic in Emerging and Young

Adulthood 342

10.4 Who Do You Want to Be? Personality in Young Adulthood 345 Creating Life Stories 346 Possible Selves 347 Personal Control Beliefs 348

    Summary 350

    Key Terms 352

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

318 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Marcus woke up with the worst headache he could ever remember having. “If this is adult- hood, they can keep it,” he muttered to himself. Like many young adults in the United States, Marcus spent his 21st birthday celebrating at a bar with his friends. But the phone call from his mother that woke him in the first place reminds him that he isn’t an adult in every way; she called to see if he needs money.

Think for a minute about the first time you felt like an adult. When was it? What was the context? Who were you with? How did you feel?

Even though becoming an adult is one of the most important life transitions, it is dif- ficult to pin down exactly when this occurs in Western societies. In the United States, for example, the age needed to achieve “adult” status includes 12 or so for purchasing movie theater tickets, 16 for driving a car (in many states), 18 for voting and joining the military, 21 for consuming alcoholic beverages, and 26 for losing medical insurance coverage on the policy of one’s parents under the 2010 Affordable Care Act. Clearly, in the United States, there is no age that signals a clean break with adolescence and full achievement of adult- hood. Certainly Marcus may feel like an adult because he can purchase alcohol legally, but he may not feel that way in other respects, such as financially.

Some human developmentalists view the period from the late teens to the mid- to late 20s as distinctive. They refer to it as emerging adulthood, a period when individuals are not ado- lescents but are not yet fully adults (Arnett, 2013, 2016). Emerging adulthood is a time to explore careers, self-identity, and commitments. It is also a time when certain biological and physiological developmental trends peak and brain development continues in different ways.

In this section, we examine some of the ways societies mark the transition to adult- hood, and we’ll see that the criteria vary widely from culture to culture.

emerging adulthood Period between late teens and mid- to late 20s when individuals are not adolescents but are not yet fully adults.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What role transitions mark entry into adulthood?

●● What evidence from neuroscience helps us understand behavioral development in young adulthood?

●● How do going to college, obtaining career training, and entering the workforce reflect the transition to adulthood?

●● When do people achieve adulthood inn contemporary American society?

10.1 Emerging Adulthood

Milestone birthdays such as turning 21 are often marked with celebrations. as

ta ro

t/ Sh

ut te

rs to

ck .c

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 319

Role Transitions Marking Adulthood When people become adults in different cultures depends on how you define adulthood and the kind of role transitions cultures create. Consider the following three women. Ganika looked older than her 20 years; her son was playing quietly on the floor. She and her husband live in a small house in a village in rural India. Sheree graduated from high school a few years ago. She works full time at the Old Navy store and rents a small apartment nearby. Claudia recently graduated from college in Germany. She just started a job at a tech firm, so until she saves enough money for her own apartment she is living with her mother.

Are these women adults? Yes and no. As we will see, it all depends on how you define adulthood and the kind of role transitions cultures create. A role transition is movement into the next stage of development marked by assumption of new responsibilities and duties.

Cross-Cultural Evidence of Role Transitions Cultures in the developing world tend to be clear about when a person becomes an adult (Mazur & Li, 2016; Nelson, Badger, & Wu, 2004). Rituals marking initiation into adult- hood, often among the most important ones in a culture, are termed rites of passage. Rites of passage may involve highly elaborate steps that take days or weeks, or they may be com- pressed into a few minutes. Initiates are usually dressed in apparel reserved for the ritual to denote their special position. We still have traces of these rites in Western culture; con- sider, for example, the ritual attire for graduations and weddings.

In many cultures, rites of passage to adulthood are connected with religious rituals (Levete, 2010). For example, Christian traditions use a ritual called confirmation to mark the transition from being a child spiritually to being an adult. Judaism celebrates bar and bat mitzvahs. In many Latin American countries, a girl’s 15th birthday is celebrated as the transition between childhood and young womanhood. In some countries, this ceremony, called the quinceañera, is preceded by a Mass if the girl’s family is Catholic.

Marriage is the most important rite of passage to adulthood in many cultures because it is a prelude to childbearing, which in turn provides clear evidence of achieving adult- hood (Hall & Willoughby, 2016; Mensch, Singh, & Casterline, 2006). In this sense, Ganika is an adult in her culture. In many cultures in developed economies, establishing a career is the most likely alternative pathway to adulthood (Hall & Willoughby, 2016).

Some tribal cultures mark the transition to adulthood in public ways so that the whole community witnesses it. In a few cultures, it may involve pain or mutilation, such as the con- troversial practice of female circumcision (Castro, 2010), or may involve being given a spe- cific tattoo (Irish, 2009). Because rites in such cultures change little over time, they provide continuity across generations (Keith, 1990); older adults lead young people through the same rites they themselves experienced years earlier. American counterparts not tied directly to specific ethnic groups are less formalized and are hard to identify; indeed, you may be hard- pressed to think of any. A father buying his son his first razor or a mother helping her daugh- ter with her first menstrual period may be as close as we get in American society.

Role Transitions in Western Cultures In Western cultures, the most widely used criteria for deciding whether a person has reached adulthood are role transitions, which involve assuming new responsibilities and duties. Certain role transitions are key markers for attaining adulthood: voting, completing one’s educa- tion, beginning full-time employment, leaving home and establishing financial independence, getting married, and becoming a parent. How- ever, many people in their 20s in industrialized countries spread these achievements over several years (Arnett, 2013, 2016).

The point in the life span when these marker events for role tran- sitions happen, though, has changed over the years. Such changes are examples of cohort effects, described in Chapter 1. For example, in the United States, the average age for completing all of one’s formal schooling

rites of passage Rituals marking initiation into adulthood.

role transitions Movement into the next stage of devel- opment marked by assumption of new responsibilities and duties.

This couple from India reflects a culture in which the passage to adulthood for many occurs with marriage.

Ro be

rt H

ar di

ng Im

ag es

/M as

te rf

ile

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

320 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

rose steadily during the 20th century as the proportion of people going to college at some point in adulthood increased from roughly 10% in the early part of the century to roughly two- thirds today. Likewise, the age at first marriage has increased in the United States to about 29 for men and 27 for women (Census Bureau, 2016), a remarkable change of over 7 years since the 1970s. Similarly, the number of adult children living with their parents increased signifi- cantly during and after the Great Recession (Davis, Kim, & Fingerman, in press).

What does this mean for the transition from adolescence to adulthood? It means that Western society has no clear age-constant rituals that clearly mark the transition to adult- hood (Ivory, 2004). As a result, young people create their own, such as being initiated into student organizations (e.g., fraternities and sororities) and drinking alcohol (a topic we explore later in this chapter). The lack of clearly defined rituals makes it difficult to use any one event as the marker for becoming an adult. Although the trend is that living indepen- dently from one’s parents, financial independence, and romantic involvement are associ- ated with increased assumption of adult roles, individual patterns are diverse. So depending on how you look at it, Sheree and Claudia may—or may not—be considered adults.

We will consider some of the components of the role transitions in Western culture and how they help shape role transitions. First, let’s consider new evidence from neurosci- ence about important changes during emerging adulthood.

Neuroscience, Behavior, and Emerging Adulthood Emerging adulthood is a time of very important developments in the brain, especially in terms of strengthening interconnections across different brain structures that help inte- grate different modes of thinking and feeling. This neuroscience research has raised intriguing questions: Could continuing brain development during early adulthood explain shifts in thinking and behavior, and possibly provide a physiological marker for adulthood?

There is considerable evidence that the prefrontal cortex, a part of the brain involved in high-level thinking, is not fully developed until a person reaches his or her mid-20s (Cohen et al., 2016). Additionally, important connections between the prefrontal cortex and the parietal lobe involved in the processing of emotional information also matures in adulthood. The key point is that the brain structures and connections that govern much of

the higher level reasoning we associate with adulthood is not possible prior to early adulthood. We will come back to these aspects of brain development later in this chapter.

From a psychological perspective, the neurological developments are observed through changes in behavior. Cognitively, young adults think in dif- ferent ways than do adolescents, especially in their ability to take different points of view (King & Kitchener, 2015). We will consider this developmental change later in this chapter.

Emerging adults engage in more risky behaviors than do any other age group of adults (Arnett, 2013, 2015). The desire to live life more on the edge through physically and emotionally threatening situations on the boundary between life and death is termed edgework (Brymer & Mackenzie, 2017; Lyng, 2012; PetersonLund, 2013). Edgework is seen through such sports as base jumping, waterfall kayaking, free solo climbing, and other extreme sports in which the slightest mistake likely results in death.

There is an important distinction between edgework, which involves high levels of risk but a great deal of expertise and planning, and reckless behavior, which involves high levels of risk but tends to be more spontaneous (Zinn, 2016). People who engage in edgework activities tend to share their experiences with other like-minded individuals across the spectrum of socioeconomic dimen- sions and as a mode of escape from the modern everyday world (Lyng, 2012).

Managing to deal with intense emotions when faced with real danger is a delicate balance, and how men and women accomplish this differs. For exam- ple, in thinking about or planning future dangerous activities, men tend to be highly confident in their ability to extricate themselves from tough positions

edgework The desire to live life more on the edge through physically and emotionally threat- ening situations on the boundary between life and death.

Risky behavior such as this tends to decrease over the course of young adulthood.

Pe op

le Im

ag es

/E +/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Think About It Why does Western society lack clear rites of passage?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 321

and do not feel a need to rehearse just in case. Women, though, are more likely to have qualms, which they ease by rehearsing.

On the psychosocial front, young adulthood marks the transition from concern with identity (see Chapter 9) to concern with behaviors related to autonomy and intimacy, which we explore later in this chapter and in Chapter 11 (Erikson, 1982). Becoming inde- pendent from one’s parents entails being able to fend for oneself, but it does not imply a complete severing of the relationship. On the contrary, adult children usually establish a rewarding relationship with their parents, as we will see in Chapter 13.

Achieving Milestones: Education, Workforce, and Erikson’s Intimacy As we noted earlier, a key marker of the transition to adulthood is achieving certain mile- stones, such as the launching of one’s career and reaching developmental markers such as Erikson’s stage of intimacy. Let’s take a look at how they are accomplished.

Education and Workforce Attainment For nearly 70% of all high school graduates in the United States, a marker of the transi- tion to adulthood is going straight to college—although the rates vary significantly across income groups. As shown in ◗ Figure 10.1, enrollment of high school graduates from high-income families was 26 points higher (84%) than from low-income families (58%) (National Center for Educational Statistics, 2016). Racial and ethnic differences are also found, but these gaps are closing. For example, Asian Americans had the highest overall participation rate immediately after high school at 91%, followed by African Americans at 70%, whites at 68%, and Latinos at 65% (National Center for Education Statistics, 2016).

Considerable research has documented how students develop while they are in college (King & Kitchener, 2015; Patton et al., 2016; Perry, 1970). This work provides a theoretical and practical frame for understanding how adult students learn specific content ( Nickerson & Thurkettle, 2013; Sargent & Borthick, 2013). Students start acting and thinking like adults because of advances in intellectual development and personal and social identity. It is thought that much of this developmental change in college occurs through social interaction, especially when it is supported within an active learning process. Indeed there is a belief that the college social experience can either facilitate or frustrate the development of one’s sense of identity, including ethnic/racial identity (Guiffrida, 2009). Campus discussions and activities around such movements as Black Lives Matter and safe spaces are examples of these processes.

●◗ Figure 10.1 Percentage of high school completers who were enrolled in 2- or 4-year colleges by the October immediately following high school completion, by family income: 1975–2014.

100

80

60

40

20

0 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000

Year

P er

ce n

t

2005 2010 2014

Low income

Middle income

High income

SOURCE: National Center for Education Statistics. Retrieved from nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d15/tables/dt15_302.30.asp and nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d15/tables/dt15_302.20.asp.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

322 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Those changes may well describe the experience of people who go directly from high school to college. But take a look around, literally, at your classmates. In addition to the ethnic and racial diversity you see, the age diversity you also likely experience is a reflec- tion of the changing nature of college and university campuses.

Large numbers of students are in at least their mid-20s or older. For them, going to col- lege isn’t the marker of adulthood. Colleges usually refer to students over age 25 as returning adult students, which implies that these individuals have already reached adulthood.

Overall, returning adult students tend to be problem solvers, self-directed, and prag- matic; may have increased stress due to work-family-school conflict; may prefer having clearer understanding of the pathways from courses to careers; and have relevant life experiences they can integrate with their course work (Cleary et al., 2017; Patton et al., 2016; Swingle, 2013). Balancing employment and families along with their college courses often causes stress, especially early in returning adult students’ academic studies. How- ever, support from family and employers, as well as the positive effects of continuing one’s education, are stress reducers. But many returning adult students, especially middle- aged women, express a sense of self-discovery they had not experienced before (Miles, 2009). The main conclusion here is that going to college impacts students of all ages; for traditional-aged students (ages 18 to 25), it helps foster the transition to adulthood.

As we noted earlier, a key indicator of becoming an adult for many people is establishing financial independence by joining the workforce. For some of the 30% of high school grad- uates who do not go on to college and for some of those who do not finish high school, an intermediary step may include learning a trade such as plumbing, welding, auto mechan- ics, and so forth. In any case, there are many pathways to financial independence: working a series of part-time jobs, finding full-time employment, or starting one’s own business.

The ease with which one can launch one’s career is related to educational attainment. As shown in ◗ Figure 10.2, for each increased educational level beyond high school, the odds of being employed increase. Estimates are that as of 2016, people with some college

returning adult students College students over age 25.

●◗ Figure 10.2 Percent of 20- to 24-year-olds employed, by sex and educational attainment: 2015.

100

P er

ce n

t

90

80

70

Total Males

Sex

Females

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

Total Less than high school completion

High school completion

Some college, no bachelor’s degree

Bachelor’s or higher degree

71

51

67

76

89

75

60

71

80

91

68

42

62

73

88

NOTE: For each group presented, the employment to population ratio, or employment rate, is the number of persons in that group who are employed as a percentage of the civilian population in that group. Data exculde persons enrolled in school. “Some college, no bachelor’s degree” inculdes persons with an associate’s degree. “High school completion” inculdes equivalency credentials, such as the GED credential.

SOURCE: National Center for Education Statistics. (2016). Employment rates of college graduates. Retrieved from nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/ display.asp?id=561.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 323

education obtained 11.5 of the 11.6 million jobs created in the recovery from the Great Recession (Carnevale, Jayasundera, & Gulish, 2016). Clearly, educational attainment is increasingly the pathway to financial independence.

Regardless of when and how it occurs, reaching financial independence is a major achievement and serves as a marker of becoming an adult. There are barriers, though. College graduates who have significant student loan debt may experience delays in achiev- ing financial independence and turn to living with parents to make quicker progress.

Establishing Intimacy According to Erikson, the major task for young adults is dealing with the psychosocial con- flict of intimacy versus isolation. This is the sixth step in Erikson’s theory of psychoso- cial development (see Chapter 1). Once a person’s identity is established, Erikson (1982) believed that he or she is ready to create a shared identity with another—intimacy. With- out a clear sense of identity, Erikson argued, young adults would be afraid of committing to a long-term relationship with another person or might become overly dependent on the partner for his or her identity.

But is this explanation, developed in the mid-20th century, applicable to contempo- rary emerging adults? Research evidence for Erikson’s view is conflicting. For example, a meta-analytic study (see Chapter 1) of the connection between identity and intimacy showed that men and women resolve identity and intimacy issues differently under certain circumstances (Årseth et al., 2009). For example, some women resolve intimacy issues before identity issues by marrying and rearing children, and only later do they deal with the question of their own identity. Middle-aged women who go to college for the first time is an example of this form of identity development (Miles, 2009).

The challenges adults face in their 20s in modern society can be difficult and present challenges for Erikson’s model. Robbins and Wilner (2001) coined the term quarter-life crisis to describe how life in one’s 20s is far from easy as individuals struggle to find their way. Adjusting to the “real world” is increasingly difficult for college graduates who face signifi- cant debt from educational loans, and for all early adults who face challenges from workplace politics, networking, time management, and other daily hassles. Indeed, Byock (2010) and Robinson, Wright, and Smith (2013) argue that a crisis in emerging or early adulthood repre- sents a period of self-exploration and search for meaning. Jung described a similar search for meaning in his original theory of the midlife crisis (a topic we will explore more in Chapter 13). It may be that adults reflect on their life from time to time and seek clarity on its direction.

Robinson (2015) lays out an expanded approach based on both Erikson and mod- ern theory and research about emergent adulthood. Robinson argues that early adulthood should be separated into two distinct stages: emerging adulthood, which pertains to the flux and instability of the 18 to 25 age range, and early adulthood, which is a more settled phase. At the beginning of early adulthood is a normative period of turmoil, referred to as the “quarter-life crisis” (Robinson, 2015; Robinson & Smith, 2010; Robinson & Wright, 2013; Robinson et al., 2013), which was not accounted for in Erikson’s model.

Robinson proposes that the quarter-life crisis can take two forms; the locked-out form (feeling unable to enter adult roles) or the locked-in form (feeling trapped in adult roles). Further, Robinson suggests that the issues in early adulthood be reframed away from Erikson’s intimacy–isolation toward commitment–independence. This extension of Erikson’s theory reflects the longer time it takes more recent generations to traverse the challenges of early adulthood compared to earlier generations. As noted earlier and will be repeated in subsequent chapters, this is reflected in later ages at first marriage and other key life indicators, such as lower home ownership rates.

So When Do People Become Adults? Evidence is clear that the years between late adolescence and the late twenties to early thirties may reflect a distinct life stage researchers call emerging adulthood (Arnett, 2013, 2015). Additional evidence is mounting that social and demographic trends are perhaps

intimacy versus isolation Sixth stage in Erikson’s theory and the major psychosocial task for young adults.

Think About It Why do you think it mattered to Erikson whether identity issues are resolved before intimacy issues?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

324 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

creating two developmental periods between adolescence and middle age, much as simi- lar changing circumstances created the period of adolescence in the early 20th century. All of the evidence points to the conclusion that achieving adulthood takes much lon- ger now than in generations past, and that the psychosocial issues people face are more complex.

As society continues to shed formal rites of passage, especially those connected to religious ceremonies, the exact moment a person becomes an adult will become less and less certain. The perspectives considered in this section do not provide any defini- tive answers to the question of when people become adults. All we can say is that the transition depends on numerous cultural and psychological factors. In cultures without clearly defined rites of passage, defining oneself as an adult rests on one’s perception of whether personally relevant key criteria have been met. In Western society, this can be very complicated—for example, when success comes at a young age, as discussed in the Real People feature. Read the story of Lorde to see how.

recall 1. The most widely used criteria for deciding whether a person

has reached adulthood are _______________.

2. Rituals marking initiation into adulthood are called _______________.

3. Students over 25 are referred to as _______________.

4. Behaviorally, a major difference between adolescence and adulthood is a significant drop in the frequency of _______________.

5. According to Erikson, young adults must resolve the psychosocial conflict of _______________.

Interpret ●● Why are formal rites of passage important? What has Western

society lost by eliminating them? What has it gained?

●● Why is understanding how rites of passage occur in different cultures important?

Apply ●● When do you think people become adults? Why?

●● How do legal definitions of adulthood matter in professions such as law enforcement and health care?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 10.1

Ella Maria Lani Yelich-O’Connor, bet-ter known as Lorde, is a world-famous singer-songwriter from New Zealand. Born November 7, 1996, Lorde’s talent was recog- nized early—she received a contract to work with songwriters at Universal when she was barely in her teens.

Lorde’s leap to world stardom came with the release of her first single, “Royals,” which hit number one in the Billboard Hot 100 in 2013. Her first album, Pure Heroine, was an international best-seller. She had a hit single, “Yellow Flicker Beat” from The Hunger Games: Mockingjay—Part 1 soundtrack. Music critics have already noted her talent by bestowing numerous awards and having her

Ke vi

n W

in te

r/ G

et ty

Im ag

es E

nt er

ta in

m en

t/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

perform at major ceremonies. Her recording and publishing contracts are worth millions of dollars, so she is already wealthy at a young age.

Lorde has also risen to prominence in other ways, too. She has become a sought- after spokesperson on a number of major issues, and her endorsement of other enter- tainers carries significant weight. Her loyal fol- lowing has a great deal of purchasing power, so she has developed important financial influence. There is no question that Lorde is famous and wealthy. The question is whether that is enough to make her an adult. What do you think about that? Is fame and wealth enough?

Real People Applying Human Development

Does Being Rich and Famous Mean You’re an Adult?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 325

Juan is a 25-year-old who started smoking cigarettes in high school to be popular. Juan wants to quit, but he knows it will be difficult. He also heard that it doesn’t really matter if he quits because his health will never recover. Juan wonders whether it is worthwhile to try.

Juan is at the peak of his physical functioning. Most young adults are in the best physical shape of their lives. Indeed, the early 20s are the best years for strenuous work, trouble- free reproduction, and peak athletic performance. But people’s physical functioning is affected by several health-related behaviors, including smoking.

Growth, Strength, and Physical Functioning Physical functioning generally peaks during young adulthood (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013). You’re as tall as you will ever be. Physical strength, coordination, and dexterity in both sexes peaks during the late 20s and early 30s, declining slowly throughout the rest of life even when you maintain an active lifestyle. Because of these trends, few professional ath- letes remain at the top of their sport in their mid-30s. Indeed, individuals such as Brett Favre, who played quarterback in the National Football League into his early 40s, Jaromír Jágr, who played in his mid-40s in the National Hockey League, Diana Nyad, who swam roughly 90 miles from Cuba to Key West at age 64, and Dara Torres, who set the record for the oldest swimmer to win Olympic medals by winning three in Beijing in 2008 at age 41, are famous partly because they are exceptions.

Sensory acuity is also at its peak in the early 20s (Fozard & Gordon-Salant, 2001). Visual acuity remains high until middle age, when people tend to become farsighted and require glasses for reading. Hearing begins to decline somewhat by the late 20s, especially for high-pitched tones.

Lifestyle Factors in Health Because they are so healthy overall, American young adults rarely die from disease (Heron, 2016). So what is the leading cause of death among young adults in the United States? Between the ages of 25 and 44, it’s accidents.

There are important gender and ethnic differences in these statistics. Young adult men aged 25 to 34 are nearly 2.5 times as likely to die as women of the same age. African American and Latino young adult males are 2 to 2.5 times as likely to die as their European American male counterparts, but Asian and Pacific Islander young adult males are likely to die at only about half the rate of their European American male counterparts (Heron, 2016).

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● In what respects are young adults at their physical peak?

●● How healthy are young adults in general?

●● How do smoking, alcohol, and nutrition affect young adults’ health?

●● How does the health of young adults differ as a function of socioeconomic status, ethnicity, and education?

10.2 Physical Development and Health

Dara Torres, shown on the right, won three medals at the Beijing Olympics in 2008 at age 41.

A P

Im ag

es /M

ar k

Ba ke

r

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

326 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

In emerging and young adulthood, three behaviors set the stage for health across the rest of adulthood: smoking, alcohol use, and nutrition. Let’s see how people can lay a good foundation for health.

Smoking Smoking is the single biggest contributor to health problems. In the United States alone, roughly 480,000 people die each year from tobacco use and exposure to secondhand smoke (American Lung Association, 2016), and medical treatment of smoking-related ailments alone costs over $170 billion annually (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016a).

The risks of smoking are many. ◗ Figure 10.3 shows the various forms of cancer and other chronic diseases that are caused by smoking. And smoking during one’s lifetime has a significant negative impact on cognitive functioning in adults over age 50 (Dregan, Stewart, & Gulliford, 2013).

Juan, the young man in the vignette, is typical of people who want to stop smoking. Most people begin the process in young adulthood. More than 90% of those who stop do so on their own. But as Juan suspects, quitting is not easy; most people who try to quit smoking relapse within six months. For most people, success is attained only after a long period of stopping and relapsing.

Regardless of how it happens, quitting smoking has enormous health benefits (Ameri- can Cancer Society, 2016a). For example, in less than a year after quitting, the lungs regain their normal ability to move mucus out. The risks of stroke and coronary heart disease return to normal after a period of roughly 15 years. Even people who do not quit until late life show marked improvements in health. Check out the American Cancer Society’s Guide to Quitting Smoking for key information about how to quit and a quiz about whether you need help to quit.

Drinking Alcohol About 60% of women and 70% of men in the United States drink alcohol at least occa- sionally (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016b). Rates of at least occasional drinking increase with income for both men and women; for instance, for those below the official poverty line, only 56% of men and 43% of women drink at last occasionally.

For the majority of people, drinking alcohol poses no serious health problems as long as they do not drink and drive. Evidence suggests that moderate drinkers (one or two

●◗ Figure 10.3 Smoking can damage every part of the body.

Lung

Leukemia

Stomach

Pancreas Kidney

Colon

Cervix

Bladder

Stroke

Blindness

Gum infection

Aortic rupture

Heart disease

Pneumonia

Hardening of the arteries

Chronic lung disease & asthma

Hip fracture

Reduced fertility

Head or Neck

Cancers Chronic Diseases

SOURCE: Centers for Disease Control. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/tobacco/basic_information/health_effects/ cancer/index.htm.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 327

glasses of beer or wine per day for men, one per day for women) have a 25% to 40% reduc- tion in risk of cardiovascular disease and stroke than either abstainers or heavy drinkers, even after controlling for hypertension, prior heart attack, and other medical conditions (Harvard School of Public Health, 2010). However, moderate drinking also increases the risk for certain types of cancer, so whether moderate drinking is an appropriate health behavior depends on the balance between lowering cardiovascular risk and increasing cancer risk (LeWine, 2013).

For many students, college parties and drinking alcohol are virtually synonymous. Unfortunately, drinking among college students often goes beyond moderate intake to become binge drinking. Binge drinking is defined for men as consuming five or more drinks in a row and for women as consuming four or more drinks in a row within the past two weeks. Binge drinking has been identified as a major health problem in the United States since the 1990s (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism [NIAAA], 2007; Wechsler et al., 1994). Surveys of drinking behavior among young adults aged 18 to 24 show that the rate of binge drinking at about 34% among men and 26% among women (Centers for Dis- ease Control and Prevention, 2016c). Being a college student does not appreciably affect the likelihood of binge drinking overall, but college students tend to drink more at parties and are especially at risk during the first six weeks of freshman year (NIAAA, 2015).

Research indicates that average binge drinking among college students is an international problem (Crawford-Williams, Roberts, & Watts, 2016; Karam, Kypri, & Salamoun, 2007; Kypri et al., 2009), with rates in many countries roughly on par with those in the United States.

Students between the ages of 18 and 24 are more likely than older students to binge drink. They are significantly more likely to binge drink if alcohol is readily available, if they are a member of a social fraternity or sorority, or if they feel positively about what they are doing, which tends to make some people behave rashly (NIAAA, 2015). Women college students are more likely to exceed guidelines of weekly alcohol intake than are college men (Hoeppner et al., 2013). In addition, college men tend to mature out of weekly binge drinking more quickly than do college women. Because women metabolize alcohol differently than men do, it takes less alcohol for women to experience deterioration of judgment and behavior.

Studies of binge drinking, differential metabolism of alcohol in men and women, and the cognitive and behavioral effects of alcohol intoxication set off a firestorm of debate regard- ing whether college women should be educated about the dangers of binge drinking. On one thing, all sides agree: alcohol is a factor in most sexual assaults, especially incapacitated sexual assault, on college campuses (Bird et al., 2016). From there, people take sides. On the one hand, some (e.g., Marcus, 2013; Yoffe, 2013) argue that educating women about poten- tial unwanted and dangerous outcomes from binge drinking is simply a prudent practice. Others (e.g., Adelman, 2013; Ryan, 2013) argue that such approaches constitute “blaming the victim” and that women have every right to engage in binge drinking should they choose to do so and that unwanted outcomes such as sexual assault were the per- petrators’ fault. As we will see, binge drinking has consequences for both the drinker and the people around the drinker, and education programs take these outcomes into account.

Nearly 100,000 college students annually are victims of alcohol- related date rape, about 700,000 are assaulted by a student who has been drinking, and over 1,800 young adult college students aged 18 to 24 die every year in the United States from drinking too much (NIAAA, 2015). Estimates of sexual assault rates are likely to be low due to the tendency of victims not to report incidents. Indeed, this is a major reason why the Obama administration launched a series of major efforts to raise awareness and enforcement of sexual assault prevention on college campuses beginning in 2014. ◗ Figure 10.4 shows the rate of a variety of problems related to binge drinking.

Numerous programs aim to reduce the number of college stu- dents who binge. These efforts include establishing low tolerance lev- els for the antisocial behaviors associated with binge drinking; working with athletes, fraternities, and sororities; changing the expectations of

binge drinking Type of drinking defined for men as con- suming five or more drinks in a row and for women as consuming four or more drinks in a row within the past two weeks.

Binge drinking is viewed by many as a rite of passage in college, but it is an especially troublesome behavior for young adults that can cause academic and social problems.

D is

ob ey

A rt

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

328 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

incoming freshmen; increasing the number of nonalcoholic activities available to students; and working to enforce underage drinking laws (New York Times, 2016; NIAAA, 2015). Education programs about the hazards of binge drinking can be effectively delivered online (O’Rourke, Humphris, & Baldacchino, 2016; Weaver et al., 2014).

The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (2002, 2007, 2015) has offered several strategies that focus on all these issues. Evidence is growing that programs based on social norms and the involvement of students themselves are key and underlie the most effective strategies to reduce risky behavior both in the United States (Miller & Prentice, 2016) and around the world (McAlaney, Bewick, & Hughes, 2011). The social norms approach focuses on changing the culture of drinking in college from one that strongly supports binge drinking to one in which binge drinking is something that popular people do not do. This approach is based on the idea that many college students think that their peers’ attitudes toward drinking are more permissive than they really are (McAlaney et al., 2011; Miller & Prentice, 2016; NIAAA, 2007).

For most young adults, binge drinking declines across one’s 20s. However, in the United States, nearly 8% of adults 18 to 24 years old and 5% of those aged 25 to 44 are considered heavy drinkers and likely candidates to develop alcohol use disorder (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016c). Significantly more men than women are at risk. Rates are also higher for European Americans and Native Americans than for other ethnic groups.

What constitutes alcohol use disorder? Alcohol use disorder is a drinking pattern that results in significant and recurrent consequences that reflect loss of reliable control over alcohol use. Alcohol use disorder is diagnosed whenever anyone meets any two of the 11 criteria listed in ◗ Table 10.1 during a 12-month period.

alcohol use disorder Drinking pattern that results in significant and recurrent consequences that reflect loss of reliable control over alcohol use.

●◗ Figure 10.4 Troublesome behaviors increase with binge drinking. Note that all binge drinkers report more problems than non–binge drinkers.

0 20 40 Percentage of students reporting a problem

60 80 100

Have a hangover

Do something you regret

Miss a class

Get behind in school work

Argue with friends

Engage in unplanned sexual activity

Get hurt or injured

Damage property

Have sex without protection Frequent binge drinkers

Infrequent binge drinkers

Non–binge drinkers Get into trouble with police

Have blackouts

SOURCE: Adapted from a table in Wechsler et al. (2002). Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibrung, M., Nelson, T. F., & Lee, H. (2002). Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts. Journal of American College Health, 2002, 203–217).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 329

Neuroscience research has discovered that alcohol “does a number” on our brain, especially in disrupting the balance in neurotransmitters (Bjork & Gilman, 2014; Lovinger & Roberto, 2013) and impairing neuroplasticity (Loheswaran et al., 2016). These neurotransmitters include gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), which inhibits impulsive- ness; glutamate, which excites the nervous system; norepinephrine, which is released in response to stress; and dopamine, serotonin, and opioid peptides, which are responsible for pleasurable feelings. Excessive long-term drinking can cause the body to crave alcohol to restore good feelings or to avoid negative feelings. In addition, other factors come into play: genetics; high stress, anxiety, or emotional pain; close friends or partners who drink excessively; and sociocultural factors that glorify alcohol.

Treatment for alcohol use disorder focuses on three goals (NIAAA, 2014): stabili- zation and reduction of substance consumption, treatment of coexisting problems, and arrangement of appropriate social interventions. Three main options for treatment are available: mutual-support groups, behavioral treatments, and medications.

Mutual-support groups are the most widely known and include Alcoholics Anony- mous (AA) and other 12-step groups. These groups provide peer support to help people stop abusing various drugs and alcohol. Behavioral treatments rely on individual or group counseling to stop or reduce drinking or drugging.

The U.S. Food and Drug Administration has approved three types of medications for use in treating alcohol use disorder (NIAAA, 2014):

●● Disulfiram (Antabuse) is used to block alcohol metabolism, and causes very unpleas- ant symptoms such as nausea to help people stop drinking. Disulfiram has been used for nearly a century, but some people stop taking it to stop the unpleasant effects.

Diagnosis of alcohol use disorder (AUD) based on DSM-5

SymptomS

In the past year, have you: ●● Had times when you ended up drinking more, or longer, than you intended? ●● More than once wanted to cut down or stop drinking, or tried to, but couldn’t? ●● Spent a lot of time drinking? Or being sick or getting over other aftereffects? ●● Wanted a drink so badly you couldn’t think of anything else? ●● Found that drinking—or being sick from drinking—often interfered with taking care of your home or family? Or caused job troubles? Or

school problems? ●● Continued to drink even though it was causing trouble with your family or friends? ●● Given up or cut back on activities that were important or interesting to you, or gave you pleasure, in order to drink? ●● More than once gotten into situations while or after drinking that increased your chances of getting hurt (such as driving, swimming, using

machinery, walking in a dangerous area, or having unsafe sex)? ●● Continued to drink even though it was making you feel depressed or anxious or adding to another health problem? Or after having had a

memory blackout? ●● Had to drink much more than you once did to get the effect you want? Or found that your usual number of drinks had much less effect

than before? ●● Found that when the effects of alcohol were wearing off, you had withdrawal symptoms, such as trouble sleeping, shakiness, restlessness,

nausea, sweating, a racing heart, or a seizure? Or sensed things that were not there?

Severity

The severity of the AUD is defined as: ●● mild The presence of 2 to 3 symptoms ●● moderate The presence of 4 to 5 symptoms ●● Severe The presence of 6 or more symptoms

Table 10.1

Source: Based on NIH Publication 13-7999. (2016). Alcohol Use Disorder: A Comparison Between DSM-IV and DSM-5. National Institutes of Health (NIH), National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Available at pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/dsmfactsheet/dsmfact.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

330 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

low-density lipoproteins (LDLs) Chemicals that cause fatty deposits to accu- mulate in arteries, impeding blood flow.

high-density lipoproteins (HDLs) Chemicals that help keep arteries clear and break down LDLs.

●● Naltrexone reduces the pleasure received from drinking and reduces the cravings that compel chronic drinking by blocking the endorphin receptors in the brain. Research evidence is mostly positive on the effectiveness of naltrexone, especially when com- bined with other treatments.

●● Acamprosate (Campral) reduces the unpleasant symptoms experienced from alcohol withdrawal by stabilizing the neurotransmitters in the brain. Research results on the effectiveness of acamprosate is mixed.

Perhaps the most important thing with people who have alcohol use disorder is to encourage them to get treatment. Unfortunately, only a minority of people who need such treatment actually get it.

Nutrition Once on a vacation trip, one of us (John Cavanaugh) took this photo out- side a restaurant in Megalochori, Greece. It captures the essence of how you should approach diet as part of healthy living.

The photo may remind you of disagreements with parents (or now with your own children) about food. As an adult yourself, you may now realize those lima beans and other despised foods your parents urged you to eat really are healthy.

Experts agree nutrition directly affects one’s mental, emotional, and physi- cal functioning (Hammar & Östgren, 2013; McKee & Schüz, 2015). Diet has been linked to cancer, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, anemia, and digestive disorders. To stay maximally healthy, though, we must recognize that nutritional requirements and eating habits change across the life span. This change is due mainly to differences in metabolism, or how much energy the body needs. Body metabolism and the digestive process slow down with age (Janssen, 2005).

The U.S. Department of Agriculture publishes dietary guidelines based on current research. In its Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2015–2020 (Health.gov, 2015), the USDA recommends we eat a variety of nutrient-dense foods and beverages across the basic food groups. The general guidelines for adults can be seen in ◗ Figure 10.5.

As you can see, the USDA approaches nutrition from the perspective of ensuring people eat a healthy plate of food at each meal, and the contents of that plate be appropriately balanced. Most important, we should choose foods that limit the intake of added sugar, saturated fats, sodium, and alco- hol. And we need to keep our target calorie intake in mind. The full report contains much more detail about the specific foods that are best for you.

Of course, most people do not eat perfectly all the time. From time to time, each of us craves something—whether a triple-dip cone of premium ice cream or really high-end chocolate. If you feel even a tiny bit guilty after you enjoy that splurge, you are among the people who have taken to heart (literally) the link between diet and cardiovascular disease. The American Heart Association (2016a) makes it clear foods high in saturated fat (such as our beloved ice cream) should be replaced with foods low in fat (such as fat-free frozen yogurt). Check out their website for the latest in advice on eating a heart-healthy diet.

Healthy aging and eating is also the focus of minority communities. For example, the American Heart Association has partnered with Native American tribes to create the Seeds of Native Health campaign for indigenous nutrition as a way to connect traditional native foods to healthy eating and healthy aging (American Heart Association, 2016b).

Much of the focus of the American Heart Association’s various guidelines and initia- tives is to lower the risk of cardiovascular disease. To achieve this goal, it is important to understand an important difference between two different types of lipoproteins reflected in different types of cholesterol. Lipoproteins are fatty chemicals attached to proteins car- ried in the blood. Low-density lipoproteins (LDLs) cause fatty deposits to accumulate in arteries, impeding blood flow, whereas high-density lipoproteins (HDLs) help keep arteries clear and break down LDLs. It is not so much the overall cholesterol number but the ratio

metabolism How much energy the body needs.

Jo hn

C . C

av an

au gh

Eating a healthy diet is an important part of preventing disease.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 331

of LDLs to HDLs that matters most in cholesterol screening. High levels of LDLs are a risk factor in cardiovascular disease, and high levels of HDLs are considered a protective factor. Reducing LDL levels is effective in diminishing the risk of cardiovascular disease in adults of all ages; in healthy adults, a high level of LDL (over 160 mg/dL) is associated with higher risk for cardiovascular disease (Mayo Clinic, 2016). In contrast, higher levels of HDL are good (in healthy adults, levels at least above 40 mg/dL for men and 50 mg/dL for women). LDL levels can be lowered and HDL levels can be raised through various interventions such as exercise and a high-fiber diet. Weight control is also an important component.

If diet and exercise are not effective in lowering cholesterol, numerous medications exist for treating cholesterol problems. The most popular of these drugs are from a family of medi- cations called statins (e.g., Lipitor, Crestor). These medications lower LDL and moderately increase HDL. Before prescribing statins, healthcare professionals also assess a person’s risk of

●◗ Figure 10.5 Key Recommendations from Dietary Guidelines 2015–2020.

SOURCE: health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/resources/2015-2020_Dietary_Guidelines.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

332 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

cardiovascular disease from family history and lifestyle, among other factors. If statins are prescribed, their potential side effects on liver functioning should be monitored, and patients should consult with their physicians on a regular basis.

Obesity is a serious and growing health problem related to diet. One good way to assess your own status is to compute your body mass index. Body mass index (BMI) is a ratio of body weight and height and is related to total body fat. You can compute BMI as follows:

BMI = w/h2

where w = weight in kilograms (or weight in pounds divided by 2.2), and h = height in meters (or inches divided by 39.37).

The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2016c) defines healthy weight as having a BMI of less than 25. However, this calculation may overestimate body fat in muscular athletic people (e.g., professional athletes) and underestimate body fat in those who appear of normal weight but have little muscle mass.

Obesity is related to the risk of serious medical conditions and mortality: the higher one’s BMI, the higher one’s risk (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016d). ◗ Figure 10.6 shows the increased risk for several diseases and mortality associated with increased BMI (and obesity).

In the United States, obesity rates have increased very significantly since the 1970s. Today, nearly half of African Americans and over 40% of Latinos are obese (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016d). Across all racial and ethnic groups, over one- third of older adults are obese. Reductions in these statistics is a primary focus of the fed- eral government’s nutrition and exercise guidelines.

Based on these data, you may want to lower your BMI if it’s above 25. But be careful— lowering your BMI too much (below 18.5) may not be healthy either. Very low BMIs may indicate malnutrition, which is also related to increased mortality.

Socioeconomic, Ethnic, and Education Issues in Health The three most important social influences on health are socioeconomic status (which is a strong predictor having access to insurance and good health care), ethnicity, and educa- tion (a good predictor of being able to access a wider array of healthy living options and avoid certain diseases). In the United States, being poor in any setting is a major predictor of health challenges.

Disparities in access to quality health care and to healthy lifestyle choices (e.g., fresh food) in the United States as related to ethnicity has been documented for decades, and the

National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities is charged with conduct- ing programs and research to address these differences (National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, 2016). Even when poor minorities in the United States have access to health care, they are less likely than European Americans to receive treatment for chronic disease (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2016). Such socioeconomic differences also hold interna- tionally (Huisman et al., 2013).

Educational level also matters. The risk of dying increases as educational level decreases (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016e; Huisman et al., 2013). Quantity and

body mass index (BMI) Ratio of body weight and height related to total body fat.

●◗ Figure 10.6 Classification of overweight and obesity by BMI, waist circumference, and associated disease risks.

Disease Risk* Relative to Normal Weight and Waist Circumference

< 18.5

18.5–24.9

25.0–29.9

30.0–34.9

35.0–39.9

40.0†

Increased

High

Very High

Extremely High

High

Very High

Very High

Extremely High

Underweight

* Disease risk for type 2 diabetes, hypertension, and CVD. † Increased waist circumference can also be a marker for increased risk even in persons of normal weight.

BMI (kg/m2)

Men 102 cm (40 in) or less

Women 88 cm (35 in) or less

Men > 102 cm (40 in)

Women > 88 cm (35 in)

Normal

Overweight

Obesity

Extreme obesity

SOURCE: www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health/public/heart/obesity/lose_wt/ bmi_dis.htm.

Many inner-city residents must rely on overcrowded clinics for their primary health care.

To m

C ar

te r/

Ph ot

oE di

t

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 333

As noted in the text, access to quality health care in the United States depends a great deal on socioeconomic status, eth- nicity, and education. As a researcher, an important question to ask is whether access to quality health care is improving, thereby reducing healthcare disparities in the United States. The answer, sadly, is no.

As described in the annual National Healthcare Disparities Report (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2016) and in the annual report on the overall health in the U.S. (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2016e), many Americans,

especially Americans of color, do not get the care they need, and the care they do get may actually cause them harm. Despite some improvements in the quality of care overall and the access to quality care for certain groups who were formerly uninsured, many aspects of health disparities across demographic groups are not improving. In particular, the most trou- bling disparities apply to care for diabetes, maternal and pediatric care, and cancer care.

The most important factor in these disparities is financial status. As shown in ◗ Figure 10.7, those in poor income groups received care that was worse than people

in high income groups on 60% of the quality indicators studied.

These disparities in health care mean that millions of Americans do not get the care they need despite the United States having the best health care in the world. This and other similar disparities reflect the fact that these individuals are usually much sicker by the time they get care, meaning that it is more difficult and expensive to treat them. Although its implementation was problematic, a goal of the Affordable Care Act was to provide more people with access to quality health care that is affordable. Whether that goal can be reached fully, however, remains to be seen.

Healthcare Disparities in the United StatesWhat Do You Think?

●◗ Figure 10.7 Number and percentage of quality measures for which members of selected groups experienced better, same, or worse quality of care compared with reference group.

100%

80%

60%

40%

20%

0%

Better WorseSame

Poor vs. High Income (n = 147)

Hispanic vs. White (n = 210)

Black vs. White (n = 248)

Asian vs. White (n = 217)

AI/AN vs. White (n = 142)

88

77 102

44 46

78

18

102

71

116

30

89

44

52

7

KEY: AI/AN = American Indian or Alaska Native; n = number of measures. NOTE: Numbers of measures dif fer across groups because of sample size limitations. The relative dif ference between a selected group and its reference group is used to assess disparities. For income, the reference group is High income. For race and ethnicity, the reference group is White. • Better = Population received better quality of care than reference group. Dif ferences are statistically signif icant, are equal to or larger than 10%, and favor the selected group. • Same = Population and reference group received about the same quality of care. Dif ferences are not statistically signif icant or are smaller than 10%. • Worse = Population received worse quality of care than reference group. Dif ferences are statistically signif icant, equal to or larger than 10%, and favor the reference group.

SOURCE: Figure on p. 11 from www.ahrq.gov/sites/default/files/ wysiwyg/research/findings/nhqrdr/nhqdr15/2015nhqdr.pdf.

quality of care as well as outcomes are also poorer with lower educational levels when looking at specific conditions such as rheumatoid arthritis (McCollum & Pincus, 2009).

Does socioeconomic status, ethnicity, or education cause good (or poor) health? Not exactly. As we discussed in Chapter 1, correlation research does not address cause and effect. In this case, higher educational level is also associated with higher income and with more awareness of dietary and lifestyle influences on health. Thus, more highly educated people are in a better position to afford health care and to know about the kinds of foods and lifestyle that affect health. The discussion of these issues continues in the What Do You Think? feature.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

334 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

multidirectionality Developmental pattern in which some aspects of intelligence improve and other aspects decline during adulthood.

interindividual variability Patterns of change that vary from one person to another.

recall 1. In young adulthood, most people reach their maximum

_______________.

2. Sensory acuity peaks during a person’s _______________.

3. During the early 20s, death from disease is _______________.

4. Young adults are most likely to die from _______________.

5. _______________ is the biggest contributor to health problems.

6. In terms of alcohol drinking patterns, _______________ peaks in emerging adulthood.

7. The two most important social influences on health are education and _______________.

Interpret ●● How would you design a healthcare system that provides

strong incentives for young adults leading healthy lifestyles?

Apply ●● In your job as a nutritionist, you have been asked to design a

comprehensive educational campaign to promote healthy life- styles. What information would you include?

●● As a nurse, what information besides basic health status would you want to know from a new patient?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 10.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is intelligence in adulthood?

●● What are primary and secondary mental abilities? How do they change?

●● What are fluid and crystallized intelligence? How do they change?

●● How has neuroscience research furthered our under- standing of intelligence in adulthood?

●● What is postformal thought? How does it differ from formal operations?

●● How do emotion and logic become integrated in adult- hood? How do emotional intelligence and impression formation demonstrate this integration?

10.3 Cognitive Development

Susan, a 33-year-old woman recently laid off from her job as a secretary, slides into a seat on her first day of classes at the community college. She is clearly nervous. “I’m worried that I won’t be able to compete with these younger students—that I may not be smart enough,” she sighs. “Guess we’ll find out soon enough, huh?”

Many returning adult students such as Susan worry that they may not be “smart enough” to keep up with 18- or 19-year-olds. Are these fears realistic? We will see how the answer to this question depends on the types of intellectual skills being used.

How Should We View Intelligence in Adults? Take a sheet of paper and write down all the abilities that you think reflect intelligence in adults. It’s a safe bet that you listed more than one ability. You are not alone. Most theories of intelligence are multidimensional—that is, they identify several types of intellectual abilities.

Sternberg (1985, 2008; Sternberg, Jarvin, & Grigorenko, 2009) emphasized multidi- mensionality in his theory of successful intelligence (also discussed in Chapter 6). Based on the life-span perspective (described in Chapter 1), Baltes and colleagues (Baltes et al., 2006) introduced three other concepts as being vital to intellectual development in adults: multidirectionality, interindividual variability, and plasticity.

Over time, the various abilities underlying adults’ intelligence show multidirectionality: Some aspects of intelligence improve and other aspects decline during adulthood. Closely related to this is interindividual variability: These patterns of change also vary from one

multidimensional Characteristic of theories of intelligence that identify several types of intellectual abilities.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 335

person to another. Finally, people’s abilities reflect plasticity: They are not fixed, but can be modified under the right conditions at just about any point in adulthood. Because most research on plasticity has focused on older adults, we return to this topic in Chapter 14. In general, Baltes and colleagues emphasize that intelligence has many components and that these components show varying development in different abilities and different people.

Given that intelligence in adults is complex and multifaceted, how might we study it? Two common ways are formal testing and assessing practical problem-solving skills. Formal testing assesses a wide range of abilities and involves tests from which we can compute overall IQ scores such as those discussed in Chapter 6. Tests of practical problem solving assess people’s ability to apply intellectual skills to everyday situations. Let’s see how each approach describes intellectual development.

The psychometric approach focuses on the interrelationships among intellectual abil- ities, so a major goal has long been to describe the ways these relationships are organized (Sternberg, 1985). This organization of interrelated intellectual abilities is termed the structure of intelligence. The most common way to describe the structure of intelligence is to think of it as a five-level hierarchy, depicted in ◗ Figure 10.8 (Cunningham, 1987).

Each higher level of this hierarchy represents a higher level of organizing the compo- nents of the level below. The lowest level consists of individual test questions—the specific items people answer on an intelligence test. These items or questions can be organized into intelligence tests, which constitute the second level.

The third level reflects interrelationships among scores on intelligence tests that assess similar abilities; these clusters of abilities are called primary mental abilities. Continuing to move up the hierarchy, the interrelationships existing among the primary mental abili- ties produce the secondary mental abilities at the fourth level. Finally, general intelligence at the top refers to the interrelationships among the secondary mental abilities.

Keep in mind that each time we move up the hierarchy we move away from people’s actual performance. Each level above the first represents a theoretical description of how things fit together. Thus, there are no tests of primary abilities per se; primary abilities represent theoretical relationships among tests, that in turn represent theoretical relation- ships among actual performances.

So exactly how do researchers construct this theoretical hierarchy? The structure of intelligence is uncovered through sophisticated statistical detective work using a technique called factor analysis. First, researchers obtain people’s performances on many types of problems. Second, the results are examined to determine whether performance on one type of problem, such as filling in missing letters in a word, predicts performance on another type of problem, like unscrambling letters to form a word. If the performance on one test is highly related to the performance on another, the abilities measured by the two tests are interrelated and are called a factor.

Most psychometric theorists believe intelligence consists of several factors. However, we should note although factor analysis is a sophisticated statistical technique, it is not an exact technique. Thus, estimates of the exact number of factors vary from a few to over 100. Most researchers and theorists believe the number to be relatively small. We examine two factors: primary and secondary mental abilities.

Primary and Secondary Mental Abilities Since the 1930s, researchers agreed intel- lectual abilities can be studied as groups of related skills (such as memory or spatial ability) organized into hypothetical con- structs called primary mental abilities. In turn, related groups of primary mental abilities can be clustered into a half dozen or so broader skills termed secondary mental abilities.

plasticity Concept that intellectual abilities are not fixed but can be modified under the right conditions at just about any point in adulthood.

structure of intelligence The organization of interrelated intellectual abilities.

factor The interrelated abilities measured by two tests if the performance on one test is highly related to the performance on another.

primary mental abilities Groups of related intellectual skills (such as memory or spatial ability).

secondary mental abilities Broader intellectual skills that subsume and organize the primary abilities.

●◗ Figure 10.8 Secondary mental abilities reflect sev- eral primary mental abilities and their respective measurements. This figure shows those relations regarding crys- tallized intelligence.

Primary mental ability (e.g., verbal comprehension)

Vocabulary Similarities Social

translations Social situations

Secondary mental ability (e.g., crystallized intelligence)

Primary mental ability (e.g., experiential evaluation)

© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

336 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Roughly 25 primary mental abilities have been identified (Horn, 1982). Because it is difficult to study all of them, researchers focused on five representative ones:

●● Number: the basic skills underlying our mathematical reasoning ●● Word fluency: how easily we produce verbal descriptions of things ●● Verbal meaning: our vocabulary ability ●● Inductive reasoning: our ability to extrapolate from particular facts to general concepts ●● Spatial orientation: our ability to reason in the three-dimensional world

Even with a relatively small number of primary mental abilities, it is still hard to discuss intelligence by focusing on separate abilities. As a result, theories of intelligence emphasize clusters of related primary mental abilities as a framework for describing the structure of intelligence. Because they are one step removed from primary mental abili- ties, secondary mental abilities are not measured directly.

The Spotlight on Research feature describes the definitive developmental study of pri- mary mental abilities.

On ResearchSpotlight

The Seattle Longitudinal Study

Who was the investigator, and what was the aim of the study? To provide a more thorough picture of intellectual change, K. Warner Schaie began the Seattle Longitu- dinal Study in 1956.

How did the investigator measure the topic of interest? Schaie used standardized tests of primary mental abilities to assess a wide range of abilities such as logical reason- ing and spatial ability.

Who were the participants in the study? Over the course of the study, more than 5,000 individuals were tested at eight different times (1956, 1963, 1970, 1977, 1984, 1991, 1998, and 2005). The participants were representa- tive of the upper 75% of the socioeconomic spectrum and were recruited through a large health maintenance organization in Seattle.

What was the design of the study? Schaie invented a new type of research design—the sequential design (see Chapter 1). Partici- pants were tested every seven years.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? Because people’s names must be retained for future contact, the researchers were careful about keeping personal informa- tion secure.

What were the results? Among the many important findings from the study are differen- tial changes in abilities over time and cohort effects. As you can see in ◗ Figure 10.9, scores on tests of primary mental abilities improve gradually until the late 30s or early 40s. Small declines begin in the 50s, increase as people age into their 60s, and become

increasingly large in the 70s (Schaie & Zanjani, 2006).

Cohort differences were also found. ◗ Figure 10.10 shows that on some skills (e.g., inductive reasoning ability) but not others, more recently born younger and middle-aged

cohorts performed better than cohorts born earlier. These cohort effects probably reflect differences in educational experiences.

Schaie uncovered many individual differ- ences as well; some people showed devel- opmental patterns closely approximating the

●◗ Figure 10.9 Longitudinal estimates of age changes on observed measures of five primary mental abilities.

Word fluency

Number

Inductive reasoning

Spatial orientation

Verbal meaning

Total Sample

60

55

50

45

40

35

25 32 39 46 53 60 67 74 81 88

Age (years)

M ea

n T

-S co

re s

SOURCE: Based on Intellectual development across adulthood by Warner Schaie, K., and Zanjani, Faika A. K., in Handbook of Adult Development and Learning, ed. by Hoare, C., p. 102.

[Continued on next page]

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 337

●◗ Figure 10.10 Cohort gradients showing cumulative cohort difference on five primary mental abilities for cohorts born in 1889 to 1973.

15

10

5

0

–5 1889 19031896 1907 1910 1917 1924 1931 1938 1945 1952

Cohort (year of birth)

C u

m u

la ti

ve M

ea n

T -S

co re

D iff

er en

ce s

1959 1966 1973

Word fluency

Intellectual ability

Number

Inductive reasoning

Spatial orientation

Verbal meaning

SOURCE: Based on Intellectual development across adulthood by Warner Schaie, K., and Zanjani, Faika A. K., in Handbook of Adult Development and Learning, ed. by Hoare, C., p. 102.

overall trends, but others showed unusual patterns. For example, some individu- als showed steady declines in most abili- ties beginning in their 40s and 50s, others showed declines in some abilities but not oth- ers, and some people showed little change in most abilities over a 14-year period.

What did the investigator conclude? Three points are clear. First, intellectual development during adulthood is marked by a gradual leveling off of gains between young adult- hood and middle age followed by a period of relative stability and then a time of gradual decline in most abilities. Second, these trends vary from one cohort to another. Third, indi- vidual patterns of change vary considerably from person to person.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Although Schaie’s study is one of the most compre- hensive ever conducted, it is limited. Studying people who live in different locations around the world would provide evidence as to whether the results are limited geographically. Additional cross-cultural evidence comparing people with different economic backgrounds and different access to health care would also provide insight into the effects of these vari- ables on intellectual development.

Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence As noted earlier, primary abilities are themselves organized into clusters of second- ary mental abilities. A summary of the major secondary mental abilities is presented in ◗ Table 10.2. Two secondary mental abilities have received a great deal of attention in adult developmental research: fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence (Horn, 1982).

Fluid intelligence consists of the abilities that make you a flexible and adaptive thinker, allow you to make inferences, and enable you to understand the relations among concepts. It includes the abilities you need to understand and respond to any situation, but espe- cially new ones: inductive reasoning, integration, abstract thinking, and the like (Horn, 1982). An example of a question that taps fluid abilities is the following: What letter comes next in the series d f i m r x e?1

Crystallized intelligence is the knowledge you have acquired through life experience and education in a particular culture. Crystallized intelligence includes your breadth of knowledge, comprehension of communication, judgment, and sophistication with infor- mation (Horn, 1982). Many popular television game shows (such as Jeopardy and Wheel of Fortune) are based on contestants’ accumulated crystallized intelligence.

Developmentally, fluid and crystallized intelligence follow two different paths, as you can see in ◗ Figure 10.11. Notice that fluid intelligence declines throughout adulthood, whereas crystallized intelligence improves. Although we do not yet fully understand why fluid intelligence declines, it likely is related to underlying changes in the brain (see Chap- ter 2). In contrast, the increase in crystallized intelligence (at least until late life) indicates people continue adding knowledge every day.

1The next letter is m. The rule is to increase the difference between adjacent letters in the series by one each time and use a continuous circle of the alphabet for counting. Thus, f is two letters from d, i is three letters from f, and e is seven letters from x.

fluid intelligence Abilities that make you a flexible and adap- tive thinker, allow you to make inferences, and enable you to understand the relations among concepts.

crystallized intelligence The knowledge you have acquired through life experience and education in a particular culture.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

338 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

CRYSTALLIZED INTELLIGENCE (Gc)

Crystallized intelligence reflects the breadth of knowledge and experience, understanding com- munications and social conventions, judgment, and reason. The components of Gc include the primary abilities of verbal comprehension, concept formation, logical reasoning, and general reasoning, among others. Tests of Gc include vocabulary (What does timid mean?) and analo- gies (Plato is to Kant as Shakespeare is to _______________ ), among others. Gc is a rough estimate of the knowledge and sophistication that underlies the intelligence of a culture.

FLUID INTELLIGENCE (Gf )

Fluid intelligence reflects the abilities to see relationships among patterns, draw inferences from relationships, and comprehend the implications of relationships. The primary abilities under- lying Gf include inductive reasoning, figural flexibility, and integration, among others. Tests of Gf include letter series (What comes next in the series d f i m r x e?), matrices (Identify the relationships among elements in a 232 matrix.), and shapes (From among a set of overlapping circles, squares, and triangles, choose a figure that enables you to place a dot inside a circle and triangle but outside of a square.) Gf provides a rough estimate of a person’s problem solving and abstracting ability. It does not reflect cultural learning.

VISUAL ORGANIZATION (Gv)

Visual organization reflects the underlying primary abilities: visualization, spatial orientation, speed and flexibility of closure, among others. Gv is tested through holistic closure (Identify a figure that has missing parts.), form board (Put cutout parts together to create and match a specific figure.), and embedded figures (Find the duck in a complex visual image.). Gv differs from Gf in that Gv reflects relationships among visual patterns that are obvious, and not inferred (which would reflect Gf ).

AUDITORY ORGANIZATION (Ga)

Auditory organization reflects underlying primary abilities such as time tracking, auditory cognition of relations, and speech perception when the speech is degraded or distorted, among others. Tests of Ga include repeated tones (Identify the first occurrence of a tone that is played several times.), tonal series (Indicate which tone comes next in a specific series of tones.), and word identification in noise (Identify a specific word when it is embedded in a noisy back- ground environment.), among others.

SHORT-TERM ACQUISITION AND RETRIEVAL

This ability reflects the ability to be aware of and retain information long enough to do some- thing with it. The underlying primary abilities all reflect aspects of short-term memory. Tests of Gstar include span memory (repeat increasingly long lists of numbers or words.), associative memory (remember word pairs of related words), among others.

LONG-TERM STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL

This type of intelligence reflects the ability to store information and retrieve information that was acquired in the distant past.

Source: Horn, J. L. (1982). The aging of human abilities. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of Developmental Psychology (pp. 847–870). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Reprinted with permission.

Descriptions of Major Secondary Mental Abilities Table 10.2

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 339

●◗ Figure 10.11 Note the opposite developmental patterns for fluid and crystallized intelligence.

General IQ tests

Tests of fluid intelligence

Tests of crystallized intelligence

High

Low 14–17 18–20 21–28 29–39 40–61

Age (years)

S co

re

SOURCE: Based on Organization of data on life-span development of human abili- ties by Horn, J. L. in Goulet, L. R. and Baltes, P. B. (Eds.), Life-Span Developmental Psychology: Research and Theory, p. 463.

●◗ Figure 10.12 Brain imaging research indicates that active connections between the pari- etal and frontal lobes, shown in the colored areas here, are key to under- standing intelligence.

What do these different developmental trends imply? First, they indicate that— although it continues through adulthood—performance or learning that depends on basic underlying skills becomes more difficult with age, whereas performance or learning that is based on what we already know continues to improve, at least until very late in life.

Second, intellectual development varies a great deal from one set of skills to another. Whereas individual differences in fluid intelligence remain relatively uniform over time, individual differences in crystallized intelligence increase with age, mainly because main- taining crystallized intelligence depends on being in situations that require its use (Horn, 1982; Horn & Hofer, 1992). For example, few adults get much practice in solv- ing complex letter series tasks like the one on page 23, so individual differences tend to be minimal. But because people improve their vocabulary skills by reading and they vary considerably in how much they read, individual differences are likely to increase.

Neuroscience Research and Intelligence Considerable research shows specific areas in the brain are associated with intellectual abilities, and developmental changes in these areas are related to changes in performance.

Research now shows that the prefrontal cortex, along with the parietal lobe (an area of the brain at the top of the head), plays an important role in general intel- lectual abilities. Based on 37 studies using various types of neuroimaging tech- niques, Jung and Haier (2007) proposed the parieto-frontal integration theory. The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) proposes that intelligence comes from a distributed and integrated network of neurons in the parietal and frontal areas of the brain. ◗ Figure 10.12. Shows these key brain areas. In general, P-FIT accounts for individual differences in intelligence as having their origins in individual differ- ences in brain structure and function. For example, research indicates that the P-FIT predicts fluid intelligence (Nikolaidis et al., in press; Pineda-Pardo et al., 2016).

In general, P-FIT accounts for individual differences in intelligence as having their ori- gins in individual differences in brain structure and function. The P-FIT model has been tested in several studies. Results indicate support for the theory when measures of fluid

parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) Theory that proposes that intelligence comes from a distributed and integrated network of neurons in the parietal and frontal lobes of the brain.

Parietal cortex

Frontal cortex ©

2 01

9 C

en ga

ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

340 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

and crystallized intelligence are related to brain structures (Basten, Hilger, & Fiebach, 2015; Brancucci, 2012; Pineda-Pardo et al., 2016). It is also clear performance on measures of spe- cific abilities is likely related to specific combinations of brain structures (Di Domenico et al., 2015; Haier et al., 2010; Kievit et al., 2016; Pineda-Pardo et al., 2016).

A second theory of intelligence based on neuroscience evidence is based on how effi- ciently the brain works (Brancucci, 2012; Di Domenico et al., 2015; Kievit et al., 2016; Langer et al., 2012). The neural efficiency hypothesis states intelligent people process information more efficiently, showing weaker neural activations in a smaller number of areas than less intelligent people. Research evidence is mounting that this idea holds merit, and with greater intelligence does come demonstrably increased efficiency in neural pro- cessing (e.g., Kievit et al., 2016; Langer et al., 2012; Lipp et al., 2012; Pineda-Pardo et al., 2016). However, how this neural efficiency develops is not yet known, nor are its develop- mental pathways understood.

It is clear neuroscience and related research on intelligence will continue to provide many insights into the bases for both the development of fluid and crystallized intelligence as well as understanding individual differences in each. As neuroimaging and other tech- niques continue to improve, it is likely that our understanding of both the brain structure- intelligence relations as well as their development will improve.

Going Beyond Formal Operations: Thinking in Adulthood Suppose you are faced with the following dilemma:

You are a member of your college’s or university’s student judicial board and are cur- rently hearing a case involving plagiarism. The student handbook states plagiarism is a serious offense resulting in expulsion. The student accused of plagiarizing a paper admits copying from Wikipedia but says she has never been told she needed to use a formal citation and quotation marks. Do you vote to expel the student?

When this and similar problems are presented to older adolescents and young adults, interesting differences emerge. Adolescents tend to approach the problem in formal- operational terms and point out the student handbook is clear and the student ignored it, concluding the student should be expelled. Formal-operational thinkers are certain such solutions are right because they are based on their own experience and are logically driven.

But many adults are reluctant to draw conclusions based on the limited information in the problem, especially when the problem can be interpreted in different ways (Commons, 2016). They point out there is much about the student we don’t know: Has she ever been taught the proper procedure for using sources? Was the faculty member clear about what plagiarism is? For adults, the problem is more ambiguous. Adults may eventually decide

neural efficiency hypothesis States intelligent people process informa- tion more efficiently, showing weaker neu- ral activations in a smaller number of areas than less intelligent people.

Th e

W as

hi ng

to n

Po st

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Because people using the initial stages of reflective judgment perceive commentators such as Megyn Kelly as authority figures, they believe that what the commentators say must be true.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 341

the student is (or is not) expelled, but they do so only after considering aspects of the situ- ation that go well beyond the information given in the problem.

Based on numerous investigations, researchers concluded this different type of thinking represents a qualitative change beyond formal operations (King & Kitchener, 2015; Lemieux, 2012; Sinnott, 2014). Postformal thought is characterized by recognition that truth (the cor- rect answer) may vary from situation to situation, solutions must be realistic to be reasonable, ambiguity and contradiction are the rule rather than the exception, and emotion and subjec- tive factors usually play a role in thinking. In general, the research evidence indicates post formal thinking has its origins in young adulthood (King & Kitchener, 2015; Sinnott, 2014).

Several research-based descriptions of the development of thinking in adulthood have been offered. One of the best is the description of the development of reflective judgment, a way adults reason through dilemmas involving current affairs, religion, science, personal rela- tionships, and the like. Based on decades of longitudinal and cross-sectional research, King and Kitchener (2002; 2015) refined descriptions and identified a systematic progression of reflective judgment in young adulthood. A summary of these stages is shown in ◗ Table 10.3.

The first three stages in the model represent prereflective thought. People in these stages typically do not acknowledge and may not even perceive that knowledge is uncer- tain. Consequently, they do not understand some problems exist when there is not a clear and absolutely correct answer. A student pressuring her instructor for the “right” theory to explain human development reflects this stage. She is also likely to hold firm positions on controversial issues and does so without acknowledging other people’s ability to reach a different (but nevertheless equally logical) position.

About halfway through the developmental progression, students think differently. In Stages 4 and 5, students are likely to say nothing can be known for certain and to change their conclusions based on the situation and the evidence. At this point, students argue knowl- edge is quite subjective. They are also less persuasive with their positions on controversial issues: “Each person is entitled to his or her own view; I cannot force my opinions on anyone else.” King and Kitchener refer to thinking in these stages as “quasi-reflective” thinking.

As they continue their development into Stages 6 and 7, individuals begin to show true reflective judgment, understanding that people construct knowledge using evidence and argument after careful analysis of the problem or situation. They once again hold firm

postformal thought Thinking characterized by recognizing that the correct answer varies from one situ- ation to another, that solutions should be realistic, that ambiguity and contradiction are typical, and that subjective factors play a role in thinking.

reflective judgment Way in which adults reason through real- life dilemmas.

PREREFLECTIVE REASONING (STAGES 1–3)

Belief that “knowledge is gained through the word of an authority figure or through firsthand observation, rather than, for example, through the evaluation of evidence. [People who hold these assumptions] believe that what they know is absolutely correct, and that they know with complete certainty. People who hold these assumptions treat all problems as though they were well-structured” (King & Kitchener, 2004, p. 39). Example statements typical of Stages 1–3: “I know it because I see it.” “If it’s on Fox News, it must be true.”

QUASI-REFLECTIVE REASONING (STAGES 4 AND 5)

Recognition “that knowledge—or more accurately, knowledge claims—contain elements of uncertainty, which [people who hold these assumptions] attribute to missing information or to methods of obtaining the evidence. Although they use evidence, they do not understand how evidence entails a conclusion (especially in light of the acknowledged uncertainty), and thus tend to view judgments as highly idio- syncratic” (King & Kitchener, 2004, p. 40). Example statement typical of stages 4 and 5: “I would believe in climate change if I could see the proof. How can you be sure the scientists aren’t just making up the data?”

REFLECTIVE REASONING (STAGES 6 AND 7)

People who hold these assumptions accept “that knowledge claims cannot be made with certainty, but [they] are not immobilized by it; rather, [they] make judgments that are ‘most reasonable’ and about which they are ‘relatively certain,’ based on their evaluation of available data. They believe they must actively construct their decisions, and that knowledge claims must be evaluated in relationship to the context in which they were generated to determine their validity. They also readily admit their willingness to reevaluate the adequacy of their judg- ments as new data or new methodologies become available” (King & Kitchener, 2004, p. 40). Example statement typical of stages 6 and 7: “It is difficult to be certain about things in life, but you can draw your own conclusions about them based on how well an argument is put together based on the data used to support it.”

Description of the Stages of Reflective Judgment Table 10.3

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

342 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

convictions but reach them only after careful consideration of several points of view. They also realize they must continually reevaluate their beliefs in view of new evidence.

Even though people are able to think at complex levels, do they? Not usually (King & Kitchener, 2004, 2015). Why? Mostly because the environment does not provide the sup- ports necessary for using one’s highest-level thinking, especially for issues concerning knowledge and experience you already have. People may not always purchase the product with the least impact on the environment, such as a fully electric car, even though philo- sophically they are strong environmentalists, because recharging stations are currently not widely available. However, if pushed and if given the necessary supports (e.g., easily available charging stations), people demonstrate a level of thinking and performance far higher than they typically show on a daily basis.

Integrating Emotion and Logic in Emerging and Young Adulthood You may have noticed that a hallmark of postformal thinking is the movement from think- ing “I’m right because I’ve experienced it” to thinking “I’m not sure who’s right because your experience is different from mine.” Problem situations that seemed fairly straightfor- ward in adolescence appear more complicated to young adults; the “right thing to do” is much tougher to figure out.

In addition to an increased understanding there is more than one “right” answer, adult thinking is characterized by the integration of emotion with logic (Labouvie-Vief, 2015). Labouvie-Vief (2015; Labouvie-Vief, Grühn, and Mouras, 2009) describes this emotional development as paralleling intellectual development, demonstrating both gains and losses with increasing age. These parallel processes create tension, resulting in the cognitive- emotional integration and interplay that middle-aged and older adults use when con- fronted with real-life problems.

As they mature, adults tend to make decisions and analyze problems not so much on logical grounds as on pragmatic and emotional grounds. Rules and norms are viewed as relative, not absolute. Mature thinkers realize thinking is an inherently social enterprise that demands making compromises with other people and tolerating contradiction and ambiguity. Such shifts mean one’s sense of self also undergoes a fundamental change.

A good example of this developmental shift is the difference between how late adoles- cents or emerging adults view an emotionally charged issue—such as unethical behavior at work—compared to the views of middle-aged adults. Emerging adults may view such behav- ior as completely inexcusable, with firing of the employee an inescapable outcome. Middle- aged adults may take contextual factors into account and consider what factors may have forced the person to engage in the behavior. Some might argue this is because the topic is too emotionally charged for adolescents to deal with intellectually whereas young adults are

better able to incorporate emotion into their thinking. As people grow older, two things happen in terms

of the integration of emotion and thought (Labouvie- Vief, 2015). First, the rich emotional experience people have accumulated can be brought to bear on tasks that are not too difficult in terms of cognitive demands, meaning that in these situations older adults have an easier time than younger adults at integrating emo- tions and thought. In contrast, when the demands of the task are great, the arousal that is created narrows their ability to bring emotions to bear.

Neuroimaging Evidence We noted earlier that important changes occur in the brain beginning in emerging adulthood. Evidence from neuroimaging research indicates emotion and

Blogging has become a common way of documenting the integration of thought and emotion.

W es

te nd

61 /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 343

logic processing is indeed integrated in adults (Gu et al., 2013). This integration occurs in the prefrontal cortex and the anterior insula (an area of the brain deep inside the cortex). Additional research indicates the amygdala is also involved in processing emotion, and this information is also integrated with thought.

Evidence of certain neural pathways in the brain associated with cognition-emotion inte- gration can be used as a baseline for understanding what happens when the neural connec- tions are altered or absent. Evidence is now clear that these interconnections are different in some forms of mental disorders (Anticevic, Repovs, & Barch, 2012; Hamilton, Hiatt Racer, & Newman, 2015). This means intellectual and emotion processing share common brain path- ways in healthy adults, but various other pathways in adults who experience mental disorders.

The integration of emotion with logic that happens in adulthood provides the basis for decision making in the personal and sometimes difficult arenas of love and work that we examine in detail in Chapters 11 and 12, respectively. It also provides the basis for broader perspectives about life and the ability to see points of view different from one’s own.

Emotional Intelligence The increased integration of emotion and thought across emerging and young adult- hood provides a way of examining how cognitive abilities operate in social situations. The basic goal of the social cognition approach is to understand how people make sense of themselves, others, and events in everyday life (Fiske & Taylor, 2013; Frith & Frith, 2012; Kornadt et al., in press; Weiss, in press).

A key ability in social contexts is emotional intelligence (Goleman, 1995; Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Emotional intelligence (EI) refers to people’s ability to recognize their own and others’ emotions, to correctly identify and appropriately tell the difference between emotions, and use this information to guide their thinking and behavior. Emotional intelligence consists of two aspects. First, EI can be viewed as a trait that reflects a person’s self-perceived disposi- tions and abilities. Second, EI can be viewed as an ability that reflects the person’s success at processing emotional information and using it appropriately in social contexts. EI has been applied to a wide variety of situations, from everyday social cognition and problem solving to bullying to business to leadership. Although still controversial (Hunt & Fitzgerald, 2013, 2014), EI has been mapped onto brain structures (Barbey et al., 2014; Operskalski et al., 2015).

Research indicates that emotional intelligence increases with age (Chen, Peng, & Fang, 2016; Mankus, Boden, & Thompson, 2016), though there is some evidence that the spe- cific ability to perceive others’ emotions in the work context declines late in life (Doerwald et al., 2016). An important finding is that older adults’ increased emotional intelligence may be a source of their higher subjective well-being (Chen et al., 2016).

EI may well be an underlying factor in the social cognitive situations, one of which, impression formation, we will now consider. Keep in mind that variations in a person’s ability to sort out the emotional information available in any given social situation may play an important role in how he or she behaves.

Impression Formation When people meet each other, they tend to immediately come to conclusions about them on many dimensions. Researchers (e.g., Adams et al., 2012; Kotter-Grühn, 2016; Leshikar, Cassidy, & Gutchess, 2016) examine age differences in social judgments by examining impression formation. Impression formation is the way we form and revise first impres- sions about others. Researchers examine how people use diagnostic trait information (aspects about people that appear critical or unique) in making initial impressions of an individual, and how this process varies with age.

A common way of studying impression formation is to have two groups of adults pre- sented with information about a person, either through descriptions or inferences. One group gets positive information first, such as evidence of honesty. The other group is pre- sented with negative information first, such as incidents of dishonest behavior. Each group then subsequently gets the opposite information about the person (e.g., the group that got positive information first then gets negative information).

emotional intelligence (EI) The ability to recognize their own and others’ emotions, to correctly identify and appropriately tell the difference between emotions, and use this information to guide their thinking and behavior.

impression formation The way we form and revise first impressions about others.

Think About It Why are formal operations inadequate for integrating emotion and thought?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

344 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

What happens to people’s first impressions as a function of age is a well-established finding. As you can see from ◗ Figure 10.13, in a study that helped create this area of research focus, Hess and Pullen (1994) found all study participants modified their impres- sions. When new negative information was presented after the initial positive portrayal of the target person, older adults modified their impression of the target from positive to negative. Interestingly, however, they modified their first impression less when the negative portrayal was followed by positive information.

In contrast, younger adults did not show this pattern. Instead, they were more con- cerned with making sure the new information was consistent with their initial impression. To do so, they modified their impressions to correspond with the new information regard- less of whether it was positive or negative. Younger adults, then, make their impression based on the most recent information they have.

●◗ Figure 10.13 Mean trait ratings before and after presentation of new negative or posi- tive information.

0

1

2

3

4

5

6 M

ea n

tr ai

t ra

ti ng

s

7

Positive traits

Negative traits

Positive traits

Negative traits

Younger adults Older adults

Positive initial portrayal of target

Before new negative information After new negative information

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

M ea

n tr

ai t

ra ti

ng s

Positive traits

Negative traits

Positive traits

Negative traits

Younger adults Older adults

Negative initial portrayal of target

Before new positive information After new positive information

SOURCE: A modified graph of the Hess, T. M., & Pullen, S. M. (1994). Adult age differences in impression change processes. Psychology and Aging, 9, p. 239.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 345

Felicia is a 19-year-old sophomore at a community college. She expects her study of early childhood education to be difficult but rewarding. She figures that along the way, she will meet a great guy and marry him soon after graduation. They will have two children before she turns 30. Felicia sees herself getting a good job teaching preschool children and someday owning her own day-care center.

Maya Angelou (1969) perhaps said it best: “There is no agony like bearing an untold story inside of you.” True to her conviction, she spent a lifetime writing her story in numerous books, poems, and other literary works. She described an incredible developmental path of oppression, hatred, and hurt that ultimately resulted in self-awareness, understanding, and compassion.

In Chapter 9, we saw how children and adolescents begin creating their stories by answering the question, What do you want to be when you grow up? As a young adult, Felicia has arrived at the “grown-up” part and is experimenting with some idealistic answers to the question. Are Felicia’s answers typical of most emerging adults?

recall 1. Most modern theories of intelligence are _______________ in

that they identify many domains of intellectual abilities.

2. Number, verbal fluency, and spatial orientation are some of the _______________ mental abilities.

3. _______________ reflects knowledge that you have acquired through life experience and education in a particular culture.

4. Kitchener and King describe a kind of postformal thinking called _______________.

5. Life problems provide a context for understanding the inte- gration of _______________ and _______________.

Interpret ●● Many young adult college students seemingly get more con-

fused about what field they want to major in and less certain about what they know as they progress through college. From a cognitive-developmental approach, why does this happen?

●● Political movements often are led by a combination of late adolescents/young adults and older adults. From a cognitive development perspective, why would this tend to be the case?

Apply ●● Two of LuSharon’s friends explained their decision to sup-

port different candidates during the 2016 U.S. presidential campaign. One friend said that she had an intuition that her candidate’s campaign statements were based on facts and were absolutely trustworthy; the other friend said that her candidate’s statements always had to be put into a specific context in order to be analyzed. Based on the reflective judg- ment model, what levels of thinking do LuSharon’s friends demonstrate?

●● How does experience on the job relate to the stages of reflec- tive judgment?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 10.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do adults create scenarios and life stories?

●● What are possible selves? Do they show differences during adulthood?

●● What are personal control beliefs?

10.4 Who Do You Want to Be? Personality in Young Adulthood

Why do younger and older adults differ? Hess and Pullen suggest older adults may rely more on life experiences and social rules of behavior when making their interpretations, whereas younger adults may be more concerned with situational consistency of the new information presented.

Given the consistency of these patterns over many studies, knowing how to create positive (or negative) impressions about people, such as in political campaigns, job inter- views, or dating, requires understanding of the order in which certain personal character- istics should be presented.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

346 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

In this section, we examine how the search for identity gets transformed through the cognitive, social, and personal reality of adulthood. As a result, people create life scenarios and life stories, possible selves, self-concept, and personal control beliefs. Let’s begin by considering how Felicia and the rest of us construct images of our adult lives.

Creating Life Stories Figuring out what (and whom) you want to be as an adult takes a great deal of thought, hard work, and time. Based on personal experience and input from other people, young adults create a life-span construct that represents a unified sense of the past, present, and future. Several factors influence the development of a life-span construct; identity, values, and society are only a few. Together they not only shape the creation of the life- span construct but also influence how it is played out and whether it remains stable (Fraley & Roberts, 2005). The life-span construct represents a link between Erikson’s notion of identity, which is a major focus during adolescence, and our adult view of ourselves.

The first way the life-span construct is manifested is through the scenario, which con- sists of expectations about the future. The scenario takes aspects of a person’s identity that are particularly important now and projects them into a plan for the future. For example, you may find yourself thinking about the day you will graduate and will be able to apply all of the knowledge and skills you have learned. In short, a scenario is a game plan for how your life will play out in the future.

Felicia, the sophomore human development student, has a fairly typical scenario. She plans on completing a degree in early childhood education, marrying after graduation, and having two children by age 30. Tagging future events with a particular time or age by which they are to be completed creates a social clock. This personal timetable gives people a way to track progress through adulthood, and it may use biological markers of time (such as menopause), social aspects of time (such as marriage), and historical time (such as the turn of the century) (Hagestad & Neugarten, 1985).

Felicia will use her scenario to evaluate progress toward her personal goals. With each new event, she will check where she is against where her scenario says she should be. If she is ahead of her plan, she may be proud of having made it. If she is lagging behind, she may chastise herself for being slow. But if she criticizes herself too much, she may change her scenario altogether. For example, if she does not go to college, she may decide to change her career goals entirely: Instead of owning her own day care center, she may aim to be a manager in a department store.

How Felicia and the rest of us actually create the stories about ourselves is a fascinating process of integrating thought and emotion, along with the lived experiences we have. Let’s see in more detail how this happens by examining the best known explanation of the process.

McAdams’s Life-Story Model McAdams (2001, 2015) argues a person’s sense of identity cannot be understood using the language of dispositional traits or personal concerns. Identity is not just a collection of traits, nor is it a collection of plans, strategies, or goals. Instead, it is based on a story of how the person came into being, where the person has been, where he or she is going, and who he or she will become, much like Felicia’s story. McAdams argues that people create a life story that is an internalized narrative with a beginning, middle, and an anticipated ending. The life story is created and revised throughout adulthood as people change and the changing environment places different demands on them.

McAdams’s research indicates people in Western societies begin forming their life story during late adolescence and emerging adulthood, but its roots lie in one’s earliest attachments in infancy. As in Erikson’s theory, adolescence marks the full initiation into forming an identity, and thus, a coherent life story begins. In emerging adulthood it is con- tinued and refined through intimacy, and from midlife and beyond it is refashioned in the wake of major and minor life changes.

life-span construct Unified sense of the past, present, and future based on personal experience and input from other people.

scenario Manifestation of the life-span construct through expectations about the future.

social clock Tagging future events with a particular time or age by which they are to be completed.

life story An internalized narrative with a beginning, middle, and an anticipated ending.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 347

Paramount in these life stories is the changing personal identity reflected in the emo- tions conveyed in the story (from tragedy to optimism or through comic and romantic descriptions). In addition, motivations change and are reflected in the person repeatedly trying to attain his or her goals over time. The two most common goal themes are agency (reflecting power, achievement, and autonomy) and communion (reflecting love, intimacy, and a sense of belonging). Finally, life stories indicate one’s beliefs and values, or the ideol- ogy a person uses to set the context for his or her actions.

Every life story contains episodes that provide insight into perceived change and continuity in life. People prove to themselves and others they have either changed or remained the same by pointing to specific events supporting the appropriate claim. The main characters, representing the roles we play, in our lives represent idealizations of the self, such as “the dutiful mother” or “the reliable worker.” Integrating these various aspects of the self into a coherent whole begins in emerging adulthood, and continues to be a major challenge of midlife and later adulthood. Finally, all life stories need an ending so the self can leave a legacy that creates new beginnings. As we will see in Chapters 13 and 15, life stories in middle-aged and older adults have a clear quality of “giving birth to” a new generation.

One of the more popular methods for examining the development of life stories is through autobiographical memory (Dunlop, Guo, & McAdams, 2016; Lilgendahl & McAdams, 2011; McLean, 2016; McLean & Pasupathi, 2012). When people tell their life stories to others, the stories are a joint product of the speaker and the audience, which includes other key people in a person’s life, such as family (Pasupathi, 2013; McLean, 2016). This co-construction of identity is a good example of conversational remember- ing, much like collaborative cognition discussed in Chapter 8.

Overall, McAdams (2001, 2015) believes the model for change in identity over time is a process of fashioning and refashioning one’s life story. This process appears to be strongly influenced by culture. At times, the reformulation may be at a conscious level, such as when people make explicit decisions about changing careers. At other times, the revision process is unconscious and implicit, growing out of everyday activities. The goal is to create a life story that is coherent, credible, open to new possibilities, richly differenti- ated, reconciling of opposite aspects of oneself, and integrated within one’s sociocultural context.

Possible Selves When we are asked questions like, “What do you think you’ll be like a few years from now?” it requires us to imagine ourselves in the future. When we speculate like this, we create a possible self (Markus & Nurius, 1986). Possible selves represent what we could become, what we would like to become, and what we are afraid of becoming. What we could or would like to become often reflects personal goals; we may see ourselves as lead- ers, as rich and famous, or in great physical shape. What we are afraid of becoming may show up in our fear of being alone, or overweight, or unsuccessful. Our possible selves are powerful motivators (Ko, Mejia, & Hooker, 2014); indeed, how we behave is largely an effort to achieve or avoid these various possible selves and protect the current view of self ( Baumeister, 2010).

In a rare set of similar studies conducted across time and research teams by Cross and Markus (1991) and Hooker and colleagues (Frazier et al., 2000; Frazier et al., 2002; Hooker, 1999; Hooker et al., 1996; Morfei et al., 2001), people across the adult life span were asked to describe their hoped-for and feared possible selves. The responses were grouped into categories (e.g., family, personal, material, relationships, and occupation).

Several interesting age differences emerged. In terms of hoped-for selves, young adults listed family concerns—for instance, marrying the right person—as most impor- tant. In contrast, adults in their 30s listed family concerns last; their main issues involved personal concerns, such as being a more loving and caring person. By ages 40 to 59, family issues again became most common—such as being a parent who can “let go” of the chil- dren. Reaching and maintaining satisfactory performance in one’s occupational career as

possible selves Representations of what we could become, what we would like to become, and what we are afraid of becoming.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

348 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

well as accepting and adjusting to the physiological changes of middle age were important to this age group.

For adults over 60, researchers find personal issues are most prominent—like being active and healthy for at least another decade. The greatest amount of change occurred in the health domain, which predominated the hoped-for and feared-for selves. The health domain is the most sensitive and central to the self in the context of aging and people’s possible self with regard to health is quite resilient in the face of health chal- lenges in later life.

Overall, young adults have multiple possible selves and believe they can actually become the hoped-for self and successfully avoid the feared self. Their outlook tends to be quite positive (Remedios, Chasteen, & Packer, 2010). Life experience may dampen this outlook. By old age, both the number of possible selves and the strength of belief have decreased. Older adults are more likely to believe neither the hoped-for nor the feared-for self is under their personal control. These findings may reflect differences with age in personal motivation, beliefs in personal control, and the need to explore new options.

The emergence of online social media has created new opportunities for young adults to create possible selves (Lefkowitz, Vukman, & Loken, 2012). Such media present differ- ent ways for them to speculate about themselves to others.

The connection between possible selves and how we construct meaning in our lives is important. The link is through the process of setting personal goals that derive from the possible selves we envision.

Personal Control Beliefs Do you feel you have control over your life? Personal control beliefs reflect the degree to which you believe your performance in a situation depends on something you do. For example, sup- pose you are not offered a job when you think you should have been. Was it your fault? Or was it because the company was too shortsighted to recognize your true talent? The option you select provides insight into a general tendency. Do you gener- ally believe that outcomes depend on the things you do? Or are they due to factors outside yourself, such as luck or the power of others?

Answering these questions requires the integration of sev- eral different sources of information, along with one’s emotions. The result of this integration are various conclusions about how much  control one has in any specific situation. A high sense of personal control implies a belief that performance is up to you, whereas a low sense of personal control implies that your perfor- mance is under the influence of forces other than your own.

Personal control is an extremely important idea in a variety of settings and cultures because of the way it guides behavior (Brandtstädter, 1997, 1999; Fung & Siu, 2010). Successful peo- ple such as Stefani Joanne Angelina Germanotta (better known as Lady Gaga) need to exude a high sense of personal control to demonstrate that they are in charge.

Personal control is an important concept that can be applied broadly to several domains, including social networks, health, and careers (Fung & Siu, 2010). It is related to the nearly uni- versal desire to make a difference in the world through one’s life (Dowd, 2012).

Personal control beliefs are important not only in personality development but also (as we will see in Chapter 14) in memory

personal control beliefs The degree to which you believe your per- formance in a situation depends on some- thing you do.

Em m

an ue

l D un

an d/

A FP

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Larry Page, cofounder of Google, is likely to have a high sense of personal control.

Think About It How might the development of possible selves be related to cognitive development?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 349

performance in late life. Research indicates that people experience four types of personal control (Tiffany & Tiffany, 1996): control from within oneself, control over oneself, control over the environment, and control from the environment.

Despite its importance, we do not have a clear picture of the developmental course of personal control beliefs. Evidence from both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (Lang & Heckhausen, 2006) is contradictory. Some data indicate that younger adults are less likely to hold internal control beliefs (i.e., believe they are in control of outcomes) than are older adults. Other research finds the opposite.

The contradiction may derive from the complex nature of personal control beliefs in the context of the situation (Vazire & Doris, 2009). These beliefs vary depending on which domain, such as intelligence or health, is being assessed. Indeed, other research shows that perceived control over one’s development declines with age, whereas per- ceived control over marital happiness increases (Brandtstädter, 1989). In addition, younger adults are more satisfied when attributing success in attaining a goal to their own efforts, whereas older adults are more satisfied when they attribute such success to their ability (Lang & Heckhausen, 2001, 2006). Clearly, people of all ages and cul- tures try to influence their environment regardless of whether they believe they will be successful.

Heckhausen and Wrosch (Haase, Heckhausen, & Wrosch, 2013; Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, 2010) pulled together the various perspectives on control beliefs and proposed a motivational theory of life-span development to describe how people opti- mize primary and secondary control. Primary control is behavior aimed at affecting the individual’s external world; working a second job to increase one’s earnings is an exam- ple. One’s ability to influence the environment is heavily influenced by biological factors (e.g., stamina to work two jobs); so it changes over time—from very low influence dur- ing early childhood to high influence during middle age and then to very low again in late life. Secondary control is behavior or cognition aimed at affecting the individual’s internal world; an example is believing that one is capable of success even when faced with challenges.

The developmental patterns of both types of control are shown in ◗ Figure 10.14. The figure also shows that people of all ages strive to control their environment, but how they do this changes over time. Note that for the first half of life, primary and secondary con- trol operate in parallel. During midlife, primary control begins to decline but secondary control does not. Thus, the desire for control does not change; what differs with age is whether we can affect our environment or whether we need to think about things differ- ently. We will return to these ideas in Chapter 15 when we see how our interaction with the environment changes in late life.

primary control Behavior aimed at affecting the individual’s external world.

secondary control Behavior or cognition aimed at affecting the individual’s internal world.

●◗ Figure 10.14 Hypothetical life-span trajectories for primary control potential and primary and secondary control striving.

Primary Control Striving

Secondary Control Striving

Primary Control Capacity

Old AgeMidlifeChildhood© 2

01 9

C en

ga ge

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

SUMMARY

10.1 Emerging Adulthood

What role transitions mark entry into adulthood?

●● The most widely used criteria for deciding whether a person has reached adulthood are role transitions, which involve assuming new responsibilities and duties.

●● Some societies use rituals, called rites of passage, to mark this transition clearly. However, such rituals are largely absent in Western culture.

What evidence from neuroscience helps us understand behavioral development in young adulthood?

●● The prefrontal cortex fully develops during young adult- hood and forms key connections with other areas of the brain.

●● Edgework may reflect aspects of this continued neurologi- cal development.

●● Young adults make the transition to Erikson’s struggle of intimacy and isolation.

How do going to college, obtaining career training, and entering the workforce reflect the transition to adulthood?

●● Roughly 70% of high school graduates continue their edu- cation at the postsecondary level. College students experi- ence transitions in thinking. Returning adult students tend to be more focused on their academic program, but face challenges balancing education, career, and family.

●● Many emerging adults obtain career or job training as a way to further education without obtaining a college degree.

●● Joining the workforce is a major step toward financial inde- pendence, which in turn is a major marker of achieving adulthood.

10.2 Physical Development and Health

In what respects are young adults at their physical peak?

●● Young adulthood is the time when certain physical abilities peak: strength, muscle development, coordination, dex- terity, and sensory acuity. Most of these abilities begin to decline in middle age.

How do smoking, alcohol, and nutrition affect young adults’ health?

●● Young adults are at the peak of health. Death from disease is relatively rare, especially during the 20s. Accidents are the leading cause of death. However, homicide and violence are major factors in some groups. Poor ethnic minorities have less access to good health care, and poverty is also a major barrier to good health.

●● Smoking is the single biggest contributor to health problems. One is never too old to quit smoking. Smoking causes several forms of cancer and is a primary cause of respiratory and car- diovascular disease. Although it is difficult, quitting smoking has many health benefits.

●● For most people, drinking alcohol poses few health risks. Binge drinking has numerous negative effects, but peaks in emerging adulthood. Campus sexual assault is related to alcohol use. Several treatment approaches are available for people with alcohol use disorder.

recall 1. A(n) _______________ is a unified sense of a person’s past,

present, and future.

2. A personal narrative that organizes past events into a coher- ent sequence is a(n) _______________.

3. Representations of what we could become, what we would like to be, and what we are afraid of becoming are our _______________.

4. _______________ reflect the degree to which a person’s per- formance in a situation is believed to be under his or her control.

Interpret ●● How might people’s scenarios, life stories, and other aspects

of personality vary as a function of cognitive developmental level and self-definition as an adult?

●● How do possible selves reflect people’s life experiences?

Apply ●● In the context of a bad economy, how could you determine

whether your performance on job interviews is under your control?

●● How has your view of your possible selves influenced your choice of career?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 10.4

350 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T E r 1 0 : B E C O M I N G A N A D U LT 351

●● Nutritional needs change somewhat during adulthood, mostly due to changes in metabolism. Some nutrient needs, such as carbohydrates, change. The ratio of LDLs to HDLs in serum cholesterol, which can be controlled through diet or medication in most people, is an important risk factor in cardiovascular disease.

How does young adults’ health differ as a function of socio- economic status, ethnicity, and education?

●● The three most important social factors in health are socio- economic status, ethnicity, and education. Poverty is the most important factor in limiting access to quality health care.

●● Even with other variables held equal, people of color have poorer access to quality health care than European Americans.

●● Higher education is associated with better health via bet- ter access to health care and more knowledge about proper diet and lifestyle.

10.3 Cognitive Development

What is intelligence in adulthood?

●● Most modern theories of intelligence are multidimensional. Research reveals interindividual variability and plasticity.

●● Intellectual abilities are organized into factors of interre- lated abilities that form the structure of intelligence.

What are primary and secondary mental abilities? How do they change?

●● Intellectual abilities can be studied as groups of related skills known as primary mental abilities.

●● Clusters of related primary abilities are called secondary mental abilities. Secondary mental abilities are not mea- sured directly.

●● These abilities develop differently and change in succeed- ing cohorts. More recent cohorts perform better on some skills, such as inductive reasoning, but older cohorts per- form better on number skills.

What are fluid and crystallized intelligence? How do they change?

●● Fluid intelligence consists of abilities that make people flex- ible and adaptive thinkers. Fluid abilities generally decline during adulthood.

●● Crystallized intelligence reflects knowledge that people acquire through life experience and education in a particu- lar culture. Crystallized abilities improve until late life.

How has neuroscience research furthered our understanding of intelligence in adulthood?

●● Neuroscience research has begun mapping specific areas in the brain that relate to intelligence. One prominent theory based on this work is the parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT).

What is postformal thought? How does it differ from formal operations?

●● Postformal thought is characterized by a recognition that truth may vary from one situation to another, that solu- tions must be realistic, that ambiguity and contradiction are the rule, and that emotion and subjectivity play a role in thinking. One example of postformal thought is reflective judgment.

How do emotion and logic become integrated in adult- hood? How do emotional intelligence and impression formation demonstrate this integration?

●● Cognition (logic) and emotion become integrated during young adulthood and middle age. This means that the way people approach and solve practical problems in life differs from adolescence through middle age.

●● Emotional intelligence refers to the ability to recognize and correctly identify one’s own and other’s emotion, and to use this information to guide thinking and behavior. Emotional intelligence increases with age and likely underlies many social cognitive situations.

●● Impression formation is the way we form and revise first impressions. How this process influences judgments about people differs with age.

10.4 Who Do You Want to Be? Personality in Young Adulthood

What is the life-span construct? How do adults create scenarios and life stories?

●● Young adults create a life-span construct that represents a unified sense of the past, present, and future. This is mani- fested in two ways: through a scenario that maps the future based on a social clock and in the life story, which creates an autobiography.

What are possible selves? Do they show differences during adulthood?

●● People create possible selves by projecting themselves into the future and thinking about what they would like to become, what they could become, and what they are afraid of becoming.

●● Age differences in these projections depend on the dimen- sion examined. In hoped-for selves, 18- to 24-year-olds and 40- to 59-year-olds report family issues as most important, whereas 25- to 39-year-olds and older adults consider personal issues to be most important. However, all groups include physical aspects as part of their most feared selves.

What are personal control beliefs?

●● Personal control is an important concept with broad appli- cability. However, the developmental trends are complex because personal control beliefs vary considerably from one domain to another.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

352 PA rT T H r E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Test Yourself Recall Answers 10.1 1. role transitions 2. rites of passage 3. returning adult students 4. reckless behavior 5. intimacy versus isolation 10.2 1. height 2. 20s 3. rare 4. accidents 5. Smoking 6. binge drinking 7. socioeconomic status 10.3 1. multidimensional 2. primary 3. Crystallized intelligence 4. reflective judgment 5. emotion, logic 10.4 1. life-span construct 2. life story 3. possible selves 4. Personal control beliefs

Key Terms emerging adulthood (318) rites of passage (319) role transitions (319) edgework (320) returning adult students (322) intimacy versus isolation (323) binge drinking (327) alcohol use disorder (328) metabolism (330) low-density lipoproteins

(LDLs) (330) high-density lipoproteins

(HDLs) (330)

body mass index (Bmi) (332) multidimensional (334) multidirectionality (334) interindividual variability (334) plasticity (335) structure of intelligence (335) factor (335) primary mental abilities (335) secondary mental abilities (335) fluid intelligence (337) crystallized intelligence (337) parieto-frontal integration theory

(p-Fit) (339)

neural efficiency hypothesis (340) postformal thought (341) reflective judgment (341) emotional intelligence (ei) (343) impression formation (343) life-span construct (346) scenario (346) social clock (346) life story (346) possible selves (347) personal control beliefs (348) primary control (349) secondary control (349)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Pe te

r B er

ni k/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

355

Imagine yourself years from now. Your children are grown and have children and grandchildren of their own. In honor of your 80th birthday, they

have all come together, along with your friends, to

celebrate. Their present to you is a video made from

hundreds of photographs and dozens of videos cre-

ated over the decades of your life. As you watch it,

you realize how lucky you’ve been to have so many

wonderful people in your life. Your relationships have

made your adult life fun and worthwhile. As you

watch, you wonder what it must be like to go through

life alone—no family, no friends (even on Facebook),

no followers of your Twitter postings. You think of all

the wonderful experiences you would have missed in

early and middle adulthood—never knowing what friendship is all about, never

being in love, never dreaming about children and becoming a parent.

That is what we’ll explore in this chapter—the ways in which we share our

lives with others. First, we consider what makes good friendships and love

relationships. Because these relationships form the basis of our lifestyle, we

examine these lifestyle influences next. In the third section, we consider what it

is like to be a parent. Finally, we see what happens when marriages or partner-

ships end. Throughout this chapter, the emphasis is on aspects of relationships

that nearly everyone experiences during young adulthood and middle age.

In Chapter 13, we examine aspects of relationships specific to middle-aged

adults; in Chapter 14, we do the same for relationships in later life.

Being with Others: Relationships in Young and Middle Adulthood

11.1 Relationship Types and Issues 356 Friendships 356 Love Relationships 358 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH Patterns and Universals of

Romantic Attachment Around the World 361 Violence in Relationships 363

11.2 Lifestyles and Relationships 365 Singlehood 365 Cohabitation 366 LGBTQ Relationships 367 REAL PEOPLE James Obergefell and John Arthur Make

History Marriage 369

11.3 Family Dynamics and the Life Course 372 The Parental Role 372 WHAT DO YOU THINK? Paid Family Leave 374 Diverse Family Forms 376

11.4 Divorce and Remarriage 379 Divorce 379 Remarriage 382

    Summary 384

    Key Terms 385

11

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

356 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Jamal and Kahlid have known each other all their lives. They grew up together in New York, attended the same schools, and even married sisters. Their business careers took them in different directions, but they and their families always got together on major holidays. Now as older men, they feel a special bond; many of their other friends have died.

Having other people in our lives we can count on is essential to our well-being. Just imagine how difficult life would be if you were totally alone, without even a Facebook “friend” to communicate with. In this section, we consider the different types of rela- tionships we have with other people, and learn how these relationships help—and some- times hurt us.

Friendships Jamal and Kahlid remind us some of the most important people in our lives are our friends. They are often the people to whom we are closest, and are there when we need someone to lean on.

What is an adult friend? Someone who is there when you need to share? Someone that’s not afraid to tell you the truth? Someone you have fun with? Friends, of course, are all of these and more. Researchers define friendship as a mutual relationship in which those involved influence one another’s behaviors and beliefs, and define friendship qual- ity as the satisfaction derived from the relationship (Blieszner, 2014; Blieszner & Roberto, 2012; Hall, 2016).

The role and influence of friends for young adults is extremely important from the late teens through the 20s, and friends continue to be a source of support across adult- hood (Arnett, 2013; Nehamas, 2016). Friendships are predominantly based on feelings and grounded in reciprocity and choice. Friendships are different from love relationships mainly because friendships are less emotionally intense and usually do not involve sex (Nehamas, 2016). Having good friendships boosts self-esteem, especially early in emerg- ing adulthood (Miething et al., 2016), and happiness across adulthood (Adams & Taylor, 2015; Demir et al., 2015; Fiori & Denckla, 2015). Friendships also help us become social- ized into new roles throughout adulthood.

Friendship in Adulthood From a developmental perspective, adult friendships can be viewed as having identifi- able stages (Levinger, 1980, 1983): Acquaintanceship, Buildup, Continuation, Deteriora- tion, and Ending. This ABCDE model describes the stages of friendships and how they change. Whether a friendship develops from Acquaintanceship to Buildup depends on where the individuals fall on several dimensions, such as the basis of the attraction, what each person knows about the other, how good the communication is between the part- ners, the perceived importance of the friendship, and so on. Although many friendships reach the Deterioration stage, whether a friendship ultimately ends depends heavily on the availability of alternative relationships. If potential friends appear, old friendships may end; if not, they may continue even though they are no longer considered important by either person.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What role do friends play across adulthood? How do they

develop?

●● What characterizes love relationships? How do they vary across cultures?

●● What is the nature of violence in some relationships?

11.1 Relationship Types and Issues

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 357

Longitudinal research shows how friendships change across adulthood, some in ways that are predictable and others not. As you probably have experienced, life transitions (e.g., going away to college, getting married) usually result in fewer friends and less contact with the friends you keep (Blieszner, 2014; Blieszner & Roberto, 2012). People tend to have more friends and acquaintances during young adulthood than at any subsequent period (Demir et al., 2015). Friendships are important throughout adulthood, in part because a person’s life satisfaction is strongly related to the quantity and quality of contacts with friend. College students with strong friendship networks adjust better to stressful life events whether those networks are face-to-face (e.g., Brissette, Scheier, & Carver, 2002) or through online social networks (Antheunis, 2016; DeAndrea et al., 2012).

The importance of maintaining contacts with friends cuts across ethnic lines as well. People who have friendships that cross ethnic groups have more positive attitudes toward  people with different backgrounds (Aberson, Shoemaker, & Tomolillo, 2004), including Facebook networks (Schwab & Greitemeyer, 2015). Thus, regardless of one’s background, friendships play a major role in determining how much we enjoy life.

Why does friendship have such positive benefits for us? Although scientists do not know for certain, they are gaining insights through neuroscience research. Coan and colleagues (Beckes & Coan, 2013; Beckes, Coan, & Hasselmo, 2013; Coan, 2008; Coan & Sbarra, 2015) propose Social Baseline Theory, a perspective that integrates the study of social relation- ships with principles of attachment, behavioral ecology, cognitive neuroscience, and per- ception science. Social Baseline Theory suggests the human brain expects access to social relationships that mitigate risk and diminish the level of effort needed to meet a variety of goals by incorporating relational partners into neural representations of the self.

When people are faced with threatening situations, their brains process the situation differently when faced alone compared to with a close friend. Specifically, neuroimaging definitively shows the parts of the brain that respond to threat operate when facing threat alone but do not operate when facing the same threat with a close friend. A close friend- ship literally changes the way the brain functions, resulting in our perception of feeling safer and the trials we face are more manageable with friends than without them.

Three broad themes characterize both traditional (e.g., face-to-face) and new forms (e.g., online) of adult friendships (de Vries, 1996; Ridings & Gefen, 2004):

●● The affective or emotional basis of friendship refers to self-disclosure and expressions of intimacy, appreciation, affection, and support, and all are based on trust, loyalty, and commitment.

●● The shared, or communal nature, of friendship reflects how friends partici- pate in or support activities of mutual interest.

●● The sociability and compatibility dimension represents how our friends keep us entertained and are sources of amusement, fun, and recreation.

In the case of online friendships (e.g., through social media), trust devel- ops on the basis of four sources: (1) reputation; (2) performance, or what users do online; (3) precommitment, through personal self-disclosure; and (4) situ- ational factors, especially the premium placed on intimacy and the relation- ship (Håkansson & Witmer, 2015; Henderson & Gilding, 2004). Online social network friendships develop much like face-to-face ones in that the more time people spend online with friends the more likely they are to self-disclose (Chang & Hsiao, 2014). Online environments are more conducive to people who are lonely (Blachnio et al., 2016). A study of Facebook users in Australia showed that they were extremely positive about their online friendship activ- ity (Young, 2013). However, they mentioned that the risk of “de-friending” is a real phenomenon, so that the psychological consequences can be both positive (gaining new friends and a social network) and negative (dealing with the hurt of being de-friended).

A special type of friendship exists with one’s siblings, who are the friends people typically have the longest and that share the closest bonds; the importance of these relationships varies with age (Carr & Moorman, 2011;

Despite concerns, social networking web- sites such as Facebook have not reduced the quality of friendships.

iS to

ck .c

om /D

em 10

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

358 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Merz & De Jong Gierveld, 2016; Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). The centrality of siblings depends on several things, such as proximity, health, prior relationship, and degree of relatedness (full, step-, or half-siblings). No clear pattern of emotional closeness emerges when viewing sibling relationships on the basis of gender.

Men’s, Women’s, and Cross-Sex Friendships Men’s and women’s friendships tend to differ in adulthood, reflecting continuity in the learned behaviors from childhood (Blieszner & Roberto, 2012; Levine, 2009). Four char- acteristics of same-sex friends do not appear to differ between men and women and are similar across cultures and age groups: geographic proximity, similarity of interests and values, inclusion, and symmetrical reciprocity (Hall, 2016). Three characteristics that dis- tinguish female same-sex friendships from males’ same-sex friendships are: communion and self-disclosure, greater effort and expectations from friends in general, and a greater risk of corumination (extensively discussing and re-visiting problems, and focusing on negative feelings) (Hall, 2016). In contrast, men tend to base friendships on shared activi- ties or interests.

What about friendships between men and women? These friendships are mainly a product of the 20th century (Hart, Adams, & Tullett, 2016), and have a beneficial effect, especially for men (Piquet, 2007). Cross-sex friendships help men have lower levels of dating anxiety and higher capacity for intimacy. These patterns hold across ethnic groups, too. Cross-sex friendships can also prove troublesome because of misperceptions. Misper- ception about one’s own or one’s partner’s sexual attractiveness to others is common and can be the basis for relationship difficulties (Hart et al., 2016; Haselton & Galperin, 2013). Some research shows men overestimate and women underestimate their friends’ sexual interest in them (Koenig, Kirkpatrick, & Ketelaar, 2007). Maintaining cross-sex friend- ships once individuals enter into exclusive dating relationships, marriage, or committed relationships is difficult, and often results in one partner feeling jealous (Hart et al., 2016; Haselton & Galperin, 2013).

Love Relationships Love is one of those things we feel but cannot fully describe. (Test yourself: Can you explain fully what you mean when you look at someone special and say, “I love you”?) One way researchers try to understand love is to think about what components are essential. In an interesting series of studies, Sternberg (2006) found love has three basic components: (1) passion, an intense physi- ological desire for someone; (2) intimacy, the feeling that you can share all your thoughts and actions with another; and (3) commitment, the willingness to stay with a person through good and bad times. Ideally, a true love relationship has all three components; when couples have equivalent amounts and types of love, they tend to be happier, even as the balance among these components shifts over time.

Love Through Adulthood The different combinations of love help us understand how relationships develop (Sternberg, 2006). Research shows the development of romantic rela- tionships in emerging adulthood is a complex process influenced by relation- ships in childhood and adolescence (Collins & van Dulmen, 2006; Oudekerk et al., 2015). Early in a romantic relationship, passion is usually high whereas intimacy and commitment tend to be low. This is infatuation: an intense, physically based relationship when the two people have a high risk of mis- understanding and jealousy. Indeed, it is sometimes difficult to establish the boundaries between casual sex and hook-ups and dating in young adulthood (Giordano et al., 2012).

Physical attraction tends to be high early in a relationship.

Ca ro

lin e

Sc hi

ff /B

le nd

Im ag

es /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 359

Infatuation is short-lived. As passion fades, either a relationship acquires emotional intimacy or it is likely to end. Trust, honesty, openness, and acceptance must be a part of any strong relationship; when they are present, romantic love develops.

Although it may not be the basis for best-selling romance novels or movies, this pat- tern is a good thing. Research shows people who select a partner for a more permanent relationship (e.g., marriage) during the height of infatuation are likely to support the notion of “love at first sight” and are more likely to divorce (Hansen, 2006). If the couple spends more time and works at their relationship, they may become committed to each other. By spending much of their time together, making decisions together, caring for each other, sharing possessions, and developing ways to settle conflicts, they increase the chances that their relationship will last. Such couples usually show outward signs of commitment, such as wearing a lover’s ring, having children together, or simply sharing the mundane details of daily life, from making toast at breakfast to following before-bed rituals.

Campbell and Kaufman (2015) surveyed 1,529 people across the United States in order to better understand the connections among love, personality, and creativity. They found that as time goes on, physical intimacy and passion decrease but emotional intimacy and commitment increase. This and related research indicate that good relationships tend to deepen as time goes on, even if intense physical passion decreases.

Falling in Love In his book The Prophet, Kahlil Gibran (1923) points out love is two-sided: Just as it can give you great ecstasy, so can it cause you great pain. Yet most of us are willing to take the risk.

As you may have experienced, taking the risk is fun (at times) and difficult (at other times). Making a connection can be ritualized, as when people use pickup lines in a bar, or it can happen almost by accident, as when two people literally run into each other in a crowded corridor. The question that confronts us is “How do people fall in love?” Do birds of a feather flock together? Or do opposites attract?

The best explanation of the process is the theory of assortative mating, that states people find partners based on their similarity to each other. Assortative mating occurs along many dimensions, including education, religious beliefs, physical traits, age, socio- economic status, intelligence, and political ideology, among others (Horwitz et al., 2016). Such nonrandom mating occurs most often in Western societies that allow people to have more control over their own dating and pairing behaviors. Common activities are one basis for identifying potential mates, except, that is, in speed dating situations. In that case, it comes down to physical attractiveness (Luo & Zhang, 2009).

People meet people in all sorts of places, both face-to-face and virtually. Does how people meet influence the likelihood they will “click” on particular dimensions and form a couple? Kalmijn and Flap (2001) found that it does. Using data from more than 1,500 cou- ples, they found meeting at school, for example, was most likely to result in homogamy— the degree to which people are similar. Not surprisingly, the pool of available people to meet is strongly shaped by the opportunities available, that in turn constrain the type of people one is likely to meet.

Speed dating provides a way to meet several people in a short period of time. Speed dating is practiced most by young adults (Fein & Schneider, 2013; Whitty & Buchanan, 2009). The rules governing partner selection during a speed dating session seem quite similar to traditional dating: physically attractive people, outgoing and self-assured peo- ple, and moderately self-focused people are selected more often and their dates are rated as smoother (Herrenbrueck et al., 2016).

The popularity of online dating means an increasing number of people meet this way (Fein & Schneider, 2013; Lomanowska & Guitton, 2016; Whitty & Buchanan, 2009). Sur- veys indicate nearly 1 in every 5 couples in the United States meet online (compared with 1 in 10 in Australia, and 1 in 20 in Spain and the United Kingdom; Dutton et al., 2009). Emerging research indicates virtual dating sites offer both problems and possibilities, espe- cially in terms of the accuracy of personal descriptions. As in the offline world, physical

assortative mating theory stating that people find partners based on their similarity to each other.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

360 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

attractiveness strongly influences initial selections online (Sritharan et al., 2010), especially on sites such as Tinder and Grindr (Sumter, Vandenbosch, & Ligtenberg, 2017). Over a third of couples who marry first met online, often via online dating sites (Johnson, 2016).

One increasing trend among emerging adults is the hookup cul- ture of casual sex, often without even knowing the name of one’s sexual partner (Garcia et al., 2013; Kratzer & Stevens Aubrey, 2016). Research indicates both men and women are interested in having hookup sex but also prefer a more romantic relationship over the long run. How- ever, the perception there are no strings attached to hookup sex appear wrong, as nearly three-fourths of both men and women eventually expressed some level of regret at having hookup sex.

How do couple-forming behaviors compare cross-culturally? A few studies have examined the factors that attract people to each other in different cultures. In one now classic study, Buss and a large team of researchers (1990) identified the effects of culture and gender on heterosexual mate preferences in 37 cultures worldwide. Men and women in each culture displayed unique orderings of their preferences concerning the ideal char- acteristics of a mate. When all of the orderings and preferences were compared, two main dimensions emerged.

In the first main dimension, the characteristics of a desirable mate changed because of cultural values—that is, whether the respondents’ country has more traditional values or Western-industrial values. In traditional cultures, men place a high value on a woman’s chastity, desire for home and children, and ability to be a good cook and housekeeper; women place a high value on a man being ambitious and industrious, being a good finan- cial prospect, and holding favorable social status. China, India, Iran, and Nigeria represent the traditional end of this dimension. In contrast, people in Western-industrial cultures value these qualities to a much lesser extent. The Netherlands, Great Britain, Finland, and Sweden represent this end of the dimension; people in these countries place more value on Western ideals.

The second main dimension reflects the relative importance of education, intelli- gence, and social refinement—as opposed to a pleasing disposition—in choosing a mate. For example, people in Spain, Colombia, and Greece highly value education, intelligence, and social refinement; in contrast, people in Indonesia place a greater emphasis on having a pleasing disposition. Note that this dimension emphasizes the same traits for both men and women.

Chastity proved to be the characteristic showing the most variability across cultures, being highly desired in some cultures but mattering little in others. It is interesting that in their respective search for mates, men around the world value physical attractive- ness in  women, whereas women around the world look for men capable of being good providers. But men and women around the world agree that love and mutual attraction are most important, and nearly all cultures rate dependability, emotional stability, kindness, and understanding as important factors. Attraction, it seems, has some characteristics that transcend culture.

Overall, Buss and his colleagues concluded that mate selection is a complex process no matter where you live. However, each culture has a describable set of high-priority traits that men and women look for in the perfect mate. The study also shows that social- ization within a culture plays a key role in being attractive to the opposite sex; characteris- tics that are highly desirable in one culture may not be so desirable in another.

As described in the Spotlight on Research feature, Schmitt and his team of col- leagues (2004) studied 62 cultural regions. They showed secure romantic attachment was the norm in nearly 80% of cultures and “preoccupied” romantic attachment was particularly common in East Asian cultures. In general, multicultural studies show there are global patterns in mate selection and romantic relationships. The romantic attach- ment profiles of individual nations were correlated with sociocultural indicators in ways that supported evolutionary theories of romantic attachment and basic human mating strategies.

These Egyptian women, performing tradi- tional cultural tasks, are more likely to be desired as mates.

Co ur

te sy

o f J

oh n

C. C

av an

au gh

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 361

On ResearchSpotlight

Patterns and Universals of Roman- tic Attachment Around the World

Who were the investigators and what was the aim of the study? One’s attachment style may have a major influence on how one forms romantic relationships. In order to test this hypothesis, David Schmitt (2004) assembled a large international team of researchers.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Great care was taken to ensure equivalent translation of the survey across the 62 cultural regions included. The survey was a two-dimension four-category measure of adult romantic attachment (the Relationship Questionnaire) that measured models of self and others relative to each other: secure romantic attachment (high scores indicate positive models of self and others), dismissing romantic attachment (high scores indicate a positive model of self and a negative model of others), preoccupied romantic attachment (high scores indicate a negative model of self and a positive model of others), and fearful romantic attachment (high scores indicate negative models of self and others). An overall score of model of self is computed by adding together the secure and dismissing scores and subtracting the combi- nation of preoccupied and fearful scores. The overall model of others score is computed by adding together the secure and preoccupied scores and subtracting the combination of dismissing and fearful scores.

Additionally, there were measures of self- esteem, personality traits, and sociocultural correlates of romantic attachment (e.g., fertil- ity rate, national profiles of individualism ver- sus collectivism).

Who were the participants in the study? A total of 17,804 people (7,432 men and 10,372 women) from 62 cultural regions around the world took part in the study. Such large and diverse samples are unusual in developmental research.

What was the design of the study? Data for this cross-sectional, nonexperimental study were gathered by research teams in each country. The principal researchers asked the research collaborators to administer a nine-page survey to the participants that took 20 minutes to complete.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? Because the study involved volun- teers, there were no ethical concerns. How- ever, ensuring all participants’ rights were protected was a challenge because of the number of countries and cultures involved.

What were the results? The research- ers first demonstrated the model of self and others measures were valid across cultural regions, that provided general support for the independence of measures (i.e., they measure different things). Specific analyses

showed 79% of the cultural groups studied demonstrated secure romantic attachments, but North American cultures tended to be dismissive and East Asian cultures tended to be high on preoccupied romantic attachment. These patterns are shown in ◗ Figure 11.1. Note all the cultural regions except East Asia showed the pattern of model of self-scores higher than model of others scores.

What did the investigators conclude? Overall, Schmitt and colleagues concluded although the same attachment pattern holds across most cultures, no one pattern holds across all of them. East Asian cultures in particular tend to fit a pattern in which people report others do not get as emotionally close as the respondent would like, and respondents find it difficult to trust others or to depend on them.

●◗ Figure 11.1 In research across 10 global regions, note that only in East Asian cultures were the “model of others” scores higher than the “model of self” scores.

In te

rn al

w or

k in

g m

od el

s

World region

–1.0

–0.5

0.0

0.5

1.0

2.0

2.5

3.0

1.5

N or

th A

m er

ic a

S ou

th A

m er

ic a

W es

te rn

E u

ro p

e

E as

te rn

E u

ro p

e

S ou

th er

n E

u ro

p e

M id

d le

E as

t

A fr

ic a

O ce

an ia

S ou

th ea

st A

si a

E as

t A

si a

Model of self Model of others

SOURCE: Data from Schmitt et al. (2004).

Culture and evolution are powerful forces shaping mate selection choices (Buss, 2016). Specifically, Buss (2016) argues that mate selection serves many deeply held, and deeply programmed, human needs. The kinds of partners we choose are driven by certain hardwired biological and psychological needs and desires that play out in many ways.

Love styles within European cultures also predict relationship satisfaction (Rohmann, Führer, & Bierhoff, 2016). Cultural norms are sometimes highly resistant to change. Loyalty of the individual to the family is an important value in India, so despite many changes in

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

362 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

mate selection, about 95% of marriages in India are carefully arranged to ensure an appro- priate mate is selected (Dommaraju, 2010).

Similarly, Islamic societies use matchmaking as a way to preserve family consistency and continuity and ensure couples follow the prohibition on premarital relationships between men and women (Adler, 2001). Matchmaking in these societies occurs both through family connections and personal advertisements in newspapers. To keep up with the Internet age, Muslim matchmaking has gone online too (Lo & Aziz, 2009), increasing the pressure for the individuals in the couple to make their own decisions (Ahmad, 2012). As urbanization and globalization effects continue, pressures to move toward individual selection and com- mitment and away from traditional matchmaking continues to increase in many societies (Abela & Walker, 2014).

Developmental Forces, Neuroscience, and Love Relationships As you no doubt know from your own experience, finding a suitable relationship, espe- cially a love relationship, is tough. Many things must work just right: timing, meeting the right person, luck, and effort are but a few of the factors that shape the course of a relation- ship. Centuries of romance stories describe this magical process and portray it as one of life’s great mysteries.

From our discussion here, you know that who chooses whom (and whether the feel- ings will be mutual) results from the interaction of developmental forces described in the biopsychosocial model presented in Chapter 1. Neuroscience research is revealing how.

Love is one of three discrete, interrelated emotion systems (the sex drive and attach- ment are the other two; Fisher, 2016; Pfaff & Fisher, 2012; see also Helen Fisher’s series of TED Talks). The brain circuitry involved in romantic love, maternal love, and long-term attachment overlap (Fisher, 2016; Stein & Vythilingum, 2009). In terms of love, neuro- chemicals related to the amphetamines come into play early in the process, providing a biological explanation for the exhilaration of falling madly in love. Aron and colleagues (2005) reported that couples who were in the early stages of romantic love showed high levels of activity in the dopamine system, which is involved in all of the basic biological drives. Once the relationship settles into what some people might call long-term commit- ment and tranquility, the brain processes switch neurochemically to substances related to morphine, a powerful narcotic. People with a predilection to fall in love also tend to show left hemisphere chemical dominance and several changes in neurochemical processing (Fisher, 2016; Kurup & Kurup, 2003).

Additional research indicates that the hormone oxytocin may play an important role in attachment. In men, it enhances their partner’s attractiveness compared to other females (Scheele et al., 2013); in women, it enhances their orgasms, among other things (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2013), which has earned it the nickname of the “cuddle hormone” (Lee et al., 2009). Love really does a number on your brain!

And that’s not all. The interactions among psychological aspects, neurological aspects, and hormonal aspects of romantic love help explain why couples tend to have exclusive relationships with each other. For women (but not men), blood levels of serotonin increase during periods of romantic love (Langeslag, van der Veen, & Fekkes, 2012). In addition, the stronger the romantic bond with their boyfriend, the less likely they are to be able to iden- tify the body odor of a different male friend (Lundström & Jones-Gotman, 2009). That means that women’s attention is deflected from other potential male partners the more they are romantically involved with one specific male.

Psychologically, as we saw in Chapter 10, an important developmental issue is inti- macy; according to Erikson, mature relationships are impossible without it. In addi- tion, the kinds of relationships you saw and experienced as a child (and whether they involved violence) affect how you define and act in relationships you develop as an adult. Sociocultural forces shape the characteristics you find desirable in a mate and deter- mine whether you are likely to encounter resistance from your family when you have made your choice. Life-cycle forces matter, too; different aspects of love are more or less important depending on your stage in life. For example, romantic love tends to be most

Think About It What are the effects of increasing interactions among cultures on mate selection?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 363

prominent in young adulthood, whereas the aspect of companionship becomes more important later in life.

In short, to understand adult relationships, we must take the forces of the biopsy- chosocial model into account. Relying too heavily on one or two of the forces provides an incomplete description of why people are or are not successful in finding a partner or a friend. Unfortunately, the developmental forces do not influence only good relationships. As we will see next, sometimes relationships turn violent.

Violence in Relationships Up to this point, we have been considering relationships that are healthy and positive. Sadly, this is not always the case. Sometimes relationships become violent; one person becomes aggressive toward the partner, creating an abusive relationship. Such relation- ships have received increasing attention since the early 1980s, when the U.S. criminal jus- tice system ruled that, under some circumstances, abusive relationships can be used as an explanation for one’s behavior (Walker, 1984). For example, battered woman syndrome occurs when a woman believes she cannot leave the abusive situation and may even go so far as to kill her abuser.

Being female, Latina, African American, having an atypical family structure (some- thing other than two biological parents), having more romantic partners, early onset of sexual activity, and being a victim of child abuse predicts a higher likelihood of being a survivor of relationship violence. Although overall national rates of sexual assault have declined more than 60% since the early 1990s, acquaintance rape or date rape is still a major problem. College women are more likely to be survivors of sexual assault than are women in other age groups (Rape, Abuse, and Incest National Network, 2016). The Obama administration made campus sexual assault a major policy focus, creating the Not Alone campaign (NotAlone.gov, 2016).

What range of aggressive behaviors occurs in abusive relation- ships? What causes such abuse? Based on considerable research on abusive partners, O’Leary (1993) proposed a continuum of aggressive behaviors toward a partner, and progresses as follows: verbally aggres- sive behaviors, physically aggressive behaviors, severe physically aggressive behaviors, and murder (see ◗ Table 11.1). The causes of the abuse also vary with the type of abusive behavior being expressed.

Two points about the continuum should be noted. First, there may be fundamental differences in the types of aggression indepen- dent of level of severity. Overall, each year about 5 million women and 3 million men experience partner-related physical assaults and rape in the United States (Rape, Abuse, and Incest National Network, 2016); worldwide, between 10% and 69% of women report being physically assaulted or raped, making it one of the priority areas for the World Health Organization (World Health Organization, 2013).

The second point, depicted in the table, is the suspected underly- ing causes of aggressive behaviors differ as the type of aggressive behav- iors change (O’Leary, 1993; Sugimoto-Matsuda & Guerrero, 2016). Although anger and hostility in the perpetrator are associated with var- ious forms of physical abuse, especially in young adulthood, the exact nature of this relationship remains elusive (Giordano et al., 2016).

Heterosexual men and members of the LGBTQ community are also the victims of violence from intimate partners, though at a reporting rate lower that of women (Rape, Abuse and Incest National Network, 2016). All victims need to be supported and provided ave- nues that provide safe ways for them to report assaults.

Culture is also an important contextual factor in understand- ing partner abuse. In particular, violence against women worldwide reflects cultural traditions, beliefs, and values of patriarchal societies;

abusive relationship relationships in which one person becomes aggressive toward the partner.

battered woman syndrome situation occurring when a woman believes that she cannot leave the abusive situation and may even go so far as to kill her abuser.

Many communities have established shel- ters for women who have experienced abuse in relationships.

Jo hn

B ird

sa ll/

Th e

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

364 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

this can be seen in the commonplace violent practices against women that include sexual slavery, female genital cutting, intimate partner violence, and “honor” killing (Ghanim, 2015; World Health Organization, 2013).

Additionally, international data indicate rates of abuse are higher in cultures that emphasize female purity, virginity, male status, and family honor. A common cause of women’s murders in Arab countries is brothers or other male relatives performing so- called honor killings, murdering the victim because she violated the family’s honor (Ghanim, 2015). Intimate partner violence is prevalent in China (43% lifetime risk in one study) and has strong associations with male patriarchal values, male unemployment, and conflict resolutions (Xu et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2016).

Alarmed by the seriousness of abuse, many communities established shelters for peo- ple who experience abuse. However, the legal system in many localities is still not set up to deal with domestic violence; for example, women in some locations cannot sue their husbands for assault, and restraining orders all too often offer little real protection from additional violence. Much remains to be done to protect people from the fear and the real- ity of abuse.

Table 11.1

Continuum of progressive behaviors in abusive relationships

Verbal aggression

Physical aggression

seVere aggression

Murder

Insults Pushing Beating

Yelling Slapping Punching

Name-calling Shoving Hitting with object

causes

Need to control*

Misuse of power*

Jealousy*

Marital discord

Accept violence as a means of control

Modeling of physical aggression

Abused as a child

Aggressive personality styles

Alcohol abuse

Personality disorders

Emotional lability

Poor self-esteem

contributing factors      

Job stresses

unemployment

NOTE: Need to control and other variables on the left are associated with all forms of aggression; acceptance of violence and other variables in the middle are associated with physical aggression, severe aggression, and murder. Personality disorders and the variables on the right are associated with severe aggression and murder. *More relevant for males than for females. SOURCE: O’Leary, K.D. (1993). Through a psychological lens: Personality traits, personality disorders, and levels of violence. In R. J. Gelles & D. R. Loseke (Eds.), Current Controversies on Family Violence (pp. 7–30). Copyright © 1993 by Sage Publications. Reprinted by permission of the publisher.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 365

Kevin and Beth are on cloud nine. They got married one month ago and have recently returned from their honeymoon. Everyone who sees them can tell that they love each other a great deal. They are highly compatible and have much in common, sharing most of their leisure activities. Kevin and Beth wonder what lies ahead in their marriage.

Developing relationships is only part of the picture in understanding how adults live their lives with other people. Putting relationships such as Kevin and Beth’s in context is impor- tant for us to understand how relationships come into existence and how they change over time. In the following sections, we explore relationship lifestyles: singlehood, cohabitation, same-sex couples, and marriage.

Singlehood Adult men and women are single—defined as not living with an intimate partner—at multiple points in their lives: before marriage or other long-term commitment, follow- ing divorce, and in widowhood are common examples. In this section, we focus most on young adult singles; elsewhere we return to singlehood in the context of divorce or the death of a spouse/partner.

What’s it like to be a single young adult in the United States? It’s tougher than you might think. Several researchers (e.g., Budgeon, 2016; Casper et al., 2016; DePaulo, 2006) point out numerous stereotypes and biases against single people, especially women. For example, married people are perceived as kinder and more giving, and public policy also tends to favor married individuals. Additionally, research indicates that rental agents and certain housing programs prefer married couples over singles (Goodsell, 2013; Morris, Sinclair, & DePaulo, 2007).

Many women and men remain single as young adults to focus on establishing their careers rather than marriage or relationships that most do later. Others report they simply did not meet “the right person” or prefer singlehood, a factor especially important among strongly religious groups (Engelberg, 2016; Ibrahim & Hassan, 2009). However, the pres- sure to marry is especially strong for women (Budgeon, 2016).

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the challenges and advantages of being single?

●● Why do people cohabit?

●● What are LGBTQ relationships like?

●● What is marriage like across adulthood?

11.2 Lifestyles and Relationships

Recall 1. Friendships based on intimacy and emotional sharing are

more characteristic of .

2. Competition is a major part of most friendships among .

3. Love relationships in which intimacy and passion are present but commitment is not are termed .

4. Chastity is an important quality that men look for in a poten- tial female mate in cultures.

5. Aggressive behavior that is based on abuse of power, jeal- ousy, or the need to control is more likely to be displayed by

.

Interpret ●● Why, according to Erikson, is intimacy (discussed in

Chapter 9) a necessary prerequisite for adult relationships? What aspects of relationships discussed here support (or refute) this view?

apply ●● Based on Schmitt and colleagues’ (2004) research, what

attachment pattern would Korean women likely have regard- ing romantic attachment?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 11.1

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

366 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Men remain single a bit longer in young adulthood because they marry about two years later on average than women (Census Bureau, 2016). Fewer men than women remain unmarried throughout adulthood, though, mainly because men find partners more easily as they select from a larger age range of unmarried women.

Ethnic differences in singlehood reflect differences in age at marriage as well as social factors. Nearly twice as many African Americans are single during young adult- hood as European Americans, and more are choosing to remain so. Singlehood is also increasing among Latinos, in part because the average age of Latinos in the United States is lower than other ethnic groups (e.g., 28 for Latinos versus 42 for white non-Hispanic) and in  part because of poor economic opportunities for many Latinos (Pew Research Center, 2015).

The millennial generation is also changing the assumptions about singlehood. The Urban Institute (Martin, Astone, & Peters, 2014) projects that the percentage of millen- nials who will remain single until at least age 40 may be as high as 30%, higher than any previous generation. However, also likely are millennials who decide to live with a partner and forego the legality of marriage.

Globally, the meanings and implications of remaining single are often tied to strongly held cultural and religious beliefs. Muslim women who remain single in Malaysia speak in terms of jodoh (the soul mate one finds through fate at a time appointed by God) as a reason; they believe God simply has not decided to have them meet their mate at this time (Ibrahim & Hassan, 2009). But because the role of Malaysian women is to marry, they also understand their marginalized position in society through their singlehood. In Southeast Asia, the number of single adults has increased steadily as education levels have risen over the past several decades (Hull, 2009). However, family systems in these cultures have not yet adapted to these changing lifestyle patterns (Jones, 2010).

An important distinction is between adults who are temporarily single (i.e., those who are single only until they find a suitable marriage partner) and those who choose to remain single. For most singles, the decision to never marry is a gradual one. This transition is represented by a change in self-attributed status that occurs over time and is associated with a cultural timetable for marriage. It marks the experience of “becoming single” that occurs when an individual identifies more with singlehood than with marriage (Davies, 2003). Choosing to remain single can also reflect an economic-based decision, especially for millennials, who are less likely to think they are better off than their parents than their own parents and grandparents thought at the same age (Kalish, 2016).

Cohabitation Being unmarried does not necessarily mean living alone. People in committed, intimate, sexual relationships but who are not married may decide living together, or cohabitation, provides a way to share daily life. Cohabitation is becoming an increasingly popular life- style choice in the United States as well as in Canada, Europe, Australia, and elsewhere, especially among millennials and older adults (Luxenberg, 2014; Martin et al., 2014).

In the United States, evidence clearly indicates that cohabitation is common, and has increased over the past several decades (Copen et al., 2013). For example, roughly half of all women cohabit with, rather than marry, a partner as a first committed relationship. Such cohabitation patterns range from about 70% for women with less than a high school diploma to about half of women with a baccalaureate degree or higher, and are longest for Latina women (33 months on average) and shortest for European American women (19 months on average). Cohabitation rates for adults 50 years of age and older have more than doubled since 2000.

The global picture differs by culture (Popenoe, 2009; Therborn, 2010). In most Euro- pean, South American, and Caribbean countries, cohabitation is a common alternative to marriage for young adults. Cohabitation is extremely common in the Netherlands, Norway, and Sweden, where this lifestyle is part of the culture; 99% of married couples in Sweden lived together before they married and nearly one in four couples are not legally married. Decisions to marry in these countries are typically made to legalize the relationship after

cohabitation people in committed, intimate, sexual relationships who live together but are not married.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 367

children are born—in contrast to Americans, who marry to confirm their love and com- mitment to each other.

Interestingly, having cohabitated does not seem to make marriages any better; in fact, under certain circumstances it may do more harm than good, resulting in lower quality marriages (Johnson, 2016). These findings reflect two underlying issues that can cause problems (Johnson, 2016): couples who have children while cohabiting, especially for European American women (as compared with African American and Latina women; Tach & Halpern-Meekin, 2009), and couples who are using cohabitation to test their rela- tionship and keep separate finances (Addo, 2017).

Essentially, the happiest cohabiting couples are those who look very much like happily married couples: they share financial responsibilities and child care. Longitudinal studies find few differences in couples’ behavior after living together for many years regardless of whether they married without cohabiting, cohabited then married, or simply cohabited (Stafford, Kline, & Rankin, 2004). Additionally, many countries extend the same rights and benefits to cohabiting couples as they do to married couples, and have done so for many years. Argentina provides pension rights to cohabiting partners, Canada extends insurance benefits, and Australia has laws governing the disposition of property when cohabiting couples sever their relationship (Neft & Levine, 1997).

LGBTQ Relationships The current generation of adults in the LGBTQ community have largely experienced vari- ous forms of oppression and discrimination throughout their adult lives (Robinson-Wood & Weber, 2016). This is especially true for those who also have low income and for people of color. However, social attitudes toward the LGBTQ community have changed and continue to change rapidly, especially in view of the legal support for same-sex marriage (King, 2016).

For the most part, the relationships of gay and lesbian couples have many similari- ties to those of heterosexual couples (Kurdek, 2004). Most gay and lesbian couples are in dual-earner relationships, much like the majority of married heterosexual couples, and are likely to share household chores. However, gay and lesbian couples differ from hetero- sexual couples in the degree to which both partners are similar on demographic character- istics such as race, age, and education; gay and lesbian couples tend to be more dissimilar, except regarding education (Ciscato, Galichon, & Gousse, 2015).

King (2016) argues that the experiences of older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults can- not be put into need categories, even generational ones. Rather, King emphasizes that the LGB community is at least as diverse as the heterosexual community, and needs to be understood as such.

With the advent of legalized same-sex marriage, numerous issues that heterosexual married couples have long taken for granted are being confronted in the LGBTQ com- munity, including end-of-life issues and legal matters regarding caregiving (Godfrey, 2016; Orel & Coon, 2016). Changes to state laws will continue as reforms and revisions continue to keep pace with change.

When compared to LGB individuals living with partners, LGB individuals living alone or with others (but not in a relationship with them) reported higher degrees of loneliness (Kim & Fredriksen-Goldsen, 2016). This finding parallels that in heterosexual individuals in similar living arrangements, and highlights the fact that there are many similarities in personal outcomes across various gender identity groups.

Little research has been conducted examining the development of transgender and gender nonconforming (TGNC) individuals across adulthood (Witten, 2016). One detailed examination of experiences of TGNC adults found that many barriers to access- ing key services such as health care and social services exist, largely due to anti-TGNC prejudice, discrimination, and lack of appropriate and adequate training of professionals (Porter et al., 2016). Additionally, research indicates that transgender older adults experi- ence social isolation more than most other groups (Harley, Gassaway, & Dunkley, 2016). However, relationship stress patterns and outcomes parallel that seen in other types of couple relationships (Randall & Bodenmann, 2017).

Think About It Why might there be large differences in cohabitation rates among countries?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

368 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

As we explore marriage in the next section, keep in mind that although most of the research has been conducted with heterosexual couples, key findings regarding what matters most in relationship satisfaction, decisions about parenting, and caring for a spouse/partner hold for LGBTQ individuals who are married or in long-term committed relationships.

Marriage Most adults want their love relationships to result in marriage. However, U.S. residents are in less of a hurry to achieve this goal; the median age at first marriage for adults in the United States has been rising for several decades. As shown in ◗ Figure 11.2, between 1970 and 2015, the median age for first marriage rose almost 7 years for both men and women (Census Bureau, 2016).

An important fact to keep in mind is that these statistics reflect heterosexual mar- riage. Until the 2015 Obergefell v. Hodges decision in the U.S. Supreme Court, same- sex marriage was still illegal in 14 states. This case is highlighted in the Real People feature.

What Is a Successful Marriage and What Predicts It? You undoubtedly know couples who appear to have a successful marriage. But what does that mean, really? Minnotte (2010) differentiates marital success, an umbrella term refer- ring to any marital outcome (such as divorce rate), marital quality, a subjective evaluation of the couple’s relationship on a number of different dimensions, marital adjustment, the degree spouses accommodate each other over a certain period of time, and marital satisfaction, a global assessment of one’s marriage. Each of these provides a unique insight into the workings of a marriage.

Marriages, like other relationships, differ from one another, but some important pre- dictors of future success can be identified. One key factor is age. In general, the younger the partners are, the lower the odds the marriage will last—especially when the people are in their teens or early 20s (Census Bureau, 2013). Other reasons that increase or decrease the likelihood a marriage will last include financial security and pregnancy at the time of the marriage.

marital success umbrella term referring to any marital outcome.

marital quality subjective evaluation of the couple’s relationship on a number of different dimensions.

marital adjustment degree to which a husband and wife accommodate to each other over a certain period of time.

marital satisfaction global assessment of one’s marriage.

●◗ Figure 11.2 Note the median age at first mar- riage has been increasing for many years during the 20th and early 21st centuries.

18

20

22

24

26

28

30

1890 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010 16

A ge

( ye

ar s)

Men

Women

SOURCE: U.S Census Bureau, Current Population Survey, Annual Social and Economic Supplements, 1947–2012. Data for years prior to 1947 are from decennial censuses. www.census.gov/hhes/families/data/marital.html.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 369

A second important predictor of successful marriage is homogamy, or the similarity of values and interests a couple shares. As we saw in relation to choosing a mate, the extent the partners share similar age, values, goals, attitudes (especially the desire for children), socioeconomic status, certain behaviors (such as drinking alcohol), and ethnic background increases the likelihood their relationship will succeed (Kippen, Chapman, & Yu, 2009).

A third factor in predicting marital success is a feeling the relationship is equal. According to exchange theory, marriage is based on each partner contributing something to the relationship the other would be hard-pressed to provide. Satisfying and happy mar- riages result when both partners perceive there is a fair exchange, or equity, in all the dimensions of the relationship. Problems achieving equity arise because of the competing demands of work and family, an issue we take up again in Chapter 12.

Cross-cultural research supports these factors. Couples in the United States and Iran (Asoodeh et al., 2010; Hall, 2006) say trust, consulting each other, honesty, making joint decisions, and commitment make the difference between a successful marriage and an unsuccessful marriage. Couples for whom religion is important also point to commonly held faith as a key factor.

So what really matters in predicting whether a relationship is likely to be successful? Dey and Ghosh (2016)’s findings point to several key predictors: respect for emotion, atti- tude toward marriage, expression of love, regard for views and importance to the likings of the spouse, ignoring weaknesses of the spouse, sexual adjustment, temperament, value, taste, and interest. We will see in the next few sections how these, and other factors, play out at specific times during the course of marriage across adulthood.

homogamy similarity of values and interests.

exchange theory relationship, such as marriage, based on each partner contributing something to the relationship that the other would be hard- pressed to provide.

James Obergefell and John Arthur Make History

Marshall Airport on July 11, 2013. Arthur died three months later.

Shortly before Arthur’s death, they sued the state of Ohio in order for Jim to be listed as the surviving spouse on Arthur’s death cer- tificate. They lost the case but were granted a hearing by the U.S. Supreme Court. Their case was combined with several others for the hearing. By a 5 to 4 decision, the Court ruled

that the U.S. Constitution supports same-sex marriage, thereby making these marriages legal in the entire country.

Together with the Loving v. Virginia (1967) decision (depicted in the movie Loving) that banned anti-miscegenation laws, the Obergefell v. Hodges decision opened mar- riage to all couples who had, in one way or another, been refused that right in U.S. history.

Real People

James (Jim) Obergefell

When James (Jim) Obergefell met John Arthur in Cincinnati in 1992, there was no way they thought that their love relationship would make history. Neither one of them had ever been an activist, and it took tragedy and loss to set them on their mission.

After they fell in love, they lived and worked together as consultants. They were avid art col- lectors and had a large social network of friends and family. Nearly 20 years into their relation- ship, though, everything changed. Arthur, who had been experiencing severe mobility prob- lems, was diagnosed with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), also known as Lou Gehrig’s dis- ease. As the disease progressed, Jim became Arthur’s primary caregiver. By 2013, Arthur’s condition had progressed very significantly.

What both of them wanted was to be married so that Jim could be recognized as Arthur’s surviving spouse on Arthur’s death certificate. Unfortunately for them, Ohio vot- ers had passed a constitutional amendment banning same-sex marriage. However, such marriages were legal in Maryland. With sup- port from friends, Jim hired a medical plane to fly them to Maryland, where Arthur’s aunt officiated at their wedding on the tarmac at Baltimore-Washington International Thurgood

A nd

re w

H ar

re r/

Bl oo

m be

rg /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

370 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Do Married Couples Stay Happy? Few sights are happier than a couple on their wedding day. Newlyweds, like Kevin and Beth in the vignette, are at the peak of marital bliss. The beliefs people bring into a marriage influence how satisfied they will be as the marriage develops. But as you may have experienced, feelings change over time, sometimes getting better and stronger, sometimes not.

Research shows for most couples, overall marital satisfaction is highest at the begin- ning of the marriage, falls until the children begin leaving home, and stabilizes or continues to decline in later life; this pattern holds for both married and never-married cohabit- ing couples with children (Kulik, 2016). However, there is considerable variability across couples. For some couples, satisfaction declines only slightly, while for others it rebounds in late life, while for still others it declines more precipitously and the couple becomes, in essence, emotionally divorced (Proulx, 2016).

The pattern of a particular marriage over the years is determined by the nature of the dependence of each spouse on the other. When dependence is mutual and about equal and both people hold similar values that form the basis for their commitment to each other, the marriage is strong and close (Givertz, Segrin, & Hanzal, 2009). When the dependence of one partner is much higher than that of the other, however, the marriage is likely to be characterized by stress and conflict. Learning how to deal with these changes is the secret to long and happy marriages.

The fact that marital satisfaction has a general downward trend but varies widely across couples led Karney and Bradbury (1995) to propose a vulnerability–stress–adaptation model of marriage, depicted in ◗ Figure 11.3. The vulnerability–stress–adaptation model sees marital quality as a dynamic process resulting from the couple’s ability to handle stressful events in the context of their particular vulnerabilities and resources. As a couple’s ability to adapt to stressful situations gets better over time, the quality of the marriage will  probably improve. How well couples adapt to various stresses on the relationship determines whether the marriage continues or they get divorced. Let’s see how this works over time.

Setting the Stage: The Early Years of Marriage Marriages are most intense in their early days. Early on, husbands and wives share many activities and are open to new experiences together, so bliss results (Olson & McCubbin, 1983). But bliss doesn’t come from avoiding tough issues. Discussing financial matters honestly is key since many newly married couples experience their first serious marital stresses around money issues (Parkman, 2007). How tough issues early in the marriage are handled sets the stage for the years ahead.

Early in a marriage, couples tend to have global adoration for their spouse regard- ing the spouse’s qualities (Karney, 2010; Seychell, 2016). For wives, but not for husbands, more accurate specific perceptions of what their spouses are really like were associated

vulnerability–stress–adaptation model model that proposes that marital quality is a dynamic process resulting from the couple’s ability to handle stressful events in the context of their particular vulnerabili- ties and resources.

●◗ Figure 11.3 The vulnerability-stress-adaptation model shows how adapting to vul- nerabilities and stress can result in either adaptation or dissolution of the marriage.

Enduring Vulnerabilities

Initial Satisfaction

External Stressors

Adaptive Processes

Change in Marital

Satisfaction

Marital Dissolution

SOURCE: From “Keeping marriages healthy, and why it’s so difficult,” by B. R. Karney.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 371

with more supportive behaviors, feelings of control in the marriage, and a decreased risk of divorce. Couples who are happiest in the early stage of their marriage focus on the good aspects, not the annoyances; nit-picking and nagging do not bode well for long-term wedded bliss (Seychell, 2016).

As time goes on and stresses increase, marital satisfaction declines (Kulik, 2016). For many couples, the primary reason for this drop is having children. It’s not just a matter of having a child. The tempera- ment of the child matters, with fussier babies creating more marital problems (Greving, 2007; Meijer & van den Wittenboer, 2007). Par- enthood also means having substantially less time to devote to the marriage.

However, using the birth of a child as the explanation for the drop in marital satisfaction is much too simplistic, because child-free cou- ples also experience a decline in marital satisfaction (Kulik, 2016). Longitudinal research indicates disillusionment—as demonstrated by a decline in feeling in love, in demonstra- tions of affection, and in the feeling that one’s spouse is responsive, as well as an increase in feelings of ambivalence—and other personality characteristics such as narcissism are key predictors of marital dissatisfaction (Lavner et al., 2016).

During the early years of their marriage, many couples may spend significant amounts of time apart, especially those who are in the military (Fincham & Beach, 2010). Spouses who serve in combat areas on active duty assignment and who suffer from post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are particularly vulnerable, as they are at greater risk for other spouse-directed aggression.

What the nondeployed spouse believes turns out to be important. If the nonde- ployed spouse believes the deployment will have negative effects on the marriage, then problems are much more likely. In contrast, if the nondeployed spouse believes such challenges make the relationship stronger, then they typically can do so (Lewis, Lamson, & White, 2016; Renshaw, Rodrigues, & Jones, 2008). Research indicates the effects of deployment may be greater on wives than on husbands; divorce rates for women ser- vice members who are deployed is higher than for their male counterparts (Karney & Crown, 2007).

Keeping Marriages Happy Although no two marriages are exactly the same, couples must be flexible and adaptable. Couples who have been happily married for many years show an ability to “roll with the punches” and to adapt to changing circumstances in the relationship. For example, a serious problem of one spouse may not be detrimental to the relationship and may even make the bond stronger if the couple use good stress- and conflict-reduction strate- gies. Successful couples also find a way to keep the romance in the relationship, a very important determinant of marital satisfaction over the long run. Being motivated to meet a partner’s sexual needs seems to be a key factor (Birnbaum et al., 2016; Muise et al., 2013).

Sharing religious beliefs and spirituality with one’s spouse is another good way to ensure higher quality marriages, and that’s especially the case among couples in lower socioeconomic groups (Lichter & Carmalt, 2009). It appears that this effect goes beyond merely doing an activity together, as religion and spirituality may provide a framework for conflict resolution and a way to put one’s marriage in a bigger, more significant context.

But when you get down to basics, it’s how well couples communicate their thoughts, actions, and feelings to each other and show intimacy and support each other that largely determines the level of conflict couples experience and, by extension, how happy they are likely to be over the long term (Patrick et al., 2007). This is especially important with regard to high-stress areas such as children and work and after long separations such as military deployments (Wadsworth, Hughes-Kirchubel, & Riggs, 2014).

Young married military couples face special types of stress on their relationship.

Sa nd

y H

uf fa

ke r/

G et

ty Im

ag es

N ew

s/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Think About It What types of interventions would help keep married couples happier?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

372 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

So what are the best ways to “stack the deck” in favor of a long, happy marriage? Based on research, here are the best:

●● Make time for your relationship. ●● Express your love to your spouse. ●● Be there in times of need. ●● Communicate constructively and positively about problems in the relationship. ●● Be interested in your spouse’s life. ●● Confide in your spouse. ●● Forgive minor offenses and try to understand major ones.

Recall 1. A difficulty for many single people is that other people may

expect them to .

2. Young adults view cohabitation as a(n) marriage.

3. Same-sex relationships are similar to .

4. According to , marriage is based on each partner contributing something to the relationship that the other would be hard-pressed to provide.

5. For most couples, marital satisfaction after the birth of the first child.

Interpret ●● What sociocultural forces affect decisions to marry rather than

to cohabit indefinitely?

apply ●● Ricardo and Maria are engaged to be married. Ricardo works

long hours as a store manager at a local coffee shop, while Maria works regular hours as an administrative assistant for a large communications company. Based on your understand- ing of the factors that affect marital success, what other char- acteristics would you want to know about Ricardo and Maria before evaluating the likely success of their marriage?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 11.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is it like to be a parent? ●● What diverse forms of parenting are there?

11.3 Family Dynamics and the Life Course

Bob, 32, and Denise, 33, just had their first child, Matthew, after several years of trying. They’ve heard that having children in their thirties can have advantages, but Bob and Denise wonder whether people are just saying that to be nice to them. They are also concerned about the financial obligations they are likely to face.

Bob and Denise are increasingly typical of first-time parents, as more people than ever delay having children. As all parents discover, having a child changes everything. New parents are often overwhelmed with the responsibility.

In this section, we consider the dynamics of families, from deciding whether to have children through caring for aging parents and grandparenthood. As we do so, we must recognize the concept of “family” is undergoing change.

The Parental Role The birth of a child transforms a couple (or a single parent) into a family. The most common form of family in most Western societies is the nuclear family, consisting only of parent(s) and child(ren). However, the most common family form globally is the extended family, in which grandparents and other relatives live with parents and children. Let’s see how add- ing children to either family form matters.

nuclear family most common form of family in Western societies, consisting only of parent(s) and child(ren).

extended family most common form of family around the world; one in which grandparents and other relatives live with parents and children.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 373

Deciding Whether to Have Children One of the biggest decisions couples (and many singles) make is whether to have children. You would think that potential parents must weigh the many benefits of child rearing— such as feeling personal satisfaction, fulfilling personal needs, continuing the family line, and enjoying companionship—with the many drawbacks, including expense and lifestyle changes, especially the balance between work and family. But you would be wrong—this is not what most people actually do.

Rijken (2009; Rijken & Knijn, 2009) reports potential parents actually don’t think deliberately or deeply about when to have a child, and those who are career-oriented or like their freedom do not often deliberately postpone parenthood because of those factors. Rather, thoughts about having children are implicit and do not cross their minds until they are ready to begin thinking about having children.

Whether the pregnancy is planned or not (and more than half of all U.S. pregnan- cies are unplanned), a couple’s first pregnancy is a milestone event in a relationship, with both benefits and stresses (Lavner & Bradbury, 2017; Meijer & van den Wittenboer, 2007). Parents largely agree children add affection, improve family ties, and give parents a feeling of immortality and sense of accomplishment. Most parents willingly sacrifice a great deal for their children and hope they grow up to be happy and successful. In this way, children bring happiness to their parents’ relationship.

Nevertheless, finances are of great concern to most parents because children are expensive. How expensive? According to the U.S. Department of Agriculture (2017), a typ- ical family who had a child born in 2015 would spend about $ 233,610 for food, housing, and other necessities by the time the child turns 18 years old. College expenses would be additional. These costs do not differ significantly between two-parent and single-parent households but clearly are a bigger financial burden for single parents. Costs vary sub- stantially across levels of household income. Take a look at ◗Figure 11.4 and see where the money goes.

For many reasons that include personal choice, financial instability, and infertility, an increasing number of couples are child-free. Social attitudes in many countries (Austria, Germany, Great Britain, Ireland, Netherlands, and United States) are improving toward child-free couples (Blackstone & Stewart, 2016; Guberns- kaya, 2010).

Couples without children have some advantages: higher marital satisfaction, more freedom, and higher standards of living on average. Yet, they also must deal with societal expec- tations regarding having children and may feel defensive about their decision not to be a parent. A major international study of older adult couples without children in Australia, Finland, Germany, Japan, the Netherlands, the United King- dom, and the United States revealed highly similar patterns across all countries except Japan (Koropeckyj-Cox & Call, 2007). In Japan, the cultural norm of children caring for older parents created difficulties for childless older couples.

The factors that influence the decision to be child- free appear to differ for women and men (Waren & Pals, 2013). For women, higher levels of education and economic factors increase the likelihood of remaining child-free, whereas holding traditional sex role beliefs decrease the chances of that decision. For men, though, neither educa- tion nor economic factors predict deciding to remain child- free, but traditional sex role beliefs lower the odds of that decision.

Today, parents in the United States typically have fewer children and have their first child later than in the past. The average age at the time of the birth of a woman’s first child is nearly 26.3 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016c).

●◗ Figure 11.4 The Cost of Raising a Child from Birth to 18.

$30,000

$454,770 total for parents making more than $107,400*

A new USDA report found that parents in each income bracket will spend vastly dif ferent sums of money each year as their children grow from infancy to age 17. This chart shows the estimated annual cost of raising a child born in 2015 for married parents in each income group.

*Annual pre-tax income.

$284,570 total for parents making $59,200 to $107,400

$212,300 total for parents making less than $59,200

20,000

10,000

0 Infant 5 10 15

SOURCE: USDA Expenditures on Children by Families, 2013, United States Department of Agriculture, Center for Nutrition Policy and Promotion, Miscellaneous Publication Number 1528–2013. Retrieved from www.cnpp.usda.gov/sites/default/files/ expenditures_ on_children_by_families/crc2013.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

374 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

This average age has been increasing steadily since 1970 as a result of many women postponing having children because they marry later, they want to establish careers first, or they make a deliberate choice to delay childbearing.

Being older at the birth of one’s first child is advantageous. Older moth- ers, like Denise in the vignette, are more at ease being parents, spend more time with their babies, and are more affectionate, sensitive, and supportive to them. In addition, there is a higher maternal investment in middle child- hood and less child-perceived conflict in adolescence (Camberis et al., 2016; Schlomer & Belsky, 2012). The age of the father also makes a difference in how he interacts with children (Palkovitz & Palm, 2009). Compared to men who become fathers in their 20s, men such as Bob who become fathers in their 30s are generally more invested in their paternal role and spend up to three times as much time caring for their preschool children as younger fathers do. Father involvement has increased significantly, due in part to social attitudes that sup- port it (Fogarty & Evans, 2010). Research clearly shows that being a father is an important aspect of men’s lives across adulthood (Marsiglio & Roy, 2013).

Parenting skills do not come naturally; they must be acquired. Having  a child changes all aspects of couples’ lives. As we have seen, children place a  great deal of stress on a relationship. Both motherhood and fatherhood require major commitment and cooperation. Parenting is full of rewards, but it also takes a great deal of work. Caring for young children is demanding. It may create disagreements over division of labor, especially if both parents are employed outside the home (see Chapters 4 and 12). Even when mothers are  employed outside the home (and roughly 70% of women with children under age 18 in the United States are), they still perform most of the child- rearing tasks. Even when men take employment leave, although more likely to

share tasks, they still do not spend more time with children than fathers who do not take leave (Seward & Stanley-Stevens, 2014).

The United States is the only major industrialized country not to provide man- datory paid leave for new parents. Although some states and cities have passed laws requiring paid parental leave, most have not. The What Do You Think? feature explores the issue of paid parental leave in more detail.

Having a child later in adulthood has many benefits.

Ro hi

t S et

h/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Does your employer provide paid time off if you have a new child? In order to care for an ill child or older parent? If you have a serious illness or need to recover after surgery? If so, you are fortunate. One of the biggest needs for most employees is access to these types of benefits, espe- cially employees in smaller organizations or employers that do not provide paid time off. In fact, the United States is the only industrialized country that does not either have a national government program or an employer mandate to provide such benefits (A Better Balance, 2016).

Currently, the federal Family and Medical Leave Act of 1993 (FMLA) is the only national program in the U.S. It requires employers to provide unpaid leave for up to 12 weeks for

employees to care for a new child or seri- ously ill family member, to recover from one’s own serious health condition, or to deal with certain obligations (including child care and related activities) arising from a spouse, par- ent or child being on, or called to, active duty in the military. The FMLA also provides up to 26 weeks of unpaid leave per year for workers whose spouse, child, parent or next of kin is a seriously ill or injured member of the armed services (A Better Balance, 2016).

However, due to the specific provisions that create coverage gaps in the law, roughly 40% of employees are not covered. And, the unpaid nature of the law means that only those who can afford to lose their income take it. To address these gaps and provide some income coverage, the Family Act was

introduced in Congress in 2015. It would pro- vide 12 weeks of paid leave for the situations covered in FMLA. It has yet to be enacted.

Fortunately, a few states have taken the lead to create paid family leave programs. For example, California, New Jersey, New York, Rhode Island, and Washington, DC have different types of paid leave. Several other states are working to enact other versions. (Find out what the policy is in your home state.) Many advocates and employees agree that paid leave for family reasons is important. Many businesses do as well. As we have seen, the evidence is strong that being able to care for one’s family or oneself improves well-being, and helps the employee be more productive. What do you think should be done?

Paid Family LeaveWhat Do You Think?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 375

In general, parents manage to deal with the many challenges of child rearing reason- ably well. They learn how to compromise when necessary and when to apply firm but fair discipline. Given the choice, most parents do not regret their decision to have children.

Ethnic Diversity and Parenting Ethnic background matters a great deal in terms of family structure and the parent–child relationship. African American husbands are more likely than their European American counterparts to help with household chores, regardless of their wives’ employment status (Dixon, 2009). Overall, most African American parents provide a cohesive, loving envi- ronment that often exists within a context of strong religious beliefs (Smith-Bynum, 2013; Teachman, Tedrow, & Kim, 2013), pride in cultural heritage, self-respect, and cooperation with the family (Coles, 2016).

As a result of several generations of oppression, many Native American parents have lost the cultural parenting skills that were traditionally part of their culture: children were valued, women were considered sacred and honored, and men cared for and provided for their families (Davis, Dionne, & Fortin, 2014). Thus, restoring and retaining a strong sense of tribalism is an important consideration for Native American families. Indeed, research shows that American Indian families receive more support from relatives in child rearing than do European Americans (Limb & Shafer, 2014). This support helps with the trans- mission of cultural values.

Nearly 25% of all children under 18 in the United States are Latino, and most are at least second generation (ChildStats.gov, 2016). Among two-parent families, Mexican American mothers and fathers both tend to adopt similar authoritative behaviors toward their preschool children (Gamble, Ramakumar, & Diaz, 2007).

Latino families demonstrate two key values: familism and the extended family. Familism refers to the idea the well-being of the family takes precedence over the concerns of individual family members. This value is a defining characteristic of Latino families; Bra- zilian and Mexican families consider familism a cultural strength (Carlo et al., 2007; Lucero-Liu, 2007). Indeed, familism accounts for the significantly higher trend for Latino college students to live at home (Desmond & López Turley, 2009). The extended family is also strong among Latino families and serves as the venue for a wide range of exchanges of goods and services, such as child care and financial support (Almeida et al., 2009).

Asian American adolescents report very high feelings of obligation to their fami- lies compared with European American adolescents, although in fact, most caregiving is done by daughters or daughters-in-law, not sons (Rodriguez-Galán, 2014). Contrary to commonly portrayed stereotypes, Asian American families do not represent the “Model Minority,” but experience the same challenges with parenting as all other groups (Xia, Do, & Xie, 2013). In general, males enjoy higher status in traditional Asian families (Tsuno & Homma, 2009). Among recent immigrants, though, women are expanding their role by working outside the home. Research shows that Chinese American parents experi- ence less marital stress during the transition to parenthood than European American couples do, perhaps because of the clearer traditional cultural division of tasks between husbands and wives (Burns, 2005).

Raising multiethnic children presents challenges not experi- enced by parents of same-race children. For example, parents of biracial children report feeling discrimination and being targets of prejudicial behavior from others (Hubbard, 2010; Kilson & Ladd, 2009). These parents also worry that their children may be rejected by members of both racial communities. Perhaps that is why par- ents of multiracial children tend to provide more economic and cultural resources to their children than do parents of single-race children (Cheng & Powell, 2007).

In multiethnic families, you might think that the parent from a minority group takes primary responsibility for guiding that aspect

familism idea that the family’s well-being takes pre- cedence over the concerns of individual family members.

Multiethnic families in the U.S. are increasing in number.

Fa tC

am er

a/ E+

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

376 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

of the child’s ethnic identity. However, it is the mothers who are key in most respects (Schlabach, 2013). A study of children of European mothers and Maori fathers in New Zealand showed that the mothers played a major role in establishing the child’s Maori identity (Kukutai, 2007). Similarly, European American mothers of biracial children whose fathers were African American tended to raise them as African American in terms of pub- lic ethnic identity (O’Donoghue, 2005). In general, multiracial adolescents experience more negative outcomes socially and emotionally if their mothers, rather than fathers, are a minority (Schlabach, 2013).

It is clear that ethnic groups vary a great deal in how they approach the issue of par- enting and what values are most important. Considered together, there is no one parenting standard that applies equally to all groups.

Diverse Family Forms The traditional family form of two married parents with their biological parents does not reflect the wide diversity of family forms in American society. You likely are in or know people who live in one of these forms. Let’s take a closer look.

Single Parents About 40% of births in the U.S. are to mothers who are not married, a rate that has declined 14% since peaking in 2008 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016d). As can be seen in ◗ Figure 11.5, the percentage of births to nonmarried mothers differs considerably across race and ethnicity, from 72% among African Americans to 29.2% among European Ameri- cans. Among African American mothers, two-thirds of these mothers have no high school diploma, and evidence suggests that the education disadvantage significantly affects the child’s future development (McLanahan & Jencks, 2015).

Single parents, regardless of gender, face considerable obstacles. Financially, they are usually much less well-off than their married counterparts. Integrating the roles of work and parenthood are more difficult. Single mothers are hardest hit, mainly because women typically are paid less than men.

Many divorced single parents report complex feelings toward their children, such as frustration, failure, guilt, and a need to be overindulgent (Amato & Boyd, 2014). Loneli- ness when children grow up and leave or are visiting the noncustodial parent can be espe- cially difficult to deal with (Langlais, Anderson, & Greene, 2016).

●◗ Figure 11.5 Percentage of All Births that Were to Unmarried Women, by Race and Hispanic Origin: Selected Years, 1960–2014

100

80

60

P er

ce n

ta ge

o f

al l

b ir

th s

40

20

0 1959 1963 1967 1971 1975 1979 1983 1987 1991 1995 1999 2003 2007 2011 2015

5.3 7.7 10.7

14.3

23.6

9.5 12.4 16.9

21.2

29.5

57.2 62.0

66.7 70.7 68.5

72.8 70.9

36.7 43.1 42.5

53.2

41.0

52.9

40.3

29.2 29.0

22.5

32.6

Total Non-Hispanic white Non-Hispanic black Hispanic

SOURCE: www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/03/75_Births_to_Unmarried_Women.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 377

Single parents, regardless of gender, face considerable obstacles. Financially, they are usually less well-off than their married counterparts, especially single mothers. Having only one source of income puts additional pressure on single par- ents to provide all of the necessities. Integrating the roles of work and parenthood are difficult enough for two people; for the single parent, the hardships are compounded. Financially, single mothers are hardest hit, mainly because women typi- cally are paid less than men and because single mothers may not be able to afford enough child care to provide the work schedule flexibility needed for higher-paying jobs.

One particular concern for many divorced single  parents is dating. Several common questions asked by single parents  involve dating: “How do I become available again?” “How will my children react?” “How do I cope with my own sexual needs?” They have reason to be concerned. Research indicates that single parents tend to report that children may interfere with dating and romance (Sommer et al., 2013). They report feeling insecure about sexuality and won- dering how they should behave around their children in terms of having partners stay overnight (Langlais et al., 2016; Lampkin-Hunter, 2010).

Military families experience unique aspects of single parenting. When one parent in a two-parent household deploys, the remaining parent becomes a single parent. Separa- tion anxiety is common among deployed parents, and the cycling of experience as a single parent and partner in a two-parent household with both parents present can create stress (Wadsworth, Hughes-Kirchubel, & Riggs, 2014).

Step-, Foster-, Adoptive, and Same-Sex Couple Parenting Roughly one-third of North American couples become stepparents or foster or adoptive parents at some time during their lives. In general, there are few differences among par- ents who have their own biological children or who become parents in some other way, but there are some unique challenges (Doss & Rhoades, 2017; Ganong & Coleman, 2017).

A big issue for foster parents, adoptive parents, and stepparents is how strongly the child will bond with them. Although infants less than 1 year old will probably bond well, children who are old enough to have formed attachments with their biological parents may have com- peting loyalties. For example, some stepchildren remain strongly attached to the noncustodial parent and actively resist attempts to integrate them into the new family (“My real mother wouldn’t make me do that”), or they may exhibit behavioral problems. As a result, the dynam- ics in blended families can best be understood as a complex system (Dupuis, 2010). Stepparents must often deal with continued visitation by the noncustodial parent, which may exacerbate any difficulties. These problems are a major reason that second marriages are at high risk for dissolution, as discussed later in this chapter. They are also a major reason why behavioral and emotional problems are more common among stepchildren (Ganong & Coleman, 2017).

Still, many stepparents and stepchildren ultimately develop good relationships with each other (Coleman, Ganong, & Russell, 2013). Stepparents must be sensitive to the rela- tionship between the stepchild and his or her biological, noncustodial parent. Allowing stepchildren to develop a relationship with the stepparent at their own pace also helps. What style of stepparenting ultimately develops is influenced by the expectations of the stepparent, stepchild, spouse, and nonresidential parent (Ganong & Coleman, 2017).

Adoptive parents also contend with attachment to birth parents, but in different ways. Adopted children may wish to locate and meet their birth parents. Such searches can strain the relationships between these children and their adoptive parents who may inter- pret these actions as a form of rejection (Curtis & Pearson, 2010).

Families with children adopted from another culture pose challenges of how to estab- lish and maintain connection with the child’s culture of origin (Yngvesson, 2010). For mothers of transracially adopted Chinese and Korean children, becoming connected to the appropriate Asian American community is a way to accomplish this (Johnston et al., 2007).

Being a single parent presents many chal- lenges, but also satisfaction.

M on

ke y

Bu si

ne ss

Im ag

es /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

378 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Research in the Netherlands found children adopted from Columbia, Sri Lanka, and Korea into Dutch homes struggled with looking different, and many expressed desires to be white (Juffer, 2006). Canadian parents who adopted children from China took several different approaches to introducing their children to Chinese culture, from not at all (“my child is simply Canadian” to deliberately blending both cultures to leaving is up to the child (Bian, Blachford, & Durst, 2015).

Foster parents have the most tenuous relationship with their children because the bond can be broken for any of a number of reasons having nothing to do with the quality of the care being provided. Dealing with attachment is difficult; foster parents want to provide secure homes, but they may not have the children long enough to establish continuity. Furthermore, because many children in foster care have been unable to form attachments at all, they are less likely to form ones will inevitably be broken. Despite the challenges, placement in good foster care results in the development of attachment between foster parents and children who were placed out of institutional settings (Alper & Howe, 2015; Smyke et al., 2010).

Finally, many gay men and lesbian women also want to be parents. Although changes in laws regarding same-sex couples and parenting have changed in important ways, bar- riers remain (Frank, 2016). Some have biological children themselves, whereas others choose adoption or foster parenting.

Research indicates that children reared by gay or lesbian parents do not experience any more problems than children reared by heterosexual parents and are as psychologi- cally healthy as children of heterosexual parents (Bos et al., 2016; Frank, 2016). Substantial evidence exists that children raised by gay or lesbian parents do not develop sexual identity problems or any other problems any more than children raised by heterosexual parents (Goldberg, 2009). Children of gay and lesbian parents were no more likely than children of heterosexual parents to identify as gay, lesbian, bisexual, transgendered, or questioning.

The evidence is clear that children raised by gay or lesbian parents suffer no adverse consequences compared with children raised by heterosexual parents. Children of lesbian couples and heterosexual couples are equally adjusted behaviorally, show equivalent cog- nitive development, have similar behaviors in school, and do not show different rates of use of illegal drugs or delinquent behavior (Biblarz & Savci, 2010; Patterson, 2013).

Children of gay or lesbian parents might be better adjusted than adult children of heterosexual parents in that the adult children of gay and lesbian parents exhibit lower levels of homophobia and less fear of negative evaluation than do the adult children of heterosexual parents. Gay men are often especially concerned about being good and nur- turing fathers, and they try hard to raise their children with nonsexist, egalitarian attitudes (Goldberg, 2009; Patterson, 2013). Evidence shows that gay parents have more egalitarian sharing of child rearing than do fathers in heterosexual households (Biblarz & Savci, 2010).

These data will not eliminate the controversy, much of which is based on long-held beliefs (often religion-based) and prejudices. In the United States, the topic of lesbian and gay cou- ples’ right to be parents is likely to continue to play out in political agendas for years to come.

Recall 1. The series of relatively predictable changes that families

experience is called .

2. Major influences on the decision to have children are marital factors, career factors, lifestyle factors, and

.

3. A new father who is invested in his parental role, but who may feel ambivalent about time lost to his career, is probably over age .

4. A major issue for foster parents, adoptive parents, and step- parents is .

Interpret ●● What difference would it make to view children as a financial

asset (i.e., a source of income) as opposed to a financial burden (i.e., mainly an expense)? Which of these attitudes characterizes most Western societies? Give an example of the other type.

apply ●● Would northern European cultures be likely to demonstrate

familism? Why or why not?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 11.3

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 379

Frank and Marilyn, both in their late 40s, thought that their marriage would last forever. However, they weren’t so lucky and have just been divorced. Although two of their children are married, their youngest daughter is still in college. The financial pressures Marilyn feels now that she’s on her own are beginning to take their toll. She wonders whether her financial situation is similar to that of other recently divorced women.

Despite what Frank and Marilyn pledged on their wedding day, their marriage did not last until death parted them; they dissolved their marriage through divorce. Even though divorce is stressful and difficult, thousands of people each year choose to try again. Most enter their second (or third or fourth) marriage with renewed expectations of success. Are these new dreams realistic? As we’ll see, it depends on many things; among the most important is whether children are involved.

Divorce Most couples enter marriage with the idea their relationship will be permanent. Rather than growing together, though, many couples grow apart.

Who Gets Divorced and Why? You or someone you know has experienced divorce. No wonder. Divorce in the United States is common, and the divorce rate is substantially higher than in many other coun- tries around the world. As you can see in ◗ Figure 11.6, American couples have roughly a 50–50 chance of remaining married for life. In contrast, the ratio of divorces to marriages in Japan, Israel, and Greece are substantially lower as are rates in Africa and Asia. Divorce rates in nearly every developed country increased over the past several decades, and tend to vary as a function of religion.

Of those marriages in the U.S. ending in divorce within 20 years, Asian American couples have the lowest risk and African Americans the highest. College educated women have much lower divorce rates than women who have a high school or lower education (Wang, 2015).

Research indicates that men and women tend to agree on the reasons for divorce (Braver & Lamb, 2013). Infidelity is the most commonly reported cause, followed by incompatibility, drinking or drug use, and growing apart. An individual couple’s specific reasons for divorcing vary with gender, social class, and life-course variables. Former hus- bands and wives are more likely to blame their ex-spouses than themselves for the prob- lems that led to the divorce. Former husbands and wives agree, however, that the women were more likely to have initiated the divorce.

Why people divorce has been the focus of much research. Divorce touches every aspect of relationships: emotional, psychological, social, economic, and more (Coates, 2017). Still, we can gain insight into who is most likely to divorce, and why they do.

A great deal of attention has been devoted to the notion that success or failure depends critically on how couples handle conflict. Although conflict management is important, it has become clear from research in couples therapy that the reasons couples split are com- plex (Kayser, 2010).

Gottman and Levenson (2000; Gottman & Silver, 2015) developed two models that predicted divorce early (within the first 7 years of marriage) and later (when the first child

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Who gets divorced? How does divorce affect parental

relationships with children? ●● What are remarriages like? How are they similar to and

different from first marriages?

11.4 Divorce and Remarriage

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

380 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

covenant marriage expands the marriage contract to a lifelong commitment between the partners within a supportive community.

reaches age 14) with 93% accuracy over the 14-year period of their study. Negative emotions displayed during conflict between the couple predicted early divorce, but not later divorce. In general, this reflects a destructive interaction pattern of demand-withdraw (Baucom et al., 2015; Christensen, 1990) in which, during conflict, one partner places a demand on the other, who then withdraws either emotionally or physically. In contrast, the lack of positive emotions in a discussion of events-of-the-day and during conflict predicted later divorce, but not early divorce. An example would be a wife talking excitedly about a project she had just been given at work and her husband showing disinterest. Such “unrequited” interest and excitement in discussions likely carries over to the rest of the relationship.

Gottman’s and other similar (e.g., Baucom et al., 2015) research is important because it clearly shows how couples show emotion is critical to marital success. Couples who divorce earlier typically do so because of high levels of negative feelings such as contempt, criticism, defensiveness, and stonewalling experienced as a result of intense marital con- flict. But for many couples, such intense conflict is generally absent. But the mere absence of conflict does not mean the marriage is full of positive feelings. Although the absence of intense conflict makes it easier to stay in a marriage longer, the lack of positive emotions eventually takes its toll and results in later divorce. For a marriage to last, people need to be told regularly they are loved and that what they do and feel really matters to their partner.

We must be cautious about applying Gottman’s model to all married couples. Kim, Capaldi, and Crosby (2007) reported in lower-income high-risk couples, the variables Gottman says predict early divorce did not hold for that sample. The priority given to cou- ples’ communication as the source of marital satisfaction is not always apparent (Lavner, Karney, & Bradbury, 2016). For older, long-term married couples, the perception of the spouse’s support is the most important predictor of remaining married (Landis et al., 2013).

The high divorce rate in the United States has led to many approaches to increase the likelihood that marriages will last. One approach to keeping couples together, termed covenant marriage, makes divorce much harder to obtain. Covenant marriage expands the marriage contract to a lifelong commitment between the partners within a supportive community. This approach is a religious-centered view founded on the idea that if getting

Vietnam

Mexico

Greece, Italy, Thailand

Israel, Singapore

Japan

Australia

Costa Rica, United Kingdom

Canada

Germany

Russia, Switzerland

Cuba

USA

France

Spain

Czech Republic

Portugal

Belgium

China

Egypt, South Africa

71%

68%

66%

61%

55%

53%

56%

51%

49%

48%

47%

43%

36%

28%

25%

22%

17%

15%

4%

Selected Global Divorce Rates

SOURCE: U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, updated and revised from “Families and Work in Transi- tion in 12 Countries, 1980–2001,” Monthly Labor Review, September 2003, with unpublished data. www.bls.gov/opub/mlr/2003/09/art1full.pdf.

●◗ Figure 11.6 The United States has one of the highest divorce rates in the world.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 381

married and getting divorced were grounded in religious and cultural values and divorce was made more difficult, couples would be more likely to stay together (Felkey, in press). The couple wanting to celebrate a covenant marriage agrees to participate in mandatory premarital counseling and, should problems arise later, the grounds for divorce become very limited (White, 2010).

Other approaches to decreasing the likelihood of divorce focus on teaching couples the skills necessary for maintaining strong relationships, such as good communication skills and joint problem-solving strategies. Research related to these initiatives has focused on the positive aspects of marriage and on the need to do a better job with marriage edu- cation (Fincham & Beach, 2010). Will they succeed in helping couples stay married longer? That remains to be seen.

Effects of Divorce on the Couple Divorce takes a high toll on the couple. Unlike the situation when one’s spouse dies, divorce often means the person’s ex-spouse is present to provide a reminder of the failure. As a result, divorced people are typically unhappy in general, at least for a while.

Research in the United States and Spain shows great similarity in how both partners in a failed marriage feel: deeply disappointed, misunderstood, and rejected ( Doohan, Carrère, & Riggs, 2010; Yárnoz-Yaben, 2010). Unlike the situation of a spouse dying, divorce often means that one’s ex-spouse is present to provide a reminder of the unpleas- ant aspects of the relationship and, in some cases, feelings of personal failure. Divorced people suffer negative health consequences as well (Lamela, Figueiredo, & Bastos, 2014). Especially because of the financial effects of divorce, the effects can even be traced to future generations due to long-term negative consequences on education and on quality of parenting (Amato & Cheadle, 2005; Friesen et al., 2017). Divorced people suffer negative health consequences as well (Lamela et al., 2016).

Divorced people sometimes find the transition difficult; researchers refer to these problems as “divorce hangover” (Walther, 1991). Divorce hangover reflects divorced part- ners’ inability to let go, develop new friendships, or reorient themselves as single parents. Forgiving the ex-spouse is also important for eventual adjustment postdivorce (Rye et al., 2004; Sbarra, 2015). Both low preoccupation and forgiveness may be indicators ex-spouses are able to move on with their lives.

Divorce in middle age has some special characteristics. If women initiate the divorce, they report self-focused growth and optimism; if they did not initiate the divorce, they tend to ruminate and feel vulnerable. Many middle-aged women who divorce also face significant financial challenges if their primary source of income was the ex-husband’s earnings (Sakraida, 2005).

We must not overlook the financial problems that many divorced women face (Braver & Lamb, 2013). These problems are especially keen for the middle-aged divorcee who may have spent years as a homemaker and has few marketable job skills. For her, divorce presents an especially difficult financial hardship, which is intensified if she has children in college and the father provides little support.

Relationships with Young Children When it involves children, divorce becomes a complicated matter, especially when viewed from a global perspective (Amato & Boyd, 2014). In most countries, mothers tend to obtain custody but often do not obtain sufficient financial resources to support the chil- dren. This puts an extreme financial burden on divorced mothers, whose standard of liv- ing is typically reduced.

In contrast, divorced fathers often pay a higher psychological price. Although many would like to remain active in their children’s lives, few actually do. Child support laws in some states also may limit fathers’ contact with their children (Wadlington, 2005). When mothers who have custody remarry, visits from noncustodial fathers usually decline (Anderson & Greene, 2013).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

382 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

One hopeful direction that addresses the usually difficult custody situations following divorce is the Collaborative Divorce Project, based on collaborative law (Mosten, 2009; Pruett, Insabella, & Gustafson, 2005). Collaborative divorce is a voluntary, contractually based alternative dispute resolution process for couples who want to negotiate a resolution of their situation rather than have a ruling imposed on them by a court or an arbitrator (Ballard et al., 2014). Collaborative divorce is an intervention designed to assist the parents of children 6 years and younger as they begin the separation/divorce process.

Results from this approach are positive (DeLucia-Waack, 2010). In addition to posi- tive evaluations from both parents, couples benefited in terms of less conflict, greater father involvement, and better outcomes for children than in the control group. Attorneys and court records indicate that intervention families were more cooperative and were less likely to need custody evaluations and other costly services. The Collaborative Divorce Project is evidence that programs can be designed and implemented to benefit all members of the family.

Divorce and Relationships with Adult Children We saw in Chapter 5 that young children can be seriously affected by their parents’ divorce. But what happens when the parents of adult children divorce? Are adult children affected, too? It certainly looks that way. Young adults whose parents divorce experience a great deal of emotional vulnerability and stress (Cooney & Uhlenberg, 1990). One young man put it this way:

The difficult thing was that it was a time where, you know, [you’re] making the transi- tion from high school to college . . . your high school friends are dispersed . . . they’re all over the place. . . . It’s normally a very difficult transition [college], new atmosphere, new workload, meeting new people. You’ve got to start deciding what you want to do, you’ve got to sort of start getting more independent, and so forth. And then at the same time you find out about a divorce. You know, it’s just that much more adjustment you have to make. (Cooney et al., 1986)

The effects of experiencing the divorce of one’s parents while growing up can be quite long-lasting. College-age students report poorer relations with their parents if their par- ents are divorced (Yu et al., 2010). Parental divorce also affects young adults’ views on inti- mate relationships and marriage, often having negative effects on them (Ottaway, 2010). Wallerstein and Lewis (2004) report the findings from a 25-year follow-up study of indi- viduals whose parents divorced when they were between 3 and 18 years old. Results show an unexpected gulf between growing up in intact versus divorced families as well as the difficulties that children of divorce encounter in achieving love, sexual intimacy, and com- mitment to marriage and parenthood. Even when the length of time spent in the intact two-parent family was taken into account, negative effects of divorce on adult children were still found in a large Dutch study (Kalmijn, 2013). The “marriage protection” factor outweighed biological relatedness, especially for fathers. There is no doubt that divorce has significant effects regardless of when it occurs in a child’s life.

Remarriage The trauma of divorce does not always deter people from beginning new relationships that often lead to another marriage. As you can see in ◗ Figure 11.7, though, rates of remarriage differ dramatically by age (Livingston, 2014). Additionally, the likelihood of ever remarry- ing has dropped in nearly all age groups since the mid-20th century. For example, for those under age 35, remarriage declined from 72% in 1960 to 42% in 2013, from 76% to 57% in those 35 to 44, and from 69% to 63% in those 45 to 54. Interestingly, for those in older age groups, the trend is reversed: for those 55 to 64, remarriage increased from 55% to 67% and for those over 65, it increased from 34% to 50%.

Overall, women are less likely to remarry than are men, but this gender gap is closing, mainly because men are less likely in general to remarry now than in the past (Livingston, 2014). European Americans are the most likely group to remarry (60% of those couples do),

collaborative divorce voluntary, contractually based alternative dispute resolution process for couples who want to negotiate a resolution of their situ- ation rather than have a ruling imposed on them by a court or an arbitrator.

Think About It Given the serious impact of divorce, what changes in mate selection might lower the divorce rate?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 383

and African Americans (48%) and Asian Americans (46%) least likely. Finally, the age dif- ference between spouses is likely to be greater in remarriage; whereas among wives in a first marriage only 14% are at least 6 years younger than their spouse, for remarried women that increases to 31%.

Cultural differences are apparent in the ability of women, in particular, to remarry; in Namibia widows are constrained in their options and typically must depend on others (Thomas, 2008). Among older adults, adult children may voice strong opposition to their parent remarrying that can put sufficient pressure on the parent that they remain single.

Adapting to new relationships in remarriage is stressful. Partners may have unre- solved issues from the previous marriage that may interfere with satisfaction with the new marriage (Faber, 2004; Gold, 2016; Martin-Uzzi & Duval-Tsioles, 2013). The challenges can include antagonism toward ex-spouses that interferes with child custody, differing loyalties among stepchildren, and financial difficulties. The effects of remarriage on chil- dren is complicated, at least for emerging adults (Collardeau & Ehrenberg, 2016; Gold, 2016). Parental divorce can have long-term consequences on children’s attitudes toward marriage and divorce. The extent to which parental conflict was openly present and the religious affiliation of emerging adults influences their attitudes. For instance, those who were exposed to high levels of parental conflict have more positive views of divorce.

Remarriages tend to be less stable than first marriages, and have become less so since the 1990s (Council on Contemporary Families, 2015). The typical first marriage lasts 13 years, whereas the typical remarriage lasts 10. These differences do not reflect relationship quality, which is equivalent; rather, pressures from complex family relationships, lower commitment, and financial pressures take a higher toll on remarriages. Nevertheless, couples who are very committed to and work diligently at making the remarriage strong are usually successful.

Recall 1. Following divorce, most women suffer disproportionately in

the _______________ domain compared with most men.

2. On average, within two years after a divorce, _______________ fathers remain central in their children’s lives.

3. Even many years later, divorced _______________ may not experience positive relationships with their adult children.

4. Remarriages tend to be _______________ than first marriages.

Interpret ●● Despite greatly increased divorce rates over the past few

decades, the rate of marriage has not changed very much. Why?

apply ●● Ricardo and Maria are engaged to be married. Ricardo works

long hours as the manager of a local coffee shop, while Maria works regular hours as an administrative assistant at a large communications company. Based on what you know about why couples get divorced, what factors may increase the like- lihood that Ricardo and Maria’s marriage will fail?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 11.4

●◗ Figure 11.7 Remarriage as a Function of Age

65 and older

55–64

25–34

18–24

35–44

45–54

29

43

57

63

67

50

Note: Previously married are those eligible for remarriage.

Remarriage by age % of the previously married who ever remarried

SOURCE: Pew Research Center (2014). Retrieved from www.pewsocialtrends.org/ 2014/11/14/four-in-ten-couples-are-saying-i-do-again/st_2014-11-14_remarriage-06/.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

384 pa RT T h R e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

SUMMARY

11.1 Relationship Types and Issues

What role do friends play across adulthood? how do they develop?

●● People tend to have more friendships during young adult- hood than during any other period. Face-to-face and virtual friendships are based on the same principles: affective or emotional self-disclosure, sharing activities of mutual inter- est, and as sources of fun.

●● Men have fewer close friends and base them on shared activities. Women have more close friends and base them on emotional sharing. Cross-gender friendships may create challenges due to misperceptions.

What characterizes love relationships? how do they vary across cultures?

●● Passion, intimacy, and commitment are the key compo- nents of love.

●● The theory of assortative mating does the best job explain- ing the process of forming love relationships.

●● Selecting a mate works best when there are shared values, goals, and interests. There are cross-cultural differences in which specific aspects of these are most important.

●● Neuroscience research shows a great deal of brain activity and related neurochemical activity when people are in love.

What is the nature of violence in some relationships?

●● Levels of aggressive behavior range from verbal aggression to physical aggression to murdering one’s partner. People remain in abusive relationships for many reasons, including cultural factors, low self-esteem, and the belief they cannot leave.

11.2 Lifestyles and Relationships

What are the challenges and advantages of being single?

●● Most adults in their 20s are single. People remain single for many reasons; gender differences exist. Ethnic differences in rates of singlehood reflect differences in age at marriage and social factors.

●● Singles recognize the pluses and minuses in the lifestyle. There are health and longevity consequences from remain- ing single for men but not for women.

Why do people cohabit?

●● Cohabitation is on the increase globally. ●● Three primary reasons for cohabiting are convenience

(e.g., to share expenses), trial marriage, or substitute

marriage. Cultural differences abound; in some cultures, cohabitation is a viable alternative to marriage.

What are LGBTQ relationships like?

●● LGBTQ couples are similar to married heterosexual cou- ples in terms of relationship issues. Lesbian couples tend to be more egalitarian. The option to marry is changing the nature of LGBTQ relationships.

What is marriage like across adulthood?

●● The most important factors in creating stable marriages are maturity, similarity (called homogamy), and conflict resolu- tion skills. Exchange theory is an important explanation of how people contribute to their relationships.

●● Exchange theory argues that happy marriages result when both partners perceive they contribute roughly equally.

●● For couples with children, marital satisfaction tends to decline until the children leave home, although individual differences are apparent, especially in long-term marriages.

●● Marriages are most intense during the early years. Most long-term marriages tend to be happy, and partners in them express fewer negative emotions. Sharing values and beliefs helps sustain marriages.

11.3 Family Dynamics and the Life Course

What is it like to be a parent? What are the key issues across ethnic groups?

●● Most couples choose to have children, although for many different reasons. The timing of parenthood determines in part how involved parents are in their families as opposed to their careers. Couples without children can be happier.

●● Being older at the birth of one’s first child is an advantage. Instilling cultural values in children is important for par- ents. In some cultures, familism changes the unit of analysis from the individual to the family.

What diverse forms of parenting are there?

●● Single parents face many problems, especially if they are women and are divorced. The main problem is reduced financial resources.

●● A major issue for adoptive parents, foster parents, and step- parents is how strongly the child will bond with them. Each of these relationships has special characteristics. Same- sex couple parents still face discrimination, and are good parents.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p T e R 1 1 : B E I N G W I T H O T H E R S : R E L AT I O N S H I P S I N Y O U N G A N D   M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D 385

11.4 Divorce and Remarriage

Why do couples divorce?

●● Currently, half of all new marriages end in divorce. Reasons for divorce include a lack of the qualities that make a strong marriage. Also, societal attitudes against divorce have eased and expectations about marriage have increased.

●● Recovery from divorce is different for men and women. Men tend to have a tougher time in the short run. Women clearly have a harder time in the long run, often for

financial reasons. Difficulties between divorced partners usually involve visitation and child support.

Why do people remarry?

●● Most divorced couples remarry. Second marriages are especially vulnerable to stress if stepchildren are involved. Remarriage in later life tends to be happy, but may be resisted by adult children.

11.1 1. women 2. men 3. romantic love 4. traditional 5. men 11.2 1. marry 2. step toward 3. heterosexual marriages 4. exchange theory 5. decreases 11.3 1. the family life cycle 2. psychological factors 3. 30 4. how strongly the child will bond with them 11.4 1. financial 2. few 3. fathers 4. less stable

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

assortative mating (359) abusive relationship (363) battered woman syndrome (363) cohabitation (366) marital success (368) marital quality (368)

marital adjustment (368) marital satisfaction (368) homogamy (369) exchange theory (369) vulnerability-stress-adaptation

model (370)

nuclear family (372) extended family (372) familism (375) covenant marriage (380) collaborative divorce (382)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

La ur

a D

os s/

A G

E Fo

to st

oc k

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

387

When you were young, you were undoubtedly asked, “What do you want to be when you grow up?” Now that we are “grown up,” the ques-

tion has changed to “What do you do?”

Work is a central aspect of life and a defin-

ing characteristic of who we are. For some, work is

life; for all, our work is a prime source of identity in

adulthood.

The world of work has changed dramatically over

the past few decades in large part to the continuing

replacement of jobs with technology (witness the

new use of robots and iPads in restaurants instead

of human servers, and the advent of driverless cars

that could eliminate taxi and share-service drivers).

The so-called gig economy, based on short-term

contracts rather than on long-term employment, is

the fastest growing sector of the labor force (Intuit,

2016; Irwin, 2016). Employment as a series of short-

term jobs largely eliminates the traditional employer–

employee relationship, and mostly eliminates the availability of key benefits

such as employer-paid health care, paid family leave, and retirement savings

plans to workers. Intuit (2016) predicts that by 2020, 40% of all American work-

ers will be in the gig economy and not employed in a traditional long-term way.

Aging baby boomers are also redefining work and retirement. The Great

Recession of the late 2000s and early 2010s affected the workplace by forcing

Working and Relaxing

12.1 Occupational Selection and Development 388 The Meaning of Work 388 Occupational Choice Revisited 389 Occupational Development 392 Job Satisfaction 394

12.2 Gender, Ethnicity, and Discrimination Issues 397 Gender Differences in Occupational Selection 397 Women and Occupational Development 398 Ethnicity and Occupational Development 399 Bias and Discrimination 400 Gender Bias, Glass Ceilings, and Glass Cliffs 400 WHAT DO YOU THINK? Helping Women Lean In 401

12.3 Occupational Transitions 404 Retraining Workers 404 Occupational Insecurity 405 Coping with Unemployment 405 REAL PEOPLE: The Politics of Unemployment 406

12.4 Work and Family 408 The Dependent Care Dilemma 408 Juggling Multiple Roles 410

12.5 Taking Time to Relax: Leisure Activities 413 Types of Leisure Activities 413 Developmental Changes in Leisure 414 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Long-Term Effects of

Leisure Activities 415 Consequences of Leisure Activities 415

    Summary 417

    Key Terms 419

12

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

388 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

boomers who lost a significant part of their retirement savings to con-

tinue working, making it more difficult for younger adults to enter or

advance in the workforce.

As a result, our understanding of what work is, what it means, and

how people engage in it is undergoing fundamental change. We con-

sider the complexities of the world of work throughout this chapter

as we confront the reality of rapidly changing occupational conditions

and opportunities, and how those are shaping adult development and

aging, and raising issues about basic assumptions people have about

retirement.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do people view work?

●● How do people choose their occupations?

●● What factors influence occupational development?

●● What is the relationship between job satisfaction and age?

12.1 Occupational Selection and Development

Fatima, a 28-year-old senior communications major, wonders about careers. Should she enter the broadcast field as a behind-the-scenes producer, or would she be better suited as a public relations spokesperson? She thinks her outgoing personality is a factor she should consider in making this decision.

Choosing one’s work is serious business. Like Fatima, we try to select a field in which we are trained and maximizes the odds that we can meet our goal of doing meaningful work. You may be taking this course as part of your preparation for a career in human develop- ment, social services, psychology, nursing, allied, health, or other field, so in that sense it is career preparation. But work is more than that. Work is a source for friends and often for spouses or partners. People arrange personal activities around work schedules. Parents often choose child care centers on the basis of proximity to where they work. People often choose where they live in terms of where they work.

The Meaning of Work Studs Terkel, author of the fascinating classic book Working (1974), writes work is “a search for daily meaning as well as daily bread, for recognition as well as cash, for astonishment rather than torpor; in short, for a sort of life rather than a Monday through Friday sort of dying” (xiii). Kahlil Gibran (1923), in his mystical book The Prophet, put it this way: “Work is love made visible.”

The meaning most of us derive from working includes both the money that can be exchanged for life’s necessities (and perhaps a few luxuries) and the possibility of per- sonal growth (Lips-Wiersma, Wright, & Dik, 2016; Rosso, Dekas, & Wrzesniewski, 2010). Schwartz (2015) says the belief that people only work for a paycheck is wrong; most people want to do something meaningful with people who they respect and who respect them. To achieve that true goal, Schwartz says most people would even be willing to make less money.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 389

The upshot is that the specific occupation a person holds appears to have no effect on his or her need to derive meaning from work. Finding meaning in one’s work can mean the difference between feel- ing work is the source of one’s life problems or a source of fulfillment and contentment (Grawitch, Barber, & Justice, 2010). Blue- and pink- collar workers tend to derive meaning most from finding unity with others and developing the inner self, whereas white-collar workers place more emphasis on expressing full potential.

Contemporary business theory supports the idea that meaning matters. The concept called meaning-mission fit explains how corpo- rate executives with a better alignment between their personal inten- tions and their firm’s mission care more about their employees’ happiness, job satisfaction, and emotional well-being (Abbott, Gilbert, & Rosinski, 2013; Salomaa, 2014). Ensuring such fit is a major focus of talent management efforts in organizations.

Given the various meanings that people derive from work, occu- pation is clearly a key element of a person’s sense of identity and self- efficacy (Lang & Lee, 2005). This can be readily observed when adults introduce themselves socially. You’ve probably noticed that when peo- ple are asked to tell something about themselves, they usually provide information about what they do for a living. Occupation affects your life in a host of ways and often influences where you live, what friends you make, and even what clothes you wear. In short, the impact of work cuts across all aspects of life. Work, then, is a major social role and influence on adult life. Occupation is an important anchor that complements the other major role of adulthood—love relationships.

As we will see, occupation is part of human development. Young children, in their pretend play, are in the midst of the social prepa- ration for work. Adults are always asking them, “What do you want to be when you grow up?” School curricula, especially in high school and college, are geared toward preparing people for particular occupations. Young adult college students as well as older returning students have formulated perspectives on the meanings they believe they will get from work. Hance (2000) organized these beliefs into three main categories: working to achieve social influence; working to achieve personal fulfillment; and working because of economic reality. These categories reflect fairly well the actual meanings that working adults report.

Because work plays such a key role in providing meaning for people, an important question is how people select an occupation. Let’s turn our attention to two theories explaining how and why people choose the occupations they do.

Occupational Choice Revisited Decisions about what people want to do in the world of work do not initially happen in adulthood. Even by adolescence, there is evidence occupational preferences are related to their personalities. But what are people preparing for? Certainly, much has been written about the rapidly changing nature of work and how people cannot prepare for a stable career where a person works for the same organization throughout his or her working life (Fouad et al., 2016; Savickas, 2013).

Currently, it is more appropriate to consider careers as something people construct themselves rather than enter (Di Fabio, 2016; Hartung & Santilli, 2017; Savickas, 2013). Career construction theory posits people build careers through their own actions that result from the interface of their own personal characteristics and the social context. What people “do” in the world of work, then, results from how they adapt to their environment, that in turn is a result of bio-psychosocial processes grounded in the collection of experi- ences they have during their life.

meaning-mission fit alignment between people’s personal intentions and their company’s mission.

career construction theory posits that people build careers through their own actions that result from the inter- face of their own personal characteristics and the social context.

Hassling with our commute makes us think about why we work.

Th in

ks to

ck /S

to ck

by te

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

390 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

In this regard, two specific theories about how people adapt themselves to their envi- ronment have influenced research. First, Holland’s (1997) personality-type theory pro- poses people choose occupations to optimize the fit between their individual traits (such as personality, intelligence, skills, and abilities) and their occupational interests. Second, social cognitive career theory (SCCT) proposes career choice is a result of the application of Bandura’s social cognitive theory, especially the concept of self-efficacy.

Holland categorizes occupations by the interpersonal settings that people must func- tion and their associated lifestyles. He identifies six personality types that combine these factors: investigative, social, realistic, artistic, conventional, and enterprising, that he believes are optimally related to occupations.

How does Holland’s theory help us understand the continued development of occu- pational interests in adulthood? Fatima, the college senior in the vignette, found a good match between her outgoing nature and her major, communications. Indeed, college stu- dents of all ages prefer courses and majors that fit well with their own personalities. You are likely to be one of them. Later on, that translates into the tendency of people to choose occupations and careers they like.

Complementarily, social cognitive career theory proposes people’s career choices are heavily influenced by their interests (Brown & Lent, 2016; Lent, 2013; Sheu et al., 2010). As depicted in ◗ Figure 12.1, SCCT has two versions. The simplest includes four main factors: Self-Efficacy (your belief in your ability), Outcome Expectations (what you think will happen in a specific situation), Interests (what you like), and Choice Goals (what you want to achieve). The more complex version also includes Supports (environmental things that help you) and Barriers (environmental things that block or frustrate you). Several studies show support for the six-variable version of the model (Brown & Lent, 2016; Sheu et al., 2010).

Over four decades, Donald Super (1957, 1976, 1980; Super, Savickas, & Super, 1996) developed a theory of occupational development based on self-concept, first introduced in Chapter 9. He proposed a progression through five distinct stages during adulthood as a result of changes in individuals’ self-concept and adaptation to an occupational role: implementation, establishment, maintenance, deceleration, and retirement (see ◗ Figure 12.2). People are located along a continuum of vocational maturity through their working years; the more congruent their occupational behaviors are with what is expected of them at different ages, the more vocationally mature they are.

Super proposed five developmental tasks, the first two (crystallization and specification) occurring primarily in adolescence. The remaining three (implementation, stabilization, and consolidation) occur over the course of adulthood. Each of the tasks in adulthood has distinctive characteristics, as follows.

social cognitive career theory (SCCT) proposes career choice is a result of the application of Bandura’s social cogni- tive theory, especially the concept of self-efficacy.

vocational maturity degree of congruence between people’s occupational behavior and what is expected of them at different ages.

●◗ Figure 12.1 The four-variable (paths 1–6) and six- variable (paths 1–13) versions of the Social Cognitive Career Theory inter- est and choice models.

Self-Efficacy

Supports 13

12 11

10

9

5

2 1

3

6

7

4

8

Barriers

Outcome expectations

Interests Choice goals

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 391

●● The implementation task begins in the early 20s, when people take a series of tempo- rary jobs to learn firsthand about work roles and to try out possible career choices. Summer internships that many students use to gain experience are one example.

●● The stabilization task begins in the mid-20s with selecting a specific occupation dur- ing young adulthood. It continues until the mid-30s as the person confirms the occu- pational choice that was made.

●● The consolidation task begins in the mid-30s and continues throughout the rest of the person’s working life as he or she advances up the career ladder. Taking a position in a law firm and working one’s way up to partner or beginning as a salesclerk in a store at a mall and moving up to store manager are two examples.

These adult tasks overlap a sequence of developmental stages, beginning at birth, that continues during adulthood: exploratory (age 15 to 24), establishment (age 24 to 44), main- tenance (age 45 to 64), and decline (age 65 and beyond). These stages reflect the overall occupational cycle from choosing what one wants to do through achieving the maximum possible in a career to the reduction in work in late adulthood.

Super’s theory applies to people who enter and stay in a particular career their entire adult lives and to those who change occupations. Because it is now typical for Americans to have a series of careers, Super’s notion is that we cycle and recycle through the tasks and stages as we adapt to changes in ourselves and the workplace (Super et al., 1996).

Super may be right. A longitudinal study of 7,649 individuals born in the United Kingdom showed that occupational aspirations at age 16 in science and health fields predicted actual occupational attainments in science, health professions, or engineer- ing at age 33 (Schoon, 2001). Adult occupational attainment was also related to belief in one’s ability, mathematical test performance, several personality characteristics, sociocultural background, and gender. These results point to the importance of view- ing occupational development as a true developmental process, as Super claimed, as well as the importance of personal characteristics, as proposed in occupational selec- tion theories.

Perspectives on Theories of Career Development How well do these theories work in actual practice, particularly in the rapidly changing world in which we live and where people’s careers are no longer stable? Certainly, the rela- tions among occupation, personality, and demographic variables are complex (Brown & Lent, 2016; Hartung & Santilli, 2017). However, even given the lack of stable careers and the real need to change jobs frequently, there is still a strong tendency on people’s part to find occupations in which they feel comfortable and they like (Brown & Lent, 2016; Hartung & Santilli, 2017). As we will see later, loss of self-efficacy through job loss and long-term unemployment provides support for the role the self-statements underling self- efficacy and SCCT are key.

SCCT has also been used as a framework for career counselors and coaches to help people identify and select both initial occupations and navigate later occupational changes. The goal is to have people understand the work world changes rapidly and they need to develop coping and compensatory strategies to deal with that fact.

Although people may have underlying tendencies relating to certain types of occu- pations, unless they believe they could be successful in those occupations and careers they are unlikely to choose them. These beliefs can be influenced by external factors. Occupational prestige and gender-related factors need to be taken into account (Deng, Armstrong, & Rounds, 2007).

●◗ Figure 12.2 Super’s occupational stages during adulthood.

Implementation Establishment Maintenance Deceleration Retirement ©

2 01

9 Ce

ng ag

e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

392 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Occupational Development It is said that advancing through one’s career is not just a function of being smart and doing all of the written requirements of a job. It also depends on the socialization that occurs through learning the unwritten rules of an organization, in combination with one’s own expectations of what the career should entail.

Certainly, the relations among occupation, personality, and demographic variables are complex (e.g., Barrick, Mount, & Li, 2013). However, even given the lack of stable careers and the real need to change jobs frequently, there is still a strong tendency on people’s part to find occupations in which they feel comfortable and that they like (Lent, 2013). As we will see later, loss of self-efficacy through job loss and long-term unemployment supports the fact that the self-statements that underlie self-efficacy and SCCT are key.

SCCT has also been used as a framework for career counselors and coaches to help people identify and select initial occupations and navigate later occupational changes. The goal is for people to understand that the work world changes rapidly and that they need to develop coping and compensatory strategies to deal with that fact.

Although people may have underlying tendencies that relate to certain types of occu- pations, unless they believe they could be successful in those occupations and careers, they are unlikely to choose them. These beliefs can be influenced by external factors. For example, occupational prestige and gender-related factors need to be taken into account (Deng, Armstrong, & Rounds, 2007).

Occupational Expectations Especially in adolescence, people begin to form opinions about what work in a particular occupation will be like, based on what they learn in school and from their parents, peers, other adults, and the media. These expectations influence what they want to become and when they hope to get there.

In adulthood, personal experiences affect people’s opinions of themselves as they con- tinue to refine and update their occupational expectations and development (Fouad, 2007; Fouad et al., 2016). This usually involves trying to achieve an occupational goal, monitor- ing progress toward it, and changing or even abandoning it as necessary. Modifying the goal happens for many reasons, such as realizing interests have changed, the occupation was not a good fit for them, they never got the chance to pursue the level of education nec- essary to achieve the goal, or because they lack certain essential skills and cannot acquire them. Still other people modify their goals because of age, race, or sex discrimination, a point we consider later in this chapter.

Research shows most people who know they have both the talent and the opportunity to achieve their occupational and career goals often attain them. When high school stu- dents identified as academically talented were asked about their career expectations and outcomes, it turned out that 10 and even 20 years later they had been surprisingly accurate (Perrone et al., 2010).

In general, research shows young adults modify their expecta- tions at least once, usually on the basis of new information, especially about their academic ability. The connection between adolescent expectations and adult reality reinforces the developmental aspects of occupations and careers.

Many writers believe occupational expectations also vary by gen- eration. Nowhere has this belief been stronger than in the supposed differences between the baby boom generation (born between 1946 and 1964) and the current millennial generation (born since 1983). What people in these generations, on average, expect in occupations appears to be different (Stewart et al., 2017). Millennials are more likely to change jobs more often than the older generations did, and are likely to view traditional organizations with more distrust and  cynicism. Experts recommend that metrics used to evaluate job performance need to reflect these generational differences.

Think About It How does one’s level of cognitive development relate to one’s choice of occupation?

Reality shock typically hits younger workers soon after they begin an occupation.

M ar

c Ro

m an

el li/

Th e

Im ag

e Ba

nk /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 393

Contrary to most stereotypes, millennials are no more egotistical, and are just as happy and satisfied as young adults in every generation since the 1970s (Trzesniewski & Donnellan, 2010). However, millennials tend to have an inherent mistrust in organizations, prefer a culture focused on employee development, create information through interactive social media, are more globally aware and comfortable working with people from diverse socio-ethnic backgrounds, and do best in situations that value innovation through team- work (Dannar, 2013; Stewart et al., in press).

The importance of occupational expectations can be seen clearly in the transition from school to the workplace (Moen, 2016a, 2016b; Moen & Roehling, 2005). The 21st- century workplace is not one where hard work and long hours necessarily lead to a stable career. It can also be a place where you experience reality shock, a situation that what you learn in the classroom does not always transfer directly into the “real world” and does not represent all you need to know. When reality shock sets in, things never seem to happen the way we expect. Reality shock befalls everyone. You can imagine how a new teacher feels when her long hours preparing a lesson result in students who act bored and unap- preciative of her efforts.

Many professions, such as nursing and teaching, have gone to great lengths to allevi- ate reality shock (Hinton & Chirgwin, 2010; Shayshon & Popper-Giveon, in press). This problem is one best addressed through internship and practicum experiences for students under the careful guidance of experienced people in the field.

The Role of Mentors and Coaches Entering an occupation involves more than the relatively short formal training a person receives. Instead, most people are oriented by a more experienced person who makes a specific effort to do this, taking on the role of a mentor or coach.

A mentor is part teacher, sponsor, model, and counselor who facilitates on-the job learning to help the new hire do the work required in his or her present role and to prepare for future career roles (Volpe et al., 2016). A developmental coach is an individual who helps a person focus on their goals, motivations, and aspirations to help them achieve focus and apply them appropriately (Hunt & Weintraub, 2016). Mentoring and coaching are viewed as primary ways that organizations invest in developing their talent and future leadership (Smits & Bowden, 2013). Although mentors and coaches work with people at all career stages, mentoring is found most often with people new to a position, whereas coaching tends to focus on those with more experience.

The mentor helps a young worker avoid trouble and also provides invaluable informa- tion about the unwritten rules governing day-to-day activities in the workplace, and being sensitive to the employment situation (Smith, Howard, & Harrington, 2005). Good men- tors makes sure their protégés are noticed and receives credit from supervisors for good work. Thus, occupational success often depends on the quality of the mentor–protégé relationship and the protégé’s perceptions of its importance (Eddleston, Baldridge, & Veiga, 2004).

What do mentors get from the relationship? Helping a younger employee learn the job is one way to achieve Erikson’s phase of gen- erativity (see Chapter 9; Marcia & Josselson, 2013).

Developmental coaching is a process that helps people make fundamental changes in their lives by focusing on general skill devel- opment and performance improvement (Volpe et al., 2016). It tends not to focus on specific aspects of a job; rather, the intent is more general improvement of one’s overall career success. Thus, coach- ing complements mentoring and helps people develop all of the key aspects of themselves.

Women and minorities have an especially important need for both mentors and coaches (Hunt & Weintraub, 2016; Ortiz- Walters  & Gilson, 2013; Pratt, 2010; Williams, Phillips, & Hall, 2014). When paired with mentors and coaches, women benefit by

reality shock situation in which what you learn in the classroom does not always transfer directly into the “real world” and does not represent all you need to know.

mentor a person who is part teacher, sponsor, model, and counselor who facilitates on-the-job learning to help a new hire do the work required in his or her present role and to prepare for future roles.

developmental coach individual who helps a person focus on their goals, motivations, and aspirations to help them achieve focus and apply them appropriately.

Women employees typically prefer and may achieve more from a female mentor.

Sp en

ce r G

ra nt

/P ho

to Ed

it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

394 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

having higher expectations; mentored women also have better perceived career develop- ment (Schulz & Enslin, 2014). Latina nurses in the U.S. Army benefitted from mentors in terms of staying in the military and getting better assignments (Aponte, 2007). It is also critical to adopt a culturally conscious model of mentoring and coaching in order to enhance the advantages of developing minority mentees (Campinha-Bacote, 2010). Cul- turally conscious mentoring and coaching involves understanding how an organization’s and employee’s cultures mutually affect employees, and explicitly building those assump- tions, interrelationships, and behaviors into the mentoring or coaching situation.

Despite the evidence that having a mentor or coach has many positive effects on one’s occupational development, there is an important caveat; the quality of the mentor or coach really matters (Tong & Kram, 2013; Volpe et al., 2016). Having a poor mentor or coach is worse than having no mentor at all. Consequently, people must be carefully matched. It is in the best interest of the organization to get the match correct. How can prospective matches happen more effectively? Some organizations have taken a page from dating and created speed mentoring as a way to create better matches (Berk, 2010; Cook, Bahn, & Menaker, 2010).

Job Satisfaction What does it mean to be satisfied with one’s job or occupation? Job satisfaction is the positive feeling that results from an appraisal of one’s work. Research indicates that job sat- isfaction is a multifaceted concept but that certain characteristics—including hope, resil- ience, optimism, and self-efficacy—predict both job performance and job satisfaction. This research has resulted in the creation of psychological capital theory, the notion that having a positive outlook improves processes and outcomes (Luthans et al., 2007; Youssef- Morgan & Luthans, 2013).

Satisfaction with some aspects of one’s job increases gradually with age, and success- ful aging includes a workplace component (Robson et al., 2006). Why is this? Is it because people sort themselves out and end up in occupations they like? Is it they simply learn to like the occupation they are in? What other factors matter?

So how does job satisfaction evolve over young and middle adulthood? You may be pleased to learn research shows, given sufficient time, most people eventually find a job where they are reasonably happy (Hom & Kinicki, 2001). Optimistically, this indicates there is a job out there, somewhere, where you will be happy. That’s good, because research grounded in positive psychology theory indicates happiness fuels success (Achor, 2010).

It’s also true job satisfaction does not increase in all areas and job types with age. White- collar professionals show an increase in job satisfaction with age, whereas those in blue- collar positions generally do not, and these findings hold with both men and women (Aasland, Rosta, & Nylenna, 2010). This is also true across cultures. A study of Filipino and Taiwanese workers in the long-term healthcare industry in Taiwan showed workers with 4 or 5 years’ experience had lower job satisfaction than workers with less experience, but job satisfaction among older physicians in Norway increases over time (Aasland et al., 2010; Tu, 2006).

However, the changes in the labor market in terms of lower prospects of having a long career with one organization have begun to change the notion of job satisfaction (Bidwell, 2013; Böckerman et al., 2013). Specifically, the fact that companies may eliminate jobs and workers not based on performance, but on stereotypes about older workers, for instance, makes it more difficult for employees to develop a sense of organizational commitment, thereby making the relationship between worker age and job satisfaction more compli- cated (Abrams, Swift, & Drury, 2016; Bayl-Smith & Griffin, 2014).

Also complicating traditional relations between job satisfaction and age is the fact that the type of job one has and the kinds of family responsibilities one has at different career stages—as well as the flexibility of work options such as telecommuting and family leave benefits to accommodate those responsibilities—influence the relationship between age and job satisfaction (Marsh & Musson, 2008). For instance, family caregiving responsibilities may collide with work demands, resulting in lower job satisfaction, especially if the employer has no flexible work options to accommodate the employee’s needs (Paulson et al., 2016).

job satisfaction positive feeling that results from an appraisal of one’s work.

psychological capital theory notion that having a positive outlook improves processes and outcomes.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 395

This suggests the accumulation of experience, changing context, and the stage of one’s career development all influence job satis- faction over time. The general increase in job satisfaction, then, may reflect the reality of workers figuring out how to manage their lives, understanding the limited alternative options they have, or taking advantage of alternative work assignments, such as telecommuting.

Alienation and Burnout All jobs create a certain level of stress. For most workers, such negatives are merely annoyances. But for others, extremely stress- ful situations on the job may result in alienation and burnout.

When workers feel what they are doing is meaningless and their efforts are devalued, or when they do not see the connection between what they do and the final product, a sense of alienation is likely to result. Terkel (1974) reported employees are most likely to feel alienated when they perform routine, repetitive actions. Alienation can also result from employees feeling aban- doned by their employer, such as experiencing long periods without pay increases.

It is essential for companies to provide positive work envi- ronments to ensure the workforce remains stable and committed (Griffin et al., 2010). How can employers avoid alienating workers and improve organizational commitment? Research indicates that leaders who show trust and ethics are key (Bachman, 2017; Bligh, 2017), as is a perception among employees the employer deals with people fairly and impartially (How- ard & Cordes, 2010). It is also helpful to involve employees in the decision-making pro- cess, create flexible work schedules, and institute employee development and enhancement programs.

Sometimes the pace and pressure of the occupation becomes more than a person can bear, resulting in burnout, a depletion of a person’s energy and motivation, the loss of occu- pational idealism, and the feeling that one is being exploited. Burnout is a state of physical, emotional, and mental exhaustion that negatively affects self-esteem as a result of job stress (Shoji et al., 2016). Burnout is most common among people in the helping profes- sions—such as police (McCarty & Skogan, 2013), teaching, social work, health care ( Bermejo-Toro, Prieto-Ursúa, & Hernández, 2016; Shanafelt et al., 2016), and for those in the military (Simons et al., 2016). The tendency of organizations to keep employee num- bers smaller during times of economic uncertainty adds to the workload for people on the job, increasing the risk of burnout (Bosco, di Masi, & Manuti, 2013).

First responders and people in helping professions must constantly deal with other people’s complex problems, usually under time constraints. Dealing with these pressures every day, along with bureaucratic paperwork, may become too much for the worker to bear. Frustration builds, and disillusionment and exhaustion set in—burnout. Importantly, burnout negatively affects the quality of the services people are supposed to receive from the burned-out employee (Rowe & Sherlock, 2005).

Burnout has several bad effects on the brain (Michel, 2016). For instance, highly stressed workers are much less able to regulate negative emotions, resulting from weak- ened connections between the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, and prefrontal cortex (Golkar et al., 2014). Such structural changes probably underlie episodes of poorer judg- ment and emotional outbursts seen in highly stressed people.

We know burnout does not affect everyone in a particular profession. Why? Vallerand (2015) proposes the difference relates to people feeling different types of passion (obses- sive and harmonious) toward their jobs. A passion is a strong inclination toward an activ- ity individuals like (or even love), they value (and thus find important), and where they invest time and energy (Vallerand, 2015). Vallerand’s (2015) Passion Model proposes peo- ple develop a passion toward enjoyable activities that are incorporated into identity.

alienation situation in which workers feel they are doing is meaningless and their efforts are devalued, or when they do not see the con- nection between what they do and the final product.

burnout the depletion of a person’s energy and motivation, the loss of occupational ideal- ism, and the feeling of being exploited.

passion a strong inclination toward an activity that individuals like (or even love), that they value (and thus find important), and in which they invest time and energy.

High-stress jobs such as intensive care nursing often result in burnout.

ER pr

od uc

tio ns

L td

/B le

nd Im

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

396 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Vallerand’s model differentiates between two kinds of passion: obsessive and harmonious. A critical aspect of obsessive passion is the internal urge to engage in the passionate activity, which makes it difficult for the person to fully disengage from thoughts about the activity, leading to conflict with other activities in the person’s life (Vallerand, 2015).

In contrast, harmonious passion results when individuals do not feel compelled to engage in the enjoyable activity; rather, they freely choose to do so, and it is in harmony with other aspects of the person’s life (Vallerand, 2015).

Research in France and Canada indicates the Passion Model accurately predicts employees’ feelings of burnout (Vallerand, 2008; Vallerand et al., 2010). As shown in ◗ ●Figure 12.3, obsessive passion predicts higher levels of conflict that in turn predicts higher levels of burnout. In contrast, harmonious passion predicts higher levels of satis- faction at work, and predicts lower levels of burnout.

The best ways to lower burnout are intervention programs that focus on both the organi- zation and the employee (Awa, Plaumann, & Walter, 2010; Bagnall et al., 2016) and foster pas- sion (Vallerand, 2015). At the organizational level, job restructuring and employee-provided programs are important. For employees, stress-reduction techniques, lowering other people’s expectations, cognitive restructuring of the work situation, and finding alternative ways to enhance personal growth and identity are most effective (e.g., Allexandre et al., 2016).

●◗ Figure 12.3 Model of the relations among pas- sion, satisfaction at work, conflict, and burnout. Harmonious passion predicts higher levels of satisfaction at work that predict lower levels of burnout. In contrast, obsessive pas- sion predicts higher levels of conflict, predicting higher levels of burnout.

.74

.78

.33***

–.33***

.49***

.51***

–.12 .35***

.79

Obsessive passion

Harmonious passion

Conflict

Satisfaction at work

Burnout

***p < 0.001.

SOURCE: From “On the role of passion for work in burnout: A process model,” by R. J. Vallerand, Y. Paquet, F. L. Philippe, and J. Charest, in Journal of Personality, vol. 78.

Recall 1. For most people, the main reason to work

is _______________.

2. Holland’s theory deals with the relationship between occu- pation and _______________.

3. _______________ says that occupational selection is based in part on a person’s self-efficacy regarding his or her occupation.

4. The role of a mentor is part teacher, part sponsor, part model, and part _______________.

5. For many workers, job satisfaction tends to _______________ in midlife.

6. Two salient aspects of job dissatisfaction are alienation and _______________.

Interpret ●● What is the relation between occupational development and

job satisfaction? Would these relations be different in the case of a person with a good match between personality and occu- pation versus a person with a poor match?

●● How could interventions that help people avoid alienation be made culturally sensitive?

Apply ●● If you were the director of the campus career services office,

what would you do to provide students with realistic and accurate information about potential careers?

●● If you were a company’s director of human resources, how would you design a new employee orientation program?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 12.1

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 397

Janice, a 35-year-old African American manager at a business consulting firm, is con- cerned because her career is not progressing as rapidly as she hoped. Janice works hard and receives excellent performance ratings every year. She noticed there are few women in upper management positions in her company. Janice wonders whether she will ever be promoted.

Occupational choice and development are not equally available to all, as Janice is expe- riencing. Gender, ethnicity, and age may create barriers to achieving one’s occupational goals. Men and women in similar occupations may nonetheless have different life experi- ences and probably received different socialization as children and adolescents that made it easier or difficult for them to set their sights on a career. Bias and discrimination also create barriers to occupational success.

Gender Differences in Occupational Selection About 58% of all women over age 16 in the United States participate in the labor force (down from its peak of 60% in 1999), and they represent roughly 47% of the total work- force (U.S. Department of Labor Women’s Bureau, 2016a). Across ethnic groups, Afri- can American women participate the most (about 59%) and Latina women the least (about 56%). Compared to other countries, women in the United States tend to be employed at a higher rate (see ◗ Figure 12.4). Still, structural barriers remain for women in the United States. Let’s take a look at both traditional and nontraditional occupations for women.

What work people do has changed in many ways, not the least in terms of the gender breakdown of workers in specific jobs. A growing number of women work in occupations that have been traditionally male-dominated, such as construction and engineering. The U.S. Department of Labor (2016a) categorizes women’s nontraditional occupations as those in which women constitute 25% or less of the total number of peo- ple employed; the skilled trades (electricians, plumbers, carpenters) still have among the lowest participation rates of women. Data for several occupations can be seen in ◗ Figure 12.5. Note the pay differentials between women and men; we will consider that a bit later.

Despite the efforts to counteract gender stereotyping of occupations, male- dominated occupations tend to pay more than women-dominated occupations (Price, 2016). Although the definition of nontraditional varies across cultures, women who choose nontraditional occupations and are successful in them are viewed negatively as compared with similarly successful men.

In patriarchal societies, both women and men gave higher “respectability” ratings to males than females in the same occupation (Sharma & Sharma, 2012). In the United States, research shows men prefer to date women who are in traditional occupations (Kapoor et al., 2010). Additionally, sexual objectification in the workplace, especially in nontraditional occupations, results in higher sexual harassment and worse work performance (Gervais et al., 2016).

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How do women’s and men’s occupational selection dif-

fer? How are people viewed when they enter occupations not traditional for their gender?

●● What factors are related to women’s occupational development?

●● What factors affect ethnic minority workers’ occupational experiences and occupational development?

●● What types of bias and discrimination hinder the occu- pational development, especially of women and ethnic minority workers?

12.2 Gender, Ethnicity, and Discrimination Issues

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

398 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Women and Occupational Development The characteristics and aspirations of women who entered the workforce in the 1950s and those from the baby boomers (born between 1946 and 1964), generation X (born between 1965 and 1982), and the millennials (born since 1983) are significantly different (Wegman et al., in press). The biggest difference across generations is the progressive increase in opportunities for employment choice.

In the 21st century, women entrepreneurs are starting small businesses but are dis- advantaged in gaining access to capital (McLymont, 2016). As the millennial generation heads into the workforce, it will be interesting to see whether their high degree of tech- nological sophistication and broader experience and background in entrepreneurship will provide more occupational and career options (Wegman et al., in press).

In the corporate world, unsupportive or insensitive work environments, organizational politics, and the lack of occupational development opportunities are most important for women working full-time. Greater empowerment of women is an essential element in ensuring occupational development and remaining in their jobs (Cornwall, 2016). Female professionals leave their jobs for two main reasons. First, the organizations where women work are felt to idealize and reward masculine values of working— individuality, self- sufficiency, and individual contributions—while emphasizing tangible outputs, competitiveness, and rationality. Most women prefer organizations that highly value rela- tionships, interdependence, and collaboration.

●◗ Figure 12.4 Labor force participation rate by sex, race, and Hispanic ethnicity: 2015 annual averages and 2024 projections.

Total Women Men

100

80

60

40

20

90

70

50

30

10

0

P er

ce n

t

100

80

60

40

20

90

70

50

30

10

0

P er

ce n

t

2015 Annual Averages

Total Women Men

Highlight population group

Total

White

Black

Asian

Other

Hispanic

2024 Projections

SOURCE: U.S. Department of Labor. (2016). Latest annual data: Women of working age. Retrieved from www.dol. gov/wb/stats/latest_annual_data.htm.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 399

Second, women may feel disconnected from the workplace. By midcareer, women may conclude they must leave these unsupportive organizations in order to achieve sat- isfaction, growth, and development at work and rewarded for the relational skills they consider essential for success. As we see a bit later, whether women leave their careers or plateau before reaching their maximum potential level in the organization because of lack of support, discrimination, or personal choice is controversial.

Ethnicity and Occupational Development Unfortunately, little research has been conducted from a developmental perspective related to occupational selection and development for people from ethnic minorities. Rather, most researchers focused on the limited opportunities ethnic minorities have and on the structural barriers, such as discrimination, they face.

Women do not differ significantly in terms of participation in nontraditional occupations across ethnic groups (Hegewisch & Hartman, 2014). However, African American women who choose nontraditional occupations tend to plan for more formal education than necessary to achieve their goal. This may actually make them overqualified for the jobs they get; a woman with a college degree may be working in a job that does not require that level of education.

73.7%

71.2%

90.7%

75.8%

70.0%

83.9%

79.0%

73.6%

86.6%

88.4%

87.3%

72.8%

Women as a Percentage of Total Employed in Selected Non-Traditional Occupations, 2014 National Averages

Women’s Median Earnings as a Percentage of Men’s Earnings in Those Occupations, 2014 National Averages

100 020 30 40 50 100

Percent

10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

Percent

Driver/sales workers and truck drivers

Police and sheriff’s patrol officers

Civil engineers

Clergy

FIrst-line supervisors of production and operating workers

Software developers, applications and systems software

Metal workers and plastic workers, all others

Engineering technicians, except drafters

Chefs and head cooks

Computer programmers

Security guards and gaming surveillance officers

Production workers, all others

5.8%

12.4%

16.5%

18.6%

18.6%

19.8%

20.4%

20.5%

21.4%

21.4%

22.6%

24.7%

Notes: • Nontraditional or male-dominated occupations are those in which women represent 25 percent or less of total employed. Occupations include those with a sample size of at least 50,000 people employed. • Median weekly earnings are 2014 annual averages based on full-time wage and salary workers only. http://www.bls.gov/cps/cpsaat39.ht

●◗ Figure 12.5 Women’s nontraditional occupations and median weekly earnings, 2014 annual averages.

SOURCE: www.dol.gov/wb/stats/Nontraditional%20Occupations.pdf.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

400 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Whether an organization is responsive to the needs of ethnic minorities makes a big difference for employees. Ethnic minority employees of a diverse organization in the sev- eral countries report more positive feelings about their workplace when they perceived their organizations as responsive and communicative in supportive and transparent ways (Boehm et al., 2014; Hofhuis, van der Rijt, & Vlug, 2016).

Bias and Discrimination Since the 1960s, numerous laws have been enacted in the United States to prohibit vari- ous types of bias and discrimination in the workplace. Despite anti-discrimination laws, though, bias and discrimination still occur far too frequently. Let’s consider some of the most common forms.

Gender Bias, Glass Ceilings, and Glass Cliffs More than half of all people employed in management, professional, and related occupa- tions are women (U.S. Department of Labor Women’s Bureau, 2016a). However, women are still underrepresented at the top. Janice’s observation in the vignette that few women serve in the highest ranks of major corporations is accurate.

Why are there so few women in such positions? The most important reason is gender discrimination: denying a job to someone solely on the basis of whether the person is a man or a woman. Gender discrimination is still pervasive, and gets worse the higher up the corporate ladder one looks (LeanIn.org, 2016).

Women themselves refer to a glass ceiling, the level they may rise within an organiza- tion but beyond which they may not go. The glass ceiling is a major barrier for women (LeanIn.org, 2016), and one of the most important sources of loss of women leaders ( Heppner, 2013). Men are largely blind to the existence of the glass ceiling.

The glass ceiling is pervasive across higher management and professional workplace settings (Heppner, 2013; LeanIn.org, 2016). Despite decades of attention to the issue, little overall progress is being made in the number of women who lead major corpora- tions or serve on their boards of directors (Cundiff & Stockdale, 2013; LeanIn.org, 2016). The glass ceiling has also been used to account for African American men’s and Asian American men’s lack of advancement in their careers as opposed to European American men ( Cundiff & Stockdale, 2013; Hwang, 2007). It also provides a framework for under-

standing limitations to women’s careers in many countries around the world such as South Africa (Kiaye & Singh, 2013).

Interestingly, a different trend emerges if one examines who is appointed to critical positions in organizations in times of crisis. Research shows that women are more likely put in leadership positions when a company is in crisis. Consequently, women often confront a glass cliff where their leadership position is precarious. Evidence shows companies are more likely to appoint a woman to their board of direc- tors if their financial performance had been poor in the recent past, and women are more likely to be political candidates if the seat is a highly contested one (Ryan, Haslam, & Kulich, 2010; Ryan et al., 2016).

What can be done to eliminate the glass ceiling and the glass cliff? Kolb, Williams, and Frohlinger (2010) argue women can and must be assertive in getting their rightful place at the table by focusing on five key things: drilling deep into the organization so you can make informed decisions, getting critical support, getting the necessary resources, getting buy-in, and making a difference.

Much debate has erupted over the issue of women rising to the top. There is no doubt the glass ceiling and glass cliff exist. The contro- versy surrounds the extent women decide not to pursue or reluctance to pursue the top positions. As discussed in the Current Controversies feature, this debate is likely to rage for years.

gender discrimination denying a job to someone solely on the basis of whether the person is a man or a woman.

glass ceiling the level to which a woman may rise in an organization but beyond which they may not go.

glass cliff a situation in which a woman’s leadership position in an organization is precarious.

Women CEOs of major corporations, such as Mary Barra at General Motors, are still few in number.

ST A

N H

O N

D A

/A FP

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 401

Equal Pay for Equal Work In addition to discrimination in hiring and promotion, women are also subject to salary discrimination. According to the National Women’s Law Center (2016), 98% of all occu- pations show a gender-based wage gap. Overall, the U.S. Department of Labor Women’s Bureau (2016b) notes that women who work full-time earn about 79% of men’s median annual earnings. As you can see in ◗ Figure 12.6, the wage gap depends on ethnicity and has been narrowing in some groups since the 1980s.

Many people have argued that there are legitimate reasons for the wage gap, such as women stepping out of their careers to raise children, or their taking lower paying jobs in the first place. However, evidence of discrimination is apparent; for instance, research indicates that average earnings for individuals who undergo gender reassignment from male to female fall by about one-third (National Women’s Law Center, 2016).

In the United States, the first law regarding pay equity was passed by Congress in 1963. Forty-six years later in 2009, President Obama signed the Lilly Ledbetter Fair Pay Act, showing clearly that gender-based pay inequity still exists. Nearly a decade after this law was enacted, inequity remains (Lips, 2016).

Sheryl Sandberg is unquestionably success-ful. She has held the most important, power- ful positions in the most recognizable technology companies in the world. When she published her book Lean In: Women, Work, and the Will to Lead in 2013, she set off a fierce debate. Sandberg claimed there is discrimination against women in the corporate world. She also argued an important reason women do not rise to the top more often is because of their own unintentional behavior that holds them back. She claimed women do not speak up enough, need to abandon the myth of “having it all,” set boundaries, get a mentor, and not to “check out of work” when thinking about starting a family.

The national debate around these issues raised many issues: Sandberg’s ability to afford to pay for support may make her points irrelevant for women who do not have those resources; she was “blaming the victim”; no one ever puts men in these situations of hav- ing to choose; and so on. When her husband died unexpectedly in 2015, she reassessed some of her arguments and acknowledged her privileged position. Still, she pushed hard on the issue of gender discrimination in the work- place, and founded LeanIn.org, an organiza- tion dedicated to helping women advance in their careers and to educate all in the overt and subtle ways gender discrimination operates.

Through LeanIn.org, Sheryl has also focused on documenting the problems and highlighting best practices in stopping gen- der discrimination. The report Women in the Workplace 2016 provided rich data on numer- ous aspects of the problem.

Sheryl Sandberg has made gender discrimination an important focus of her life. She, and many others, are convinced that limits on women’s careers will never be eliminated without a focused effort on everyone’s part. Can gender discrimination be eliminated? Will women eventually be welcomed into every occupation? What do you think?

Helping Women Lean In?What Do You Think?

●◗ Figure 12.6 Women’s earnings by race and ethnicity as a percentage of white, non-Hispanic men’s earnings, March 1987–2014.

P er

ce n

t

1990 1995 2000 2005 2010 2014

100

95

90

85

80

75

70

65

60

55

50

45

40

Year

White, non-Hispanic Black Asian Hispanic

SOURCE: www.dol.gov/wb/stats/earnings_race_ethnic_percent_white_nonhisp _mens_earn_87_14_txt.htm.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

402 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Why? Consider this: Only one year out of college, a woman earns on average about $0.80 for every $1.00 a male college graduate earns. Over time, the difference gets bigger, not smaller. This is even after controlling for such important variables as occupation, hours worked, parenthood, and other factors associated with pay (Maatz & Hedgepath, 2013).

What if women choose a college major associated with high-paying jobs, such as those in science, technology, engineering, mathematics, and medicine (STEMM)? Will that help reduce the pay differential? No. Even after controlling for differences in academic field, women earn on average 11% less than men in first-year earnings after receiving their doc- toral degree (Shen, 2016). Does choosing to enter a traditionally male-dominated major will solve the problem? No. Women software developers, for example, earn only 81% of their male counterparts earn.

A woman is also significantly disadvantaged when it comes to the division of labor at home if she is married to or living with a man (Shen, 2016). Despite decades of effort in getting men to do more of the housework and child care tasks, little has changed in terms of the amount of time men actually spend on these tasks. In effect, this means women have two careers, one in the workplace and the other at home. If a college-educated woman stays at home to care for a child or parent, then her return to the workforce will be at a lower salary than it would have been otherwise.

Only through a concerted effort on the part of employers and policymakers can the gender-based pay gap be addressed. But more will be needed. Besides fairer pay rules and policies, we will also need a change in attitudes in the men who are the women’s spouses and partners. They will need to step up and do an equal share of the work around the house and in child care. Because unless both aspects are addressed, the pay gap will never be closed.

Sexual Harassment Suppose you have been working hard on a paper for a course and think you’ve done a good job. When you receive an “A” for the paper, you are elated. When you discuss your paper (and your excitement) with your instructor, you receive a big hug. How do you feel? What if this situation involved a major project at work and the hug came from your boss? Your coworker? What if it were a kiss on your lips instead of a hug?

Whether such behavior is acceptable, or whether it constitutes sexual harassment, depends on many situational factors, including the setting, people involved, and the rela- tionship between them.

How many people have been sexually harassed? That’s a hard question to answer for several reasons: there is no universal definition of harassment, men and women have dif- ferent perceptions, and many victims do not report it (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, 2016a). Even given these difficulties, global research indicates between 40% and 50% of women in the European Union, and 30% to 40% of women in Asia-Pacific countries experience workplace sexual harassment (International Labour Organization, 2013). Complaints are filed most often by women, but about 17% of workplace cases are filed by men (Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, 2016b). Although the number of formal complaints in the United States is declining, it is unclear whether this is because of increased sensitivity and training by employers, reluctance of victims to report for fear of losing their jobs during economically difficult times, or both.

What are the effects of being sexually harassed? As you might expect, research evi- dence clearly shows negative job-related, psychological, and physical health outcomes (Holland & Cortina, 2016). These outcomes can affect people for many years after the harassment incident(s).

What can be done to provide people with safe work and learning environments, free from sexual harassment? Training in gender awareness is a common approach that often works, especially given that gender differences exist in perceptions of behavior. However, even very high profile cases of sexual harassment, such as those involving famous people (e.g., Bill Cosby) or well-known companies (e.g., Fox News) show that much remains to be done to eradicate sexual harassment.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 403

Age Discrimination Another structural barrier to occupational development is age discrimination, that involves denying a job or promotion to someone solely on the basis of age. The U.S. Age Discrimination in Employment Act of 1986 protects workers over age 40. A law that brought together all of the anti-discrimination legislation in the United Kingdom, the Equality Act of 2010, includes a prohibition against age discrimination, and more European countries are protecting middle-aged and older workers (Equality and Human Rights Commission, 2016; Lahey, 2010). These laws stipulate people must be hired based on their ability, not their age and that employers cannot segregate or classify workers or otherwise denote their status on the basis of age.

Age discrimination is difficult to document, because employers can use such things as earnings history or other variable appear to be a deciding factor. Or they can attempt to get rid of older workers by using retirement incentives. Or supervisors can let their stereotypes about aging interfere with their assessment of the quality of older workers’ performance. As noted by the Equal Employment Opportunity Com- mission (2016c), treating an older worker less favorably because of her or his age is illegal.

Employment prospects for middle-aged people around the world are lower than for their younger counterparts (Lahey, 2010; Vansteen- kiste, Deschacht, & Sels, 2015). For example, age discrimination toward those over age 45 is common in Hong Kong (Cheung, Kam, & Ngan, 2011), resulting in longer periods of unemployment. Such practices may save companies money in the short run, but the loss of expertise and knowledge comes at a high price. Indeed, an emerging model of employ- ment is boomerang employees, individuals who terminate employment at one point in time but return to work in the same organization at a future time. Boomerang employees sometimes return as employees on the company’s payroll but increasingly are returning as contract workers who are not eligible for benefits, thereby meeting the company’s needs for both expertise and lower costs (Shipp et al., 2014).

Age discrimination usually happens before or after interaction with professional human resources staff by other employees making the hiring decisions, and it can be covert (Lahey, 2010; Pillay, Kelly, & Tones, 2006). For example, employers can make cer- tain types of physical or mental performance a job requirement and argue that older work- ers cannot meet the standard prior to an interview. Or they can attempt to get rid of older workers by using retirement incentives. Supervisors’ stereotyped beliefs sometimes factor in performance evaluations for raises or promotions or in decisions about which employ- ees are eligible for additional training (Sterns & Spokus, 2013).

age discrimination denying a job or a promotion to a person solely on the basis of age.

boomerang employees individuals who terminate employment at one point in time but return to work in the same organization at a future time.

Employers cannot make a decision not to hire this woman solely on the basis of her age.

D en

ni s

W is

e/ D

ig ita

l V is

io n/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Recall 1. Women who choose nontraditional occupations are viewed

_______________ by their peers.

2. Among the reasons women in well-paid occupations leave, _______________ are most important for part-time workers.

3. Ethnic minority workers are more satisfied with and com- mitted to organizations that are responsive and provide _______________.

4. Three barriers to women’s occupational development are sex discrimination, the glass ceiling, and _______________.

Interpret ●● What steps need to be taken to eliminate gender, ethnic, and

age bias in the workplace?

Apply ●● Suppose you are the CEO of a large organization and you

need to make personnel reductions through layoffs. Many of your most expensive employees are over age 40. How can you accomplish this without being accused of age discrimination?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 12.2

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

404 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Fred has 32 years of service for an automobile manufacturer making pickup trucks. Over the years, more and more assembly-line jobs have been eliminated by new technology (including robots) and the export of manufacturing jobs to other countries. Although Fred has been assured his job is safe, he isn’t so sure. He worries he could be laid off at any time.

In the past, people like Fred commonly chose an occupation during young adulthood and stayed in it throughout their working years. Today, however, not many people have that option. Corporations have restructured so often employees now assume occupational changes are part of the career process. Such corporate actions mean people’s conceptions of work and career are in flux and losing one’s job no longer has only negative meanings (Biggs et al., in press; Haworth & Lewis, 2005).

Several factors have been identified as important in determining who will remain in an occupation and who will change. Some factors—such as whether the person likes the occupation—lead to self-initiated occupation changes.

However, other factors—such as obsolete skills and larger economic trends—may cause forced occupational changes. Continued improvement of robots has caused some auto industry workers to lose their jobs; corporations send jobs overseas to increase profits; and economic recessions usually result in large-scale layoffs and high levels of unemployment.

Retraining Workers When you are hired into a specific job, you are selected because your employer believes you offer the best fit between the abilities you already have and those needed to perform the job. As most people can attest, though, the skills needed to perform a job usually change over time. Such changes may be based in the introduction of new technology, addi- tional responsibilities, or promotion.

Unless a person’s skills are kept up-to-date, the outcome is likely to be either job loss or a career plateau (da Costa & Oliveira, 2016; Jiang, 2016). Career plateauing occurs when there is a lack of challenge in one’s job or promotional opportunity in the organization or when a person decides not to seek advancement. Research in Canada (Foster, Lonial, & Shastri, 2011), Asia (Lee, 2003), and Australia (Rose & Gordon, 2010) shows feeling one’s career has plateaued usually results in less organizational commitment, lower job satisfac- tion, and a greater tendency to leave. But attitudes can remain positive if it is only the lack of challenge and not a lack of promotion opportunity responsible for the plateauing (da Costa & Oliveira, 2016; Jiang, 2016).

In cases of job loss or a career plateau, retraining may be an appropriate response. Around the world, large numbers of employees participate each year in programs and courses offered by their employer or by a college or university and aimed at improving existing skills or adding new job skills. For midcareer employees, retraining might focus on how to advance in one’s occupation or how to find new career opportunities—for example, through résumé preparation and career counseling. Increasingly, such programs are offered online in order to make them easier and more convenient for people to access. For people who were involuntarily separated from their employer, severance packages may provide a way to pay for these courses.

career plateauing situation occurring when there is a lack of challenge in the job or promotional oppor- tunity in the organization or when a person decides not to seek advancement.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Why do people change occupations?

●● Is worrying about potential job loss a major source of stress?

●● How does job loss affect the amount of stress experienced?

12.3 Occupational Transitions

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 405

Alternatively, mid-career individuals may choose to change fields altogether. In this case, people may head back to college and earn a credential in a completely different field. Increasingly, middle-aged adults are seeking coaches to help them navigate through the decision to change careers (Stoltz, 2016).

The retraining of midcareer and older workers highlights the need for lifelong learn- ing as a way to stay employable (Froehlich, Beausaert, & Segers, 2016). If corporations are to meet the challenges of a global economy, it is imperative they include retraining in their employee development programs. Such programs will improve people’s chances of advancement in their chosen occupations and also assist people in making successful tran- sitions from one occupation to another.

Occupational Insecurity Over the past few decades, changing economic realities (e.g., increased competition in a global economy), changing demographics, continued advancements in technology, and a global recession forced many people out of their jobs. Heavy manufacturing and sup- port businesses (such as the steel, oil, and automotive industries) and farming were the hardest-hit sectors during the 1970s and 1980s. The service sector (e.g., financial ser- vices) were hard hit during the Great Recession. No one is immune any more from layoff. The Great Recession that began in 2008 put many middle- and upper-level corporate executives out of work worldwide; previously, recession-driven layoffs hit lower-level employees harder.

As a result, many people feel insecure about their jobs much of the time. Economic downturns create significant levels of stress, especially when such downturns create mas- sive job loss (Sinclair et al., 2010). Continued shifts from in-person retail to online retail result in nearly constant retrenchment in retail jobs. The advent of driverless cars even threatens the jobs of taxi, Uber, and Lyft drivers.

Like Fred, the autoworker in the vignette, many worried workers have numerous years of dedicated service to a company. Unfortunately, people who worry about their jobs tend to have poorer physical and psychological well-being (Gonza & Burger, in press; McKee-Ryan et al., 2005). Anxiety about one’s job may result in negative attitudes about one’s employer or even about work in general, and in turn may result in diminished desire to be successful. Whether there is an actual basis for people’s feelings of job insecurity may not matter; sometimes what people think is true about their work situation is more important than what is actually the case. Just the possibility of losing one’s job can negatively affect physical and psychological health.

So how does the possibility of losing one’s job affect employees? Mantler and colleagues (2005) examined coping strategies for comparable samples of laid-off and employed high- technology workers. They found although unemployed participants reported higher levels of stress compared with employed participants, employment uncertainty mediated the asso- ciation between employment status and perceived stress. That is, people who believe their job is in jeopardy—even if it is not—show levels of stress similar to unemployed participants.

This result is due to differences in coping strategies. There are several ways people deal with stress, and two of the more common are emotion-focused coping and problem- focused coping. Some people focus on how the stressful situation makes them feel, so they cope by making themselves feel better about it. Others focus on the problem itself and do something to solve it. People who used emotional avoidance as a strategy reported higher levels of stress, particularly when they were fairly certain of the outcome. Thus, even peo- ple whose jobs aren’t really in jeopardy can report high levels of stress if they tend to use emotion-focused coping strategies.

Coping with Unemployment Losing one’s job can have enormous personal impact that can last a long time ( Gregor-Gonza  & Burger, in press; McKee-Ryan et al., 2005; Norris, 2016). When the overall U.S. unemployment rates hit 10.6% in January 2010, millions of people could relate. Years later, the psychological impact remained (Gregor-Gonza & Burger, in press), even

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

406 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

for people who found other jobs. When unemployment lasts and re-employment does not occur soon, unemployed people commonly experience a wide variety of negative effects (Norris, 2016) that range from a decline in immune system functioning (Cohen et al., 2007) to decreases in well-being (Gregor-Gonza & Burger, in press).

Coping with unemployment involves both financial and personal issues. As noted in the Discovering Development feature, the financial support people receive varies across states and situations. Unemployment compensation is typically much lower than one’s original salary, often resulting in severe financial hardship and difficult choices for indi- viduals. People risk losing their homes to foreclosure, for instance, as well as encountering difficulties with other everyday expenses.

In a comprehensive study of the effects of unemployment, McKee-Ryan and col- leagues (2005) found several specific results from losing one’s job. Unemployed work- ers had significantly lower mental health, life satisfaction, marital or family satisfaction, and subjective physical health (how they perceive their health to be) than their employed counterparts. With reemployment, these negative effects disappear. ◗ Figure 12.7 shows physical and psychological health following job displacement is influenced by several fac- tors (McKee-Ryan et al., 2005).

The effects of job loss vary with age, gender, and education (Norris, 2016). In the United States, middle-aged men are more vulnerable to negative effects than older or younger men—largely because they have greater financial responsibilities than the other two groups and they derive more of their identity from work—but women report more negative effects over time (Bambra, 2010; Norris, 2016). Research in Spain indicates gen- der differences in responding to job loss are complexly related to family responsibilities and social class (Artazcoz et al., 2004). Specifically, to the extent work is viewed as your

Millions of workers in the U.S. struggle daily with the cold reality of employment uncertainty, either because they have already lost their jobs or are worried that they are about to lose them. Perhaps you, or someone you know, is in this situation. As noted in the text, there are many reasons for this, with the most important ones being education (e.g., the need for increased technical skills) and technology (e.g., human jobs replaced by robots).

Acknowledgment of the anxiety and other emotions felt by these workers as well as how to bring them back into the labor force was a central issue in the presidential election of 2016. Hillary Clinton emphasized the need for better access to education and the need to shift jobs from traditional manufacturing and extraction industries (e.g., coal mining) to more technology-focused opportunities. Donald Trump argued that he could and would restore the traditional opportunities for these displaced workers.

In the aftermath of Donald Trump’s victory, analysts (e.g., Kolko, 2016) pointed out that Trump did best in those areas of the country where the economy was in the worst shape

and jobs were most in jeopardy in the future. Many union members, who traditionally had voted for Democratic candidates including Barack Obama, backed Trump because of his message. First-person accounts, such as in J. D. Vance’s Hillbilly Elegy (2016), provide pictures of people whose understanding of the American Dream has been completely disrupted.

In areas of the United States with high unemployment and underemployment, one of the most visible and deadly problems has been a rapid increase in opioid and heroin addiction and overdoses. This, along with other health, personal, and mental effects of the actual or feared loss of one’s job has brought issues once associated with urban areas into the sub- urbs and rural parts of the country.

What to do about the human cost of con- tinuing economic modernization will be one of the most important issues facing the United States and other countries the rest of the 21st century. The solutions are neither obvious nor easy. Without them, though, the human cost will continue to mount.

Real People The Politics of Unemployment

Displaced workers can feel forgotten and can become a potent political force.

Xi nh

ua /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 407

expected contribution to the family, losing one’s job has a more substantial negative effect. Because this tends to be more the case for men than for women, it helps explain the gen- der differences. The higher one’s education levels, the less stress one typically feels imme- diately after losing a job, probably because higher education level usually result in faster re-employment (Mandemakers & Monden, 2013).

Because unemployment rates are substantially higher for African Americans and Latinos than for European Americans (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2016a), the effects of unemployment are experienced by a greater proportion of people in these groups. Economic consequences of unemployment are often especially difficult. Compared to European Americans, it usually takes minority workers significantly longer to find another job.

How long you are unemployed also affects how people react. People who are unem- ployed for at least a year perceive their mental health significantly more negatively than either employed people or those who have removed themselves from the labor force (e.g., have stopped looking for work). For example, suicide risk increases the longer unemployment lasts (Gunnell & Chang, 2016) Those who lost their jobs involuntarily feel a loss of control over their “work” environment and feel less demand placed on them. Importantly, a reasonable amount of “demand” is critical to maintaining good health, whereas too little demand lowers health.

Research also offers some advice for adults who are trying to manage occupational transitions (Ebberwein, 2001):

●● Approach job loss with a healthy sense of urgency. ●● Consider your next career move and what you must do to achieve it, even if there are

no prospects for it in sight. ●● Acknowledge and react to change as soon as it is evident. ●● Be cautious of stopgap employment. ●● Identify a realistic goal and then list the steps you must take to achieve it.

Additionally, the U.S. Department of Labor offers tips for job seekers, as do online ser- vices such as LinkedIn that also provides networking groups. These steps may not guarantee you will find a new job quickly, but they will help create a better sense that you are in control.

●◗ Figure 12.7 Psychological and physical well-being after losing one’s job are affected by many variables.

Cognitive Appraisal • Stress appraisal • Internal attribution • Reemployment

expectation

Coping Strategies

Work-role Centrality

Human Capital and Demographics

Coping Resources • Personal • Social • Financial • Time structure

Psychological Well-being

Physical Well-being

• Mental health • Life satisfaction • Domain satisfaction

• Subjective • Objective

• Job search effort • Problem-focused coping • Emotion-focused coping

SOURCE: From “Psychological and physical well-being during unemployment: A meta-analytic study,” by F.  McKee-Ryan, Z. Song, C. R. Wanberg, and A. J. Kinicki, in Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 90.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

408 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Jennifer, a 38-year-old sales clerk at a department store, feels her husband, Mustafa, doesn’t do his share of the housework or child care. Mustafa says men don’t do housework and he’s really tired when he comes home from work. Jennifer thinks this isn’t fair, especially because she works as many hours as her husband.

One of the most difficult challenges facing adults like Jennifer is trying to balance the demands of occupation with the demands of family. Over the past few decades, the rapid increase in the number of families where both parents are employed has fundamentally changed how we view the relationship between work and family. This can even mean tak- ing a young child to work as a way to deal with the pushes and pulls of being an employed parent. In roughly half of married-couple families, both spouses are employed (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2016b). Why? Families need the dual income to pay their bills and main- tain a moderate standard of living, especially given the relatively flat incomes since the Great Recession.

We will see dual-earner couples with children experience both benefits and disadvan- tages. The stresses of living in this arrangement are substantial, and gender differences are clear—especially in the division of household chores.

The Dependent Care Dilemma Many employed adults must also provide care for dependent children or parents. Deciding how to divide the chores is a major source of stress, as we will see.

Employed Caregivers Many mothers have no option but to return to work after the birth of a child. In fact, about 64% of mothers with children under the age of 6 years, and roughly 75% of those with chil- dren between 6 and 17-years old are in the labor force (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2016c).

Recall 1. One response to the pressures of a global economy and an

aging workforce is to provide _______________.

2. Two factors that may cause involuntary occupational change are economic trends and _______________.

3. Fear of job loss is often a more important determinant of stress than is _______________.

4. The age group that is most at risk for negative effects of job loss is _______________.

Interpret ●● The trend toward multiple careers is likely to continue and

become the norm. What implications will this have for theories of career development?

Apply ●● You have been asked to design a program to help employees

cope with losing their job. What key components would you include in this program?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 12.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the issues faced by employed people who care

for dependents? ●● How do partners view the division of household chores?

What is work–family conflict, and how does it affect couples’ lives?

12.4 Work and Family

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 409

Despite high participation rates, mothers grapple with the decision of whether they want to return to work. Surveys of mothers with preschool children reveal the motivation for returning to work tends to be related to financial need and how attached mothers are to their work. The amount of leave time a woman has matters; the passage of the Family and Medical Leave Act (FMLA) in 1993 entitled workers to take unpaid time off to care for their dependents with the right to return to their jobs. (It should be noted that the United States is the only developed country in the world that does not have a national mandatory paid family leave benefit.)

FMLA resulted in an increase in the number of women who returned to work at least part-time (Schott, 2010). Evidence from the few states with paid family leave show simi- lar trends; for instance, mothers in California who take paid leave extend their time off roughly five weeks longer, and show more work hours during the second year of the child’s life (Baum & Ruhm, 2016).

A concern for many women is whether stepping out of their occupations following childbirth will negatively affect their career paths. Indeed, evidence clearly indicates it does (Evertsson, Grunow, & Aisenbrey 2016; LeanIn.org, 2016). Women in the United States are punished, even for short leaves. In women-friendly countries such as Sweden, long leaves typically result in a negative effect on upward career movement, but with shorter leaves in Sweden and Germany no negative effects are observed. These universally negative impacts in the U.S. need to be taken into account as more states consider imple- menting paid family leave programs.

Often overlooked is the increasing number of workers who must also care for an aging parent or partner. As we saw in Chapter 11, providing this type of care takes a high toll through stress and has a generally negative impact on one’s career.

Whether assistance is needed for one’s children or parents, key factors in selecting an appropriate care site are quality of care, price, and hours of availability (Helpguide.org, 2016; National Association for the Education of Young Children, 2016). Depending on one’s economic situation, it may not be possible to find affordable and quality care avail- able when needed. In such cases, there may be no option but to drop out of the workforce or enlist the help of friends and family.

Dependent Care and Effects on Workers Being responsible for dependent care has significant negative effects on caregivers. Whether responsible for the care of an older parent or a child, women and men report negative effects on their work, higher levels of stress, and problems with coping (Neal & Hammer, 2007). Roxburgh (2002) introduced the notion that parents of families dealing with time pressures feel much more stress; indeed, subsequent research clearly shows not only are stress levels higher, but “fast-forward families” also often deal with negative impacts on career advancement and physical and mental health consequences of this life style (Ochs & Kremer-Sadlik, 2013). Unsurprisingly, women’s careers are usually affected more negatively than men’s.

How can these negative effects be lessened? When women’s partners provide good support and women have average or high control over their jobs, employed mothers are significantly less distressed than employed nonmothers or mothers without support (Cram, Alkadry, & Tower, 2016). One of the most important factors in this outcome is the realization that it is impossible to “have it all” for either mothers or fathers (Cram et al., 2016; LeanIn.org, 2016).

Dependent Care and Employer Responses Employed parents with small children or dependent spouses/partners or parents are confronted with the difficult prospect of leaving them in the care of others. This is especially problematic when the usual care arrangement is unavailable, such as due to weather-related closures of the care facility. A growing need in the workplace is for backup care, that provides emergency care for dependent children or adults so

backup care emergency care for dependent children or adults so the employee does not need to lose a day of work.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

410 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

the employee does not need to lose a day of work. Does provid- ing a workplace care center or backup care make a difference in terms of an employee’s feelings about work, absenteeism, and productivity?

There is no simple answer. Making a child care center available to employees does tend to reduce employee stress, but does not necessarily reduce parents’ work–family conflict or their absentee- ism (Hipp, Morrissey, & Warner, 2017). A “family-friendly” com- pany must also pay attention to the attitudes of their employees and make sure the company provides broad-based support (Aryee et al., 2013; Hill et al., 2016). The keys are how supervisors act and the number and type of benefits the company provides. Cross-cul- tural research in Korea confirms having a family-friendly supervi- sor matters (Aryee et al., 2013). The most important single thing a company can do is allow the employee to leave work without penalty to tend to family needs (Lawton & Tulkin, 2010).

Research also indicates there may not be differences for either mothers or their infants between work-based and nonwork-based child-care centers in terms of the mothers’ ease in transitioning back to work or the infants’ ability to settle into day care (Hill et al., 2016; Hipp et al., 2016; Skouteris et al., 2007).

It will be interesting to watch how these issues— especially flexible schedules—play out in the United States, where such practices are not yet common. A global study of parental leave showed the more generous parental leave policies are, the lower the infant mortality rates, clearly indicating parental leave policies are a good thing (Ferrarini & Norström, 2010).

Juggling Multiple Roles When both members of a heterosexual couple with dependents are employed, who cleans the house, cooks the meals, and takes care of the children when they are ill? This question goes to the heart of the core dilemma of modern, dual-earner couples: How are household chores divided? How are work and family role conflicts handled?

Dividing Household Chores Despite much media attention and claims of increased sharing in the duties, women still perform the lion’s share of housework, regardless of employment status. As shown in ◗ Figure 12.8, this is true globally (OECD, 2016). This unequal division of labor causes the most arguments and the most unhappiness for dual-earner couples. This is the case with Jennifer and Mustafa, the couple in the vignette; Jennifer does most of the housework.

The additional burden women carry with respect to household chores, including child rearing, is still reflected in millennials, despite their endorsement of more gender-equal views on the matter. It appears that deeply held cultural beliefs about gender-based divi- sions of labor are difficult to change. There are indications, though, that change may be happening, slowly. The gap between women’s and men’s time spent on household chores and child rearing is narrowing.

Ethnic differences in the division of household labor are also apparent. In Mexican American families with husbands born in Mexico, men help more when family income is lower and their wives contribute a proportionately higher share of the household income (Pinto & Coltrane, 2009). Comparisons of Latino, African American, and European American men consistently show European American men help with the chores less than Latino or African American men (Omori & Smith, 2009).

Work–Family Conflict When people have both occupations and children, they must figure out how to balance the demands of each. These competing demands cause work–family conflict, the feeling of being pulled in multiple directions by incompatible demands from one’s job and one’s family.

work–family conflict the feeling of being pulled in multiple directions by incompatible demands from job and family.

Employers who provide day care and after-school centers on-site have more satisfied employees.

M ik

e G

re en

la r/

Th e

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 411

●◗ Figure 12.8 Time spent in unpaid work and leisure (minutes per day).

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

0

50

100

150

Care for Household Members

Routine Housework

TV or Radio at Home

Sports Sleeping M

in u

te s

p er

d ay

M in

u te

s p

er d

ay M

in u

te s

p er

d ay

M in

u te

s p

er d

ay M

in u

te s

p er

d ay

M in

u te

s p

er d

ay M

in u

te s

p er

d ay

Women

Men

514

Canada (2010)

44

133

83 99

123

21 32

507 493

21

Finland (2009-10)

137 91 111

147

30 3731 13

507

France (2009) 158

98 103

124

24 3735

15

513 506

Italy (2008-09) 204

57

106 123

25 37

23 10

526 520

Japan (2011)

27 7

199

24

140 127

14 17

456 472

Mexico (2009)

53

15

280

75 71 86

8 15

488 496

United States (2010)

41 19

126

82

136

162

12 25

522 509

SOURCE: OECD. (2016). Balancing paid work, unpaid work and leisure. Retrieved from www.oecd.org/gender/data/ balancingpaidworkunpaidworkandleisure.htm.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

412 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Dual-earner couples must find a balance between their occupational and family roles. Many people believe work and family roles influence each other: When things go badly at work, the family suffers, and when there are troubles at home, work suf- fers. That’s true, but the influence is not the same in each direction (Andreassi, 2011). Whether work influences family or vice versa is a complex function of personality, cop- ing skills, support resources, type of job, and a host of other issues that interact (Repetti & Wang, 2017). One key but often overlooked factor is whether the work schedules of both partners allow them to coordinate activities such as child care (van Klaveren, van den Brink, & van Praag, 2013). Another is the ability of a spouse/partner to provide emotional and social support for the other spouse/partner when work stress increases (Repetti & Wang, 2017).

Understanding work–family conflict requires taking a life-stage approach to the issue (Blanchard-Fields, Baldi, & Constantin, 2004). The availability of support for employed parents that takes the child’s developmental age into account (e.g., day care for young chil- dren, flexible work schedules when children are older) goes a long way to helping parents balance work and family obligations.

A comprehensive review of the research on the experience of employed mothers sup- ports this conclusion (Edwards, 2012). How juggling the demands of housework and child care affect women depends on the complex interplay among the age of the children, the point in career development and advancement the woman is, and her own developmental phase. The combination of challenges that any one of these reflects changes over time. Because all of these factors are dynamic, how they help or hinder a woman in her career changes over time. What is certain is that women’s careers are likely to be irreparably harmed without it (LeanIn.org, 2016).

In addition to having impacts on each individual, dual-earner couples often have dif- ficulty finding time for each other, especially if both work long hours. The amount of time together is not necessarily the most important issue; as long as the time is spent in shared activities such as eating, playing, and conversing, couples tend to be happy (Ochs & Kre-

mer-Sadlik, 2013). Actively soliciting support from one’s spouse or part- ner generally brings results, that in turn results in happier relationships (Wang & Repetti, 2016)

When both partners are employed, getting all of the schedules to work together smoothly can be a major challenge. However, ensuring joint family activities are important for creating and sustaining strong relations among family members. Unfortunately, many couples find by the time they have an opportunity to be alone together, they are too tired to make the most of it.

The issues faced by dual-earner couples are global: burnout from the dual demands of work and parenting is more likely to affect women across many cultures (Aryee et al, 2013; van Klaveren et al., 2013; Spector et al., 2005). Japanese career women’s job satisfaction declines, and turnover becomes more likely, to the extent they have high work– family conflict (Honda-Howard & Homma, 2001). Research in China revealed a signifi- cant relation between overcommitment and work-family conflict on the one hand with symptoms of depression on the other (Kan & Yu, 2016).

The work-family conflicts described here are arguably worse for cou- ples in the United States because Americans work more hours with fewer vacation days than any other developed country (Frase & Gornick, 2013). You may be thinking that the obvious solution is to legislate shorter work schedules. Surprisingly, it’s not that simple. It turns out that in the 32 countries Ruppanner and Maume (2016) studied, there is no reduction in work-to-family interference. In fact, the opposite occurs—there is an increase in such interference when work hours are reduced. Why? The most likely answer is that reduced work hours results in higher expecta- tions of better work-family balance, so people get more sensitive to even little disruptions, thereby increasing feelings of unhappiness.

Dual-earner couples must learn how to grapple with work–family conflict in balanc- ing job and family demands.

Pu re

st oc

k/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 413

Claude is a 55-year-old electrician who has enjoyed outdoor activities his whole life. From the time he was a boy he fished the trout rivers and snow-skied in the mountains of Montana. Although he doesn’t ski double black diamond runs any more, Claude still enjoys both sports every chance he gets.

Adults do not work every waking moment of their lives. As each of us knows, we need to disconnect from our smartphones, relax, and engage in leisure activities. Intuitively, lei- sure consists of activities not associated with work. Leisure is discretionary activity that includes simple relaxation, activities for enjoyment, and creative pursuits. Simply finding the time to fit leisure into an already busy schedule can be challenging. For too many people, leisure just becomes another scheduled component in our overall time manage- ment problem (Corbett & Hilty, 2006).

Types of Leisure Activities Leisure can include virtually any activity. To organize the options, researchers classified leisure activities into several categories. Jopp and Hertzog (2010) developed an empiri- cally based set of categories that includes a wide variety of activities: physical (e.g., lifting weights, backpacking, jogging), crafts (e.g., woodworking, household repairs), games (e.g., board/online games, puzzles, card games), watching TV, social-private (e.g., going out with a friend, visiting relatives, going out to dinner), social-public (e.g., attending a club meet- ing, volunteering), religious (e.g., attending a religious service, praying), travel (e.g., travel

leisure a discretionary activity that includes simple relaxation, activities for enjoyment, and creative pursuits.

Recall 1. Parents report lower work–family conflict and have lower

absenteeism when supervisors are sympathetic and sup- portive regarding _______________.

2. Men are satisfied with an equitable division of labor based on _______________, whereas women are satisfied _______________.

Interpret ●● What can organizations do to help ease work–family conflict?

Apply ●● Suppose you are the vice president for human resources and

you are thinking about creating a way to help employees who face dependent care issues. What factors will you need to consider before implanting a plan?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 12.4

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What activities are leisure activities? How do people

choose among them? ●● What changes in leisure activities occur with age?

●● What do people derive from leisure activities?

12.5 Taking Time to Relax: Leisure Activities

So what is a couple to do? For one thing, they can work together to help mitigate the stress. Most important, they can negotiate schedules around work commitments through- out their careers, taking other factors such as child care and additional time demands into account (van Wanrooy, 2013). These negotiations should include discussion of such joint activities as meals and other family activities, too (Ochs and Kremer-Sadlik, 2013). In short, communicate about all of the demands and come to an understanding and compro- mise that provides an optimal solution for all concerned.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

414 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

abroad, travel out of town), experiential (e.g., collect stamps, read for leisure, gardening, knitting), devel- opmental (e.g., read as part of a job, study a foreign language, attend public lecture), and technology use (e.g., photography, use computer software, play an instrument).

More complete measures of leisure activi- ties not only provide better understanding of how adults spend their time, but can help in clinical set- tings. Declines in the frequency of leisure activi- ties is associated with depression (Schwerdtfeger & Friedrich-Mei, 2009), with lower well-being (Paggi, Jopp, & Hertzog, 2016), and with a later diagnosis of dementia (Hertzog et al., 2009). Monitoring changes in leisure activity levels during and after intervention programs can provide better outcomes assessments.

Given the wide range of options, how do people pick their leisure activities? Apparently, each of us

has a leisure repertoire, a personal library of intrinsically motivated activities we do regu- larly and we take with us into retirement (Nimrod, 2007a, b). The activities in our reper- toire are determined by two things: perceived competence (how good we think we are at the activity compared to other people our age) and psychological comfort (how well we meet our personal goals for performance). As you might expect, men and women differ in their views of leisure, as do people in different ethnic and age groups (van der Pas & Koopman-Boyden, 2010).

Personality factors are related to one’s choice of leisure activities (Gaudron & Vautier, 2007), and it is possible to construct interest profiles that map individuals to specific types of leisure activities, and to each other (Leuty, Hansen, & Speaks, 2016). Other fac- tors are important as well: income, health, abilities, transportation, education, and social characteristics. Some leisure activities, such as downhill skiing, are relatively expensive and require transportation and reasonably good health and physical coordination for maximum enjoyment. In contrast, reading requires minimal finances (if one uses a public library or other free sources) and is far less physically demanding.

The use of computer technology in leisure activities has increased dramatically. Most usage involves e-mail, Facebook, Twitter, or other social networking tools for such activi- ties as keeping in touch with family and friends, pursuing hobbies, and lifelong learning. Multi-participant video streaming and multiplayer interactive computer gaming have also increased among adult players. All of these online activities provide opportunities to cre- ate virtual friendship networks that provide the same types of support as traditional face- to-face networks.

Developmental Changes in Leisure Cross-sectional studies report age differences in leisure activities. Emerging adults partici- pate in a greater range of activities than middle-aged adults. Furthermore, emerging adults prefer intense leisure activities that provide a “rush,” such as rock climbing, whitewater kayaking, and hang gliding. In contrast, middle-aged adults focus more on home-based and family-oriented activities. In later middle age, they spend less of their leisure time in strenuous physical activities and more in sedentary activities such as reading and watching television and in moderately strenuous activities such as tennis and hiking (van der Pas & Koopman-Boyden, 2010).

Longitudinal studies of changes in individuals’ leisure activities over time show con- siderable stability in leisure interests over reasonably long periods. Specifically, stud- ies in the United States, Finland, Great Britain, and Japan show that level of activity in young adulthood predicts activity level later in life, and leisure physical activity in emerg- ing adulthood bodes well for health later in life (Hillsdon et al., 2005; Lahti et al., 2016).

Participating in leisure activities improves one’s well-being.

M au

ro R

od rig

ue s/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 415

Claude, the 55 -year-old in the vignette who likes to fish and ski, is a good example of this overall trend. As Claude demonstrates, frequent participation in particular leisure activi- ties earlier in life tends to continue into adulthood. Similar findings hold for the pre- and postretirement years. Apparently, one’s preferences for certain types of leisure activities are established early in life; they tend to change over the life span primarily in terms of how physically intense they are. Explore these findings in more detail in the Spotlight On Research feature.

On ResearchSpotlight

Long-Term Effects of Leisure Activities

Who were the investigators and what was the aim of the study? It is well established that physical activity is related to better health at all ages in adulthood. However, much of the existing research has focused on more formal exercise pro- grams, with much less attention paid to leisure forms of activity, and whether lei- sure time physical activity during emerging adulthood is related to subsequent health and well-being in midlife. To answer these questions, Jouni Lahti and colleagues (2016) followed adults in Finland, Great Britain, and Japan.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? The investigators used the Short Form-36 health questionnaire to assess physical and mental health. The eight subscales of the SF-36 include: physical functioning, role limitations due to physical problems, bodily pain, general health percep- tions, mental health, role limitations due to

emotional problems, social functioning and vitality. Scores can range from 0 to 100. Low scores imply poor health functioning, whereas high scores imply good health function- ing. The SF-36 has very good reliability and validity.

Who were the participants in the study? Participants in the study were prospective employee cohorts originally recruited for the Finnish HHS study (2000–2002 and 2007, N 59585 ), British WHII I study (1997–1999 and 2003–2004, N 41425 ) and Japanese Civil Servants Study (JACS) (1998–1999 and 2003, N 17685 ). Across the three study samples, participants ranged in age from 20 to 60.

What was the design of the study? The research used a longitudinal design. As noted in Chapter 1, the strength of longitudinal designs is being able to measure a behavior over time in the same individual; the con- found is the age of the person at the time of measurement.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? All participants were informed of the

nature of the larger study and had given their consent to participate.

What were the results? Leisure physical activity was associated with better subse- quent physical health functioning in all three cohorts. However, results varied somewhat across country and gender. Differences were the clearest among Finnish women (inactive: 46.0, active vigorous: 49.5) and men (inac- tive: 47.8, active vigorous: 51.1) and British women (inactive: 47.3, active vigorous: 50.4). For mental health functioning, the differences were generally smaller and not clearly related to the intensity of physical activity.

What did the investigators conclude? Lahti and colleagues concluded that vigorous physical activity was associated with better subsequent physical health functioning in all three cohorts, although with some differences in magnitude across countries. For mental health functioning, the intensity of physical activity was less important. Promoting leisure time physical activity may prove useful for the maintenance of health functioning among midlife employees.

Consequences of Leisure Activities What do people gain from participating in leisure activities? Researchers have long known involvement in leisure activities is related to well-being (Paggi et al., 2016; Warr, Butcher, & Robertson, 2004). This relation holds in other countries, such as China, as well (Dai, Zhang, & Li, 2013). Research shows participating in leisure activities helps promote bet- ter mental health (e.g., Lahti et al., 2016). This is especially true for spouses who use family-based leisure as a means to cope during their spouse/partner’s military deployment (Werner & Shannon, 2013), and buffers the effects of stress and negative life events. It even helps lower the risk of mortality (Talbot et al., 2007).

Studies show leisure activities provide an excellent forum for the interaction of the biopsychosocial forces discussed in Chapter 1 (Cheng & Pegg, 2016; Kleiber, 2013). Leisure activities are a good way to deal with stress, which—as we have seen in Chapter 4—has significant biological effects. This is especially true for unforeseen negative events, such as cancer (Chun et al., 2016). Psychologically, leisure activities have been well documented as one of the primary coping mechanisms people use, such as providing a sense of purpose in life (Chun et al., 2016; Patry, Blanchard, & Mask, 2007).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

416 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

How people cope by using leisure varies across cultures depending on the various types of activities that are permissible and available. Likewise, leisure activities vary across social class; basketball is one activity that cuts across class because it is inexpensive, whereas downhill scuba diving is more associated with people who can afford to travel to diving resorts and pay the fees.

How do leisure activities provide protection against stress? Kleiber and colleagues (2002; Kleiber, 2013) offer four ways leisure activities serve as a buffer against negative life events:

●● Leisure activities distract us from negative life events. ●● Leisure activities generate optimism about the future because they are pleasant. ●● Leisure activities connect us to our personal past by allowing us to participate in the

same activities over much of our lives. ●● Leisure activities can be used as vehicles for personal transformation.

Whether the negative life events we experience are personal, such as the loss of a loved one, or societal, such as a terrorist attack, leisure activities are a common and effec- tive way to deal with them. They truly represent the confluence of biopsychosocial forces and are effective at any point in the life cycle.

Participating with others in leisure activities may also strengthen feelings of attach- ment to one’s partner, friends, and family (Carnelley & Ruscher, 2000). Adults use leisure as a way to explore interpersonal relationships or to seek social approval. In fact, research indicates marital satisfaction is linked with leisure time; marital satisfaction is even helped when couples spend some leisure time with others in addition to spending it just as a couple (Zabriskie & Kay, 2013). But there’s no doubt couples who play together are happier (Johnson, Zabriskie, & Hill, 2006).

There is a second sense of attachment that can develop as a result of leisure activi- ties: place attachment. Place attachment occurs when people derive a deep sense of per- sonal satisfaction and identity from a particular place (Di Masso, Dixon, & Hernández, 2017). Place attachment is an active process, bringing the individual a sense of belonging that might not be experienced elsewhere. As a result, place attachment generally drives people to return to the location over and over, and the meaning of being there continues to deepen over time.

What if leisure activities are pursued seriously? In some cases, people create lei- sure–family conflict by engaging in leisure activities to extremes (Heo et al., 2010). When things get serious, problems may occur. Only when there is support from others for such extreme involvement are problems avoided. For instance, professional quilters felt much more valued when family members were supportive (Stalp & Conti, 2011). As in most things, moderation in leisure activities is probably best, unless you know you have excel- lent support.

You have probably heard the saying “no vacation goes unpunished.” It appears to be true, and the trouble is not just afterward. Research shows pre-vacation workload is associated with lower health and well-being for both men and women, and pre-vacation homeload (extra work that needs to be done at home) has the same negative effect for women (Nawijn, de Bloom, & Geurts, 2013).

Once on vacation, it matters what you do. If you detach from work, enjoy the activi- ties during vacation, and engage in conversation with your partner, then the vacation can improve health and well-being, even after you return home (de Bloom, Geurts, & Kompier, 2012). Indeed, some have suggested that vacations and tourism be prescribed like a medication due to its positive benefits. However, workers report high post-vacation workloads eliminate most of the positive effects of a vacation within about a week (de Bloom et al., 2010).

One frequently overlooked outcome of leisure activity is social acceptance. For persons with disabilities, this is a particularly important consideration (Choi, Johnson, & Kriewitz, 2013). There is a positive connection between frequency of leisure activities and positive identity, social acceptance, friendship development, and acceptance of differences. These findings highlight the importance of designing inclusive leisure activity programs.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Recall 1. Activities in which people engage for relaxation or, enjoy-

ment or as creative pursuits are considered _______________ activities.

2. Compared with younger adults, middle-aged adults prefer leisure activities that are more family- and home-centered and _______________.

3. Being involved in leisure activities is related to _______________.

Interpret ●● How are choices of leisure activities related to physical, cogni-

tive, and social development?

Apply ●● Workers in the United States tend to take fewer vacation days

compared with workers in European countries. What might the consequences of this be for U.S. workers?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 12.5

12.1 Occupational Selection and Development

how do people view work?.

●● Although most people work for money, other reasons are highly variable.

●● Meaning-mission fit can set the tone for an organization and help people derive meaning.

how do people choose their occupations?

●● Holland’s theory is based on the idea people choose occu- pations to optimize the fit between their individual traits and their occupational interests. Six personality types, representing different combinations of these, have been identified. Support for these types has been found in several studies.

●● Social cognitive career theory emphasizes how people choose careers is also influenced by what they think they can do and how well they can do it, as well as how moti- vated they are to pursue a career.

●● Super’s developmental view of occupations is based on self-concept and adaptation to an occupational role. Super describes five stages in adulthood: implementation, estab- lishment, maintenance, deceleration, and retirement.

What factors influence occupational development?

●● Reality shock is the realization one’s expectations about an occupation are different from what one actually experi- ences. Reality shock is common among young workers.

●● Few differences exist across generations in terms of their occupational expectations.

●● A mentor or developmental coach is a co-worker who teaches a new employee the unwritten rules and fosters occupational development. Mentor–protégé relationships, like other relationships, develop through stages over time.

What is the relationship between job satisfaction and age?

●● Older workers report higher job satisfaction than younger workers, but this may be partly due to self-selection; unhappy workers may quit. Other reasons include intrinsic satisfaction, good fit, lower importance of work, finding nonwork diversions, and life-cycle factors.

●● Alienation and burnout are important considerations in understanding job satisfaction. Both involve significant stress for workers.

●● Vallerand’s Passion Model proposes people develop a pas- sion toward enjoyable activities that are incorporated into identity. Obsessive passion happens when people experience an uncontrollable urge to engage in the activity; harmonious passion results when individuals freely accept the activity as important for them without any contingencies attached to it.

12.2 Gender, Ethnicity, and Discrimination Issues

how do women’s and men’s occupational selection differ? how are people viewed when they enter occupations that are not traditional for their gender?

●● Boys and girls are socialized differently for work, and their occupational choices are affected as a result. Women choose nontraditional occupations for many reasons, including expectations and personal feelings. Women in such occupations are still viewed more negatively than men in the same occupations.

What factors are related to women’s occupational development?

●● Women leave well-paid occupations for many reasons, including family obligations and workplace environment. Women who continue to work full-time have adequate child care and look for ways to further their occupational development.

SUMMARY

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 417

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

418 pA RT T h R E E : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

●● The glass ceiling, that limits women’s occupational attain- ment, and the glass cliff, that puts women leaders in a precarious position, affect how often women achieve top executive positions and how successful women leaders are.

What factors affect ethnic minority workers’ occupational experiences and occupational development?

●● Vocational identity and vocational goals vary in different ethnic groups. Whether an organization is sensitive to ethnicity issues is a strong predictor of satisfaction among ethnic minority employees.

What types of bias and discrimination hinder the occupa- tional development, especially of women and ethnic minority workers?

●● Gender bias remains the chief barrier to women’s occupa- tional development. In many cases, this operates as a glass ceiling. Pay inequity is also a problem; women are often paid less than what men earn in similar jobs.

●● Sexual harassment is a problem in the workplace. Current criteria for judging harassment are based on the “reason- able person” standard.

●● Denying employment to anyone over 40 because of age is age discrimination and is illegal.

12.3 Occupational Transitions

Why do people change occupations?

●● Important reasons people change occupations include per- sonality, obsolescence, and economic trends. Occupational change can be voluntary or involuntary.

●● To adapt to the effects of a global economy and an aging workforce, many corporations are providing retraining opportunities for workers. Retraining is especially impor- tant in cases of outdated skills and career plateauing.

Is worrying about potential job loss a major source of stress?

●● Occupational insecurity is a growing problem. Fear that one may lose one’s job is a better predictor of anxiety than the actual likelihood of job loss.

how does job loss affect the amount of stress experienced?

●● Job loss is a traumatic event that can affect every aspect of a person’s life. The degree of financial distress and the extent

of attachment to the job are the best predictors of distress. Job loss and job insecurity an influence political outcomes through people’s anxieties about their future.

12.4 Work and Family

What are the issues faced by employed people who care for dependents?

●● Caring for children or aging parents creates dilemmas for workers. Whether a woman returns to work after having a child depends largely on how attached she is to her work. Simply providing child care on-site does not always result in higher job satisfaction. A more important factor is the degree that supervisors are sympathetic.

how do partners view the division of household chores? What is work–family conflict? how does it affect couples’ lives?

●● Although women have reduced the amount of time they spend on household tasks over the past two decades, they still do most of the work. European American men are less likely than either African American or Latino American men to help with traditionally female household tasks.

●● Flexible work schedules and the number of children are important factors in role conflict. Recent evidence shows work stress has a much greater impact on family life than family stress has on work performance. Some women pay a high personal price for having careers.

12.5 Taking Time to Relax: Leisure Activities

What activities are leisure activities?

●● Leisure activities can be simple relaxation, activities for enjoyment, or creative pursuits. Views of leisure activities varies by gender, ethnicity, and age.

What changes in leisure activities occur with age?

●● As people grow older, they tend to engage in leisure activi- ties that are less strenuous and more family-oriented. Lei- sure preferences in adulthood reflect those earlier in life.

What do people derive from leisure activities?

●● Leisure activities enhance well-being and can benefit all aspects of people’s lives.

12.1 1. to earn a living 2. personality 3. Social Cognitive Career Theory or SCCT 4. counselor 5. increase 6. burnout 12.2 1. negatively 2. family obligations 3. positive work environments 4. pay discrimination 12.3 1. worker retraining 2. obsolete skills 3. actual likelihood of job loss 4. middle-aged adults 12.4 1. family issues and child care 2. the number of hours spent; when men perform traditionally female chores 12.5 1. leisure 2. less physically intense 3. well-being

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A p T E R 1 2 : W O R K I N G A N D R E L A x I N G 419

meaning-mission fit (389) career construction theory (389) social cognitive career theory (SCCT)

(390) vocational maturity (390) reality shock (393) mentor (393) developmental coach (393)

job satisfaction (394) psychological capital theory (394) alienation (395) burnout (395) passion (395) gender discrimination (400) glass ceiling (400) glass cliff (400)

age discrimination (403) boomerang employee (403) career plateauing (404) backup care (409) work–family conflict (410) leisure (413)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

A nd

y D

ea n

Ph ot

og ra

ph y/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

421

There’s an old saying that life begins at 40. Per-haps that was society’s way of saying that the changes that occur around this time of life—wrinkles,

gray hair, and a bulging waistline—were OK.

But times—and attitudes—have changed. Increas-

ingly, middle age is being viewed as a time when

adults achieve new heights in cognitive development,

reevaluate their personal goals and change their

behavior if they choose, develop adult relationships

with their children, and ease into grandparenthood.

Along the way, they must deal with stress, changes

in the way they learn, and the challenges of helping

their aging parents. People’s health is generally good,

and their earnings are at their peak.

These days, people think that it’s much harder to

define middle age and put much less emphasis on

chronological age than on perceived age. Eight in ten

Brits believe that the line between young and old is much fuzzier than it used to

be (benenden health, 2013). Let’s take a closer look at the issues confronting

people during midlife.

Making It in Midlife The Biopsychosocial Challenges of Middle Adulthood

13.1 Physical Changes and Health 422 Changes in Appearance 422 Changes in Bones and Joints 423 Reproductive Changes 427

WHAT DO YOU THINK? Menopausal Hormone Therapy 429

Stress and Health 430 Exercise 434

13.2 Cognitive Development 436 Practical Intelligence 436 Becoming an Expert 438 Lifelong Learning 439

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Designing Software for Middle-Aged and Older Nonexperts 440

13.3 Personality 441 The Five-Factor Trait Model 441 Changing Priorities in Midlife 443

13.4 Family Dynamics and Middle Age 447 Letting Go: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Children 447 Giving Back: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Aging

Parents 448

REAL PEOPLE: APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Taking Care of Mom 448

Grandparenthood 450

    Summary 453

    Key Terms 454

13

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

422 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

By all accounts, LuTimothy is extremely successful. Among other things, he became the head of a moderate-sized manufacturing firm by the time he was 43. LuTimothy has always considered himself to be a rising young star in the company. Then one day he found more than the usual number of hairs in his brush. “Oh no!” he exclaimed. “I can’t be going bald! What will people say?” What does LuTimothy think about these changes?

One morning when you least expect it, you’ll be staring into the bathroom mirror when the reality of middle age strikes. Standing there, peering through half-awake eyes, you see it. One solitary gray hair or one tiny wrinkle at the corner of your eye—or, like LuTimothy, some excess hairs falling out—and you worry that your youth is gone, your life is over, and you will soon be acting the way your parents did when they embarrassed you in your younger days. Middle-aged people become concerned that they are over-the-hill, some- times going to great lengths to prove that they are still vibrant.

Crossing the boundary to middle age in the United States is typically associated with turning 40 (or “the big four-oh,” as many people term it). This event is frequently marked with a special party, and the party often has an over-the-hill motif. Such events are soci- ety’s attempt to create a rite of passage between youth and maturity.

As people move into middle age, they begin experiencing some of the physical changes associated with aging. In this section, we focus on the changes most obvious in middle- aged adults: appearance, reproductive capacity, and stress and coping. In Chapter 14, we will consider changes that may begin in middle age but are usually not apparent until later in life, such as slower reaction time and sensory changes. A critical factor in setting the stage for healthy aging is living a healthy lifestyle in young adulthood and middle age. Eat- ing a healthy diet and exercising regularly across adulthood can help reduce the chances of chronic disease later in life (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013).

Changes in Appearance On that fateful day when the hard truth stares back at you in the bathroom mirror, it probably doesn’t matter to you that getting wrinkles and gray hair is universal and inevitable. Wrinkles are caused by changes in the structure of the skin and its con- nective and supporting tissues as well as by the cumulative effects of damage from exposure to sunlight and cigarette smoke (Blume-Peytavi et al., 2016; Tobin, 2017). You have control over how quickly your face wrinkles, mainly by limiting your exposure to the sun.

It may not make you feel better to know that gray hair is perfectly natural and caused by a normal cessation of pigment production in hair follicles. Male pattern baldness, a genetic trait in which hair is lost progressively beginning at the top of the head, often begins to appear in middle age. No, the scientific evidence that gradual thinning and gray- ing of hair with age is nearly universal isn’t what matters most (Adhikari et al., 2016). What matters is that these changes are affecting you.

To make matters worse, you also may have noticed that your clothes aren’t fitting properly even though you carefully watch what you eat. You remember a time not very long ago when you could eat whatever you wanted; now it seems that as soon as you look at food you put on weight. Your perceptions are correct; most people gain weight between

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How does appearance change in middle age?

●● What changes occur in bones and joints?

●● What reproductive changes occur in men and women in middle age?

●● What is stress? How does it affect physical and psycho- logical health?

●● What benefits are there to exercise?

13.1 Physical Changes and Health

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 423

their early 30s and mid-50s, producing the infamous “middle-aged bulge” as metabolism slows down (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013).

People’s reactions to these changes in appearance vary. LuTimothy wonders how peo- ple will react to him now that he’s balding. Some people rush out to purchase hair coloring and wrinkle cream, or investigate hair plug procedures. Others just take it as another stage in life. You’ve probably experienced several different reactions yourself. There is a wide range of individual differences, especially those between men and women and across cul- tures. For example, certain changes on men in Western society are viewed as positive, but the same changes in women are not.

Changes in Bones and Joints The bones and the joints change with age, sometimes in potentially preventable ways and sometimes because of genetic predisposition or disease. Let’s take a closer look.

Osteoporosis One physical change that can be potentially serious is loss of bone mass. Skeletal maturity, the point at which bone mass is greatest and the skeleton is at peak development, occurs at around 18 for women and 20 in men (Gilsanz & Ratib, 2012; National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, 2015a). Bone mass stays about the same until women experience menopause and men reach late life. For women, there is a rapid loss of bone mass in the first few years after menopause, which greatly increases the risk of prob- lems with disease and broken bones.

Loss of bone mass makes bones weaker and more brittle, thereby making them easier to break. Because there is less bone mass, bones also take longer to heal in middle-aged and older adults. Severe loss of bone mass results in osteoporosis, a disease in which bones become porous and extremely easy to break (see ◗ Figure 13.1). In severe cases, osteoporosis can cause spinal vertebrae to collapse, causing the person to stoop and to become shorter (Bartl, Bartl, & Frisch, 2015; National Osteoporosis Foundation, 2016a; see ◗ Figure 13.2). About 40 million Americans either have osteoporosis or are at high risk due to low bone density. Non-Latina white women and Asian women are at highest risk, and osteoporosis is the leading cause of broken bones in older women. Although the severe effects of osteopo- rosis typically are not observed until later life, this disease can occur in people in their 50s.

Osteoporosis is caused in part by having low bone mass at skeletal maturity (the point at which your bones reach peak development), deficiencies in calcium and vitamin D, estrogen depletion, and lack of weight-bearing exercise that builds up bone mass. Other risk factors include smoking; high-protein diets; and excessive intake of alcohol, caffeine, and sodium. Women who are being treated for asthma, cancer, rheumatoid arthritis, thy- roid problems, or epilepsy are also at increased risk because the medications used can lead to the loss of bone mass.

skeletal maturity The point at which bone mass is greatest and the skeleton is at peak development, occurs at around 18 for women and 20 in men.

osteoporosis disease in which bones become porous and extremely easy to break.

●◗ Figure 13.1 The difference between normal bone (on the right) and osteoporosis (on the left) is easy to see.

Osteoporotic bone tissue Normal bone tissue

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

424 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

The National Osteoporosis Foundation (2016b) recommends getting enough vita- min D and dietary calcium as ways to prevent osteoporosis. There is evidence that cal- cium supplements after menopause may slow the rate of bone loss and delay the onset of osteoporosis, but benefits appear to be greater when the supplements are provided before menopause. People should consume foods (such as milk, sardines, collard greens, broccoli, or kale) that are high in calcium and should also take calcium supplements if necessary. Recommended calcium intake for men and women of various ages is shown in ◗ Table 13.1. Although sunlight is a good source for vitamin D, due to the risk of skin cancer from overexposure to the sun, most people get their recommended amounts of vitamin D from dietary sources (National Osteoporosis Foundation, 2016c).

Women who are late middle-aged or over age 65 are encouraged to have their bone mineral density (BMD) tested by having a dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) test, which measures bone density at the hip and spine. The DXA test results are usually com- pared with the ideal or peak bone mineral density of a healthy 30-year-old adult, and you are given a T-score. A score of 0 means that your bone mineral density is equal to the norm for a healthy young adult. A T-score between +1 and -1 is considered normal or healthy. The greater the negative number, the more severe the osteoporosis (National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, 2015b).

Lowering the risk of osteoporosis involves dietary, medication, and activity approaches (National Osteoporosis Foundation, 2016a, b). Some evidence also supports the view that

dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) test Test of bone mineral density (BMd) at the hip and spine.

●◗ Figure 13.2 Notice how osteoporosis eventually causes a person to stoop and to lose height, owing to compression of the vertebrae.

Based on Ebersole, P., & Hess, P. (1998). Toward healthy aging (5th ed., p. 395).

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 425

taking supplemental magnesium, zinc, vitamin K, and special forms of fluoride may be effective. Estrogen replacement is effective in preventing women’s bone loss after meno- pause but is controversial because of potential side effects (as discussed later). There is also evidence that regular weight-bearing exercise (e.g., weight lifting, jogging, or other exercise that forces you to work against gravity) is beneficial.

In terms of medication interventions, bisphosphonates are the most commonly used and are highly effective, but can have serious side effects if used over a long time (Adler et al., 2016). Bisphosphonates slow the bone breakdown process by helping to maintain bone density during menopause. Research indicates that using bisphosphonates for up to five years appears relatively safe if followed by stopping the medication (called a “drug holi- day”); there is evidence for protective effects lasting up to five years more. Periodic reeval- uation of people taking medications to assess whether continued medication treatment is needed is crucial to appropriate and effective use of these drugs (Adler et al., 2016).

Raloxifene (e.g., Evista) is also approved for the treatment and prevention of osteopo- rosis. It is one of a relatively new group of drugs known as selective estrogen receptor modulators. Selective estrogen receptor modulators (SerMs) are not estrogens, but are compounds that have estrogen-like effects on some tissues and estrogen-blocking effects on other tissues.

The newest classes of medications for osteoporosis are the RANK ligand inhibitors, such as denosumab (Lipton et al., 2014). Related research indicates that romosozumab is effective in stimulating bone growth in postmenopausal women (McClung et al., 2014). Although these medications are very promising, additional research will be needed to ensure that they are effective over the long run and that side effects are minimal.

selective estrogen receptor modulators (SerMs) Compounds that are not estrogens, but have estrogen-like effects on some tissues and estrogen-blocking effects on other tissues.

Table 13.1

Recommended Calcium and Vitamin D Intakes

Age CAlCium

(milligrAms) VitAmin D

(internAtionAl units)

infAnts

Birth–6 months 200 400

6 months–1 year 260 400

ChilDren/Young ADults

1–3 years 700 600

4–8 years 1,000 600

9–13 years 1,300 600

14–18 years 1,300 600

ADult Women AnD men

19–30 years 1,000 600

31–50 years 1,000 600

51- to 70-year-old males 1,000 600

51- to 70-year-old females 1,200 600

Over 70 years 1,200 800

PregnAnt or lACtAting Women

18 years or younger 1,300 600

19–50 years 1,000 600

SOURCE: Food and Nutrition Board, Institute of Medicine, National Academy of Sciences, 2010, www.niams.nih.gov/ Health_Info/Bone/Osteoporosis/osteoporosis_hoh.asp#calcium.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

426 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Arthritis Many middle-aged and older adults have good reason to complain of aching joints. Begin- ning in the 20s, the protective cartilage in joints shows signs of deterioration, such as thin- ning and becoming cracked and frayed. Two types of arthritis can result: osteoarthritis and rheumatoid arthritis. These diseases are illustrated in ◗ Figure 13.3.

Over time and repeated use, the bones underneath the cartilage become damaged, which can result in osteoarthritis, a disease marked by gradual onset and progression of pain and disability, with minor signs of inflammation (National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, 2014a). The disease usually becomes noticeable in late middle age or early old age, and it is especially common in people whose joints are subjected to routine overuse and abuse, such as athletes and manual laborers. Because it is caused by overuse and abuse of joints, osteoarthritis is considered a wear-and-tear dis- ease. Pain from osteoarthritis typically is worse when the joint is used, but skin redness, heat, and swelling are minimal or absent. Osteoarthritis usually affects the hands, spine, hips, and knees, sparing the wrists, elbows, shoulders, and ankles. Effective management consists mainly of certain steroids and anti-inflammatory drugs, rest, nonstressful exer- cises that focus on range of motion, diet, and a variety of homeopathic remedies.

A second form of arthritis is rheumatoid arthritis, a more destructive disease of the joints that also develops slowly and typically affects different joints and causes other types of pain and more inflammation than osteoarthritis (National Institute of Arthritis and Musculo- skeletal and Skin Diseases., 2014b). Most often, a pattern of morning stiffness and aching develops in the fingers, wrists, and ankles on both sides of the body. Joints appear swollen.

The American College of Rheumatology (Singh et al., 2016) has adopted guidelines for treating rheumatoid arthritis. The typical low-level therapy consists of aspirin or other nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, such as Advil or Aleve. Newer treatments include disease-modifying anti-rheumatic drugs (DMARDs) (such as hydroxychol- orquine and methotrexate) that limit the damage occurring in the joints, glucocorticoids, and TNF-alpha inhibitors that act as an anti-inflammatory agent and have been shown to stop the disease’s progression in some patients. Rest and passive range-of-motion exercises are also helpful.

Both osteoarthritis (Warner & Valdes, 2016) and rheumatoid arthritis (Yarwood, Huizinga, & Worthington, 2016) have genetic components. Although the exact nature of these inheritance factors are unknown, several potential locations have been identified as possible markers. Further advances in our knowledge of these genetic links could result in more effective and more individualized treatments.

osteoarthritis Most common form of arthritis, a disease marked by gradual onset of bone dam- age with progression of pain and disability together with minor signs of inflammation from wear and tear.

rheumatoid arthritis More destructive disease of the joints that develops slowly; it typically affects differ- ent joints and causes different types of pain than osteoarthritis does.

●◗ Figure 13.3 Rheumatoid arthritis versus osteoarthritis. Osteoarthritis, the most common form of arthritis, involves the wearing away of the cartilage that caps the bones in your joints. With rheumatoid arthritis, the synovial membrane that protects and lubri- cates joints becomes inflamed, causing pain and swelling. Joint erosion may follow.

Bone

Fibrous sheath

Synovial membrane

Cartilage

Normal Joint Rheumatoid Arthritis Osteoarthritis

Bone spurs Bone loss

Worn cartilage

Worn cartilage

Synovial changesThickened

synovial membrane

SOURCE: MayoClinic.com. 2016. Arthritis. www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-conditions/arthritis/home/ovc-20168903

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 427

Surgical interventions may be an option if medications do not provide relief. For example, arthroplasty, or the total replacement of joints damaged by arthritis, contin- ues to improve as new materials help artificial joints last longer. Hip and knee replace- ment surgery is becoming both more common and more effective as less invasive surgical techniques are developed that dramatically reduce recovery time. When joints become inflamed, surgeons may be able to remove enough affected tissue to provide relief. In some cases, cartilage may be transplanted into a damaged joint. These latter two approaches help patients avoid full joint replacement, generally viewed as the method of last resort.

Osteoporosis, osteoarthritis, and rheumatoid arthritis can appear similar and cause similar symptoms., As we have seen, though, they are different diseases requiring different treatment approaches. Comparisons among osteoporosis, osteoarthritis, and rheumatoid arthritis can be seen in ◗ Table 3.2.

Reproductive Changes If you watched any television recently, you undoubtedly saw programs and advertisements showing middle-aged and older couples who clearly have active sex lives. Belsky (2007) reports that couples can and often do have sexual relationships that are very much alive and may be based on a newfound or re-found respect and love for each other. A major

Table 13.2

Similarities and Differences Among Osteoporosis, Osteoarthritis, and Rheumatoid Arthritis

osteoPorosis osteoAr-

thritis rheumAtoiD

Arthritis

risk fACtors X X

Age-related X X

Menopause X

Family history X X X

Use of certain medications such as glucocorticoids or seizure medications

X

Calcium deficiency or inadequate X

vitamin D

Inactivity X

Overuse of joints X

Smoking X

Excessive alcohol X

Anorexia nervosa X

Excessive weight X

PhYsiCAl effeCts

Affects entire skeleton X

Affects joints X X

Is an autoimmune disease X

Bony spurs X X

Enlarged or malformed joints X X

Height loss X

SOURCES: National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases (2010), www.niams.nih.gov/Health _Info/Bone/Osteoporosis/Conditions_Behaviors/osteoporosis_arthritis.asp.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

428 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

national survey by AARP (Fisher, 2010) found that middle-aged adults not only tend to continue to enjoy active sex lives but also enjoy romantic weekends, and about six of every ten middle-aged men and women report that a satisfying sex life is important for their quality of life.

Still, middle age brings changes to the reproductive systems of men and women. These changes are more significant for women, but men also experience certain changes. Let’s see what they are and how people learn to cope with them.

Reproductive Changes in Women As women enter midlife, they experience a major biological process called the climacteric, during which they pass from their reproductive to nonreproductive years. Menopause is the point at which menstruation stops. Men do not endure such sweeping biological changes but experience several gradual changes. These changes have important psychological implications because midlife is thought by many to be a key time for people to redefine themselves, an issue we will examine later in this chapter. For example, some women view climacteric as the loss of the ability to have children, whereas others view it as a liberating change because they no longer need to worry about getting pregnant.

The major reproductive change in women during adulthood is the loss of the natural ability to bear children. (Pregnancy and childbirth are still possible, though, through medi- cal intervention.) This change begins in the 40s as menstrual cycles become irregular, and by age 50 to 55 it is usually complete (MedlinePlus, 2014). This time of transition from regular menstruation to menopause is called perimenopause, and how long it lasts varies considerably. The gradual loss and eventual end of monthly periods is accompanied by decreases in estrogen and progesterone levels, changes in the reproductive organs, and changes in sexual functioning.

A variety of physical and psychological symptoms may accompany perimenopause and menopause with decreases in hormonal levels (WomensHealth.gov, 2010): hot flashes, night sweats, headaches, sleep problems, mood changes, more urinary infections, pain during sex, difficulty concentrating, vaginal dryness, less interest in sex, and an increase in body fat around the waist. Many women report no symptoms at all, but most women experience at least some, and there are large differences across social, ethnic, and cultural groups in how they are expressed (Nosek, Kennedy, & Gudmundsdottir, 2012). For exam- ple, women in the Mayan culture of Mexico and Central America welcome menopause

and its changes as a natural phenomenon and do not attach any stigma to aging (Mahady et al., 2008). In the United States, Latinas and African Americans, espe- cially working-class women, tend to view menopause more positively, whereas Euro- pean American women describe it more negatively (Dillaway et al., 2008). Women in South American countries report a variety of symptoms that impair quality of life, many of which persisted five years beyond menopause (Blümel et al., 2012).

The decline in estrogen that women experience after menopause is a very big deal. Estrogen loss is related to numerous health conditions, including increased risk of osteoporosis, cardiovascular disease, stress urinary incontinence (involuntary loss of urine during physical stress, as when exercising, sneezing, or laughing), weight gain, and memory loss, in short, almost every major body system (Women’s Health Research Institute, 2016). Consider just one negative effect: cardiovascular disease. At age 50 (prior to menopause) women have three times less risk of heart attacks than men on average. Ten years after menopause, when women are about 60, their risk equals that of men. Clearly, estrogen depletion has a negative effect on health.

In response to these increased risks and to the estrogen-related symptoms that  women experience, one approach is the use of menopausal hormone therapy (MHT): women take low doses of estrogen, which is often combined with progestin (syn- thetic form of progesterone). Hormone therapy is controversial and has been the focus of many research studies with conflicting results (North American Menopause Society, 2016). There appear to be both benefits and risks with MHT, as discussed in the What Do You Think? feature.

climacteric Biological process during which women pass from their reproductive to nonrepro- ductive years.

menopause point at which menstruation stops.

perimenopause individually varying time of transition from regular menstruation to menopause.

menopausal hormone therapy (MHT) Therapy in which women take low doses of estrogen, which is often combined with progestin (a synthetic form of progesterone).

Despite physical changes associated with middle age, women and men continue to enjoy sexual activity.

Bi g

Ch ee

se S

pe ci

al /B

ig C

he es

e Ph

ot o

LL C/

A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 429

For many years, women have had the choice of taking medications to replace the female hormones that are no longer pro- duced naturally by the body after menopause. Hormone therapy may involve taking estrogen alone or in combination with progesterone (or progestin in its synthetic form). Research on the effects of menopause hormone therapy have helped clarify the appropriate use of such medications.

Until about 2003, it was thought that menopausal hormone therapy was beneficial for most women, and results from several studies were positive. But results from the Women’s Health Initiative research in the United States and from the Million Women Study in the United Kingdom indicated that, for some types of MHT, there were several potentially serious side effects. As a result, physicians are now far more cautious in rec- ommending MHT.

The Women’s Health Initiative (WHI), begun in the United States in 1991, was a very large study (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Insti- tute, 2010). The estrogen plus progestin trial used 0.625 milligram of estrogen taken daily plus 2.5 milligrams of medroxyprogesterone acetate (Prempro) taken daily. This combina- tion was chosen because it is the mostly commonly prescribed form of the combined hormone therapy in the United States and, in several observational studies, had appeared to benefit women’s health. The women in the WHI estrogen plus progestin study were aged 50 to 79 when they enrolled in the study between 1993 and 1998. The health

of study participants was carefully monitored by an independent panel called the Data and Safety Monitoring Board (DSMB). The study was stopped in July 2002 because investiga- tors discovered a significant increased risk for breast cancer and that overall the risks outnumbered the benefits. However, in addi- tion to the increased risk of breast cancer, heart attack, stroke, and blood clots, MHT resulted in fewer hip fractures and lower rates of colorectal cancer.

The Million Women Study began in 1996 and includes one in four women over age 50 in the United Kingdom, the largest study of its kind ever conducted. Like the Women’s Health Initiative, the study examines how MHT (both estrogen/progestin combinations and estrogen alone) affects breast cancer, cardiovascular disease, and other aspects of women’s health. Results from this study confirmed the Wom- en’s Health Initiative outcome of increased risk for breast cancer associated with MHT.

A third important study is the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS). In the primary study, 727 women were randomly assigned to three groups. One group received a dose of Premarin that was lower than in the WHI study, another group received a patch, and the third group received a placebo. Both treatment groups experienced a drop in menopausal symptoms. Most important, all three groups showed similar changes in cardiovascular function and no significant changes in biomarkers, no significant differ- ences in breast cancer rates, or in rate of blood clots.

The combined results from the WHI, the Million Women Study, and the KEEPS led physicians to modify their recommendations regarding MHT (Moyer, Miller, & Faubion, 2016; North American Menopause Soci- ety, 2016; Women’s Health Research Institute, 2016). Specifically, the joint recommendation of the American Society for Reproductive Medicine, the Endocrine Soci- ety, and the North American Menopause Society is that MHT is reasonably safe for healthy women under age 60 who have moderate to severe menopausal symptoms. Advances in genomics is likely to make MHT more precise because of the increasing ability to tailor the hormone therapy to each woman’s own genetics (Moyer et al., 2016; Panay, 2016).

MHT is not recommend for women over age 60 or for those who have significant risk factors for side effects (e.g., blood clots). Additionally, women over age 60 who begin MHT are at increased risk for certain cancers.

In sum, women face difficult choices when deciding whether to use MHT as a means of combatting certain menopausal symptoms and protecting themselves against other dis- eases. For example, MHT can help reduce hot flashes and night sweats, help reduce vaginal dryness and discomfort during sexual inter- course, slow bone loss, and perhaps ease mood swings. On the other hand, MHT can increase a woman’s risk of blood clots, heart attack, stroke, breast cancer, and gallbladder disease.

Menopausal Hormone TherapyWhat Do You Think?

Women’s genital organs undergo progressive change after menopause. The vaginal walls shrink and become thinner, the size of the vagina decreases, vaginal lubrication is reduced and delayed, and the external genitalia shrink somewhat. These changes have important effects on sexual activity, such as an increased possibility of painful intercourse and a lon- ger time and more stimulation needed to reach orgasm. Failure to achieve orgasm is more common in midlife and beyond than in a woman’s younger years. However, maintaining an active sex life throughout adulthood lowers the degree to which problems are encountered. Despite these changes, there is no physiological reason not to continue having an active and enjoyable sex life from middle age through late life. The vaginal dryness that occurs, for example, can be countered by using personal lubricants, such as K-Y or Astroglide.

Whether women continue to have an active sex life has a lot more to do with the avail- ability of a suitable partner than a woman’s desire for sexual relations. This is especially true for older women. The AARP Modern Maturity sexuality study (AARP, 1999), the Sex in America study (AARP, 2005), and the Sex, Romance, and Relationships (AARP, 2010) studies all found that older married women were far more likely to have an active sex life than unmarried women. The primary reason for the decline in women’s sexual activity

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

430 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

with age is the lack of a willing or appropriate partner, not a lack of physical ability or desire (AARP, 1999, 2005, 2010).

Reproductive Changes in Men Unlike women, men do not have a physiological (and cultural) event to mark reproductive changes, although there is a gradual decline in testosterone levels (Gunes et al., 2016) that can occur to a greater extent in men who are obese or have diabetes (Nigro & Christ-Crain, 2012). Men do not experience a complete loss of the ability to father children, as this var- ies widely from individual to individual, but men do experience a normative decline in the quantity of sperm (Gunes et al., 2016). However, even at age 80 a man is still half as fertile as he was at age 25 and is quite capable of fathering a child.

With increasing age the prostate gland enlarges, becomes stiffer, and may obstruct the urinary tract. Prostate cancer becomes a real threat during middle age; annual screenings are often recommended for men over age 50 (American Cancer Society, 2012).

Men experience some physiological changes in sexual performance. By old age, men report less perceived demand to ejaculate, a need for longer time and more stimulation to achieve erection and orgasm, and a much longer resolution phase during which erection is impossible (Gunes et al., 2016). Older men also report more frequent failures to achieve orgasm and loss of erection during intercourse (AARP, 1999, 2005, 2010). However, the advent of Viagra, Cialis, and other medications to treat erectile dysfunction has provided older men with easy-to-use medical treatments and the possibility of an active sex life well into later life.

As with women, as long as men enjoy sex and have a willing partner, sexual activity is a lifelong option. Also as with women, the most important ingredient of sexual intimacy for men is a strong relationship with a partner (AARP, 1999, 2005, 2010). For example, married men in early middle age tend to have intercourse four to eight times per month, but this rate drops about 20% per decade. The loss of an available partner is a significant reason frequency of intercourse decreases with age. Still, late middle-aged and older people continue to enjoy themselves. Nearly three-quarters of people 57 to 64 had sex during the past year, as did over half of those 65 to 74 and over 25% of those 75 to 85 ( Watson, 2013). Practicing safe sex is equally important for older adults, as sexually transmitted disease remains a major concern.

As with women, if men enjoy sex and have a willing partner, sexual activity is a lifelong option. Like women, the most important ingredient of sexual intimacy for men is a strong relationship with a partner (Fisher, 2010).

Stress and Health You know what it feels like to be stressed. Whether it’s from the upcoming exam in this course, the traffic jam you sat in on your way home yesterday, or the demands your chil- dren or your job place on you, stress seems to be everywhere.

There is plenty of scientific evidence that over the long term, stress is very bad for your health. But despite thousands of scientific studies that result in our certainty about

what stress does to us, scientists still cannot agree on a formal defi- nition of what stress is. What is clear is that stress involves both physiological and psychological aspects (Levy & Bavishi, in press).

The most widely applied approaches to stress involve: (a) focusing on the physiological responses the body makes through the nervous and endocrine systems; and (b) the idea that stress is what people define as stressful. Let’s consider each in more detail.

Stress as a Physiological Response There is widespread agreement across many research studies that people differ in their physiological responses to stress (Campbell & Ehlert, 2012; Laurent et al., 2016). Prolonged exposure to stress results in damaging influences from the sympathetic nervous system (which controls such things as heart rate, respiration, perspiration, blood

Think About It Why does sexual desire remain largely unchanged despite the biological changes that are occurring?

Work-related stress is a major problem around the world and can have serious negative effects on physical and psychologi- cal health.

Su sa

n Vo

ge l/

U pp

er Cu

t I m

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 431

flow, muscle strength, and mental activity) and a weakening of the immune system. As Cohen and colleagues (2012) discussed in their model of these effects, prolonged stress has a direct causative effect on susceptibility to a wide range of diseases, from the common cold to cardio- vascular disease to cancer. At the cellular level, stress may play a role in shortening telomeres (see Chapter 3; Mathur et al., 2016).

Gender differences in physiological stress responses have also been documented. For example, there is some evidence that the hormone oxytocin plays a different role in women than in men. Oxytocin is the hormone important in women’s reproductive activi- ties and for establishing strong bonds with one’s children (Kim, Strathearn, & Swain, 2016). Researchers speculate that when stressed, men opt for a “flight or fight” approach whereas women opt for a “tend and befriend” approach (Taylor, 2006). Fischer-Shofty, Levkovitz, and Shamay-Tsoory (2013) showed that oxytocin improves accurate percep- tion of social interactions, but in different ways in men and women. In men, performance improved only for competition recognition, whereas in women it improved for kinship recognition.

The Stress and Coping Paradigm Suppose you are stuck in a traffic jam. Depending on whether you are late for an impor- tant appointment or have plenty of time on your hands, you will probably feel very differ- ent about your situation. The stress and coping paradigm views stress not as an environmental stimulus or as a response but as the interaction of a thinking person and an event (Lazarus, 1984; Lazarus et al., 1985; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). How we interpret an event such as being stuck in traffic is what matters, not the event itself or what we do in response to it. Put more formally, stress is “a particular relationship between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being” (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 19). Note that this definition states that stress is a transactional process between a person and the environment, that it takes into account personal resources, that the person’s appraisal of the situation is key, and that unless the situation is considered to be threatening, challeng- ing, or harmful, stress does not result. A diagram of the transactional model is shown in ◗ Figure 13.4.

stress and coping paradigm framework that views stress not as an envi- ronmental stimulus or as a response, but as the interaction of a thinking person and an event.

●◗ Figure 13.4 An example of a transactional model of stress.

Stressors (environmental

demands)

Perceived stress

Benign or positive appraisal

Negative emotional responses

Positive emotional responses

Blend Images/Shutterstock.com

Pressmaster/Shutterstock.com

ARENA Creative/Shutterstock.com

Appraisal of demands

and adaptive capacities

Physiological or behavioral

responses

Increased risk of

physical and psychiatric

disease

SOURCE: From Cohen, S., Kessler, R. C., & Gordon, L. U. (eds.). Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists, Copyright © 1995 by Oxford University Press. Used with permission from Oxford University Press.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

432 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

problem-focused coping Attempts to tackle a problem head-on.

Appraisal Lazarus and Folkman (1984) describe three types of appraisals of stress. Primary appraisal categorizes events into three groups based on the significance they have for our well-being: irrelevant, benign or positive, and stressful. Primary appraisals filter the events we experi- ence. Specifically, any event that is appraised as either irrelevant (things that do not affect us) or as benign or positive (things that are good or at least neutral) is not stressful. So, we literally decide which events are potentially stressful and which ones are not. This is an important point for two reasons. First, it means we can effectively sort out the events that may be problems and those that are not, allowing us to concentrate on dealing with life’s difficulties more effectively. Second, it means that we could be wrong about our reading of an event. A situation that may appear at first blush to be irrelevant, for example, may actu- ally be very important, or a situation deemed stressful initially may turn out not to be. Such mistakes in primary appraisal could set the stage for real (or imagined) crises later on.

If a person believes that an event is stressful, a second set of decisions, called second- ary appraisal, is made. Secondary appraisal evaluates our perceived ability to cope with harm, threat, or challenge. Secondary appraisal is the equivalent of asking three questions: “What can I do?” “How likely is it that I can use one of my options successfully?” and “Will this option reduce my stress?” How we answer these questions sets the stage for addressing them effectively. For example, if you believe there is something you can do in a situation that will make a difference, then your perceived stress may be reduced, and you may be able to deal with the event successfully. In contrast, if you believe there is little that you can do to address the situation successfully or reduce your feelings of stress, then you may feel powerless and ineffective, even if others around you believe there are steps you could take.

Sometimes, you learn additional information or experience another situation that indicates you should reappraise the original event. reappraisal involves making a new primary or secondary appraisal resulting from changes in the situation. For example, you may initially dismiss an accusation that your partner is cheating on you (i.e., make a pri- mary appraisal that the event is irrelevant), but after being shown pictures of your partner in a romantic situation with another person, you reappraise the event as stressful. Reap- praisal can either increase stress (if your partner had initially denied the encounter) or lower stress (if you discovered that the photographs were fakes).

The three types of appraisals demonstrate that determining whether an event is stress- ful is a dynamic process. Initial decisions about events may be upheld over time, or they may change in light of new information or personal experience. Different events may be appraised in the same way, and the same event may be appraised differently at any two points in time. This dynamic process helps explain why people react the way they do over the life span. For example, as our physiological abilities change with increasing age, we may have fewer physical resources to handle particular events. As a result, events that were appraised as not stressful in young adulthood may be appraised as stressful in late life.

Coping During the secondary appraisal of an event labeled stressful in primary appraisal, we may believe there is something we can do to deal with the event effectively. Collectively, these attempts to deal with stressful events are called coping. Lazarus and Folkman (1984) view coping more formally as a complex, evolving process of dealing with stress that is learned. Much like appraisals, coping is seen as a dynamic, evolving process that is fine-tuned over time. Our first attempt might fail, but if we try again in a slightly different way we may suc- ceed. Coping is learned, not automatic. That is why we often do not cope very well with stressful situations we are facing for the first time (such as the end of our first love relation- ship). The saying “practice makes perfect” applies to coping, too. Also, coping takes time and effort. Finally, coping entails only managing the situation; we need not overcome or control it. Indeed, many stressful events cannot be fixed or undone; many times the best we can do is to learn to live with the situation. It is in this sense that we may cope with the death of a spouse.

People cope in different ways. Two important ways are problem-focused coping and emotion-focused coping. Problem-focused coping involves attempts to tackle the

primary appraisal process that categorizes events into three groups based on the significance they have for one’s well-being: irrelevant, benign or positive, and stressful.

secondary appraisal process that evaluates one’s perceived abil- ity to cope with harm, threat, or challenge.

reappraisal process of making a new primary or sec- ondary appraisal resulting from changes in the situation.

coping Attempt to deal with stressful events.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 433

problem head-on. Taking medication to treat a disease and spending more time studying for an examination are examples of problem-focused coping with the stress of illness or failing a prior test. In general, problem-focused coping entails doing something directly about the problem at hand. emotion-focused coping involves dealing with one’s feelings about the stressful event. Allowing oneself to express anger or frustration over becoming ill or failing an exam is an example of this approach. The goal here is not necessarily to elimi- nate the problem, although this may happen. Rather, the purpose may be to help oneself deal with situations that are difficult or impossible to tackle head-on.

Several other behaviors can also be viewed in the context of coping. Many people use their relationship with God as the basis for their coping (Bade, 2012; Kinney et al., 2003). For believers, using religious coping strategies usually results in positives outcomes when faced with negative events.

How well we cope depends on several factors. For example, healthy, energetic people are better able to cope with an infection than frail, sick people. Psychologically, a positive attitude about oneself and one’s abilities is also important. Good problem-solving skills put people at an advantage by creating several options with which to manage the stress. Social skills and social support are important in helping people solicit suggestions and assistance from others. Finally, financial resources are important; having the money to pay a mechanic to fix your car allows you to avoid the frustration of trying to do it yourself.

The most effective ways to deal with stress are through various relaxation techniques. Whether you prefer yoga, visualization, progressive muscle relaxation, meditation or contem- plative prayer, massage, or just chilling does not really matter. All good relaxation methods have similar effects: slowing the pulse, lowering blood pressure, slowing down breathing, reducing tension, focusing concentration, lowering anger and fatigue, and boosting confidence.

Keep in mind that the number of stressful events, per se, is less important than one’s appraisal of them and whether the person has effective skills to deal with them. Of course, should the number of stressful issues exceed one’s ability to cope, then the number of issues being confronted would be a key issue.

Effects of Stress on Physical Health How does stress affect us? If the stress is short, such as being stuck in a traffic jam for an hour when we’re already late in an otherwise relaxed day, the answer is that it probably will have little effect other than on our temper. But if the stress is continuous, or chronic, then the picture changes dramatically.

There is ample evidence that perceived stress is related to brain structures; for instance, the size of the hippocampus, a brain structure intimately involved in cognition is smaller in people who report moderate to high levels of chronic stress (Lindgren, Berg- dahl, & Nyberg, 2016). Likewise, chronic stress has been clearly shown to have very signifi- cant negative effects on health, including pervasive negative effects on the immune system that cause increased susceptibility to viral infections, increased risk of atherosclerosis and hypertension, and impaired memory and cognition, as well as psychopathology (Frank et al., 2016; Webster-Marketon & Glaser, 2008). Effects can last for decades; severe stress experienced in childhood has effects that last well into adulthood (Shonkoff et al., 2012).

Research indicates that different types of appraisals that are interpreted as stressful create different physiological outcomes (Frank et al., 2016; Webster-Marketon & Glaser, 2008). This may mean that how the body reacts to stress depends on the appraisal process; the reaction to different types of stress is not the same. In turn, this implies that changing people’s appraisal may also be a way to lower the impact of stress on the body.

Research indicates that one of the most serious consequences of chronic stress is that it increases the level of LDL cholesterol, which has significant negative consequences (see Chapter 3; McKay, 2016). LDL cholesterol levels rise as a result of chronic stress for several reasons: people stop exercising, eat more unhealthy foods, and have higher levels of cor- tisol and adrenaline (which stimulate the production of triglycerides and free fatty acids, which in turn increase LDL cholesterol levels over time). High levels of LDL cholesterol are associated with cardiovascular disease and stroke (see Chapter 3).

emotion-focused coping dealing with one’s feelings about a stressful event.

Think About It How might life experience and cognitive developmental level influence one’s ability to appraise and cope with stress?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

434 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Effects of Stress on Psychological Health Experiencing stress can trigger psychological processes and reactions. Although stress does not directly cause psychopathology, it does influence how people react and behave. For example, the stress many people experienced after the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001, resulted in higher levels of anxiety experienced through nightmares, flashbacks, insomnia, traumatic grief, emotional numbing, and avoidance (LeDoux & Gorman, 2001).

One very important effect of stress on psychological health is post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The National Institute of Mental Health (2016f ) defines post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as an anxiety disorder that can develop after exposure to a terrify- ing event or ordeal in which grave physical harm occurred or was threatened. The kinds of traumatic events that may trigger PTSD include violent personal assaults, natural or human-caused disasters, accidents, or military combat.

PTSD affects nearly 8 million Americans and can be a debilitating condition. Because not everyone who is exposed to very stressful situations develops PTSD, our understand- ing of what causes it is limited. Treatment is typically done through psychotherapy, with the addition of certain antidepressant medications in limited circumstances.

Because stress is such a ubiquitous aspect of life, much effort (and consumer spending) goes into stress reduction techniques. For example, one popular approach is mindfulness- based stress reduction, being aware and nonjudgmental of whatever is happening at that moment (University of Massachusetts Medical School Center for Mindfulness, 2017). Common approaches using mindfulness-based stress reduction include yoga and medita- tion. Research indicates that using mindfulness-based techniques has many positive effects on physical and psychological health (Sharma, 2014). In addition, mindfulness techniques result in functional and structural changes in the brain, especially in areas related to control of stress and emotion as well as attention, memory, and sensory processing (Esch, 2014).

Exercise Since the ancient Greeks, physicians and researchers have known that exercise signifi- cantly slows the aging process. Indeed, evidence suggests a program of regular exercise, in conjunction with a healthy lifestyle, can slow the physiological aging process and improve the immune system (Bartlett & Huffman, 2017; Parrella & Vormittag, 2017). Being seden- tary is absolutely hazardous to your health.

post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) An anxiety disorder that can develop after exposure to a terrifying event or ordeal in which grave physical harm occurred or was threatened.

mindfulness-based stress reduction Being aware and nonjudgmental of what- ever is happening at that moment.

Engaging in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program such as yoga throughout middle age is a great way to counter a stressful lifestyle. M

ar ku

s G

an n/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 435

Adults benefit from aerobic exercise, exercise that places moderate stress on the heart by maintaining a pulse rate between 60% and 90% of the person’s maximum heart rate. You can calculate your maximum heart rate by subtracting your age from 220. Thus, if you are 40 years old, your target range would be 108 to 162 beats per minute. The minimum time necessary for aerobic exercise to be of benefit depends on its intensity; at low heart rates, sessions may need to last an hour, whereas at high heart rates, 15 minutes may suffice. Examples of aerobic exer- cise include jogging, step aerobics, Zumba®, swimming, and cross-country skiing.

What happens when a person exercises aerobically (besides becoming tired and sweaty)? Physiologically, adults of all ages show improved cardiovascular functioning and maximum oxygen consumption; lower blood pressure; and better strength, endurance, flexibility, and coordination (Mayo Clinic, 2015). Psychologically, people who exercise aerobically report lower levels of stress, better moods, and better cognitive functioning.

The best way to gain the benefits of aerobic exercise is to maintain physical fitness throughout the life span, beginning at least in middle age. The benefits of various forms of exercise are numerous, and include lowering the risk of cardiovascular disease, osteoporosis (if the exercise is weight bearing), and a host of other conditions. The Mayo Clinic’s Healthy Lifestyle Fitness websites provide an excellent place to start. In planning an exercise pro- gram, three points should be remembered. First, check with a physician before beginning an aerobic exercise program. Second, bear in mind that moderation is important. Third, just because you intend to exercise doesn’t mean you will; you must take the necessary steps to turn your intention into action (Paech, Luszczynska, & Lippke, 2016; Schwarzer, 2008). If you do, and stick with it, you may feel much younger (Joyner & Barnes, 2013).

Without question, regular exercise is one of the two most important behaviors you can do to promote healthy living and good aging (not smoking is the other). In addition to the wide variety of positive effects on health (e.g., lower risk of cardiovascular disease, diabetes, hypertension), there is also substantial evidence exercise is also connected to less cortical atrophy, better brain function, and enhanced cognitive performance (Erickson, Gildengers, & Butters, 2013; Suo et al., 2016). Specifically, exercise has a positive effect on the prefrontal and hippocampal areas of the brain, increased gray matter, and reversed the progression of white matter hypersensitivities (a biomarker of cerebrovascular disease) and is closely associated with memory and other cognitive functions.

Whether exercise can delay or prevent diseases associated with these brain structures, such as Alzheimer’s disease, remains to be seen. But the evidence to date points in that direc- tion (e.g., Suo et al., 2016), so researchers and clinicians are promoting exercise as a way to a healthy, better functioning brain in later life. A better functioning brain may well be related to the mood improvements seen as another positive benefit of exercise, as shown in ◗●Figure 13.5.

aerobic exercise exercise that places moderate stress on the heart by maintaining a pulse rate between 60% and 90% of a person’s maximum heart rate.

●◗ Figure 13.5 A schematic representation of the general path by which cognitive function and mood are improved by physical activity. It could be hypoth- esized that improvements in cognitive function mediate the improvements in mood or that improvements in mood mediate some of the improvements in cognitive function. The dotted lines represent these hypothesized paths.

SOURCE: Erickson, K. I., Gildengers, A. G., & Butters, M. A. (2013). Physical activity and brain plasticity in late adulthood. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15, 99–108. Retrieved from www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3622473/. Image retrieved from www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3622473/figure/DialoguesClinNeurosci-15-99-g001/.

Physical activity

Molecular/ cellular changes

in brain

Volumetric/ functional changes

in brain

Improved mood Cognitive

improvements

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

436 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

In summary, if you want to maximize the odds of healthy aging, exercise. Guidelines state about 150 minutes of moderate aerobic exercise weekly with additional whole-body strength training and balance work is sufficient to produce positive effects (Batt, Tanji, & Börjesson, 2013).

recall 1. Severe bone loss may result in the disease _______________.

2. The cessation of menstruation is termed _______________.

3. Reduction of fertility in men usually occurs _______________.

4. The stress and _______________ paradigm defines stress on the basis of the person’s appraisal of a situation as taxing his or her well-being.

5. Research indicates that _______________ is an effective approach for lowering stress.

Interpret ●● The media are full of advertisements for anti-aging creams,

diets, and exercise plans. Based on what you have read in this section, how would you evaluate these ads?

Apply ●● What would be an ideal stress reduction exercise program for

middle-aged adults?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 13.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How does practical intelligence develop in adulthood?

●● How does a person become an expert?

●● What is meant by lifelong learning? What differences are there between adults and young people in how they learn?

13.2 Cognitive Development

A’isha a 54-year-old social worker, is widely regarded as the resident expert when it comes to working the system of human services. Her coworkers admire her ability to get several agencies to cooperate, which they do not do normally, and to keep clients coming in for routine matters and follow-up visits. A’isha claims that there is nothing magical about it—it’s just her experience that makes the difference.

Compared with the rapid cognitive growth of childhood or the controversies about post- formal cognition in young adulthood, cognitive development in middle age is relatively quiet. For the most part, the trends in intellectual development discussed in Chapter 10 are continued and solidified. The hallmark of cognitive development in middle age involves developing higher levels of expertise like A’isha shows and being flexible in solving practical problems. We will also see how important it is to continue learning throughout adulthood.

Practical Intelligence Take a moment to think about the following problems (Denney, 1984; Denney, Pearce, & Palmer, 1982):

●● A middle-aged woman is frying chicken in her home when all of a sudden a grease fire breaks out on top of the stove. Flames begin to shoot up. What should she do?

●● A man finds that the heater in his apartment is not working. He asks his landlord to send someone out to fix it, and the landlord agrees. But after a week of cold weather and several calls to the landlord, the heater is still not fixed. What should the man do?

These practical problems are different from the examples of measures of fluid and crystallized intelligence in Chapter 10. They are more realistic; they reflect real-world situations that people routinely face. Many researchers argue that using such problems to assess cognition provides a better assessment of the kinds of skills adults actually use in

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 437

everyday life (Gamaldo & Allaire, 2016; Margrett et al., 2010; Mayer, 2014). Most people spend more time at tasks such as managing their personal finances, dealing with uncoop- erative people, and juggling busy schedules than they do solving esoteric mazes.

The shortcomings of traditional tests of adults’ intelligence led to different ways of viewing intelligence that differentiate academic (or traditional) intelligence from other skills (Diehl et al, 2005; Margrett et al., 2010). The broad range of skills related to how individuals shape, select, or adapt to their physical and social environments is termed practical intelligence. The examples at the beginning of this section illustrate how practi- cal intelligence is measured. Such real-life problems differ in three main ways from tradi- tional tests (Diehl et al., 2005): People are more motivated to solve them; personal experience is more relevant; and they have more than one correct answer. Research evi- dence shows that performance on these kinds of tests, while related to basic cognitive abilities such as fluid intelligence, are also strongly related to lived experience, age, and crystallized intelligence (Frank et al., 2016; Gamaldo & Allaire, 2016; Margrett et al., 2010).

Applications of Practical Intelligence Practical intelligence and postformal thinking (see Chapter 10) across adulthood have been linked. Specifically, the extent to which a practical problem evokes an emotional reaction, in conjunction with experience and one’s preferred mode of thinking, deter- mines whether one will use a cognitive analysis (thinking one’s way through the problem), a problem-focused action (tackling the problem head-on by doing something about it), passive-dependent behavior (withdrawing from the situation), or avoidant thinking and denial (rationalizing to redefine the problem and so minimize its seriousness).

Research indicates that adults tend to blend emotion with cognition in their approach to practical problems, whereas adolescents tend not to because they get hung up in the logic. Summarizing over a decade of her research, Blanchard-Fields (2007) notes that for late middle-aged adults, highly emotional problems (issues with high levels of feelings, such as dealing with unexpected deaths) are associated most with passive-dependent and avoidant-denial approaches. In contrast, problems concerned more with instrumental issues (issues related to daily living such as grocery shopping, getting from place to place, etc.) and home management (issues related to living in one’s household) are dealt with differently. Middle-aged adults use problem-focused strategies more frequently in dealing with instrumental problems than do adolescents or young adults. Clearly, middle-aged adults put considerable weight on the context in which the problem is set, and take situ- ational factors into account more than do younger adults. Thus, we cannot characterize problem-solving in middle age in any one way.

Mechanics and Pragmatics of Intelligence When we combine the research on practical intelligence with the research on the compo- nents or mechanics of intelligence discussed in Chapter 10, we have a more complete description of cognition in adulthood. The two-component model of life-span intelligence (Baltes et al., 2006) is grounded in the dynamic interplay among the biopsychosocial forces (see Chapter 1). The mechanics of intelligence reflects those aspects of intelligence com- prising fluid intelligence (see Chapter 10). The pragmatics of intelligence refers to those aspects of intelligence reflecting crystallized intelligence (see Chapter 10). However, as Baltes and colleagues point out, the biopsychosocial forces differentially influence the mechanics and pragmatics of intelligence. Whereas the mechanics of intelligence is more directly an expression of the neurophysiological architecture of the mind, the pragmatics of intelligence is associated more with the bodies of knowledge that are available from and mediated through one’s culture (Baltes et al., 2006).

These concepts are illustrated in the left side of ◗ Figure 13.6. The mechanics of intel- ligence in later life is more associated with the fundamental organization of the central nervous system (i.e., biological forces). Thus, it is more closely linked with a gradual loss of brain efficiency with age (Horn & Hofer, 1992), a finding supported by brain imaging studies (Sugiura, 2016).

practical intelligence Broad range of skills related to how individ- uals shape, select, or adapt to their physical and social environments.

mechanics of intelligence Those aspects of intelligence comprising fluid intelligence.

pragmatics of intelligence refers to those aspects of intelligence reflecting crystallized intelligence.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

438 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

On the other hand, the pragmatics of intelligence is more closely associated with psychological and sociocultural forces. At the psychological level, knowledge structures change as a function of the accumulated acquisition of knowledge over time. For example, the more you learn about the American Revolution, the more differentiated your knowl- edge system becomes, going, for example, from a basic knowledge that the Americans declared their independence from Great Britain and won the war to a more elaborate knowledge about the various battles, how the Americans nearly lost the war, and that many people in America sided with the British. At the sociocultural level, knowledge structures are also influenced by how we are socialized given the particular historical period in which we are raised. For example, during the Vietnam War era, the U.S. government viewed the Hanoi government as the enemy. Today, trade and other interactions occur routinely between the two, implying that on certain issues they are allies. As a result, how people view and consider dilemmas and problems that are influenced by historical context may result in very different solutions. Such differences reflect the sociocultural and historical contexts of particular points in time. Overall, these knowledge structures influence the way we implement our professional skills, solve everyday problems, and conduct the busi- ness of life (Baltes et al., 2006).

Finally, as the right portion of Figure 13.6 shows, the developmental pathways the mechanics and pragmatics of intelligence take across the course of adult life differ. Given that biological and genetic forces govern the mechanics more, there is a downward trajec- tory with age beginning by middle age. However, given that the pragmatics of intelligence is governed more by environmental and cultural factors, there is an upward trajectory that is maintained across the adult life span.

Becoming an Expert One day John Cavanaugh was driving along when his car suddenly began coughing and sputtering. As deftly as possible, he pulled over to the side of the road, turned off the engine, opened the hood, and proceeded to look inside. It was hopeless; to him, it looked like a jumble of unknown parts. After the car was towed to a garage, a middle-aged mechanic set about fixing it. Within a few minutes, the car was running like new. How?

We saw earlier in this chapter aspects of intelligence grounded in experience (crystal- lized intelligence) tend to improve throughout most of adulthood. In a real-world experi- ential perspective, each of us becomes an expert at something important to us, such as our work, interpersonal relationships, cooking, sports, or auto repair. In this sense, an expert

●◗ Figure 13.6 Conceptualization of the mechanics and pragmatics of intelligence across the life span. As described later, the mechanics of intelligence correspond to fluid intelligence and the pragmatics to crystallized intelligence.

ca. 25

Pragmatics (crystallized)

Mechanics (fluid)

Intelligence as cultural knowledge

Basic information processing Content-poor Universal, biological Genetically predisposed

Content-rich Culture-dependent Experience-based

Intelligence as basic information processing

ca. 70 Life course

Mechanics

Pragmatics

Pe rf

o rm

an ce

Based on Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. The Gerontologist, 33, 580–594.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 439

is someone who is much better at a task than people who have not put much effort into it. We tend to become selective experts in some areas while remaining amateurs or novices at others.

What makes experts better than novices? It’s how experts handle the problem (Erics- son & Towne, 2010; Hambrick et al., 2016). For novices, the goal for accomplishing the activity is to reach as rapidly as possible a satisfactory performance level that is stable and “autonomous.” In contrast, experts build up a wealth of knowledge about alternative ways of solving problems or making decisions. These well-developed knowledge structures are the major difference between experts and novices, and they enable experts to bypass steps needed by novices (Chi, 2006).

Experts don’t always follow the rules as novices do; they are more flexible, creative, and curious; and they have superior strategies grounded on superior knowledge for accomplishing a task (Ericsson & Towne, 2010). Even though experts may be slower in terms of raw speed because they spend more time planning, their ability to skip steps puts them at a decided advantage. In a way, this represents “the triumph of knowledge over reasoning” (Charness & Bosman, 1990).

What happens to expertise over the adult life span? Research evidence indicates expert performance tends to peak by middle age and drops off slightly after that (Masu- naga & Horn, 2001). However, the declines in expert performance are not nearly as great as they are for the abilities of information processing, memory, and fluid intelligence that underlie expertise, and expertise may sometimes compensate for declines in underlying cognitive abilities (Masunaga & Horn, 2001; Taylor et al., 2005).

Such compensation is seen in expert judgments about such things as how long cer- tain figure skating maneuvers will take. Older people who were experts were as good as younger adults who were still skating at predicting the amount of time skating moves take (Diersch et al., 2012). Thus, it appears knowledge based on experience is an important component of expertise. Indeed, researchers argue that people become “experts by expe- rience” (Tolkko, 2016). But how do people keep acquiring knowledge? That’s achieved through lifelong learning.

Lifelong Learning Many people work in occupations where information and technology change rapidly. To keep up with these changes, many organizations and professions now emphasize the importance of learning how to learn, rather than learning specific content that may become outdated in a couple of years. For most people, a college education will prob- ably not be the last educational experience they have in their careers. Workers in many professions— such as medicine, nursing, social work, psychology, auto mechanics, and teaching— are now required to obtain continuing education credits to stay current in their fields. Online learning has made lifelong learning more accessible to professionals and interested adults alike (Council for Adult and Experiential Learning, 2016).

Lifelong learning is gaining acceptance as the best way to approach the need for keep- ing active cognitively and is viewed as a critical part of aging globally (Formosa, 2014; Swindell, 2012), but should lifelong learning be approached as merely an extension of ear- lier educational experiences? Knowles, Holton, and Swanson (2015) argue teaching aimed at children and youth differs from teaching aimed at adults. Adult learners differ from their younger counterparts in several ways:

●● Adults have a higher need to know why they should learn something before undertaking it.

●● Adults enter a learning situation with more and different experience on which to build.

●● Adults are most willing to learn those things they believe are necessary to deal with real-world problems rather than abstract, hypothetical situations.

●● Most adults are more motivated to learn by internal factors (such as self-esteem or personal satisfaction) than by external factors (such as a job promotion or pay raise).

Think About It Can expertise be taught? Why or why not?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

440 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

On ResearchSpotlight

Designing Software for Middle- Aged and Older Nonexperts

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Ali Dare- jeh and Dalbir Singh (2014) observed that software companies such as Microsoft periodically make significant changes to their core products. Such changes are often well-founded and greatly enhance the func- tionality of the program or app. However, the changes may be difficult for some people to adapt to, thereby impairing their ability to use the program or app effectively and efficiently.

Darejeh and Singh were especially inter- ested in how well middle-aged adults could learn how to use the Ribbon interface in Microsoft (MS) Office Outlook, as the intro- duction of the Ribbon was a major design change.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Darejeh and Singh observed participants’ actions while they were using MS Outlook. Participants also completed several evaluation forms, and they were interviewed.

Who were the participants in the study? The participants were four men and six women staff or students of Apple English Institute in Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia, between the ages of 52 and 66. None of the partici- pants had significant background knowledge about computers, worked on computers daily, were very familiar with programming languages, or were proficient with Microsoft Outlook.

What was the design of the study? The design was a longitudinal design that lasted 62 days during which tasks to be completed on MS Office were assigned (e.g., create a new e-mail and attach a file to it; create a new meeting). Sessions lasted 45 minutes each.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? There were no ethical concerns as participants were appropriately informed about the study.

What were the results? Darejeh and Singh found that only two of the nine tasks could be completed by more than half the participants using MS Outlook as designed by Microsoft. However, based on inter- views and other assessments, Darejeh and

Singh redesigned the MS Outlook interface to include such changes as using more meaningful icons, showing only those tools that were useful for the tasks at hand, and providing more complete descriptions of how tools are used. When this redesigned interface was provided, all but one of the tasks was completed by at least 80% of the participants.

What did the investigators conclude? Darejeh and Singh concluded that users who are not experts learn better when provided more appropriately designed interfaces with embedded help. The redesigned interface greatly improved self-learning as measured by a decrease in the time and in the number of steps needed as well as much improved performance. In sum, upgrades to soft- ware need to take learner differences into account.

What converging evidence would strengthen their conclusions? Future research should include people at different levels of expertise with the target software or app, and participants should be of a wider age range.

recall 1. The skills and knowledge necessary for people to function in

everyday life make up _______________.

2. The two components of a life-span model of intelligence are _______________ and _______________.

3. The triumph of knowledge over reasoning is one way to describe _______________.

4. Due to rapidly changing technology and information, many educators now support the concept of _______________.

Interpret Based on the cognitive-developmental changes described in this sec- tion, what types of jobs would be done best by middle-aged adults?

Apply ●● If you were asked to design a cognitive training program for

middle-aged adults, what strategies would you include?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 13.2

Lifelong learning is becoming increasingly important, but educators need to keep in mind learning styles change as people age. Effective lifelong learning requires smart deci- sions about how to keep knowledge updated and what approach works best among the many different learning options available (Knowles et al., 2015).

As described in the Spotlight on Research feature, software companies would do well to take into account differences in expertise and in learning when designing updated ver- sions of familiar programs and apps.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 441

Abby was attending her 25th high school reunion. She hadn’t seen her friend Michelle since they graduated. Abby remembered that in high school Michelle was always surrounded by a group of people. She always walked up to people and initiated conversations, was at ease with strangers, pleasant, and often described as the “life of the party.” Abby wondered if Michelle would be the same outgoing person she was in high school.

Many of us eventually attend a high school reunion. It is amusing, so it is said, to see how our classmates changed over the years. In addition to noticing gray or missing hair and a few wrinkles, we should pay attention to personality characteristics. The questions that surfaced for Abby are similar to the ones we generate ourselves. For example, will Katy be the same outgoing person she was as captain of the cheerleaders? Will Ted still be as con- cerned about social issues at 43 as he was at 18?

To learn as much about our friends as possible we could make careful observations of our classmates’ personalities over the course of several reunions. Then, at the gathering marking 25 years since graduation, we could examine the trends we observed. Did our classmates’ personalities change substantially or did they remain essentially the same as they were 60 years earlier?

How we think these questions will be answered provides clues to our personal biases concerning personality stability or change across adulthood. As we will see, biases about continuity and discontinuity are more obvious in personality research than in any other area of adult development.

The Five-Factor Trait Model In the past few decades, one of the most important advances in research on adult development and aging has been the emergence of a personality theory aimed spe- cifically at describing adults. Due mostly to the efforts of Robert McCrae and Paul Costa, Jr. (Costa & McCrae, 2011; McCrae, 2016; McCrae & Costa, 2003), we are now able to describe adults’ personality traits using five dimensions: neuroticism, extraver- sion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. These dimensions (the five-factor model or the so-called Big Five traits) are strongly grounded in cross- sectional, longitudinal, and sequential research. First, though, let’s take a closer look at each dimension.

●● People who are high on the neuroticism dimension tend to be anxious, hostile, self-con- scious, depressed, impulsive, and vulnerable. They may show violent or negative emo- tions that interfere with their ability to get along with others or to handle problems in everyday life. People who are low on this dimension tend to be calm, even-tempered, self-content, comfortable, unemotional, and hardy.

●● Individuals who are high on the extraversion dimension thrive on social interaction, like to talk, take charge easily, readily express their opinions and feelings, like to keep busy, have boundless energy, and prefer stimulating and challenging environments. Such people tend to enjoy people-oriented jobs such as social work and sales, and they often have humanitarian goals. People who are low on this dimension tend to be reserved, quiet, passive, serious, and emotionally unreactive.

neuroticism personality trait dimension associated with the tendency to be anxious, hostile, self-conscious, depressed, impulsive, and vulnerable.

extraversion personality trait dimension associated with the tendency to thrive on social interac- tion, to like to talk, to take charge easily, to readily express their opinions and feelings, to keep busy, to have boundless energy, and to prefer stimulating and challenging environments.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is the five-factor model? What evidence is there for

stability in personality traits? ●● What changes occur in people’s priorities and personal

concerns? How does a person achieve generativity? How is midlife best described?

13.3 Personality

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

442 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

●● Being high on the openness to experience dimension tends to have a vivid imagina- tion and dream life, an appreciation of art, and a strong desire to try anything once. These individuals tend to be naturally curious about things and to make decisions based on situational factors rather than absolute rules. People who are readily open to new experiences place a relatively low emphasis on personal economic gain. They tend to choose jobs such as the ministry or counseling, which offer diversity of experience rather than high pay. People who are low on this dimension tend to be down-to-earth, uncreative, conventional, uncurious, and conservative.

●● Scoring high on the agreeableness dimension is associated with being accepting, will- ing to work with others, and caring. People who score low on this dimension (i.e., dem- onstrate high levels of antagonism) tend to be ruthless, suspicious, stingy, antagonistic, critical, and irritable.

●● People who show high levels of conscientiousness tend to be hard-working, ambitious, energetic, scrupulous, and persevering. Such people have a strong desire to make some- thing of themselves. People at the opposite end of this scale tend to be negligent, lazy, disorganized, late, aimless, and nonpersistent.

The five-factor model has been examined cross-culturally (Church, 2016; McCrae, 2016). Research evidence generally shows that the same five factors appear across at least 50 cultures, including rarely studied Arabic and Black African groups (McCrae & Terracciano, 2005). Heine and Buchtel (2009) point out, though, that much of this research has been conducted by Westerners; so it remains to be seen whether similar studies conducted by local researchers will have the same outcomes. Additionally, Church (2016) points out that it is difficult to validate the five-factor model in preliterate cultures.

What Happens to Traits Across Adulthood? Costa and McCrae have investigated whether the general traits that make up their model remain stable across adulthood (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 2011; McCrae, 2016; McCrae & Costa, 2003). They suggest personality traits stop changing by age 30, after which they appear to be “set in plaster” (Costa & McCrae, 1994, p. 21). The data from the Costa, McCrae, and colleagues’ studies came from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging for the 114 men who took the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey (GZTS) on three occasions, with each of the two follow-up tests about six years apart.

What Costa and colleagues found was surprising. Even over a 12-year period, the 10 traits measured by the GZTS remained highly stable; the correlations ranged from .68 to .85. In much of personality research we might expect to find this degree of stability over a week or two, but to see it over 12 years is noteworthy.

We would normally be skeptical of such consistency over a long period. But similar find- ings were obtained in other studies. In a longitudinal study of 60-, 80-, and 100-year-olds, Martin, Long, and Poon (2002) found stability higher for those in their 70s and 80s than for centenarians. However, some interesting changes did occur in the very old. There was an

increase in suspiciousness and sensitivity that could be explained by increased wariness of victimization in older adulthood.

Stability was also observed in longitudinal data conducted over various lengths of time, from a 7-year period (Mõttus, Johnson, & Deary, 2012; Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000) to as long as a 30-year span (Leon et al., 1979). According to this evidence, it appears indi- viduals change little in self-reported personality traits over periods of up to 30 years long and over the age range of 20 to 90 years of age.

However, there is evidence both stability and change can be detected in personality trait development across the adult life span (Allemand, Zimprich, & Hendriks, 2008; Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Debast et al., 2014; Mõttus et al., 2012; Schultz & Schultz, 2017). These findings came about because of advances in statisti- cal techniques in teasing apart longitudinal and cross- sectional data (see Chapter 1). Researchers find the way people differ in

openness to experience personality dimension that reflects a ten- dency to have a vivid imagination and dream life, an appreciation of art, and a strong desire to try anything once.

agreeableness dimension of personality associated with being accepting, willing to work with oth- ers, and caring.

conscientiousness dimension of personality in which people tend to be hard-working, ambitious, ener- getic, scrupulous, and persevering.

Most occupations require the acquisition of new information over time through lifelong learning so that workers can do their jobs well and stay up-to-date on the latest information.

Ph ot

og ra

ph ee

.e u/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 443

their personality becomes more pronounced with older age. For example, researchers find extraversion and openness decrease with age whereas agreeableness increases with age. Conscientious appears to peak in middle age. Most interestingly, neuroticism often disappears or is much less apparent in late life. Such changes are found in studies that examine larger populations across a larger age range (e.g., 16 to mid-80s) and greater geographical regions (e.g., United States and Great Britain).

Ursula Staudinger and colleagues have a perspective that reconciles both stability and change in personality traits (Mühlig-Versen, Bowen, & Staudinger, 2012; Staudinger, 2015). They suggest personality takes on two forms: adjustment and growth. Personality adjustment involves developmental changes in terms of their adaptive value and functionality, such as functioning effectively within society, and how personality contributes to everyday life running smoothly. Personality growth refers to ideal end states such as increased self-transcendence, wisdom, and integrity. Examples of this will be discussed later and includes Erikson’s theory.

Both of these personality dimensions interact because growth cannot occur without adjustment. However, Staudinger (2015) argues while growth in terms of ideal end states does not necessarily occur in everyone, since it is less easily acquired, strategies for adjust- ment develops across the latter half of the life span. This framework can be used to inter- pret stability and change in the Big Five personality factors.

First, the consensus regarding change in the Big Five with increasing age is the absence of neuroticism and the presence of agreeableness and conscientiousness. These three traits are associated with personality adjustment, especially in terms of becoming emotionally less volatile and more attuned to social demands and social roles (Mühlig-Versen et al., 2012; Staudinger, 2015). These characteristics allow older adults to maintain and regain lev- els of well-being in the face of loss, threats, and challenges; common occurrences in late life.

Studies also show a decrease in openness to new experiences with increasing age (e.g., Graham & Lachman, 2012; Helson et al., 2002; Roberts et al., 2006; Srivastava et al., 2003). Staudinger argues openness to experience is related to personal maturity because it is highly correlated with ego development, wisdom, and emotional complexity. Evidence suggests these three aspects of personality (ego level, wisdom, and emotional complexity) do not increase with age and may show decline (Grühn et al., 2013; Mühlig-Versen et al., 2012; Staudinger, Dörner, & Mickler, 2005; Staudinger, 2015). Staudinger concludes per- sonal growth in adulthood appears to be rare rather than normative.

To summarize, there appears to be increases in adjustment aspects of personality with increasing age, and it could be normative. At the same time, however, the basic indicators of personality growth tend to show stability or decline. You might ask, what’s going on?

The most likely answer is personality growth or change across adulthood does not normally occur unless there are special circumstances and with an environmental push for it to occur. Thus, the personality-related adjustment that grows in adulthood does so in response to ever-changing developmental challenges and tasks, such as establishing a career, marriage, and family.

Based upon a detailed analysis of individual patterns of personality stability and change, Wrzus and Roberts (2017) propose a model that accounts for both developmen- tal patterns. The triggering situations, expectancy, states/state expressions, and Reac- tions (TESSERA) model, shown in ◗ Figure 13.7, describes a process by which long-term personality development is the product of repeated short-term, situational processes. These short-term processes repeat and create a feedback loop. These processes in turn can result in changes in personality characteristics and behavior over time, showing up eventually as changes in personality. That these short-term processes differ across peo- ple is why some people show changes in personality traits and others do not. It can also explain why the life narratives of people also differ, a topic we will explore next.

Changing Priorities in Midlife Joyce, a 52-year-old preschool teacher, thought carefully about what she believes is impor- tant in life. “I definitely feel differently about what I want to accomplish. When I was younger, I wanted to advance and be a great teacher. Now, although I still want to be good,

personality adjustment developmental changes in terms of their adaptive value and functionality such as whether one can function effectively within society and how personality contributes to everyday life running smoothly.

personality growth form of personality that refers to ideal end states such as increased self-transcendence, wisdom, and integrity.

Think About It Does evidence of stability in traits sup- port the idea that some aspects of per- sonality are genetic? Why or why not?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

444 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

I’m more concerned with providing help to the new teachers around here. I’ve got a lot of on-the-job experience that I can pass along.”

Joyce is not alone. Despite the evidence that personality traits remain stable during adulthood, many middle-aged people report that their personal priorities change during middle age. In general, they report that they are increasingly concerned with helping younger people achieve rather than with getting ahead themselves. In his psychosocial the- ory, Erikson argued that this shift in priorities reflects generativity, or being productive by helping others to ensure the continuation of society by guiding the next generation.

Achieving generativity can be enriching. It is grounded in the successful resolution of the previous six phases of Erikson’s theory (see Chapter 1). There are numerous avenues for generativity (Kotre, 2005; McAdams, 2001, 2015; McAdams & Guo, 2015), such as parenting, mentoring, and creating one’s legacy by doing something of lasting importance. Sources of generativity do not vary across ethnic groups (Bates, 2009).

Some adults do not achieve generativity. Instead, they become bored, self-indulgent, and unable to contribute to the continuation of society. Erikson referred to this state as stagnation, in which people are unable to deal with the needs of their children or to provide mentoring to younger adults.

What Are Generative People Like? Research shows that generativity is different from traits; for example, generativity is more related to societal engagement than are traits (Wilt, Cox, & McAdams, 2010). One of the best approaches to generativity is McAdams’s model (McAdams, 2015), shown in ◗ Figure 13.8.

This multidimensional model shows how generativity results from the complex inter- connections among societal and inner forces. The tension between creating a product or an outcome that outlives oneself and selflessly bestowing one’s efforts as a gift to the next generation (reflecting a concern for what is good for society) results in a concern for the

generativity in erikson’s theory, being productive by helping others to ensure the continuation of society by guiding the next generation.

stagnation in erikson’s theory, the state in which people are unable to deal with the needs of their children or to provide mentoring to younger adults.

●◗ Figure 13.7 TESSERA framework of adult personality development

Long-term Personality Development

Short-term TESSERA Sequences

Environment PersonalityIndividual • Personality: dispositions,

goals, abilities... • Mental and physical health

• Proximate context • Cultural context

Moderators: positive–negative valence external–internal locus deliberate–automatic

T E SSE RA T E SSE RA+ +

R e �

e c ti

v e p

ro c e s s e s

e .g

., s

e lf-

re fle

c tio

n ,

a c c o m

o d

a tio

n , a ss

im ila

tio n ,

lif e r

e fle

c tio

n , se

lf- n a rr

a tio

n

A s s o

c ia

ti v e p

ro c e s s e s

e .g

., im

p lic

it le

a rn

in g

, h a b

it fo

rm a tio

n ,

re in

fo rc

e m

e n t le

a rn

in g

S it

u a ti

o n

s e le

c ti

o n

S ta

te e

li c it

a ti

o n

Explicit Implicit

State/state expression

Triggering situation

Expectancy Reaction

Time

SOURCE: Wrzus, C., & Roberts, B. W. (2017). Processes of personality development in adulthood: The TESSERA framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, doi:10.1177/1088868316652279

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 445

next generation and a belief in the goodness of the human enterprise. The positive resolu- tion of this conflict finds middle-aged adults developing a generative commitment, which produces generative actions. A person derives personal meaning from being generative by constructing a life story or narrative, which helps create the person’s identity.

The components of McAdams’s model relate differently to personality traits. For exam- ple, generative concern is a general personality tendency of interest in caring for younger individuals, and generative action is the actual behaviors that promote the well-being of the next generation. Generative concern relates to life satisfaction and overall happiness, whereas generative action does not. For example, new grandparents may derive much sat- isfaction from their grandchildren and are greatly concerned with their well-being, but have little desire to engage in the daily hassles of caring for them on a regular basis.

Although they can be expressed by adults of all ages, certain types of generativity are more common at some ages than others. For example, middle-aged and older adults show a greater preoccupation with generativity themes than do younger adults in their accounts of personally meaningful life experiences (McAdams, 2015; McAdams & Guo, 2015). Middle-aged adults make more generative commitments (e.g., “save enough money for my daughter to go to medical school”), reflecting a major difference in the inner and outer worlds of middle-aged and older adults as opposed to younger adults. An important aspect of generativity is leaving a personal legacy (Newton & Jones, 2016).

Similar research focusing specifically on middle-aged women yields comparable results. Hills (2013) argues that leaving a legacy, a major example of generativity in prac- tice, is a core concern in midlife, more so than at any other age. Schoklitsch & Bauman (2012) point out that the capacity of generativity peaks during midlife but that people con- tinue to accomplish generative tasks into late life (e.g., great-grandparenthood).

How well do these ideas generalize across ethnic groups and cultures? A study of sec- ond-generation Chinese American women found similar trends in generativity with Euro- pean American women (Grant, 2007). In one of the few studies to examine generativity across cultures, Hofer et al. (2008) examined it in Cameroon, Costa Rica, and Germany. They found that McAdams’s model could be successfully applied across the three cultures.

These data demonstrate that the personal concerns of middle-aged adults are fundamen- tally different from those of younger adults. In fact, generativity may be a stronger predictor of emotional and physical well-being in midlife and old age (Gruenewald, Liao, & Seeman, 2012; McAdams, 2015; McAdams & Guo, 2015; Wilt et al., 2010). For example, among both women and men, generativity is associated with positive emotion and satisfaction with life

narrative way in which a person derives personal meaning from being generative by con- structing a life story, which helps create the person’s identity.

●◗ Figure 13.8 McAdams’s model of generativity. Note that how one shows generativ- ity (action) is influenced by several factors.

Motivational sources

Thoughts, plans Behavior Meaning

Cultural demand

Concern (for the next generation)

Commitment

Action • creating • maintaining • offering

Inner desire • symbolic immortality • “need to be needed”

Belief “in the species”

N a r r a t i o n

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

446 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

and work, and it predicts physical health. Considered together, these findings provide con- siderable support for Erikson’s contention that the central concerns for adults change with age. However, the data also indicate that generativity is more complex than Erikson originally proposed and, while peaking in middle age, may not diminish in late life.

Life Transition in Midlife We have seen that theorists such as Erikson believe that adults face several important challenges and that by struggling with these issues, people develop new aspects of them- selves. Erikson’s notion that people experience fundamental changes in their priorities and personal concerns was grounded in the possibility that middle adulthood includes other important changes. Carl Jung, one of the founders of psychoanalytic theory, believed that adults may experience a midlife crisis. This belief led to the development of several theo- ries suggesting that adulthood consists of alternating periods of stability and transition that people experience in a fixed sequence.

Despite its appeal, though, there is no such thing as a universal midlife crisis. Instead, Labouvie-Vief and colleagues (e.g., Grühn et al., 2013; Labouvie-Vief, Grühn, & Mouras, 2009) offer some good evidence for a reorganization of self and values across the adult life span. They suggest that the major dynamic driving such changes may not be age-dependent but may fol- low general cognitive changes. Middle-aged adults show the most complex understanding of self, emotions, and motivations. Cognitive complexity also is shown to be the strongest pre- dictor of higher levels of complexity in general. From this approach, a midlife “crisis” may be the result of general gains in cognitive complexity from early to middle adulthood.

This increase in cognitive complexity may help people make what could be called midlife corrections. Abigail Stewart (Newton & Stewart, 2012; Torges, Stewart, & Duncan, 2008) found that well-educated women who reported regrets for adopting a traditional feminine role in life (i.e., they wished they had pursued an education or a career) and sub- sequently made adjustments in midlife were better off than those who did not make adjustments or had no role regrets at all. Stewart suggests that rather than a midlife crisis, such an adjustment may be more appropriately considered a midlife correction, reevaluat- ing one’s roles and dreams and making the necessary corrections.

Perhaps the best way to view midlife is as a time of both gains and losses (Lachman, 2004). That is, the changes people perceive in midlife can be viewed as representing both gains and losses. Competence, ability to handle stress, sense of personal control, purpose in life, and social responsibility are all at their peak, whereas physical abilities, women’s ability to bear children, and physical appearance are examples of changes that many view as negative. This gain–loss view emphasizes two things. First, the exact timing of change is not fixed but occurs over an extended period of time. Second, change can be both positive and negative at the same time. Thus, rather than seeing midlife as a time of crisis, one may want to view it as a period during which several aspects of one’s life acquire new meanings.

midlife correction reevaluating one’s roles and dreams and making the necessary correction.

recall 1. The dimensions in the five-factor theory of personality

include neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and _______________.

2. According to Erikson, an increasing concern with helping younger people achieve is termed _______________.

3. According to McAdams, the meaning one derives from being generative happens through the process of _______________.

4. Reevaluating one’s roles and dreams and making the neces- sary corrections in middle age is called _______________.

Interpret ●● How can you reconcile the data from trait research, which indi-

cates little change, with the data from other research, which shows substantial change in personality during adulthood?

Apply ●● If psychotherapy assumes that a person can change behavior

over time, what is the relation between personality and behav- ior from this perspective?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 13.3

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 447

Lily is facing a major milestone: Her youngest child, Megan, is about to head off to college. But instead of feeling depressed, as she thought she would, Lily feels almost elated at the prospect. She and Huang are finally free of the day-to-day parenting duties of the past 30 years. Lily is looking forward to getting to know her husband again. She wonders whether there is something wrong with her for being excited that her daughter is leaving.

People such as Lily connect generations. Family ties across the generations provide the context for socialization and for continuity in the family’s identity. At the center agewise are members of the middle-aged generation, such as Lily, who serve as the links between their aging parents and their own maturing children (Deane et al., 2016; Fingerman et al., 2012; Hareven, 2001). Middle-aged mothers (more than fathers) tend to take on this role of kinkeeper, the person who gathers family members together for celebrations and keeps them in touch with each other.

Think about the major issues confronting a typical middle-aged couple: maintaining a good marriage, parenting responsibilities, dealing with children who are becoming adults themselves, handling job pressures, and worrying about aging parents, just to name a few. Middle-aged adults truly have a lot to deal with every day in balancing their responsibili- ties to their children and their aging parents (Boyczuk & Fletcher, 2016). Indeed, middle- aged adults are sometimes referred to as the sandwich generation because they are caught between the competing demands of two gen- erations: their parents and their children.

Letting Go: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Children Sometime during middle age, most parents experience two posi- tive developments with regard to their children (Buhl, 2008). Suddenly their children see them in a new light, and the children leave home.

The extent parents support and approve of their children’s attempts at being independent matters. Most parents manage the transition successfully (Owen, 2005). That’s not to say par- ents are heartless. As depicted in the cartoon, when children leave home, emotional bonds are disrupted. Mothers in all eth- nic groups report feeling sad at the time children leave, but have more positive feelings about the potential for growth in their relationships with their children (Feldman, 2010).Still, parents provide considerable emotional support (by staying in touch) and financial help (such as paying college tuition, providing a free place to live until the child finds employment) when possible (Farris, 2016).

A positive experience with launching children is strongly influenced by the extent the parents perceive a job well done and their children have turned out well (Farris, 2016; Mitchell, 2010).

kinkeeper person who gathers family members together for celebrations and keeps them in touch with each other, usually a middle- aged mother.

sandwich generation Middle-aged adults who are caught between the competing demands of two generations: their parents and their children.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Who are the kinkeepers in families?

●● How does the relationship between middle-aged parents and their young adult children change?

●● How do middle-aged adults deal with their aging parents?

●● How do grandchildren and grandparents interact? How is grandparenthood meaningful? What issues matter for grandparents caring for grandchildren?

13.4 Family Dynamics and Middle Age

Adult children’s relationships with their parents often include a friendship dimension.

Jo hn

L un

d/ Sa

m D

ie ph

ui s/

Bl en

d Im

ag es

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

448 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Children are regarded as successes when they meet parents’ culturally based developmen- tal expectations, and they are seen as “good kids” when there is agreement between par- ents and children in basic values.

Parents’ satisfaction with the empty nest is sometimes short-lived. Roughly half of young adults in the United States return to their parents’ home at least once after mov- ing out (Farris, 2016). There is evidence these young adults, called “boomerang kids” (Farris, 2016; Mitchell, 2006), reflect a less permanent, more mobile contemporary society.

Why do children move back? A major impetus is the increased costs of living on their own when saddled with college debt, especially if the societal economic situation is bad and jobs are not available. This was especially true during the Great Recession of the late 2000s and early 2010s. It also reflects the different kind of relationship mil- lennial adults have with their parents and a different attitude about living back at home (Farris, 2016).

Giving Back: Middle-Aged Adults and Their Aging Parents Most middle-aged adults have parents who are in reasonably good health. But for nearly a quarter of adults like Joan in the Real People feature, being a child of aging par- ents involves providing some level of care (Hyer, Mullen & Jackson, 2017). How adult children become care providers varies a great deal from person to person, but the job of caring for older parents usually falls to a daughter or a daughter-in-law (Barnett, 2013), and daughters also tend to coordinate care provided by multiple siblings (Friedman & Seltzer, 2010). In Japan, even though the oldest son is responsible for parental care, it is his wife who actually does the day-to-day caregiving for her own parents and her in- laws (Lee, 2010).

Most adult children feel a sense of responsibility, termed filial obligation, to care for their parents if necessary. Adult child care providers sometimes express the feeling they “owe it to Mom or Dad” to care for them; after all, their parents provided for them for many years, and now the shoe is on the other foot (Gans, 2007). Adult children often pro- vide the majority of care when needed to their parents in all Western and non-Western cultures studied, but the same is true especially in Asian cultures (Barnett, 2013; Haley, 2013; Lai, 2010).

filial obligation sense of obligation to care for one’s parents if necessary.

Everything seemed to be going well for Joan. Her career was taking off, her youngest daughter Kelly had just entered high school, and her marriage to Bill was better than ever. So when her phone rang one June afternoon, she was taken by surprise.

The voice on the other end was matter- of-fact. Joan’s mother had suffered a major stroke and would need someone to care for her. Because her mother did not have suf- ficient medical and long-term care insurance to afford a nursing home, Joan made the only decision she could— her mom would move

in with her, Bill, and Kelly. Joan firmly believed that because her mom had provided for her, Joan owed it to her mom to do the same now that she was in need.

What Joan didn’t count on was that tak- ing care of her mom was the most difficult yet most rewarding thing she had ever done. Joan quickly realized that her days of lengthy busi- ness trips and seminars were over, as was her quick rise up the company leadership ladder. Other employees now brought back the great new ideas and could respond to out-of-town crises quickly. As hard as it was, Joan knew that

her career trajectory had taken a different turn. And she and Bill had more disagreements than she could ever remember, usually about the decreased amount of time they had to spend with each other. Kelly’s demands to be driven here and there also added to Joan’s stress.

But Joan and her mom were able to develop the kind of relationship they could not have otherwise and to talk about issues they had long suppressed. Although caring for a physically disabled mother was extremely tax- ing, Joan and her mother’s ability to connect on a different level made it worthwhile.

Real People Applying Human Development

Taking Care of Mom

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 449

Roughly 50 million Americans provide unpaid care for older parents, in-laws, grand- parents, and other older loved ones (National Alliance for Caregiving & AARP, 2015). The typical care provider is a middle-aged woman who is employed outside the home, has an average household income of under $46,000, and provides more than 20 hours per week of unpaid care. They expect to care for their loved one for about 10 years.

Stresses and Rewards of Providing Care Providing care is a major source of both stress and reward. On the stress side, adult chil- dren and other family caregivers are especially vulnerable from two main sources (Pearlin et al., 1990):

●● Adult children may have trouble coping with declines in their parents’ functioning, especially those involving cognitive abilities and problematic behavior, and with work overload, burnout, and loss of the previous relationship with a parent.

●● If the care situation is perceived as confining or seriously infringes on the adult child’s other responsibilities (spouse, parent, employee, etc.), then the situation is likely to be perceived negatively, and that may lead to family or job conflicts, economic problems, loss of self-identity, and decreased competence.

When caring for an aging parent, even the most devoted adult child caregiver will at times feel depressed, resentful, angry, or guilty (Cavanaugh, 1999; Haley, 2013; Hyer et al., 2017). Many middle-aged care providers are hard pressed financially: They may still be paying child care or college tuition expenses, perhaps trying to save adequately for their own retirement, and having to work more than one job to do it. Financial pres- sures are especially serious for those caring for parents with chronic conditions, such as Alzheimer’s disease, that require services, such as adult day care, not adequately cov- ered by medical insurance even if the older parent has supplemental coverage. In some cases, adult children may need to quit their jobs to provide care if adequate alterna- tives, such as adult day care, are unavailable or unaffordable, usually creating even more financial stress.

The stresses of caring for a parent mean the caregiver needs to carefully monitor his or her own health. Indeed, many professionals point out caring for the care provider is an important consideration to avoid care provider burnout (Ghosh, Capistrant, & Friede- mann-Sánchez, 2017; Tamayo et al., 2010).

On the plus side, caring for an aging parent also has rewards. Caring for aging parents can bring parents and their adult children closer together and provide a way for adult children to feel they are giving back to their parents (Miller et al., 2008). Cross-cultural research examining Taiwanese (Lee, 2007) and Chinese (Zhan, 2006) participants confirms adults caring for aging parents can find the experience rewarding.

Cultural values enter into the care providing relationship in an indi- rect way (Mendez-Luck et al., 2016). Care providers in all cultures stud- ied to date show a common set of outcomes: Care providers’ stressors are appraised as burdensome, that creates negative health consequences for the care provider. However, cultural values influence the kinds of social support available to the care provider.

Things aren’t always rosy from the parents’ perspective, either. Inde- pendence and autonomy are important traditional values in some ethnic groups, and their loss is not taken lightly. Older adults in these groups are more likely to express the desire to pay a professional for assistance rather than ask a family member for help; they may find it demeaning to live with their children and express strong feelings about “not wanting to burden them” (Cahill et al., 2009). Most move in only as a last resort. Many adults who receive help with daily activities feel negatively about the situation, although cultural norms supporting the acceptance of help, such as in Jap- anese culture, significantly lessen those feelings (Park et al., 2013).

Caring for an older parent creates both stresses and rewards.

Je d

Sh ar

e/ Ta

xi /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

450 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Determining whether older parents are satisfied with the help their children provide is a complex issue (Cahill et al., 2009; Park et al., 2013). Based on a critical review of the research, Newsom (1999) proposes a model of how certain aspects of care can produce negative perceptions of care directly or by affecting the interactions between care provider and care recipient (see ◗ Figure 13.9). The important thing to conclude from the model is even under the best circumstances, there is no guarantee the help adult children provide their parents will be well received. Misunderstandings can occur, and the frustration care- givers feel may be translated directly into negative interactions.

Grandparenthood Becoming a grandparent takes some help. Being a parent yourself, of course, is a prerequi- site. But it is your children’s decisions and actions that determine whether you experience the transition to grandparenthood, making this role different from most others we experi- ence throughout life.

Most people become grandparents in their 40s and 50s, though some are older, or perhaps as young as their late 20s or early 30s. For many middle-aged adults, becoming a grandparent is a peak experience (Gonyea, 2013; Hoffman, Kaneshiro, & Compton, 2012). Although most research on grandparenting has been conducted with respect to hetero- sexual grandparents, attention to lesbian, gay, and transgender grandparents is increasing as these family forms increase in society (Allen & Roberto, 2016; Orel & Fruhauf, 2013).

How Do Grandparents Interact with Grandchildren? Grandparents have many different ways of interacting with their grandchildren. Catego- rizing these styles has been attempted over many decades (e.g., Neugarten & Weinstein, 1964), but none of these attempts has been particularly successful because grandparents use different styles with different grandchildren and styles change as grandparents and grandchildren age (Gonyea, 2013; Hoffman et al., 2012).

●◗ Figure 13.9 Whether a care recipient perceives care to be good depends on inter- actions with the care provider and whether those interactions are per- ceived negatively.

Relationship variables (e.g., relationship conflict,

communication, reciprocity)

Caregiver variables (e.g., anger, depression,

burden, social skills)

Interactions with caregiver (e.g., help, support, conflict)

Negative reactions

Psychological well-being

Care recipient variables (e.g., self-esteem, control, need for independence, impairment, pessimism)

Situational variables (e.g., mood, pain,

life events, conflict)

Helping characteristics (e.g., inappropriate amount, quality of help, criticisms)

SOURCE: Newsom, J. T. (1999). Another side to caregiving: Negative reactions to being helped. Current Direc- tions in Psychological Science, 8, 185.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Think About It Why does parental caregiving fall mainly to women?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 451

An alternative approach involves considering the many functions grandparents serve and the changing nature of families (Hills, 2010). The social dimension includes societal needs and expectations of what grandparents are to do, such as passing on family his- tory to grandchildren. The personal dimension includes the personal satisfaction and indi- vidual needs fulfilled by being a grandparent. Many grandparents pass on skills—as well as religious, social, and vocational values (social dimension)—through storytelling and advice, and they may feel great pride and satisfaction (personal dimension) from working with grandchildren on joint projects.

Grandchildren give grandparents a great deal in return. Grandchildren keep grandpar- ents in touch with youth and the latest trends. Sharing the excitement of surfing the web in school may be one way grandchildren keep grandparents on the technological forefront.

Being a Grandparent Is Meaningful Being a grandparent really matters. Most grandparents derive multiple meanings, and they are linked with generativity (Gonyea, 2013; Hayslip & Blumenthal, 2016). For some, grandparenting is the most important thing in their lives. For others, meaning comes from being seen as wise, from spoiling grandchildren, from recalling the relationship they had with their own grandparents, or from taking pride in the fact they will be followed by not one but two generations.

Grandchildren also highly value their relationships with grandparents, even when they are young adults (Alley, 2004; Hayslip & Blumenthal, 2016). Grandparents are valued as role models as well as for their personalities, the activities they share, and the attention they show to grandchildren. Emerging adult grandchildren (ages 21–29) derive both stress and rewards from caring for grandparents, much the same way middle-aged adults do when they care for their aging parents (Fruhauf, 2007).

Ethnic Differences How grandparents and grandchildren interact varies in different ethnic groups. Inter- generational relationships are especially important and historically have been a source of strength in African American families (Waites, 2009) and Latino families (Hayslip & Blu- menthal, 2016). African American grandparents play an important role in many aspects of their grandchildren’s lives, such as family storytelling (Fabius, 2016) and religious educa- tion (King et al., 2006; Lewis, Seponski, & Camp, 2011). African American grandfathers, in particular, tend to perceive grandparenthood as a central role to a greater degree than do European American grandfathers (Kivett, 1991). Latino American grandparents are more likely to participate in child rearing owing to a cultural core value of family (Burnette, 1999).

Native American grandparents are impor- tant in their grandchildren’s lives, especially when the grandchildren live in urban settings and are reasonably close by (Limb, Shafer, & Sandoval, 2014). These grandparents provide grandchildren with a way to connect with their cultural heritage, and they are also likely to pro- vide a great deal of care for their grandchildren (Mutchler, Baker, & Lee, 2007). Research indi- cates Native American grandparents also use their own experiences of cultural disruption to reinvest in their grandchildren to ensure the continuity of culture (Thompson, Cameron, & Fuller-Thompson, 2013). In general, Native American grandmothers take a more active role than do grandfathers, and are more likely to pass on traditional rituals (Woodbridge, 2008).

How grandparents and grandchildren interact varies across ethnic groups.

Br ia

n Sk

yu m

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

452 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

Asian American grandparents, particularly if they are immigrants, serve as a primary source of traditional culture for their grandchildren (Yoon, 2005). When these grandpar- ents become heavily involved in caring for their grandchildren, they especially want and need services that are culturally and linguistically appropriate.

When Grandparents Care for Grandchildren Grandparenthood today is tougher than it used to be. Families are more mobile, which means grandparents are more often separated from their grandchildren by geographical distance. Grandparents also are more likely to have independent lives apart from their children and grandchildren. What being a grandparent entails in the 21st century is more ambiguous than it once was (Hayslip & Blumenthal, 2016; Hyer et al., 2017).

Perhaps the biggest change worldwide for grandparents is the increasing number serving as custodial parents or primary caregivers for their grandchildren (Choi, Sprang, & Eslinger, 2016). Estimates are that over 2.5 million grandparents provide basic needs (food, shelter, clothing) for one or more of their grandchildren (AARP, 2016). These situa- tions are due to several factors (Choi et al., 2016): the most frequent is due to both parents being employed outside the home; when the parents are deceased, addicted, incarcerated, or unable to raise their children for some other reason; or when discipline or behavior problems have been exhibited by the grandchild.

Because most grandparents in this situation do not have legal custody of their grand- child, problems and challenges such as dealing with schools and obtaining school or health records are frequent. Typically, social service workers must assist grandparents in navigat- ing the many unresponsive policies and systems they encounter when trying to provide the best possible assistance to their grandchildren (Cox, 2007). Clearly, public policy changes are needed to address these issues, especially regarding grandparents’ rights regarding schools and health care for their grandchildren (Ellis, 2010).

Raising grandchildren is not easy. Financial stress, cramped living space, and social isolation are only some of the issues facing custodial grandparents (Choi et al, 2016; Hayslip & Blumenthal, 2016). All of these stresses are also reported cross-culturally; full- time custodial grandmothers in Kenya reported higher levels of stress than part-time caregivers (Oburu & Palmérus, 2005).

Even custodial grandparents raising grandchildren without these problems report more stress and role disruption than noncustodial grandparents, though most grandparents are resilient and manage to cope (Hayslip & Blumenthal, 2016; Hayslip et al., 2013). Most custodial grandparents consider their situation better for their grand- child than any other alternative and report surprisingly few negative effects on their marriages.

recall 1. The term _______________ refers to middle-aged adults who

have both living parents and children of their own.

2. The people who gather the family together for celebrations and keep family members in touch are called_______________.

3. Most caregiving for aging parents is provided by _______________.

4. The sense of personal responsibility to care for one’s par- ents is called _______________.

5. An increasing number of children are being raised by their _______________.

Interpret ●● If you were to create a guide to families for middle-aged

adults, what would be your most important pieces of advice? Why did you select these?

Apply ●● What are the connections between the first part of this chap-

ter, on health, and this section, on caregiving?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 13.4

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 3 : M A K I N G I T I N M I D L I F E 453

13.1 Physical Changes and Health

How does appearance change in middle adulthood?

●● Some of the signs of aging appearing in middle age include wrinkles, gray hair, and weight gain.

What changes occur in bones and joints?

●● An important change— especially in women— is loss of bone mass, which in severe form may result in the disease osteoporosis. This disease can be very serious and result in broken bones and loss of height.

●● Osteoarthritis, a disease marked by gradual onset and pro- gressive pain and disability, generally becomes noticeable in late middle or early old age. Rheumatoid arthritis, a more destructive disease, is a more common form affecting fin- gers, wrists, and ankles.

What reproductive changes occur in men and women in middle age?

●● The climacteric (loss of the ability to bear children by natu- ral means) and menopause (cessation of menstruation) occur in the 40s and 50s and constitute a major change in reproductive ability in women.

●● The decline of estrogen has been correlated with certain physical changes. Menopausal hormone therapy is a con- troversial approach to treatment of menopausal symptoms.

●● Reproductive changes in men are much less dramatic; even older men usually are still fertile. Physical changes do affect sexual response.

What is stress? How does it affect physical and psychologi- cal health?

●● In the stress and coping paradigm, stress results from a person’s primary and secondary appraisal of an event as taxing his or her resources. Daily hassles are viewed as the primary source of stress.

●● Coping is the response to deal with stress. Problem-focused coping and emotion-focused coping are two examples.

●● Long-term stress has potentially serious negative health effects, such as on the immune and cardiovascular systems. It is also related to mental health problems, such as post- traumatic stress disorder.

What are the benefits of exercise?

●● Aerobic exercise has numerous benefits, especially with regard to cardiovascular health and cognitive fitness. The best results are obtained with a moderate exercise program maintained throughout adulthood.

13.2 Cognitive Development

How does practical intelligence develop in adulthood?

●● Intelligence in middle age shows more clearly the mechan- ics (the underlying skills) and pragmatics (practical appli- cations of crystallized intelligence). Research on practical intelligence reveals connections to postformal thinking. Emotion is more strongly blended with cognition in midlife than in younger adulthood.

How does a person become an expert?

●● People tend to become experts in some areas and not in others. Experts tend to think in more flexible ways com- pared with novices and can skip steps in solving problems. Expert performance tends to peak in middle age.

What is meant by lifelong learning? What differences are there between adults and young people in how they learn?

●● Adults learn differently than children and youth. Older  students need practical connections and a rationale for learning, and they are more motivated by internal factors.

13.3 Personality

What is the five-factor model? What evidence is there for stability in personality traits?

●● The five-factor model postulates five dimensions of per- sonality: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Several longitudinal studies indicate that personality traits show long-term sta- bility in general, but certain traits can change across adult- hood. The Triggering situations, Expectancy, States/State Expressions, and Reactions (TESSERA) model accounts for both stability and change.

What changes occur in people’s priorities and personal concerns? How does a person achieve generativity? How is midlife best described?

●● Erikson believed that middle-aged adults become more concerned with doing for others and passing social values and skills to the next generation— a set of behaviors and beliefs he labeled generativity. Those who do not achieve generativity are thought to experience stagnation. People derive personal meaning from constructing a life story or narrative.

●● Priorities and personal concerns change with age, shown in the experience of a midlife correction.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

454 PA rT T H r e e : Y O U N G A N D M I D D L E A D U LT H O O D

13.4 Family Dynamics and Middle Age

Who are the kinkeepers in families?

●● Middle-aged mothers tend to adopt the role of kinkeep- ers to keep family traditions alive as a way of linking generations.

●● Middle age is sometimes referred to as the sandwich gener- ation because middle-aged adults are caring for both their aging parents and their own children.

How does the relationship between middle-aged parents and their young adult children change?

●● Parent–child relations improve dramatically when chil- dren grow out of adolescence. Most parents look forward to having an empty nest. Difficulties emerge to the extent that raising children has been a primary source of personal identity for parents. However, once children leave home, parents still provide considerable support.

●● Young adult children move back home primarily for finan- cial reasons. In general, neither parents nor children prefer this arrangement.

How do middle-aged adults deal with their aging parents?

●● Caring for aging parents usually falls to a daughter or daughter-in-law. Caregiving creates a stressful situation due

to conflicting feelings and roles. The potential for conflict is high, as is financial pressure.

●● Caregiving stress is usually greater in women, who must deal with multiple roles. Older parents are often dissatisfied with the situation as well.

What styles of grandparenthood do middle-aged adults experience? How do grandchildren and grandparents interact?

●● Becoming a grandparent means assuming new roles. Styles of interaction vary among grandchildren and with the age of the grandchild. Also relevant are the social and personal dimensions of grandparenting.

●● Grandparents derive several types of meaning regardless of style. Most children and young adults report positive relationships with grandparents, and young adults feel a responsibility to care for them if necessary.

●● Ethnic differences are found in the extent to which grand- parents take an active role in their grandchildren’s lives.

●● In an increasingly mobile society, grandparents are more frequently assuming a distant relationship with their grand- children. An increasing number of grandparents serve as the custodial parent. These arrangements are typically stressful.

13.1 1. osteoporosis 2. menopause 3. gradually 4. coping 5. mind fulness-based stress reduction 13.2 1. practical intelligence 2. mechanics and pragmatics 3. expertise 4. life long learning 13.3 1. conscientiousness 2. generativity 3. narrative 4. midlife correction 13.4 1. sandwich generation 2. kinkeepers 3. daughters and daughters-in-law 4. filial obligation 5. grandparents

Test Yourself: Recall Answers

skeletal maturity (423) osteoporosis (423) dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry

(DXA) test (424) selective estrogen receptor modulators

(serms) (425) osteoarthritis (426) rheumatoid arthritis (426) climacteric (428) menopause (428) perimenopause (428) menopausal hormone therapy

(mht) (428) stress and coping paradigm (431)

primary appraisal (432) secondary appraisal (432) reappraisal (432) coping (432) problem-focused coping (432) emotion-focused coping (433) post-traumatic stress disorder

(PtsD) (434) mindfulness-based stress reduction (434) aerobic exercise (435) practical intelligence (437) mechanics of intelligence (437) pragmatics of intelligence (437) neuroticism (441)

extraversion (441) openness to experience (442) agreeableness (442) conscientiousness (442) personality adjustment (443) personality growth (443) generativity (444) stagnation (444) narrative (445) midlife correction (446) kinkeeper (447) sandwich generation (447) filial obligation (448)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Ra w

pi xe

l L td

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

457

Think about all the descriptions of older adults you have heard or used yourself, and write down as many as you can think of. Odds are, at least

several of them are not positive. Later life is a time of

life that provides much fodder for stereotypes that

often paint a not-so-pretty picture of growing older.

A quick look at birthday cards verifies that negative

stereotypes of aging are common.

How accurate are these images? In this chap-

ter and in Chapter 15, we check the facts. We will

sort through the common beliefs people have about

aging and find out that some of them, such as slow-

ing down, are based in fact, and others, such as a

general decline in all aspects of memory, are not.

Most important, we will learn that lifestyle factors

under our control can heavily influence what our

experience of later life will be.

The Personal Context of Later Life Physical, Cognitive, and Mental Health Issues

14.1 What Are Older Adults Like? 458 The Demographics of Aging 458 Longevity 461 The Third–Fourth Age Distinction 464

14.2 Physical Changes and Health 465 Biological Theories of Aging 466 Physiological Changes 468 Chronic Disease and Lifestyle Health Issues 475 REAL PEOPLE: The “Angelina Jolie Effect” 477

14.3 Cognitive Processes 479 Information Processing 479 Memory 481 Creativity and Wisdom 485 WHAT DO YOU THINK? Does Creativity Exist? 486

14.4 Mental Health and Intervention 488 Depression 488 Anxiety Disorders 490 Dementia 491 SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: Training Persons with

Dementia to Be Group Activity Leaders 496

Summary 497

Key Terms 499

14

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

458 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Sarah is an 87-year-old African American woman who comes from a family of long-lived individuals. She has never been to a physician, and she has never been seriously ill. Sarah figures it’s just as well that she has never needed a physician because for most of her life, she had no health insurance. Because she feels healthy and has more living that she wants to do, Sarah believes that she’ll live for several more years.

What is it like to be old? Do you want your own late life to be described by the words and phrases you wrote at the beginning of the chapter? Do you look forward to becoming old, or are you afraid of what may lie ahead? Most of us probably want to be like Sarah and enjoy a long, healthy life. Growing old is not something we think about very much until we have to. Most of us experience the coming of old age as a surprise. It’s as if we go to bed one night middle-aged and wake up the next day feeling old. But we can take comfort in knowing that when the day comes, we will have plenty of company.

The Demographics of Aging Did you ever stop to think about how many older adults you see in your day-to-day life?  Did you ever wonder whether your great-grandparents had the same experience? Actually, you are privileged—there have never been as many older adults alive as there are now, so you see many more older people than your great-grandparents (or even your parents) did when they were your age. The proportion of older adults in the population of developed countries has increased tremendously, mainly due to better health care over the past century (e.g., the elimination or prevention of previously fatal acute diseases, espe- cially during childhood, better treatment for chronic diseases) and to lowering women’s mortality rate during childbirth.

People who study population trends, called demographers, use a graphic technique called a population pyramid to illustrate these changes. ◗ Figure 14.1 shows average popu- lation pyramids for the most developed and least developed countries around the world. Let’s consider developed countries first (they’re down the left side of the figure). Notice the shape of the population pyramid in 1950, shown in the top panel of the figure. In the middle of the 20th century, there were fewer people over age 60 than under age 60; so the figure tapers toward the top. Compare this to projections for 2050; you can see that a dramatic change will occur in the number of people over 65.

These changes also occur in developing countries, shown in the lighter color. The figures for both 1950 and 2015 look more like pyramids when you look at both the male and female halves together because there are substantially fewer older adults than younger people. But by 2050, the number of older adults even in developing countries will have increased dramatically, substantially changing the shape of the figure.

Because the growth of the child population in the United States slowed in the 20th cen- tury and essentially stops by the middle of this century, the average age of Americans will con- tinue to rise (Pew Research Center, 2015). By 2030, all of the baby boomers will have reached at least age 65, meaning that one in five Americans will be 65 or older (Census Bureau, 2015).

The sheer number of older Americans will place enormous pressure on pension sys- tems (especially Social Security), health care (especially Medicare, Medicaid, and long- term care), and other human services. The costs will be borne by smaller numbers of taxpaying workers behind them, meaning that each worker will have a higher tax burden in the future in order to keep benefits at their current levels.

demographers People who study population trends.

population pyramid graphic technique for illustrating popula- tion trends.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the characteristics of older adults in the

population? ●● How long will most people live? What factors influence this?

●● What is the distinction between the third and fourth age?

14.1 What Are Older Adults Like?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 459

●◗ Figure 14.1 Population pyramids for developed and developing countries 1950–2050. Note the changing shapes of the distributions in terms of the proportion of the population that is young versus old over time and as a function of whether countries are considered developed or developing.

From International programs: International database, by U.S. Census Bureau. Copyright © U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from www.census.gov/ population/ international/data/idb/ (1950 developed countries). Remaining data extracted from census data tables. See Kail and Cavanaugh, Essentials of Human Development, 2nd ed., p. 369.

Age (years)

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80+

DEVELOPED COUNTRIES–1950

Population (in thousands) Population (in thousands)

Population (in millions)

Age group

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84

DEVELOPING COUNTRIES–1950

85–89 90–94 95–99 100+

Population (in millions)

Population (in millions)Population (in millions) Population (in millions)Population (in millions)

Population (in millions)Population (in millions)Age group

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84

DEVELOPED COUNTRIES–2014

85–89 90–94 95–99 100+

Age group

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84

DEVELOPING COUNTRIES–2014

85–89 90–94 95–99 100+

Age group

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84

DEVELOPED COUNTRIES–2050

85–89 90–94 95–99 100+

0–4 5–9

10–14 15–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 45–49 50–54 55–59 60–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84

DEVELOPING COUNTRIES–2050

85–89 90–94 95–99 100+

Age group

Male Female

Male Female

Male Female

Male Female

Male Female

Male Female

50 40 30 20 10 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 25 20 15 10 5 0 0 5 10 15 20 25

50 40 30 20 10 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 70 56 42 28 14 0 0 14 28 42 56 70

45 36 27 18 9 0 0 9 18 27 36 45 95 76 57 38 19 0 0 19 38 57 76 95

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

460 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

The growing strain on social service systems will intensify because the most rapidly grow- ing segment of the U.S. population is the group of people over age 85, as you can see in the graphs. The number of such people will increase nearly 500% between 2000 and 2050, com- pared with about a 50% increase in the number of 20- to 29-year-olds during the same period. Individuals over age 85 generally need more assistance with the tasks they have to accomplish in daily living than do people under 85, straining the healthcare and social service systems.

The Diversity of Older Adults The general population of the United States is changing rapidly. By 2044, it is expected that the U.S. will become majority-minority, up from 38% in 2014 (Census Bureau, 2015). Similarly, the number of older adults among minority groups is increasing faster than the number among European Americans (Pew Research Center, 2015). In terms of gender, as you can see in the graphs, older women outnumber older men in the United States. This is true also for each major ethnic and racial group.

Older adults in the future will be better educated, too. By 2030, it is estimated that 85% will have a high-school diploma and about 75% will have a college degree (Census Bureau, 2014). Better-educated people tend to live longer—mostly because they have higher incomes, which give them better access to good health care and a chance to follow healthier lifestyles (e.g., have access to and afford healthier food choices).

Internationally, the number of older adults is also growing rapidly (Pew Research Center, 2015). These rapid increases are due mostly to improved health care, lower rates of death in childbirth, and lower infant mortality. Nearly all countries are facing the need to adapt social policies to incorporate these changing demographics and resulting societal needs.

Economically powerful countries around the world, such as China, are trying to cope with increased numbers of older adults that strain the country’s resources. By 2040, China expects to have more than 300 million people over age 60. So it is already addressing issues related to providing services and living arrangements for the increasing number of older adults (Ren & Treiman, 2014).

China and the United States are not alone in facing increased numbers of older adults. As you can see in ◗ Figure 14.2, the population of many countries will include substantially more older adults over the next few decades. All of these countries will need to deal with an increased demand for services to older adults and, in some cases, competing demands with children and younger and middle-aged adults for limited resources.

●◗ Figure 14.2 The proportion of older adults (65 years and older) is increasing in many countries and will continue to do so.

P e rc

e n

t

1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010 2020 2030 0

5

10

15

20

25

30

Year

France Japan India Italy Republic of Korea Sweden U.S.A.

From United Nations, Statistics Bureau, Ministry of Public Management, Home Affairs, Post and Telecommunications, Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare. See Kail and Cavanaugh, Essentials of Human Development, 2nd ed., p. 370.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 461

The general aging of the population is a global phenomenon. It is due mainly to two factors: a declining birth rate in many countries, often tied to increases in opportunities for women (such as education and careers) and especially to more people living longer. Let’s take a closer look at longer life spans, and how they affect societies.

Longevity The length of life has an enormous impact on everything from decisions about govern- ment healthcare programs (e.g., how much money should Congress allocate to Medicare) to retirement policy (e.g., the age at which people may collect maximum retirement bene- fits) to life insurance premiums (e.g., longer lives on average mean cheaper rates for young adults).

How long we live depends on complex interactions among biological, psychologi- cal, socioeconomic, and life-cycle forces. Let’s begin by exploring the concept of longev- ity for yourself. To get started, complete the exercise at www.livingto100.com, based on research from the New England Centenarian Study. How long will you live? Only time will tell!

Average and Maximum Longevity How long you live, called longevity, is jointly determined by genetic and environmental factors. Researchers distinguish between two different types of longevity: average longev- ity and maximum longevity. Average longevity is commonly called average life expec- tancy and refers to the age at which half of the individuals who are born in a particular year will have died. Average longevity is affected by both genetic and environmental factors.

Average longevity can be computed for people at any age. The most common method is to compute average longevity at birth, which is the projected age at which half of the people born in a certain year will have died. The current average longevity is about 79 years at birth for people in the United States (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). This means that of the people born in 2016, for example, half of them will still be alive when the group reaches age 79.

When average longevity is computed at other points in the life span, the calculation is based on all the people who are alive at that age; people who died earlier are not included. For example, computing the average longevity for people currently 65 years old would provide a predicted age at which half of those people will have died. In the United States, females currently aged 65 can expect to live on average about 20 more years; men about 18 more years (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016b).

For people in the United States, average longevity has been increasing steadily since 1900; recent estimates for longevity at birth, at age 65, and at age 75 are presented in ◗ Figure 14.3. Pay attention to some important aspects of the data. First, data were only collected and reported for certain racial/ethnic groups. Second, whereas data on life expectancy at birth has been collected for over a century, data on life expectancy at ages 65 and 75 have only been collected and reported more recently. These differences reflect changes in both research design and in an understanding that data need to be reported separately for various racial/ethnic groups.

Note in the figure that the most rapid increases in average longevity at birth occurred in the first half of the 20th century. These increases in average longevity were caused mostly by better health care. The decrease in the number of women who died during child- birth was especially important in raising average life expectancies for women. Advances in medical technology and improvements in health care mean that more people survive to old age, thereby increasing average longevity in the general population.

Maximum longevity is the oldest age to which any individual of a species lives. Even if we were able to eliminate all diseases and other environmental influences, most research- ers estimate the limit to be somewhere around 120 years because key body systems such as the cardiovascular system have limits on how long they can last (Hayflick, 1996).

average longevity age at which half of the people born in a particular year will have died.

maximum longevity (life expectancy) oldest age to which any person lives.

Think About It How will the demographic changes in the first 30 years of the 21st century affect social policy?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

462 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

●◗ Figure 14.3 Average longevity for men and women in the united States 1900–2014.

1900 1910

1920 1930

1940 1950

1960 1970

1980 1990

2000 2010

2014

0

20

40

60

80

100

Year

L if

e E

x p

ec ta

n cy

I n

Y ea

rs

Life expectancy at 65 years

Life expectancy at birth

Female

Female

Male

Male

Adapted from National Center for Health Statistics. (2016a). Health, United States, 2015. Retrieved from www.cdc. gov/nchs/data/hus/hus15.pdf.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Increasingly, researchers are differentiating between active life expectancy and dependent life expectancy; the difference is between living to a healthy old age (active life expectancy) and the length of time one lives with a debilitating condition in which the per- son depends on others for care (dependent life expectancy). Said another way, it is the differ- ence between adding years to life and adding life to years. One’s active life expectancy ends at the point when one loses independence or must rely on others for most activities of daily living (e.g., cooking meals, bathing). The remaining years of one’s life constitute living in a dependent state. How many active and dependent years one has in late life depends a great deal on the interaction of genetic and environmental factors, to which we now turn.

Genetic and Environmental Factors in Average Longevity Our average longevity is influenced most by genetic, environmental, ethnic, and gender fac- tors. Clearly, these factors interact. But it is important to examine each of these factors and see how they influence our longevity. Let’s begin with genetic and environmental factors.

We have known for a long time that a good way to have a greater chance of a long life is to come from a family with a history of long-lived individuals. Research indicates that about 25% of the variation in human longevity is due to a person’s genetics (Passarino, De Rango, & Montesanto, 2016).

Although genes are a major determinant of longevity, environmental factors also affect the life span, often in combination with genes (Passarino et al., 2016). Diseases, such as cardiovascular disease and Alzheimer’s disease, and lifestyle issues, such as smoking and exercise, receive a great deal of attention from researchers. Environmental toxins, encountered mainly as air and water pollution, are a continuing problem.

Living in poverty shortens longevity. The impact of socioeconomic status on longevity results from reduced access to medical care and healthy food, that characterizes certain eth- nic minority groups, the poor, and many older adults, as clearly demonstrated in a very large study (566,402 participants) of premature mortality in community dwelling adults (Doubeni et al., 2012). Most of these people have restricted access to good health care and cannot afford healthy food. For many living in urban areas, air pollution, poor drinking water, and lead poi- soning from old water pipes are serious problems, but they simply cannot afford to move.

active life expectancy number of years that a person is free from debilitating chronic disease and impairment.

dependent life expectancy number of years a person lives with a debil- itating chronic disease in which the person depends on others for care.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 463

The sad part about most environmental factors is that human activity is responsible for most of them. Denying adequate health care to everyone, continuing to pollute our environment, and failing to address the underlying causes of poverty have undeniable consequences: These causes needlessly shorten lives and dramatically increase the cost of health care.

Ethnic and Gender Differences in Average Longevity People in different ethnic groups do not have the same average longevity at birth. For example, although African Americans’ average life expectancy at birth is about 4 years less for men and about 3 years less for women than it is for European Americans, by age 65 this gap has narrowed to about 2 and 1 years, respectively, for men and women. By age 85, African Americans tend to outlive European Americans. Why the shift over time?

Lower access to good-quality health care in general means that those African Americans who live to age 85 may be in better health on average than their European American counterparts. But this is just a guess, because Latinos have higher average life expectancies than European Americans and African Americans at all ages despite having, on average, less access to health care (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). The full explanation for these ethnic group differences remains to be discovered.

Women live longer, on average, than do men. Women’s average longevity is about 5 years more than men’s at birth, narrowing to roughly 1 year by age 85 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). Despite these patterns, no one really knows for certain why women tend to live longer.

International Differences in Average Longevity Countries around the world differ dramatically in how long their populations live on average. As you can see for countries in the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) in ◗ Figure 14.4, the current range extends from 71.7 years in Mexico to over 82 years in Japan. In contrast, some developing countries, such as Sierra Leone in Africa, have an average longevity at birth of less than 40 years. Such a wide diver- gence in life expectancy reflects vast discrepancies in genetic, sociocultural and economic conditions, health care, disease, and the like across industrialized and developing nations. The differences also mean that populations in countries such as Sierra Leone are very much younger, on average, than they are in countries such as Japan or the United States.

Coming from a family of long-lived individu- als shows the role of genetics in longevity.

Ja ck

H ol

lin gs

w or

th /P

ho to

di sc

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Think About It How do ethnic and gender differences in life expectancy relate to biological, psy- chological, sociocultural, and life-cycle factors?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

464 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

The Third–Fourth Age Distinction The development of the science of gerontology, the study of older adults, in the latter part of the 20th century led to cultural, medical, and economic advances for older adults (e.g., longer average longevity, increased quality of life) that in turn resulted in fundamental pos- itive changes in how older people are viewed in society. Gerontologists and policy makers became optimistic that old age was a time of potential growth rather than of decline. This combination of factors is termed the Third Age (Baltes & Smith, 2003). As we will see in this chapter and in Chapter 15, much research has documented that the young-old (ages 60 to 80) do have much to look forward to.

However, recent research shows conclusively that the oldest-old (over age 80) typically have a much different experience, which is referred to as the Fourth Age (Baltes & Smith, 2003; Gilleard & Higgs, 2010; Lamb, 2014). The oldest-old are at the limits of their func- tional capacity, and few interventions to reverse the effects of aging have been successful to date. A major focus is the choice of housing, whether to age in place or to move to alternative placements (Koss & Ekerdt, in press). We will see that the rates of diseases such as cancer and dementia increase dramatically in the oldest-old and that other aspects of psychological functioning (e.g., memory) also undergo significant and fairly rapid decline.

●◗ Figure 14.4 Life expectancy at birth, OECD countries, 2013.

Switzerland

Iceland

Israel

Italy

Japan

Spain

Sweden

Australia

Luxembourg

Noway

Netherlands

New Zealand

United Kingdom

France

Ireland

Greece

Austria

Germany

Korea

Denmark

Belgium

Finland

Portugal

Slovenia

United States

Czech Republic

Poland

Slovak Republic

Estonia

Hungary

Mexico

Japan

Spain

France

Italy

Korea

Switzerland

Australia

Finland

Greece

Portugal

Israel

Luxembourg

Austria

Norway

Sweden

Iceland

Slovenia

Belgium

Germany

Netherlands

New Zealand

Ireland

United Kingdom

Denmark

Estonia

Czech Republic

Poland

United States

Slovak Republic

Hungary

Mexico

Life Expectancy (years) Life Expectancy (years)

FEMALEMALE

0 70 80 90 0 70 80 90

80.7

76.4

81.2

86.6

71.1 77.4

Based on CDC/NCHS, Health, United States, 2015, Figure 1 and Tables 14 and 15. Data from the National Vital Statistics System (NVSS) and the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD).

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 465

Baltes and Smith (2003) view the differences between the Third Age and the Fourth Age as important for research and social policy. They characterize the Third Age as the “good news” about aging and the Fourth Age as the “bad news.”

The “Good News”: The Third Age (Young-Old) ●● Increased life expectancy, with more older people living longer and aging successfully ●● Substantial potential for physical and mental fitness, with improvement in each generation ●● Evidence of cognitive and emotional reserves in the aging mind ●● High levels of emotional and personal well-being ●● Effective strategies to master the gains and losses of later life

The “Bad News”: The Fourth Age (Oldest-Old) ●● Sizeable losses in cognitive potential and ability to learn ●● Increases in the negative effects of chronic stress ●● High prevalence of dementia (50% in people over age 90), frailty, and multiple chronic

conditions ●● Problems with quality of life and dying with dignity

The Third and Fourth Ages approach is grounded in the “selective optimization with com- pensation” model described in Chapter 1. The description of gains and losses in the Third and Fourth Ages flows naturally from this life-span perspective. As you proceed through this chapter and the next, keep in mind the distinction between the Third and the Fourth Ages. Note the different developmental patterns shown by the young-old and oldest-old. In Chapter 15, we will consider some of the social policy implications of this distinction.

recall 1. The fastest-growing segment of the population in the united

States is people over age _______________ .

2. The age at which half of the people born in a particular year will have died is called _______________.

3. The Fourth Age refers to the _______________ old.

Interpret ●● Think back to the lifestyle influences on health discussed in

Chapter 12. If most people actually exhibited a healthy life- style, what would happen to average life expectancy?

Apply ●● If you were to design an intervention program to maximize the

odds that people will live to maximum longevity, what would you emphasize?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 14.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What are the major biological theories of aging?

●● What physiological changes normally occur in later life?

●● What are the principal health issues for older adults?

14.2 Physical Changes and Health

Frank is an 80-year-old man who has been physically active his whole life. He still enjoys sailing, long-distance biking, and cross-country skiing. Although he considers himself to be in excellent shape, he has noticed that his endurance has decreased and that his hearing isn’t quite as sharp as it used to be. Frank wonders whether he can do something to stop these declines or whether they are an inevitable part of growing older.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

466 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

If your family has kept photograph albums for many years, you are able to see how your grandparents and/or great-grandparents changed over the years. Some of the more visible differences are changes in the color and amount of hair and the addition of wrinkles, but many other physical changes are harder to see. In this section, we con- sider some of these changes, as well as a few things that adults can do to improve their health. As noted in Chapter 13, many aging changes begin during middle age. However, most of these changes typically do not affect people’s daily lives until later in life, as Frank is discovering. But first, we will ask a basic question: Why do people grow old in the first place?

Biological Theories of Aging Much research on aging has focused on answering the question of why people and other living organisms grow old and die. With regard to human aging, three groups of theories have provided the most insights.

Metabolic Theories One theory of aging that makes apparent common sense postulates that organisms have only so much energy to expend in a lifetime. (Couch potatoes might like this theory, and may use it as a reason why they are not physically active.) The basic idea is that the rate of a creature’s metabolism is related to how long it lives (Barzilai et al., 2012).

Although some research indicates that significantly reducing the number of calories animals and people eat may increase longevity, research focusing on nonhuman primates shows that longer lives do not always result from restricting calories alone (Chaudhari et al., 2016).

Cellular Theories A second family of ideas points to causes of aging at the cellular level. One notion focuses on the number of times cells can divide, which presumably limits the life span of a complex organism. Cells grown in laboratory culture dishes undergo only a fixed number of divisions before dying, with the number of possible divisions dropping depending on the age of the donor organism; this phenomenon is called the Hayflick limit, after its discoverer, Leonard Hayflick (Bernadotte, Mikhelson, & Spivak, 2016; Hayflick, 1996).

What causes cells to limit their number of divisions? Evidence suggests that the tips of the chromosomes, called telomeres, play a major role in aging by adjusting the cell’s response to stress and growth stimulation based on cell divisions and DNA damage, and by typically shortening with each cell replication (Bernadotte et al., 2016). Healthy, normal telomeres help regulate the cell division and reproduction process.

An enzyme called telomerase is needed in DNA replication to fully reproduce the telo- meres when cells divide. But telomerase normally is not present in somatic cells, so with each replication the telomeres become shorter. Eventually, the chromosomes become unstable and cannot replicate because the telomeres become too short. This process is depicted in ◗ Figure 14.5.

Chronic stress may accelerate the changes that occur in telomeres and thereby shorten one’s life span (Oliveira et al., 2016; Spivak, Mikhelson, & Spivak, 2016). Research also shows that moderate levels of exercise may maintain telomere length or at least slow the rate at which telomeres shorten, which may help slow the aging process itself (Savela et al., 2013; Silva et al., 2016). However, the precise mechanisms for this process are not under- stood (Denham, O’Brien, & Charchar, 2016).

A second type of cellular theory proposes that aging is caused by unstable molecules called free radicals, which are highly reactive chemicals produced randomly in normal metabolism (Dutta et al., 2012). When these free radicals interact with nearby molecules,

telomeres tips of chromosomes that play a major role in aging by adjusting the cell’s response to stress and growth stimulation based on cell divisions and Dna damage.

telomerase enzyme needed in Dna replication to fully reproduce the telomeres when cells divide.

free radicals Highly reactive chemicals produced ran- domly in normal metabolism.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 467

●◗ Figure 14.5 The process by which telomeres shorten as we age.

As cells divide over time —

Telomere, a protective covering

Chromosome

— telomeres shorten, and eventually cell division stops.

WHAT WE LOSE WITH AGE

Cell

Adapted from telomeres-aging.com/images/shorteningdna.jpg

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

problems may result. For example, free radicals may cause cell damage to the heart by changing the oxygen levels in cells. There is also evidence that free radicals may have a role in the development of Alzheimer’s disease (Wojtunik-Kulesza et al., 2016).

The most important evidence that free radicals may be involved in aging comes from research with substances that prevent the development of free radicals in the first place. These substances, called antioxidants, prevent oxygen from combining with susceptible molecules to form free radicals. Common antioxidants include vitamins A, C, and E, and coenzyme Q. A growing body of evidence shows that ingesting antioxidants postpones the appearance of age-related diseases such as cancer, cardiovascular disease, and immune system dysfunction (Suja et al., 2016), but there is no direct evidence yet that eating a diet high in antioxidants actually increases the life span (Berger et al., 2012).

Genetic Programming Theories What if aging were programmed into our genetic code? This possibility seems much more likely as the extremely rapid growth of knowledge about human genetics con- tinues to unlock the secrets of our genetic code (Mitteldorf, 2016). Even when cell death appears random, researchers believe that such losses may be part of a mas- ter genetic program that underlies the aging process (Freitas & de Magalhães, 2011; Mackenzie, 2012).

Programmed cell death appears to be a function of physiological processes, the innate ability of cells to self-destruct, and the ability of dying cells to trigger key processes in other cells, all of which are also thought to be influenced by external environmental fac- tors (Arbeev, Ukraintseva, & Yashin, 2016; Ukraintseva et al., 2016). At present, we do not know how this genetic self-destruct program is activated, nor do we understand how it works. Nevertheless, there is increasing evidence that many diseases associated with aging (such as Alzheimer’s disease) have genetic aspects.

It is quite possible that the other explanations we have considered in this section and the changes we examine throughout this text are the result of a genetic program. We will consider many diseases throughout the text that have known genetic bases, such as Alzheimer’s disease. As genetics research continues, it is likely that we will have some exciting answers to the question, Why do we age?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

468 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Physiological Changes Growing older brings with it several inevitable physiological changes. Like Frank, whom we met in the vignette, older adults find that their endurance has declined relative to what it was 20 or 30 years earlier and that their hearing has declined. In this section, we consider some of the most important physiological changes that occur in neurons, the cardiovascu- lar and respiratory systems, the motor system, and the sensory systems. We also consider general health issues such as sleep, nutrition, and cancer. Throughout this discussion, you should keep in mind that although the changes we consider happen to everyone, the rate and the amount of change varies a great deal among individuals.

Changes in the Neurons Neuroscience research indicates that the most important normative changes with age involve structural changes in the neurons, the basic cells in the brain, and in how they communicate (Whalley, 2015). Recall the basic structures of the neuron we encountered in Chapter 3, shown again here in ◗ Figure 14.6. Two structures in neurons are most important in understanding aging: the dendrites, which pick up information from other neurons, and the axon, which transmits information inside a neuron from the dendrites to the terminal branches. Each of the changes we consider in this section impairs the neurons’ ability to transmit information, which ultimately affects how well the person functions. Three structural changes are most important in normal aging: neurofibrillary tangles, dendritic changes, and neuritic plaques.

For reasons that are not understood, fibers that compose the axon sometimes become twisted together to form spiral-shaped masses called neurofibrillary tangles. These tangles interfere with the neuron’s ability to transmit information down the axon. Some degree of tangling occurs normally with age, but large numbers of neurofibrillary tangles are associ- ated with Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementia (Sala Frigerio & De Strooper, 2016). ◗ Figure 14.7 shows how this process works.

Changes in the dendrites are more complicated. Some dendrites shrivel up and die, making it more difficult for neurons to communicate with each other and transmit infor- mation (Voss et al., 2013). However, research indicates that dendrites continue to grow in some areas of the brain, and embryonic stem cell research indicates that inducing growth may be a future way to treat brain disease and injury (West, 2010). This may help explain why older adults continue to improve in some areas, as we will discover later in this chap- ter. Why some dendrites degenerate and others do not is poorly understood; it may reflect the existence of two different families of neurons.

Neuritic or amyloid plaques are spherical structures consisting of a core of beta- amyloid, a protein, surrounded by degenerated fragments of dying or dead neurons. The plaques are found in various parts of the brain, with the amount of beta-amyloid moderately related to the severity of the disease (National Institute on Aging, 2015b). A depiction of beta-amyloid plaques is also shown in ◗ Figure 14.7.

neurofibrillary tangles spiral-shaped masses formed when fibers that compose the axon become twisted together.

beta-amyloid a protein that is the basis for neuritic plaques and is thought to be a basis for dementia.

●◗ Figure 14.6 Basic structure of the neuron.

Dendrites

Axon

Terminal buttons

Cell body

Direction of information flow

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 469

Considerable recent research has focused on beta-amyloid as a major factor in Alzheimer’s disease, both in terms of the cause and possible avenues for treatment. The role of beta-amyloid is controversial, though. Some researchers view concentration of beta-amyloid as a biomarker of Alzheimer’s disease (Sala Frigerio & De Strooper, 2016). Others consider it an early warning of potential cognitive decline, even in the absence of any behavioral symptoms (Gandy & DeKosky, 2013; Singh et al., 2016). We will con- sider the controversy surrounding diagnostic categories related to Alzheimer’s disease a bit later.

Because neurons do not physically touch each other, they must communicate via chemicals called neurotransmitters. With age, the levels of these neurotransmitters decline (Behl & Ziegler, 2014). An especially important neurotransmitter in this respect is dopamine, involved in several key cognitive processes (Mather, 2016; Park & Festini, 2017). These declines in dopamine as well as other neurotransmitters, are believed to be responsible for numerous age-related behavioral changes, including those in mem- ory and sleep, and for diseases such as Parkinson’s disease (Parkinson’s Disease Foun- dation, 2016a).

Because all of these changes in neurons are a normal part of aging, it is difficult to say precisely how they may also cause cognitive and other diseases late in life. May researchers believe that disease occurs when these changes occur at a much greater rate, but they cannot specify the exact point at which this happens. This point is important because it means that serious behavioral changes (e.g., severe memory impairment) are not a result of normative age changes in the brain; rather, they are indicators of disease.

Through technological advances in noninvasive imaging and in assessing psychologi- cal functioning, we are learning a great deal about the relations between changes in the brain and changes in behavior (Linden, 2016; Sugiura, 2016). Neuroimaging is an impor- tant tool for understanding both normal and abnormal cognitive aging. Two neuroimag- ing techniques are used most often:

neurotransmitters Chemicals released by neurons in order for them to communicate with each other.

●◗ Figure 14.7 Action of beta-amyloid and tau proteins in relation to neurons. Each disrupts neurons, but in different ways.

Adapted from www.nytimes.com/interactive/2012/02/02/science/in-alzheimers-a-tangled-protein.html

Normal microtubule

Stabilizing Tau molecules

Disintegrating microtubule

Neuron

Beta-amyloid plaque

Tau protein clumps

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

470 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

●● Structural neuroimaging provides highly detailed images of anatomical features in the brain. The most commonly used are X-rays, computerized tomography (CT) scans, and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Images from structural neuroimaging tech- niques are like photographs in that they document what a specific brain structure looks like at a specific point in time. Structural neuroimaging is usually effective at identifying such things as bone fractures, tumors, and other conditions that cause structural damage in the brain, such as strokes.

●● Functional neuroimaging provides an indication of brain activity but not high ana- tomical detail. The most commonly used are single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT), positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic res- onance imaging (fMRI), magnetoencephalograpy (or multichannel encephalograpy), and near infrared spectroscopic imaging (NIRSI). In general, fMRI is the most com- monly used technique in cognitive neuroscience research (Poldrack, 2012). Func- tional neuroimaging provides researchers with information about what parts of the brain are active when people are doing specific tasks. A typical image will show dif- ferent levels of brain activity as different colors; for example, red on an image might indicate high levels of brain activity in that region, whereas blue might indicate low levels of activity.

These noninvasive imaging techniques coupled with sensitive tests of cognitive pro- cessing have shown quite convincingly that age-related changes in the brain are, at least in part, responsible for the age-related declines in cognition that we will consider later (Sugiura, 2016). Why these declines occur has yet to be discovered, although fMRI offers considerable promise in helping researchers unlock this mystery.

Cardiovascular and Respiratory Systems Having a healthy heart and circulatory system, as well as healthy lungs are two key factors for people to enjoy a high quality of life as they grow older. Let’s take a closer look at the changes that occur to us all, as well as problems that can develop.

Cardiovascular System Changes. You may already know that cardiovascular disease is the most common cause of death in the United States. The incidence of cardiovascular diseases such as heart attack, irregular heartbeat, stroke, and hypertension (high blood pressure) increases dramatically with age and is much higher among African Americans, American Indians, and Native Hawaiians (Mozaffarian et al., 2016). However, the over- all death rates from these diseases have been declining over recent decades, mainly because fewer adults smoke cigarettes and many people have reduced the amount of fat in their diets.

Two important age-related structural changes in the heart are the accumulation of fat deposits and the stiffening of the heart muscle caused by tissue changes. By the late 40s and early 50s, the fat deposits in the lining around the heart may form a continuous sheet. Meanwhile, healthy muscle tissue is being replaced by connective tissue, which causes a thickening and stiffening of the heart muscle and valves that forces the remaining muscle to work harder (National Institute on Aging, 2015a).

Two important age-related changes occur in the circulatory system. First, the walls of the arteries stiffen due to calcification of the arterial walls and by replacement of elastic fibers with less elastic ones. Second, fat deposits in the arteries can create blockages that restrict blood flow and may result in a myocardial infarction (heart attack). Calcification and blockages in the arteries cause the disease atherosclerosis.

As people grow older, their chances of having a stroke increase. Strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents (CVAs), are caused by interruptions in the blood flow in the brain due to blockage or a hemorrhage in a cerebral artery. Blockages of arteries may be caused by clots or by atherosclerosis. Hemorrhages are caused by ruptures of the artery. CVAs are the leading cause of disability (and fourth leading cause of death) in the United States.

structural neuroimaging neuroimaging technique such as Ct and MRI scans that provides highly detailed images of anatomical features in the brain.

functional neuroimaging neuroimaging technique that provides an indication of brain activity but not high anatomical detail.

atherosclerosis a disease caused by calcification and blockages in the arteries.

strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents (CVAs) Interruption of the blood flow in the brain due to blockage or a hemorrhage in a cere- bral artery.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 471

Treatment of CVA has advanced significantly. The most important advance is use of the clot-dissolving drug tissue plasminogen activator (tPA) to treat CVAs (American Stroke Association, 2016). Currently, tPA is the only approved treatment for CVAs caused by blood clots, which constitute 80% of all CVAs. Not every patient should receive tPA treatment, and tPA is effective only if given promptly, which is vitally important. So if you or a person you know thinks they are experiencing a CVA, get medical attention imme- diately because to be most effective, tPA therapy must be started within three hours after the onset of a stroke. Recovery from CVA depends on the severity of the stroke, area and extent of the brain affected, and patient age.

Older adults often experience transient ischemic attacks (TIAs), which involve an interruption of blood flow to the brain and are often early warning signs of stroke. A single, large cerebral vascular accident may produce serious cognitive impairment, such as the loss of the ability to speak, or physical problems, such as the inability to move one’s arm. The nature and severity of the impairment in functioning that a person experiences are usually determined by which specific area of the brain is affected. Recovery from a single stroke depends on many factors, including the extent and type of the loss, the ability of other areas in the brain to assume the functions that were lost, and personal motivation.

Numerous small cerebral vascular accidents can result in a disease termed vascular dementia. Unlike Alzheimer’s disease, another form of dementia discussed later in this chap- ter, vascular dementia can have a sudden onset and may progress slowly (Leys, Murao, & Pasquier, 2014). Typical symptoms include hypertension, specific and extensive alterations on an MRI, and differential impairment on neuropsychological tests, although we still have a great deal to learn about precise diagnosis (Shim, 2014). The differential impairment refers to a pattern of scores showing some functions intact and others significantly below average. Individuals’ specific symptom patterns may vary a great deal depending on which specific areas of the brain are damaged. In some cases, vascular dementia has a much faster course than Alzheimer’s disease, resulting in death an average of two to three years after onset; in other cases, the disease may progress more slowly with idiosyncratic symptom patterns.

Single cerebral vascular accidents and vascular dementia are diagnosed similarly. Evidence of damage may be obtained from diagnostic structural imaging (e.g., CT scan or MRI), which provides pictures such as the one shown in ◗ Figure 14.8, that is then

transient ischemic attacks (TIAs) Interruption of blood flow to the brain; often an early warning sign of stroke.

vascular dementia Disease caused by numerous small cerebral vascular accidents.

●◗ Figure 14.8 Neuroimaging is especially helpful in diagnosing a cere- brovascular accident (shown in red).

Sc ot

t C am

az in

e/ Sc

ie nc

e So

ur ce

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

472 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

confirmed by neuropsychological tests. Known risk factors for both conditions include hypertension and a family history of the disorders.

Respiratory System Changes. Although the size of the lungs does not change with age, the maximum amount of air in one breath drops 40% from age 25 to age 85, due mostly to stiffening of the rib cage and air passages with age and to destruction of the air sacs in the lungs by pollution and smoking (Copley, 2016). This decline is the main cause of shortness of breath after physical exertion in later life.

Because of the cumulative effects of breathing polluted air over a lifetime, it is hard to say how many of these changes are strictly age-related. The most common form of inca- pacitating respiratory disease among older adults is chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD). COPD can be a debilitating condition and may result in depression, anxi- ety, and the need to be continually connected to oxygen (American Lung Association, 2016). Emphysema is the most common form of COPD; although most cases of emphy- sema are due to smoking, some forms are genetic. Chronic bronchitis and asthma are other common types of COPD. COPD can severely restrict a person’s ability to function, interact with others, and enjoy good quality of life.

Sensory Changes Growing older brings with it several normative changes in sensory abilities. These changes can affect people’s ability to enjoy life but in most cases can be adequately compensated for through various types of interventions.

Vision. Age-related changes in vision are fairly obvious—just watch as middle-aged and older adults’ arms become too short for them to see things clearly close up. This change is due to age-related changes in the lens’s ability to adjust, and focus declines as the muscles around it stiffen (Alavi, 2016). This is what causes difficulty in seeing close objects clearly (called presbyopia), necessitating either longer arms or corrective lenses. To complicate matters further, the time our eyes need to change focus from near to far (or vice versa) increases. This also poses a major problem in driving. Because drivers are constantly changing their focus from the instrument panel to other autos and signs on the highway,

older drivers may miss important information because of their slower refocusing time. In addition, as we grow older, the lens becomes more yellow, causing poorer color discrimination in the green–blue–violet end of the spectrum.

Other changes also occur that affect how well we see in later life. One is a decrease in the amount of light that passes through the eye, resulting in the need for more light to do tasks such as reading. As you might suspect, this change is one reason older adults do not see as well in the dark, which may account in part for their reluctance to go places at night. One logical response to the need for more light would be to increase illumination levels in general. However, this solution does not work in all situations because we also become increasingly sensitive to glare (Sanford, 2014).

Another major change is that our ability to adjust to changes in illumination, called adaptation, declines. Going from outside into a darkened movie theater involves dark adaptation; going back outside involves light adaptation. Research indicates that the time it takes for both types of adaptation increases with age (Andersen, 2012). These changes are especially important for older drivers, who have more difficulty seeing after being con- fronted with the headlights of an oncoming car.

chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) Most common form of incapacitating respi- ratory disease among older adults.

presbyopia Difficulty seeing close objects clearly.

Older adults must adjust to several changes in vision, including the need for reading glasses.

Ja ck

.Q /S

hu tt

er st

oc k.

co m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 473

A third structural change affects the ability to see detail and to discriminate different visual patterns, called acuity. Acuity declines steadily between ages 20 and 60, with a more rapid decline thereafter. Loss of acuity is especially noticeable at low light levels (Andersen, 2012).

Besides these normative structural changes, some people experience diseases caused by abnormal structural changes. First, opaque spots called cataracts may develop on the lens, which limits the amount of light transmitted. Cataracts often are treated by surgical removal and replacement of the lenses. Second, the fluid in the eye may not drain properly, causing very high internal pressure; this condition, called glaucoma, can cause internal damage and loss of vision. Glaucoma is a fairly common disease in middle and late adult- hood and is usually treated with eye drops.

The second major family of changes in vision results from changes in the retina. The retina lines approximately two-thirds of the interior of the eye. The specialized receptor cells for vision, the rods and cones, are contained in the retina. They are most densely packed toward the rear, especially at the focal point of vision, a region called the macula. At the center of the macula is the fovea, where incoming light is focused for maximum acuity, as when one is reading. With increasing age, the probability of degen- eration of the macula increases (Lighthouse International, 2014). Age-related macular degeneration involves the progressive and irreversible destruction of receptors from any of a number of causes. This disease results in the loss of the ability to see details; for example, reading becomes extremely difficult and television is often reduced to a blur. Roughly one in five people over age 75, especially smokers and European American women, have macular degeneration, making it the leading cause of functional blindness in older adults.

A second age-related retinal disease is a by-product of diabetes. Diabetes is accompa- nied by accelerated aging of the arteries, with blindness being one of the more serious side effects. Diabetic retinopathy, as this condition is called, can involve fluid retention in the macula, detachment of the retina, hemorrhage, and aneurysms (National Eye Institute, 2015). Because it takes many years to develop, diabetic retinopathy is more common among people who developed diabetes early in life.

Hearing. The age-related changes in vision we have considered can significantly affect people’s ability to function in their environment. Similarly, age-related changes in hear- ing can also have this effect and interfere with people’s ability to communicate with others. Hearing loss is one of the well-known normative changes associated with aging (Davis et al., 2016). A visit to any housing complex for older adults will easily verify this point; you will quickly notice that television sets and radios are turned up fairly loud in most of the apartments. But you don’t have to be old to experience significant hearing problems.

Loud noise is the enemy of hearing at any age. You probably have seen people who work in noisy environments (such as factories and airports) wearing protective gear on their ears so that they are not exposed to loud noise over extended periods of time.

However, you can do serious damage to your hearing with short exposure, too. But you don’t need to be at a loud rock concert or next to a jet engine to damage your hearing either. Using headphones or earbuds, especially at high volume, can cause the same serious damage and should be avoided (Jiang et al., 2016). It is especially easy to cause hearing loss with headphones or earbuds if you wear them while exercising; the increased blood flow to the ear during exercise makes hearing receptors more vulner- able to damage. Because young adults do not see their music listening behavior as a risk (Gilliver et al., 2012), hearing loss from this and other sources of loud noise is on the rise. The worst news is that hearing loss is likely to increase among older adults in the future (Davis et al., 2016).

The cumulative effects of noise and normative age-related changes create the most com- mon age-related hearing problem: reduced sensitivity to high-pitched tones or presbycusis, which occurs earlier and more severely than the loss of sensitivity to low-pitched tones.

cataracts opaque spots on the lens that limit the amount of light transmitted.

glaucoma Disease in which the fluid in the eye does not drain properly, causing very high inter- nal pressure that can damage the eye and cause loss of vision.

age-related macular degeneration Progressive and irreversible destruction of receptors from any of a number of causes.

diabetic retinopathy eye disease that is a result of diabetes and can involve fluid retention in the macula, detachment of the retina, hemorrhage, and aneurysms.

presbycusis Reduced sensitivity to high-pitched tones.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

474 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Research indicates that by their late 70s, roughly half of older adults have presbycusis. Men typically have greater loss than women, but this may be due to differential exposure to noisy environments. Hearing loss usually is gradual at first, but accelerates during the 40s, a pattern seen clearly in ◗ Figure 14.9.

Presbycusis results from four types of changes in the inner ear (Nagaratnam, Nagaratnam & Cheuk, 2016): sensory, consisting of atrophy and degeneration of receptor cells; neural, consisting of a loss of neurons in the auditory pathway in the brain; metabolic, consisting of a diminished supply of nutrients to the cells in the receptor area; and mechan- ical, consisting of atrophy and stiffening of the vibrating structures in the receptor area. Knowing the cause of a person’s presbycusis is important because the different causes have different implications for other aspects of hearing. Sensory presbycusis has little effect on other hearing abilities. Neural presbycusis seriously affects the ability to understand speech. Metabolic presbycusis produces severe loss of sensitivity to all pitches. Finally, mechanical presbycusis also produces loss across all pitches, but the loss is greatest for high pitches.

●◗ Figure 14.9 Hearing loss occurs in all adults but is greatest for high-pitched tones and greater for men than for women. As a reference, the highest note on a piano is 4,186 Hz; normal human hearing ranges from 27 Hz to 20,000 Hz.

0 dB

20

40

60

80

Age (years)

H ea

ri n

g le

ve l

FREQUENCY

1 KHz

2 KHz

4 KHz

8 KHz

0 dB

20

40

60

80

H ea

ri n

g le

ve l

FREQUENCY

1 KHz 2 KHz

4 KHz

8 KHz

F E

M A

L E

M A

L E

30 40 50 60 70 80

Age (years)

30 40 50 60 70 80 90

90

Based on Ordy, J. M., Brizzee, K. R., Beavers, T., and Medart, P. (1979). Age differences in the functional and structural organization of the auditory system in man. In J. M. Ordy and K. R. Brizzee (Eds.), Sensory systems and communication in the elderly, San Diego, CA: Raven Press.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 475

Loss of hearing in later life can also cause numerous adverse emotional reactions, such as loss of independence, social isolation, irritation, paranoia, and depression (Cherko, Hickson, & Bhutta, 2016). Much research indicates hearing loss per se does not cause social maladjustment or emotional disturbance. However, friends and relatives of an older person with undiagnosed or untreated hearing loss often attribute emotional changes to hearing loss, which strains the quality of interpersonal relationships (Li-Korotky, 2012). Thus, hearing loss may not directly affect older adults’ self-concept or emotions, but it may negatively affect how they feel about interpersonal communication, especially between couples. Moreover, over 11% of adults with hearing loss report having experienced mod- erate to severe depression, double the rate of adults without hearing loss (Li et al., 2014). By understanding hearing loss problems and ways to overcome them, people who have no hearing loss can play a large part in minimizing the effects of hearing loss on the older people in their lives.

Fortunately, many people with hearing loss can be helped through two types of ampli- fication systems and cochlear implants. Analog hearing aids are the most common and least expensive, but they provide the lowest-quality sound. Digital hearing aids include microchips that can be programmed for different hearing situations. Cochlear implants do not amplify sound; rather, a microphone transmits sound to a receiver, which stimulates auditory nerve fibers directly. Although technology continues to improve, none of these devices can duplicate our original equipment; so be kind to your ears.

Chronic Disease and Lifestyle Health Issues Older adults face numerous health challenges that are influenced by both lifestyle and genetics and their interaction.

Chronic Disease Nearly half of adults in the United States have a chronic health condition (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). Some of the most common are diabetes mellitus and cancer, discussed here, and arthritis, discussed in Chapter 13.

Diabetes Mellitus. The disease diabetes mellitus occurs when the pancreas produces insuf- ficient insulin. The primary characteristic of diabetes mellitus is above-normal sugar (glu- cose) in the blood and urine caused by problems in metabolizing carbohydrates. People with diabetes mellitus can go into a coma if the level of sugar gets too high, and they may lapse into unconsciousness if it gets too low.

There are two general types of diabetes (American Diabetes Association, 2016a). Type 1 diabetes usually develops earlier in life and requires the use of insulin; hence, it is sometimes called insulin-dependent diabetes. Type 2 diabetes typically develops in adulthood and is often effectively managed through diet. There are three groups of older adults with diabetes: those who develop diabetes as children, adolescents, or young adults; those who develop diabetes in late middle age and typically develop car- diovascular problems; and those who develop diabetes in late life and usually show mild problems. This last group includes the majority of older adults with diabetes mellitus. In adults, diabetes mellitus often is associated with obesity and is usually diagnosed during other medical procedures, such as eye examinations or hospitalizations for other conditions.

Diabetes is more common among older adults and members of minority groups (American Diabetes Association, 2016a). The chronic effects of increased glucose levels may result in serious complications, including nerve damage, diabetic retinopathy (dis- cussed earlier), kidney disorders, stroke, cognitive dysfunction, damage to the coronary arteries, skin problems, and poor circulation in the arms and legs, which may lead to gangrene.

diabetes mellitus Disease that occurs when the pancreas pro- duces insufficient insulin.

Type 1 diabetes type of diabetes that usually develops earlier in life and requires the use of insu- lin; sometimes called insulin-dependent diabetes.

Type 2 diabetes type of diabetes that typically develops in adulthood and is often effectively managed through diet.

Think About It How might changes in hearing affect people’s cognitive performance?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

476 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Although it cannot be cured, diabetes can be managed effectively through a low- carbohydrate and low-calorie diet; exercise; proper care of skin, gums, teeth, and feet; and medication (insulin) (American Diabetes Association, 2016b). Taking the appropriate steps to avoid diabetes in the first place is the best strategy, of course, so monitoring diet and weight are important aspects of an overall wellness-based lifestyle.

Cancer. Cancer is the second leading cause of death in the United States, behind cardiovascular disease (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). The risk of dying from cancer is slightly less than one in four for men and one in five for women ( American Cancer Society, 2016b). The risk of getting cancer increases markedly with age. Many current deaths caused by cancer are preventable: Stopping smoking, limit- ing exposure to the sun’s ultraviolet rays, and eating a healthy diet can prevent many cancers.

Why older people have a much higher incidence of cancer is not understood fully American Cancer Society, 2016a, b, c). Certainly, genetic predisposition is a major factor. Another key reason is the cumulative effect of poor health habits over a long period of time, such as cigarette smoking and poor diet. In addition, the cumulative effects of expo- sure to pollutants and cancer-causing chemicals are partly to blame. Some researchers believe that normative age-related changes in the immune system, resulting in a decreased ability to inhibit the growth of tumors, may also be responsible.

Research in molecular biology and microbiology is increasingly pointing to genetic links, likely in combination with environmental factors (Battista et al., 2012). For example, three breast cancer susceptibility genes that have been identified are BRCA1 on chro- mosome 17, BRCA2 on chromosome 13, and PALB2 which works in conjunction with BRCA2. When a woman carries a mutation in either BRCA1 or BRCA2, she is at a greater risk of being diagnosed with breast or ovarian cancer. Similarly, a potential susceptibil- ity locus for prostate cancer has been identified on chromosome 1, called HPC1, which may account for about 1 in 500 cases of prostate cancer. An additional rare mutation of HOXB13, on chromosome 17, has also been identified.

Screening for cancer remains controversial in some areas, such as for breast cancer and prostate cancer. For example, a 25-year study of nearly 90,000 Canadian women showed that annual mammography did not reduce deaths from breast cancer (Miller et al., 2014). Other research finds similar results for regular screening for breast and prostate cancer. Despite evidence that routine screening for everyone does not reduce deaths from these cancers—and can even result in unnecessary medical procedures—some physicians still encourage routine screening.

Occasionally, a famous person’s handling of a personal health issue impacts general public behavior. That was certainly the case when Angelina Jolie went public with her genetic predisposition to breast cancer, as discussed in the ◗ Real People feature.

Lifestyle Health Issues Two key factors in staying healthy are getting enough good sleep and eating a healthy diet. Let’s see how these are accomplished later in life.

Sleep. Nearly every aspect of sleep undergoes age-related changes (Mattis & Sehgal, 2016). It takes older adults longer to fall asleep, they are awake more at night, and they are more easily awakened. Older adults experience major shifts in their sleep–wake cycles, called circadian rhythms. Across adulthood, circadian rhythms move from a two-phase pattern of sleep (awake during the day and asleep at night for most people) to a multiphase rhythm reminiscent of that of infants (daytime napping and shorter sleep cycles at night).

These changes are related to the changes in regulating core body temperature. Other major causes of sleep disturbance include sleep apnea (stopping breathing for 5 to 10 sec- onds), periodic leg jerks, heartburn, frequent need to urinate, poor physical health, and

circadian rhythms sleep–wake cycle.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 477

depression. Among the most effective treatments of sleep problems are increasing physi- cal exercise, reducing caffeine intake, avoiding daytime naps, and making sure that the sleeping environment is as quiet and dark as possible (Boswell, Thai, & Brown, 2015).

Nutrition. Experts agree nutrition directly affects one’s mental, emotional, and physical functioning (Hammar & Östgren, 2013; McKee & Schüz, 2015). Diet has been linked to cancer, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, anemia, and digestive disorders. To stay maxi- mally healthy, though, we must recognize that nutritional requirements and eating habits change across the life span. This change is due mainly to differences in, or how much energy the body needs, termed metabolism. Body metabolism and the digestive process slow down with age (Janssen, 2005).

The U.S. Department of Agriculture publishes dietary guidelines based on current research. In its Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2015–2020 (Health.gov, 2015), the USDA recommends we eat a variety of nutrient-dense foods and beverages across the basic food groups. The general guidelines for adults can be seen in ◗ Figure 14.10.

metabolism the rate at which energy is needed and used in the body.

Angelina Jolie is one of the most famous actresses in the world. Because of her fame, many people watch her behavior care- fully, and base their own actions on what she does. In 2013, Angelina learned that she carried the BRCA1 gene, which, consid- ered in the context of the rest of her family health history and other tests that detected protein abnormalities, gave her an estimated 87% risk of breast cancer and 50% change of ovarian cancer. What she did next cre- ated both a media sensation and greatly increased awareness of the challenges and decisions that confront thousands of women each year.

Angelina decided to have a double mas- tectomy and reconstructive surgery, and sub- sequently had her ovaries and Fallopian tubes removed. After her mastectomies, she had reconstructive surgery using her own tissue. She decided to be public about her situation to raise awareness of breast cancer and ovar- ian cancer risks and the options women have. She wrote two op-eds in the New York Times (Jolie, 2013, 2015) to explain her decision making. Her decision, and her fame, made a very big difference.

It became clear that an “Angelina Jolie effect” resulted. Research in the united Kingdom indicates that following her announcement of her breast cancer risk and surgery in May 2013, referrals for genetic screening for breast cancer more than dou- bled (Evans et al., 2014). Such increases were found globally (Lebo et al., 2015). In general,

women expressed increased knowledge of breast cancer screening, techniques of recon- structive surgery, and genetic testing and risk.

Angelina Jolie’s public discussion of her learning of her cancer risk, how and why she made the decisions she did, and her recovery process caused some controversy. Some argued that she should have monitored her health and not undergone surgery. Others

argued that she provided additional support for the stereotype of female beauty. Still others thought her decision was a brave one. What is indisputable is that her fame raised the issues much higher in the public’s mind, resulting in many more women consulting their physicians about potential genetic risk and health screen- ing. And that is a very good and important outcome.

Real People The “Angelina Jolie Effect”

Angelina Jolie

Ka th

y H

ut ch

in s/

Sh ut

te rs

to ck

.c om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

478 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

●◗ Figure 14.10 Key Recommendations from Dietary Guidelines 2015–2020.

The importance of exercise:

Calorie limits can help individuals achieve healthy eating patterns:

A healthy eating pattern limits:

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/resources/2015-2020_Dietary_Guidelines.pdf, p. xiii.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 479

Rocio is a 75-year-old widow who believes that she does not remember recent events— such as whether she took her medicine—as well as she used to, but she has no trouble remembering things that happened in her 20s. Rocio wonders if this is normal or whether she should be worried.

Rocio, like many older people, takes medications for arthritis, allergies, and high blood pressure. However, each drug has its own pattern; some are taken only with meals, oth- ers are taken every eight hours, and still others are taken twice daily. Keeping these regimens straight is important to avoid potentially dangerous interactions and side effects, and older people face the problem of remembering to take each medication at the proper time.

Such situations place a heavy demand on cognitive resources such as attention and memory. In this section, we examine age-related changes in these and other cognitive pro- cesses, including reaction time, intelligence, and wisdom.

Information Processing In Chapter 1, we saw that one theoretical framework for studying cognition is informa- tion-processing theory. This framework provides a way to identify and study the basic mechanisms by which people take in, store, and remember information. Innovations and discoveries in neuroscience have resulted in major advances in our understand- ing of how people process information across the life span. Neuroscience has guided investigators as they examine age-related differences in basic processes such as attention

recall 1. The tips of the chromosomes, called _______________, play a

major role in aging.

2. A protein that is the basis for neuritic plaques and that plays a role in Alzheimer’s disease is _______________.

3. The risk of getting cancer _______________ markedly with age.

Interpret ●● In this section, we have concentrated on the biological forces

in development. Think about the other forces (psychological, social, and life cycle) and list some reasons why scientists have yet to propose a purely biological theory that accounts for all aspects of aging.

Apply ●● How do the sensory changes that occur with age affect an

older adult’s everyday life?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 14.2

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What changes occur in information processing as people

age? How do these changes relate to everyday life?

●● What changes occur in memory with age? What can be done to remediate these changes?

●● What are creativity and wisdom, and how do they relate to age?

14.3 Cognitive Processes

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

480 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

and reaction time, particularly through the use of neuroimaging (Linden, 2016; Sugi- ura, 2016). Earlier in this chapter, we considered ways of investigating brain processes through neuroimaging; such research is essential for understanding age-related changes in cognition.

Speed of Processing You are driving home from a friend’s house when all of a sudden a car pulls out of a driveway directly into your path. If you don’t hit the brakes as fast as possible, you will have an accident. How quickly can you move your foot from the accelerator to the brake?

This real-life situation is an example of speed of processing, how quickly and effi- ciently the early steps in information processing are completed. Speed of processing is one of the most investigated phenomena of aging, and hundreds of studies point to the same conclusion: People slow down as they get older. In fact, the slowing-with-age finding is so well documented that many researchers accept it as the only universal behavioral change in aging discovered so far (Salthouse, 2014a, b). However, data show that the rate at which cognitive processes slow down from young adulthood to late life varies a great deal depending on the task (Salthouse, 2014a, b).

The most important reason speed of processing slows down is that older adults take longer to decide that they need to respond, especially when the situation involves ambig- uous information (Salthouse, 2014a, b). Even when the information presented indicates that a response will be needed, there is an orderly slowing of responding with age. As the uncertainty of whether a response is needed increases, older adults become differen- tially slower; the difference in reaction time between older adults and middle-aged adults increases as the uncertainty level increases.

Because the decrease in processing speed is a universal phenomenon, researchers have argued that it may explain a great deal of the age differences in cognition (e.g., Sal- thouse, 2014a, b). Indeed, processing speed is a very good predictor of cognitive perfor- mance, but research has shown that there’s a catch. The prediction is best when the task requires little effort. If the task requires more effort and is more difficult, then working memory (which we consider later) is a better predictor of performance. Also, exercise can mediate the effects of normative aging and improve several aspects of cognition (Dulac & Aubertin-Leheudre, 2016).

speed of processing How quickly and efficiently the early steps in information processing are completed.

Due to sensory and attention changes, older adults are more challenged when driving.

Jo se

L ui

s Pe

la ez

In c.

/B le

nd Im

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 481

Dividing Attention Among Multiple Tasks Adults spend much of their time doing more than one task simultaneously, such as walk- ing and talking on a phone, cooking multiple items for a meal, and driving a car. Such multitasking requires us to spread our attention across all the tasks. Divided attention concerns how well people perform multiple tasks simultaneously. Driving a car is a classic divided attention task—you pay attention to other cars, the gauges in your car, pedestri- ans along the side of the street, and perhaps your passengers as you have a conversation with them.

Although it is widely believed older adults have more trouble than younger adults at dividing attention, it turns out the age differences observed are due to older adults’ difficulties with the individual tasks and not to spreading their attention across them per se (Horwood & Beanland, 2016; Rizzuto, Cherry, & LeDoux, 2012). Observations in the workplace show older workers are just as able to multitask, but they perform each task a bit more slowly than younger workers. However, when the tasks become complex, older adults encounter difficulties dividing their attention and their performance suffers as a result.

Age differences on divided-attention tasks can be minimized if older adults are given training, thereby reducing the demands on attention. Such training can even be through online computer games (Toril et al., 2016; van Muijden, Band, & Hommel, 2012). These results imply that older adults may be able to learn through experience how to divide their attention effectively between tasks.

So, you may ask, when do older adults have difficulty performing multiple tasks simultaneously? You may have observed older adults having difficulty trying to remem- ber something as they are walking down a staircase, or trying to simply walk and talk at the same time. Li and colleagues (Li et al., 2001) found older adults prioritize walk- ing and maintaining balance at the expense of memory. In other words, older adults focused on the task most important to them; walking and balancing to prevent falls. This finding is supported by neurological research (Holtzer et al., 2016). Younger adults, on the other hand, optimized their memory performance and ignored walking and balancing.

Memory Memory is the most important cognitive ability we have. It gives us our identity through the recordings of our past. It gets us through our lives by enabling us to find our way home, to our job, our school, and to so many other places. It enables us to recognize ourselves and loved ones and friends (and to know who we have never met). It provides our vast repertoire of information about everything under the sun. Memory really is the core of our being.

Perhaps that is why we put so much value on maintaining a good memory in old age. In fact, the ability to remember is a common measure of how well we are doing in late life, because older adults are stereotyped as people whose memory is on the decline, people for whom forgetting is not to be taken lightly. Like Rocio in the opening vignette, many people think forgetting to buy a loaf of bread when they are 25 is annoying but otherwise all right, but forgetting it when they are 70 is cause for concern (“Do I have Alzheimer’s disease?”). We will see that forgetting is part of daily life, and the belief it only happens in late life or the fact that it happens in later life automatically means something is seriously awry is wrong. In fact, older adults are quite adept at using strategies in their everyday life contexts to remember what they need to know.

Working Memory One day at work, you suddenly remember that your significant other’s birthday is a week from tomorrow. You decide that a romantic dinner would be a great way to celebrate; so you go online, find a restaurant that is perfect for a romantic dinner, see that you must call  to make a reservation, look at the phone number, pick up your phone, and call.

divided attention the ability of people to perform more than one task simultaneously.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

482 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Remembering the number long enough to dial it successfully requires good working mem- ory. Working memory involves the processes and structures involved in holding informa- tion in mind and simultaneously using it to solve a problem, make a decision, perform some function, or learn new information.

Researchers typically consider working memory an umbrella term for many similar short-term holding and computational processes relating to a wide range of cognitive skills and knowledge domains (Sternberg & Sternberg, 2017). This places working mem- ory right in the thick of things—it plays an active, critical, and central role in encoding, storage, and retrieval of information.

Working memory has a relatively small capacity. Because working memory deals with information that is being used at the moment, it acts as a kind of mental scratch pad or buffer. Unless we take some action to keep the information active (perhaps by rehearsal) or pass it along to long-term storage, the “page” we are using will be filled up quickly; to handle more information, some of the old information must be discarded.

Most evidence indicates there is significant age-related decline in working memory (Heathcote, 2016), although the extent of the decline is still in doubt. Neuroimaging stud- ies reveal why these age differences occur. It turns out that both younger and older adults activate the prefrontal area of their brains (an area behind the forehead) during work- ing memory tasks. But older adults activate more of it on easier tasks; they also exhaust their resources sooner (Cappell, Gmeindl, & Reuter-Lorenz, 2010; Heinzel et al., 2014). In a sense, older adults must devote more “brain power” to working memory compared with younger adults on average; so older adults run out of resources sooner, resulting in poorer performance. However, research shows that older adults can be trained to increase their working memory, which is reflected in more efficient brain processing (Heinzel et al., 2014).

Implicit and Explicit Memory In addition to working memory, we can further divide memory into two other types: implicit memory, the unconscious remembering of information learned at some earlier time, and explicit memory, the deliberate and conscious remembering of information that is learned and remembered at a specific time. Explicit memory is further divided into episodic memory, the general class of memory having to do with the conscious recollection of infor- mation from a specific time or event, and semantic memory, the general class of memory concerning the remembering of meanings of words or concepts not tied to a specific time or event.

Implicit memory is much like getting into a routine—we do things from memory, but we do not have to think about them. For example, the way we brush our teeth tends not to be something we consciously think about at the time. We just remember how to do it. Whether age differences in implicit memory are observed depends on the specific kind of implicit memory task in question (Howard & Howard, 2012, 2013, 2016). For example, learning sequences tend to show age differences, whereas learning spatial context does not.

Much more research has focused on explicit memory, and it is here where consistent age-related differences are observed, although even here there are exceptions (Light, 2012). When we probe a bit deeper, we find that on tests of memory for recall of explicit bits of information, older adults omit more information, remember information that was not actu- ally presented, and repeat more previously recalled items. These age differences have been well documented, are large, and are not reliably lowered by a slower presentation or by cues or reminders given during recall. On recognition tests, age differences are smaller but still present. Older adults also tend to be less efficient at spontaneously using memory strategies to help themselves remember, but they can learn to use such strategies effectively (Berry et al., 2010; Brehmer et al., 2016). In contrast, age differences on semantic memory tasks are typically absent in normative aging but are found in persons with dementia, making this difference one way to diagnose probable cases of abnormal cognitive aging (Grady, 2012).

working memory Processes and structures involved in hold- ing information in mind and simultane- ously using it for other functions.

implicit memory unconscious remembering of information learned at some earlier time.

explicit memory Deliberate and conscious remembering of information that is learned and remem- bered at a specific time.

episodic memory general class of memory having to do with the conscious recollection of information from a specific time or event.

semantic memory general class of memory concerning the remembering of meanings of words or con- cepts not tied to a specific time or event.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 483

A final area of memory research concerns autobiographical memory, memory for events that occur during one’s life. Autobiographical memories tend to be organized according to the periods in one’s life when they occurred such that remembering some events from a particular time period can trigger others (Mace & Clevinger, 2013). When simply asked to remember whatever events they choose, older adults tend to report fewer details than do younger adults (Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2008) and tend to remember more positive than negative events and even put a more positive spin on events once remembered more negatively (Boals, Hayslip, & Banks, 2014).

Neuroimaging studies indicate that when older adults take in and encode information, their prefrontal cortex shows overactivity, indicating that they are attempting to compen- sate for age-related brain changes (Meunier, Stamatakis, & Tyler, 2014). When older adults retrieve information, neuroimaging studies show age-related differences in how the pre- frontal cortex and hippocampus work together (Wang & Giovanello, 2016). This research also indicates age-related compensatory brain activity in older adults for retrieval, similar to that seen in other cognitive processing (Oedekoven et al., 2013). Specifically, younger adults have more extensive neural network connections in the parietal and frontal regions involved in retrieval than do older adults. However, older adults show higher levels of brain activity more generally in these regions, indicating a likely compensatory strategy for less extensive networks.

Overall, these data support the view that older adults process information in their brains differently than younger adults. These differences in part represent attempts at working around, or compensating for, the normal age-related changes that occur in information processing. In the area of memory, though, these compensation attempts are insufficient on their own, but with training they can mitigate some of the losses in performance.

When Is Memory Change Abnormal? As we noted earlier, older adults are sensitive to the stereotype that their forgetfulness is indicative of something much worse. Because people are concerned that memory fail- ures may reflect disease, identifying true cases of memory-impairing disease is extremely important. Differentiating normal and abnormal memory changes is usually accomplished through a wide array of tests that are grounded in the research findings that document the various developmental patterns discussed previously (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Such testing focuses on measuring performance and identifying declines in aspects of memory that typically do not change, such as tertiary memory (which is essentially long-term memory) (Stoner, O’Riley, & Edelstein, 2010).

Even if a decline is identified in an aspect of memory that is cause for concern, it does not automatically follow that there is a serious problem. A first step is to find out whether the memory problem is interfering with everyday functioning. When the memory problem does interfere with functioning, such as not remembering your spouse’s name or how to get home, it is appropriate to suspect a serious, abnormal underlying reason.

Once a serious problem is suspected, the next step is to obtain a thorough examina- tion (Stoner et al., 2010). This should include a complete physical and neurological exami- nation and a complete battery of neuropsychological tests. These may help identify the nature and extent of the underlying problem and provide information about what steps, if any, can be taken to alleviate the difficulties. Neuroimaging can help sort out the specific type of problem or disease the individual may be experiencing.

The most important point to keep in mind is that there is no magic number of times a person must forget something before it becomes a matter for concern. Indeed, many memory-impairing diseases progress slowly, and poor memory performance may only be noticed gradually over an extended period of time. The best course is to have the per- son examined; only with complete and thorough testing can these concerns be checked appropriately.

autobiographical memory Memory for events that occur during one’s life.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

484 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Remediating Memory Problems Remember Rocio, the person in the vignette who had to remember when to take several different medications? In the face of normal age-related declines, how can her problem be addressed?

Support programs can be designed to help people to remember. Sometimes people such as Rocio who are experiencing normal age-related memory changes need extra help because of the high memory demands they face. At other times, people need help because the memory changes they are experiencing are greater than normal.

Camp and colleagues (1993; Camp, 2005; Malone & Camp, 2007) developed the E-I-E-I-O framework to handle both situations. The E-I-E-I-O framework combines two types of memory: explicit and implicit. The framework also includes two types of memory aids. external memory aids are memory aids that rely on environmental resources, such as notebooks and calendars. Internal memory aids are memory aids that rely on mental processes, such as imagery. The “aha” experi- ence that comes with suddenly remembering something (as in, “Oh, now I remem-

ber!”) is the O that follows these Es and Is. As you can see in ◗ Figure 14.11, the E-I-E-I-O framework allows different types of memory to be combined with different types of memory aids to provide a broad range of intervention options to help people remember.

You are probably most familiar with the explicit-external and explicit-internal types of memory aids. Explicit-internal aids such as rehearsal help people remember phone num- bers. Explicit-external aids are used when information needs to be better organized and remembered, such as using a smartphone to remember appointments. Implicit-internal aids represent nearly effortless learning, such as the association between the color of the particular wing of the apartment building one lives in and the fact that one’s residence is there. Implicit-external aids such as icons representing time of day and the number of pills to take help older adults remember their medication.

In general, explicit-external interventions are most frequently used to remediate the kinds of memory problems that older adults face, probably because such methods are easy to use and widely available (Berry et al., 2010; Mercer, 2016). For example, virtually everyone owns a smartphone or an address book in which they store addresses and phone numbers.

Explicit-external interventions have other important applications, too. Ensuring that older adults take the proper medication at the proper time is a problem best solved by an explicit-external intervention: a pillbox that is divided into compartments corresponding to days of the week and different times of the day, which research shows to be the easi- est to load and results in the fewest medication errors (Ownby, Hertzog, & Czaja, 2012). Nursing homes also use explicit-external interventions, such as bulletin boards with the date and weather conditions and activities charts, to help residents keep in touch with current events.

external memory aids Memory aids that rely on environmental resources, such as notebooks and calendars.

internal memory aids Memory aids that rely on mental processes, such as imagery.

External memory aids such as pill organizers help people remember when certain medications need to be taken.

Li sa

F. Y

ou ng

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

●◗ Figure 14.11 The E-I-E-I-O model of memory helps categorize different types of memory aids.

Type of memory Type of memory aid

External Internal

Explicit Appointment book Mental imagery

Grocery list Rote rehearsal

Color-coded maps Spaced retrieval

Sandpaper letters Conditioning

Implicit

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 485

Creativity and Wisdom Two aspects of cognition that have been examined for age-related differences are creativ- ity and wisdom. Each has been the focus of stereotypes: Creativity is assumed to be a func- tion of young people, whereas wisdom is assumed to be the province of older adults. Let’s see whether these views are accurate.

Creativity Researchers define creativity in adults as the ability to produce work that is novel, high in demand, and task appropriate (Kaufman, 2016; Simonton, in press). Creative output, in terms of the number of creative ideas a person has or the major contributions a per- son makes, varies across the adult life span and disciplines (Franses, 2016; Jones, 2010; Kaufman, 2016; Kozbelt & Durmysheva, 2007; Simonton, in press). When considered as a function of age, the overall number of creative contributions a person makes tends to increase through one’s 30s, peak in the late 30s to early 40s, and decline thereafter. A typi- cal life-span trend is shown for painters and composers in ◗ Figure 14.12.

The age-related decline from midlife on does not mean people stop being creative alto- gether, just that they produce fewer creative ideas than when they were younger (Damian & Simonton, 2015). For example, the age when people made major creative contributions, such as research that resulted in winning the Nobel Prize, increased throughout the 20th century (Jones, 2010). And because creativity results from the interaction of cognitive abilities, per- sonality, and developmental forces, all of these continue to operate throughout people’s lives.

Exciting neuroimaging research is supporting previous research that one’s most inno- vative contribution tends to happen most often during the 30s or 40s, as well as showing that creative people’s brains are different. This research shows white matter brain struc- tures that connect brain regions in very different locations, and coordinate the cognitive control of information among them, are related to creativity and are more apparent in cre- ative people (Heilman, 2016; Zhu et al., 2016). Such research has also linked certain brain pathways to actual everyday creative behavior.

As an example of such focused research, additional neuroimaging studies show dif- ferent areas of the prefrontal and parietal areas are responsible for different aspects of creative thinking (Abraham et al., 2012). This research supports the belief creativity

●◗ Figure 14.12 Age at which peak creative output was reached for painters, writers, and composers.

N u

m b

e r

o f

to p

p a in

te rs

, w ri

te rs

a n

d c o

m p

o s e rs

p e a k in

g a

t g

iv e n

a g

e s

15-19 20-24

25-29 30-34

35-39 40-44

45-49 50-54

55-59 60-64

65-69 70-74

75-79 80-84

Age

When We Peak 70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

Based on img.washingtonpost.com/blogs/wonkblog/files/2016/06/creative_age.png&w=1484.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

486 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

involves connecting disparate ideas in new ways, as different areas of the brain are respon- sible for processing different kinds of information. Because white matter tends to change with age, this finding also suggests there are underlying brain maturation reasons why innovative thinking tends to occur most often during late young adulthood and early middle age.

Building upon neuroimaging research, Jung, Flores, and Hunter (2016) developed the Hunter Imagination Questionnaire (HIQ) designed to assess imagination over time in a naturalistic way. Scores on the HIQ have been shown to be related to a wide array of brain volume in areas such as the hippocampus, areas of the frontal lobe, and other brain regions. It will likely be the case that such measures will continue to be refined, and may eventually show utility in helping to identify both high levels of creativity and potential early signs of brain disorders.

Even with this evidence, though, a nagging question remains for many researchers and theorists (e.g., Ericsson, 2014; Gladwell, 2008): Is what the typical person calls creativity nothing more than a high level of expertise that is honed after years of practice? To find out why that question is not yet settled, consider the arguments in the What Do You Think? feature.

Wisdom For thousands of years, cultures around the world greatly admired people who were wise. Based on years of research using in-depth think-aloud interviews with young, middle- aged, and older adults about normal and unusual problems people face, Baltes and col- leagues (Ardelt, 2010; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000; Scheibe, Kunzmann, & Baltes, 2007) describe four characteristics of wisdom:

●● Wisdom deals with important or difficult matters of life and the human condition. ●● Wisdom is truly “superior” knowledge, judgment, and advice. ●● Wisdom is knowledge with extraordinary scope, depth, and balance that is applicable

to specific situations. ●● Wisdom, when used, is well intended and combines mind and virtue (character).

Researchers used this framework to discover that people who are wise are experts in the basic issues in life (Ardelt, 2010; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000). Wise people know a great deal about how to conduct life, how to interpret life events, and what life means. Kunz

Lin-Manuel Miranda’s Hamilton, a paint-ing by Diego Rivera, Stephanie Kwolek’s invention of Kevlar—these and many thou- sands more items in our world are generally described as creative works. For thousands of years, creativity was viewed as the purview of a few people who received inspiration from some special source (Simonton, in press). Creative people were, and still are by many, seen as different—they thought differently, they felt things differently, they did things differently.

By the 20th century, an alternative view became more prominent. That view, expressed clearly through authors and researchers such as Ericsson (2014) and Gladwell (2008), sees creativity as not

something special, but as a trainable skill that is essentially achievable by anyone who puts in the time and practice. From this perspec- tive, what people call creative output is little more than the next logical step in an ongoing process that is the result of expertise, and that is largely predictable. In his book Outliers, Gladwell (2008) made this quite prescriptive with his famous “10,000-Hour Rule,” derived from research by Ericsson, which set the typical amount of practice time it takes to become sufficiently expert to produce “cre- ative” things.

As popular and optimistic as the practice notion of creativity is, however, it is not that simple. Macnamara, Hambrick, and Oswald (2014) conducted a meta-analysis of studies

examining the relation between practice and creativity or expert performance. They found that although practice is certainly an important factor in such performance in a wide range of fields, such as music, sports, and games, it is far from the only factor. Thus, they concluded that practice, while important, does not explain creative or highly expert output.

Does creativity exist? It seems to come down to this. until we have a fuller under- standing of what happens cognitively when a person puts many, often disparate ideas together in a novel way, we are left without a good alternative explanation. Something about that outcome is different. You cannot simply practice and make it happen. What do you think?

Does Creativity Exist?What Do You Think?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 487

(2007) refers to this as the strengths, knowledge, and understanding learned only by living through the earlier stages of life.

Research studies indicate that, contrary to what many people expect, there is no association between age and wisdom (Ardelt, 2010; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000). In fact, the framework on wisdom discussed above has been applied to emerging adults (Booker & Dunsmore, 2016). As envisioned by Baltes and colleagues, whether a person is wise depends on whether he or she has extensive life experience with the type of problem given and has the requisite cognitive abilities and personality. Thus, wisdom could be related to crystallized intelligence, knowledge that builds over time and through experi- ence (Ardelt, 2010).

Culture matters, though, in understanding wisdom. For instance, younger and middle-aged Japanese adults use more wisdom-related reasoning strategies (e.g., recog- nition of multiple perspectives, the limits of personal knowledge, and the importance of compromise) in resolving social conflicts than younger or middle-aged Americans (Grossman et al., 2012). However, older adults in both cultures used similar wisdom- related strategies.

So what specific factors help one become wise? Baltes (1993) identified three factors: (1) general personal conditions such as mental ability; (2) specific expertise conditions such as mentoring or practice; and (3) facilitative life contexts such as education or leadership experience. Personal growth during adulthood, reflecting Erikson’s concepts of generativ- ity and integrity also fosters the process, as do facing and dealing with life crises (Ardelt, 2010). All of these factors take time. Thus, although growing old is no guarantee of wis- dom, it does provide the time, if used well, creates a supportive context for developing wisdom.

Becoming wise is one thing; having one’s wisdom recognized is another. Interestingly, peer ratings of wisdom are better indicators of wisdom than self-ratings (Redzanowski & Glück, 2013). It appears people draw from a wide array of examples of wisdom (Westrate, Ferrari, & Ardelt, 2016) and are better at recognizing wisdom in others than they are in themselves. Perhaps it is better that way.

Interestingly, there is a debate over whether with wisdom comes happiness. There is research evidence that wise people are happier, have better mental health, are humble, and better quality of life (Bergsma & Ardelt, 2012; Etezadi & Pushkar, 2013; Krause, 2016; Thomas et al., 2017). Wise people tend to have higher levels of perceived control over their lives and use problem-focused and positive reappraisal coping strategies more often than people who are not wise. On the other hand, some evidence indicates the attainment of wisdom brings increased distress (Staudinger & Glück, 2011). Perhaps because with the experience that brings wisdom comes an understanding that life does not always work out the way one would like.

recall 1. _______________ refers to how quickly and efficiently the early

steps of information processing are completed.

2. The two types of explicit memory are and _______________.

3. Three factors that help a person become wise are general personal conditions, special expertise conditions, and _______________.

Interpret ●● How would the view that wisdom involves life experience fit

into the discussion of expertise in Chapter 13?

Apply ●● If you were to design a training program for older drivers, what

elements would you include?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 14.3

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

488 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Mary lived by herself for 30 years after her husband died. For all but the last five years or so, she managed very well. Little by little, family members and friends began noticing that Mary wasn’t behaving quite right. For example, her memory slipped, she sounded confused sometimes, and her moods changed without warning. Her appearance deteriorated. Some of her friends attribute these changes to the fact that Mary is in her 80s. But others wonder whether it is something more than normal aging.

Suppose Mary is a relative of yours. How would you deal with the situation? How would you decide whether her behavior is normal? What would you do to try to improve Mary’s life? We’ll consider interventions later in this section that might help Mary if she has a serious disease such as Alzheimer’s.

Every day, families turn to mental health professionals for help in dealing with the psychological problems of their aging relatives. Unfortunately, myths interfere with appro- priate mental health diagnoses and interventions for older adults. For example, many peo- ple mistakenly believe that nearly all older adults are depressed, demented, or both. When they observe older adults behaving in these ways, they take no action because they believe that nothing can be done.

In this section, we will see that such beliefs are wrong. Only a minority of older adults have mental health problems, and most of these problems respond to therapy. Sometimes these problems manifest themselves differently in younger and older adults, so we need to know what to look for. Accurate diagnosis is essential. Let’s examine some of the most commonly occurring and widely known disorders: depression, anxiety disorders, and Alzheimer’s disease.

Depression Most people feel down or sad from time to time, perhaps in reaction to a problem at work or in one’s relationships. But does this mean that most people are depressed? How is depression diagnosed? Are there age-related differences in the symptoms examined in diagnosis? How is depression treated?

First of all, let’s dispense with a myth. Contrary to the popular belief that most older adults are depressed, for healthy people, the rate of severe depression declines from young adulthood to old age as shown in ◗ Figure 14.13; the average age of onset is 32 (National Institute of Mental Health, 2016a). However, this downward age trend does not hold in all cultures; for example, depressive symptoms among Chinese older adults rose over a 24-year period (1987–2010, inclusive) (Shao et al., 2013). For those people who do experi- ence depression, let’s examine its diagnosis and treatment.

How Is Depression Diagnosed in Older Adults? Depression in later life is usually diagnosed on the basis of two clusters of symptoms that must be present for at least two weeks: feelings and physical changes. As with younger peo- ple, the most prominent symptom of depression in older adults is feeling sad or down, termed dysphoria. But whereas younger people are likely to label these feelings directly as “feeling depressed,” older adults may refer to them as “feeling helpless” or in terms of physical health such as “feeling tired” (NIHSeniorHealth, 2016; Segal, Qualls, & Smyer, 2011). Older adults are also more likely than younger people to appear apathetic and expressionless, to

dysphoria feeling sad or down.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● How does depression in older adults differ from depres-

sion in younger adults? How is it diagnosed and treated?

●● How are anxiety disorders treated in older adults?

●● What is Alzheimer’s disease? How is it diagnosed and managed? What causes it?

14.4 Mental Health and Intervention

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 489

confine themselves to bed, to neglect themselves, and to make derogatory statements about themselves.

The second cluster of symptoms includes physical changes such as loss of appetite, insomnia, and trouble breathing (NIHSeniorHealth, 2016; Segal et al., 2011). In young people, these symptoms usually indicate an underlying psychological problem, but in older adults, they may simply reflect normal, age-related changes. Thus, older adults’ physical symptoms of depression must be evaluated very carefully (Chew-Graham & Ray, 2016).

An important step in diagnosis is ruling out other possible causes of the symptoms. For example, underlying health problems that appear as depression include vitamin defi- ciencies (e.g., B12), diabetes, cardiovascular disease, chronic pain, hypothyroidism, cancer, Cushing’s syndrome, certain viruses, dementia, Parkinson’s disease, rheumatoid arthritis, and medication interactions and side effects (Chew-Graham & Ray, 2016).

Finally, it is important to establish whether the symptoms interfere with daily life. Can the person carry out normal responsibilities at home? How well does he or she interact with others? There must be clear evidence that daily life is affected.

What Causes Depression? There are two main schools of thought about the causes of depression. One focuses on bio- logical and physiological processes, particularly on imbalances of specific neurotransmitters, genetic predisposition, and changes in certain types of brain cells (Ménard, Hodes, & Russo, 2016). Research evidence indicates that the most likely cause of severe depression in later life is an imbalance in neurotransmitters such as low levels of serotonin and the action of brain- derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; Hashimoto, 2013). The general view that severe depression has a biochemical basis underlies current approaches to drug therapies, discussed a little later.

The second view focuses on psychosocial factors, such as loss and internal belief sys- tems. Although several types of loss or negative events have been associated with depres- sion—including loss of a spouse, a job, or one’s health—it is how a person interprets a loss,

●◗ Figure 14.13 Twelve-month prevalence of major depressive episode among u.S. adults (2014).

Adapted from National Institute of Mental Health (2013). 12-Month Prevalence of Major Depressive Episode Among U.S. Adults. Retrieved from www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/prevalence/major-depression-among-adults.shtml.

14

12

10

8

6

4

2

0 Hispanic

White Black

Asian NH/OPI*

AI/AN**

2 or more

Female

Overall

Male 18–25

26–49 50+

12-month Prevalence of Major Depressive Episodes Among U.S. Adults (2015) P

e rc

e n

t

*NH/OPI = Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander ** AI/AN = American Indian/Alaska Native

10.3

6.7

8.5

4.7 4.8 4.8

7.5

4.9 4.1

5.2

8.9

12.2 Sex Age group Race

7.5

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

490 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

rather than the event itself, that causes depression (Segal et al., 2011). In this approach, internal belief systems, or what one tells oneself about why certain things are happening, are emphasized as the cause of depression. For example, experiencing an unpredictable and uncontrollable event such as the death of a spouse may cause depression if you believe it happened because you are a bad person (Beck, 1967). People who are depressed tend to believe that they are personally responsible for all the bad things that happen to them, that things are unlikely to get better, and that their whole life is a shambles.

How Is Depression Treated in Older Adults? Regardless of how severe depression is, people benefit from treatment, often through a combination of medication and psychotherapy (Segal et al., 2011). Most medications used to treat depression work by altering the balance of specific neurotransmitters in the brain (Jainer et al. 2013). For very severe cases of depression, medications such as selective sero- tonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), heterocyclic antidepressants (HCAs), or monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitors can be administered (National Institute of Mental Health, 2016b). SSRIs are the medication of first choice because they have the lowest overall side effects of any antidepressant. SSRIs work by boosting the level of serotonin, which is a neurotransmitter involved in regulating moods.

Either as an alternative to medication or in conjunction with it, psychotherapy is also a popular approach to treating depression. Two forms of psychotherapy have been shown to be effective with older adults. The basic idea in behavior therapy is that depressed people experience too few rewards or reinforcements from their environment. Thus, the goal of behavior therapy is to increase the good things that happen and minimize the negative things (Lewinsohn, 1975). The net increase in positive events and net decrease in negative events comes about through practice and homework assignments during the course of therapy, such as going out more or joining a club to meet new people.

A second effective approach is cognitive therapy, which is based on the idea that mal- adaptive beliefs or cognitions about oneself are responsible for depression. From this per- spective, those who are depressed view themselves as unworthy and inadequate, the world as insensitive and ungratifying, and the future as bleak and unpromising (Beck et al., 1979). In a cognitive therapy session, a person is taught how to recognize these thoughts and to reevaluate the self, the world, and the future more positively, resulting in a change in the underlying beliefs. Cognitive behavior therapy is especially effective for older adults (Carr  & McNulty, 2016; Jeste & Palmer, 2013). This is good news, because medications may not be as effective or as tolerated by older adults because of age-related changes in metabolism.

The most important fact to keep in mind about depression is that it is treatable. Thus, if an older person behaves in ways that indicate depression, it is a good idea to have him or her examined by a mental health professional. Even if the malady turns out not to be depression, another underlying and possibly treatable condition may be uncovered. A major healthcare problem in the United States is that less than 40% of adults of all ages receive minimally adequate treatment for depression (National Institute of Mental Health, 2016a).

Anxiety Disorders Imagine you are about to give a speech to an audience of several hundred people. During the last few minutes before you begin, you start to feel nervous, your heart begins to pound, your mouth gets dry, and your palms get sweaty. These feelings, common even to veteran speakers, are similar to those experienced more frequently and intensely by people with anxiety disorders.

Anxiety disorders involve excessive, irrational dread in everyday situations and include problems such as feelings of severe anxiety for no apparent reason, phobias with regard to specific things or places, and obsessions or compulsions in which thoughts or actions are performed repeatedly (National Institute of Mental Health, 2016c, d).

internal belief systems view of a cause of depression of what one tells oneself about why certain things are happening.

behavior therapy type of therapy based on the notion that depressed people experience too few rewards or reinforcements from their environment.

cognitive therapy type of therapy based on the idea that mal- adaptive beliefs or cognitions about oneself are responsible for depression.

anxiety disorders Problems such as feelings of severe anxi- ety, phobias, and obsessive-compulsive behaviors.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 491

The prevalence of anxiety disorders in adults of all ages is about 18%, but the lifetime prevalence is about twice as high in young and middle-aged adults as it is in older adults. Overall, women are 60% more likely than men to experience an anxiety disorder over their lifetime (National Institute of Mental Health, 2016e). The reasons for this gender differ- ence are unknown.

Common to all the anxiety disorders are physical changes that interfere with social functioning, personal relationships, or work. These physical changes include dry mouth, sweating, dizziness, upset stomach, diarrhea, insomnia, hyperventilation, chest pain, choking, frequent urination, headaches, and a sensation of a lump in the throat (Segal et al., 2011). These symptoms occur in adults of all ages, but they are particularly common in older adults because of loss of health, relocation stress, isolation, fear of losing control over their lives, or guilt resulting from feelings of hostility toward family and friends.

An important issue concerning anxiety disorders in older adults is that anxiety may be an appropriate response to the situation. For example, helplessness anxiety is generated by a potential or actual loss of control or mastery (Varkal et al., 2013). For example, a study in Turkey showed that older adults are anxious about their memory, reflecting at least in part a realistic assessment of normative, age-related decline.

In addition, a series of severe negative life experiences may result in a person’s reach- ing the breaking point and appearing highly anxious. Many older adults who show symp- toms of anxiety disorder have underlying health problems that may be responsible for the symptoms. In all cases, the anxious behavior should be investigated first as an appropriate response that may not warrant medical intervention. The important point is to evaluate the older adult’s behavior in context.

These issues make it difficult to diagnose anxiety disorders, especially in older adults (Carr & McNulty, 2016; Segal et al., 2011). The problem is that there usually is nothing spe- cific that a person can point to as the specific trigger or cause. In addition, anxiety in older adults often accompanies an underlying physical disorder or illness.

Anxiety disorders can be treated with medication and psychotherapy (Carr & McNulty, 2016; Chew-Graham & Ray, 2016). The most commonly used medications are benzodiazepine (e.g., Valium® and Librium®), SSRIs (Paxil®, among others), buspirone, and beta-blockers. Although moderately effective, these drugs must be monitored carefully in older adults because the amount needed to treat the disorder is very low and the potential for harmful side effects is great. For older adults, the clear treatment of choice is psycho- therapy, specifically cognitive behavioral or relaxation therapy, especially when anxiety disorders first occur in later life (Carr & McNulty, 2016; Hendriks et al., 2012). Relaxation therapy is exceptionally effective, easily learned, and presents a technique that is useful in many situations (e.g., falling asleep at night; Segal et al., 2011). The advantage of these psy- chotherapeutic techniques is they usually involve only a few sessions, have high rates of success, and offer clients proce- dures they can take with them.

Dementia Probably no other condition associated with aging is more feared than the family of disorders known as dementia. In dementia individuals can literally lose their personal identity through the loss of autobiographical memory and the ability even to recognize one’s spouse and children. Dementias serious enough to impair inde- pendent functioning affect nearly 48 million people glob- ally (World Health Organization, 2015). The Alzheimer’s Association (2016a) reports that dementia costs the United States roughly $236 billion annually, and 15 mil- lion caregivers provide well over 18 billion hours of unpaid care each year.

Alzheimer’s disease involves memory loss to an extent that may include forgetting the names of family members.

A la

n O

dd ie

/P ho

to Ed

it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

492 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Dementia is not a specific disease but rather a family of diseases characterized by cog- nitive and behavioral deficits involving some form of permanent damage to the brain. The most common and widely known of these is Alzheimer’s disease, but others are important as well: vascular dementia, Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, alcoholic dementia, Lewy body, and AIDS dementia complex.

Alzheimer’s Disease Alzheimer’s disease is the most common form of progressive, degenerative, and fatal dementia, accounting for between 60% and 80% of all cases of dementia (Alzheimer’s Association, 2016c). The prevalence increases with age, rising from extremely low rates in the 50s to about half of all people aged 85 and older. As the number of older adults increases rapidly over the next several decades, the number of cases is expected to roughly triple.

What Are the Symptoms of Alzheimer’s Disease? The key symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease are gradual declines in memory, learning, attention, and judgment; confusion as to time and place; difficulties in communicating and finding the right words; decline in personal hygiene and self-care skills; inappropriate social behavior; and changes in personality. These classic symptoms may be vague and may occur only occasionally in the beginning with little behavioral impact, but as the disease progresses, the symptoms become more pronounced and are exhibited more regularly (Alzheimer’s Association, 2016b, d). Wan- dering away from home and not being able to remember how to return increases. Delu- sions, hallucinations, and other related behaviors develop and get worse over time. Spouses become strangers. Patients may not even recognize themselves in a mirror; they wonder who is looking back at them. In its advanced stages, Alzheimer’s disease often causes incontinence, the loss of control of bladder or bowels. It may also result in a total loss of mobility.

People with Alzheimer’s disease may become completely dependent on others for care. At this point, many caregivers seek facilities such as adult day-care centers and other sources of help, such as family and friends, to provide a safe environment for the Alzheim- er’s patient while the primary caregiver is at work or needs to run basic errands.

The rate of deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease varies widely from one person to the next (Gandy & DeKosky, 2013). Alzheimer’s disease has an average duration of 9 years (but can range anywhere from 1 to over 20 years) from the onset of noticeable symptoms through death (Alzheimer’s Association, 2016e). The early stage is marked especially by memory loss, disorientation to time and space, poor judgment, and personality changes. The middle stage is characterized by increased memory problems, increased difficulties with speech, restlessness, irritability, and loss of impulse control. People in the late stage of Alzheimer’s disease experience incontinence of urine and feces, lose motor skills, have decreased appetite, have great difficulty with speech and language, may not recognize family members or oneself in a mirror, lose most if not all self-care abilities, and decreased ability to fight off infections. These stages are depicted in ◗ Figure 14.14.

How Is Alzheimer’s Disease Diagnosed? Given that the behavioral symptoms of Alzheim- er’s disease eventually become quite obvious, one would assume that diagnosis would be straightforward. Quite the contrary. Accurate early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease depends on a thorough assessment of the number and severity of neurological and behav- ioral changes (Ismail et al., 2016).

For an early diagnosis to be accurate, however, it must be comprehensive and broad. ◗ Figure 14.15 provides an overview of the process used to differentiate Alzheimer’s dis- ease from other conditions. Note a great deal of the diagnostic effort goes into ruling out other possible causes for the observed cognitive deficits: All possible treatable causes for the symptoms must be eliminated before a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease can be made.

A great deal of attention has been given to the development of more definitive tests for Alzheimer’s disease while the person is still alive. Much of this work has focused on beta- amyloid, a protein that is produced in abnormally high levels in persons with Alzheimer’s

dementia family of diseases involving serious impair- ment of behavioral and cognitive function- ing and some form of permanent damage to the brain.

Alzheimer’s disease Disease marked by gradual declines in memory, attention, and judgment; confu- sion as to time and place; difficulties in communicating; decline in self-care skills; inappropriate behavior; and personality changes.

incontinence loss of bladder or bowel control.

Think About It How do the memory problems in Alzheimer’s disease differ from those in normal aging?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 493

disease, perhaps causing the neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques described earlier. Considerable recent research has focused on beta-amyloid as a major factor in Alzheim- er’s disease in terms of potential diagnosis as well as a possible cause and avenue for treat- ment. The role of beta-amyloid is controversial, though (Molin & Rockwood, 2016). As we will see in the next section, some researchers view concentration of beta-amyloid as a bio- marker of Alzheimer’s disease, whereas others caution that many people with increased levels of beta amyloid do not develop Alzheimer’s disease.

What Causes Alzheimer’s Disease? We do not know for sure what causes Alzheimer’s disease (Scheff, Neltner, & Nelson, 2014). What we do know is that early-onset (before age 65) forms of Alzheimer’s disease are mostly caused by single-gene mutations related to beta-amyloid protein production (Nicolas et al., 2016). Single-gene mutation forms of Alzheimer’s disease usually involve mutations in the presenillin-1 (PSEN1), presenillin-2 (PSEN2) and amyloid (A4) precursor protein (APP) genes. The genetic inheritance aspect of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is a major concern of families.

Other genetic links to later-onset (after age 65) Alzheimer’s disease involve the complex interaction of several genes, processes that are not yet well documented or under- stood. Several sites on various chromosomes have been tentatively identified as being potentially involved in the transmission of Alzheimer’s disease, including chromosomes 1, 12, 14, 19, and 21. The most promising work noted links between the genetic markers and the production of beta-amyloid protein, the major component of neuritic plaques (as noted earlier). Much of this research focuses on apolipoprotein «4 (APOE-«4), associ- ated with chromosome 19, that may play a central role in creating neuritic plaques. People with the APOE-«4 trait are more likely to get Alzheimer’s disease than those with the more common APOE-«3 trait (Di Battista, Heinsinger, & Rebeck, 2016). Additionally, a related mutation (TREM2) may be involved with APOE-«4 as well by interfering with the brain’s ability to contain inflammation (Jonsson et al., 2013; Sala Frigerio & De Strooper, 2016).

●◗ Figure 14.14 Progression of Alzheimer’s disease through the brain.

Plaques and tangles (shown in the blue- shaded areas) tend to spread through the cortex in a predictable pattern as Alzheimer’s disease progresses. The rate of progression varies greatly. People with Alzheimer’s live an average of eight years, but some people may survive up to 20 years. The course of the disease depends in part on age at diagnosis and whether a person has other health conditions.

Earliest Alzheimer’s Changes may begin 20 years or more before diagnosis.

Mild to moderate Alzheimer’s stages Generally last from 2 to 10 years

Severe Alzheimer’s May last from 1 to 5 years.

Progression of Alzheimer’s Through the Brain

From Alzheimer’s Association. (2016). Progression through the brain. Retrieved from www.alz.org/braintour/early_ stage.asp (Slide 13).

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Think About It If an accurate diagnostic test for Alzheim- er’s disease is developed and there is no treatment for the disease, should the test be made available?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

494 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

●◗ Figure 14.15 Differential diagnosis in Alzheimer’s disease algorithm.

Progressive change in memory or function

Dementia

Medication review

Progressive change in memory or function

CT or MRI/Optional*

Depressed mood

Alzheimer’s disease

Refer for assessment

Medication-induced dementia Reduce/change medication

Hypothyroidism B12 deficiency Systemic illness

Vascular dementia Hydrocephalus Tumors Subdural hematoma

Depression–Treat

Atypical disorders**

MSE: AHRQ guidelines PSMS

DSM-IV criteria AHRQ guidelines

AHRQ guidelines

AAN guidelines

NINDS criteria

DSM-IV criteria AHRQ guidelines

NINCDS-ADRDA criteria

If no improvement in cognition after treatment

If no improvement in cognition after treatment

If no improvement in cognition

Minor vascular changes

Yes

Abnormal

Abnormal

TOOLBOX INSTRUMENTS

It is required in patients with focal signs, rapid progression, and headache.

This category will contain rare dementias (e.g., frontotemporal degenerations, Jakob-Creutzfeldt disease, Parkingon’s disease [and other movement disorders that present with dementias] ) that should be considered when unusual clinical features are present or a rapidly progressive course is noted.

*

**

From Alzheimer’s Association online document. www.alz.org/medical/rtalgrthm.htm. Developed and endorsed by the TriAD Advisory Board. Copy- right 1996 Pfizer Inc. and Esai Inc. with special thanks to J. L. Cummings. Algorithm reprinted from TriAD, Three for the Management of Alzheimer’s Disease, with permission.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 495

Interestingly, another version, APOE-«2, seems to have the reverse effect from APOE-«4: It decreases the risk of Alzheimer’s disease (Liu et al., 2013). Despite the rela- tion between APOE-«4 and neuritic plaques, and between APOE-«4 and beta-amyloid buildup, researchers have yet to establish strong relations directly between apolipoprotein « and general cognitive functioning (Wu & Zhao, 2016).

A mutation on the ABCA7 gene has been identified that roughly doubles the chances for African Americans to get late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Cukier et al., 2016). This mutation creates overproduction of cholesterol and lipids, which in turn are known to be risks for cardiovascular disease and strokes, both of which have higher rates in African Americans.

Much of the genetics and related biomarker research focuses on beta-amyloid and its proposed relation to Alzheimer’s disease reviewed earlier. When viewed as a cause of Alzheimer’s disease, researchers refer to the beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis as the pro- cess by which this occurs (Selkoe, 2016). The beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis refers to the process by which beta-amyloid deposits create neuritic plaques, that in turn lead to neurofibrillary tangles, that cause neuronal death and, when this occurs severely enough, Alzheimer’s disease. As noted earlier, there is considerable evidence beta-amyloid is involved in Alzheimer’s disease. Although there is insufficient evidence at this point to conclude it is the main cause, data are quite clear that it is at least an initiating factor ( Selkoe, 2016).

What Can Be Done for Victims of Alzheimer’s Disease? Currently there is no effective treatment for Alzheimer’s disease and no way to prevent it. The best we can do today is alleviate some of the symptoms. Most of the research is focused on drugs aimed at improving cognitive functioning. Unfortunately, the medications approved by the Food and Drug Administration to date provide little relief over the long run, and few medica- tions in development show promising results.

To date, the most effective interventions for Alzheimer’s disease are behavioral strate- gies; these approaches are recommended over medications because they give better and more effective outcomes (Barton et al., 2016). These strategies can be used from the time of initial diagnosis throughout the duration of the disease. Behavioral strategies range from simple interventions, such as large calendars to help with orientation to time, to the more complex, such as more elaborate memory interventions based on the E-I-E-I-O approach discussed earlier in the section on memory.

In designing interventions for those with Alzheimer’s disease, the guiding principle should be optimizing the person’s functioning. Regardless of the level of impairment, attempts should be made to help the person cope as well as possible with the symp- toms. The key is helping all individuals maintain their dignity as human beings. This can be achieved in some very creative ways, such as adapting the principles of Montessori methods of education to bring older adults with Alzheimer’s disease together with pre- school children so that they can perform tasks together (Malone & Camp, 2007; Materne, Luszcz, & Goodwin-Smith, 2014). One example of this approach is discussed in the Spot- light on Research feature.

Parkinson’s Disease Parkinson’s disease is known primarily for its characteristic motor symptoms: very slow walking, difficulty getting into and out of chairs, and a slow hand tremor, but it can develop into a form of dementia. These problems are caused by a deterioration of neurons in the midbrain that produce the neurotransmitter dopamine. Former boxing champion Muhammad Ali (who died in 2016) and actor Michael J. Fox are some of the more famous individuals who have Parkinson’s disease. Over 1 million people in the United States (over 4 million globally) have Parkinson’s disease (Parkinson’s Disease Foundation, 2016a).

Symptoms are treated effectively with several medications (Parkinson’s Disease Foun- dation, 2016b); the most popular are levodopa, which raises the functional level of dopa- mine in the brain; Sinemet® (a combination of levodopa and carbidopa), that gets more levodopa to the brain; and Stalevo® (a combination of Sinemet® and entacapone), that

beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis theory of alzheimer’s disease in which beta-amyloid deposits create neuritic plaques, that in turn lead to neurofibril- lary tangles, that cause neuronal death and alzheimer’s disease.

Parkinson’s disease brain disease known primarily for its char- acteristic motor symptoms: very slow walk- ing, difficulty getting into and out of chairs, and a slow hand tremor, but it can develop into a form of dementia.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

496 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTe) form of dementia caused by repeated head trauma such as concussions.

extends the effective dosage time of Sinemet®. Research indicates a device called a neuro- stimulator, that acts like a brain pacemaker by regulating brain activity when implanted deep inside the brain, may prove effective in significantly reducing the tremors, shaking, rigidity, stiffness, and walking problems when medications.

For reasons we do not yet understand, some people with Parkinson’s disease also develop severe cognitive impairment and eventually dementia (Zheng et al., 2014). Lewy bodies characterize dementia in Parkinson’s disease; the key factor in the latter case is that the dementia component occurs later.

Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy An issue of growing concern is the long-term effects of experiencing brain concussions, or traumatic brain injury (TBI; White & Venkatesh, 2016). Two situations brought TBI to the forefront: war veterans and sports injuries (Finkbeiner et al., 2016; Schulz-Helk et al., 2016). Estimates are about 350,000 U.S. veterans have been diagnosed with TBI since 2000 (Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center, 2016). Regarding sports, a significant num- ber of athletes who received numerous concussions during their playing careers have evi- dence of chronic traumatic encephalopathy.

Chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTe) is a form of dementia caused by repeated head trauma such as concussions. CTE can occur as the result of repeated brain trauma (Sorg et al., 2014). Emerging evidence shows that irrespective of the cause, there is

On ResearchSpotlight

Training Persons with Dementia to Be Group Activity Leaders

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Dementia is marked by progressive and severe cogni- tive decline. But despite these losses, can people with dementia be trained to be group leaders? Most people might think the answer is no, but Cameron Camp, Michael Skrajner, and Marty Kelly (2005) decided to find out by using a training technique based on the Montessori method.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? The Montessori method is based on self-paced learning and develop- mentally appropriate activities. As Camp and Skrajner point out, many techniques used in rehabilitation (e.g., task breakdown, guided repetition, moving from simple to complex and concrete to abstract) and in intervention programs for people with dementia (e.g., use of external cues and implicit memory) are consistent with the Montessori method.

For this study, a program was developed to train group leaders for memory bingo (see Camp, 1999a and 1999b, for details about this game). Group leaders had to learn which cards to pick for the game, where the answers were located on the card, where

to “discard” the used (but not the winning) cards, and where to put the winning cards. Success in the program was measured by research staff raters, who made ratings of the type and quality of engagement in the task shown by the group leader.

Who were the participants in the study? Camp and colleagues tested four people who had been diagnosed as probably having dementia who were also residents of a special care unit of a nursing home.

What was the design of the study? The study used a longitudinal design so that Camp and Skrajner could track participants’ performance over several weeks.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? Having persons with dementia as research participants raises important issues regarding informed consent. Because of their serious cognitive impairments, these individuals may not fully understand the procedures. Thus, family members such as a spouse or adult child caregiver are also asked to give informed consent. Additionally, researchers must pay careful attention to participants’ emotions; if participants become agitated or frustrated, the training or testing session must be stopped. Camp and Skrajner took all these precautions.

What were the results? Results showed that at least partial adherence to the

established game protocols was achieved at a very high rate. Indeed, staff assistance was not required at all for most of the game sessions for any leader. All of the leaders said that they enjoyed their role, and one recruited another resident to become a leader in the next phase of the project.

What did the investigators conclude? It appears that persons with dementia can be taught to be group activity leaders through a procedure based on the Montessori method. This is important because it pro- vides a way for such individuals to become more engaged in an activity and to be more productive.

Although more work is needed to continue refining the technique, applications of the Montessori method offer a promising inter- vention approach for people with cognitive impairments.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Camp and Skrajner studied only four residents; more evidence that the approach works with dif- ferent types of people would bolster their conclusions. Although the Montessori method is effective for training persons with dementia, the approach has not yet been demonstrated to be effective with other diseases that cause serious memory loss.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 497

structural damage to various parts of the brain that have to do with executive functions and memory.

Because of the concern over CTE, the National Institute of Neurological Disease and Stroke (NINDS) and the National Institute of Biomedical and Bioengineering established specific neuropathological criteria for the diagnosis of CTE (McKee et al., 2016). These criteria, established following the death of the individual, can now be used to definitively diagnose cases of CTE.

Clearly, there is more awareness of the problems associated with repeated TBI. Whether sports, or at least certain sports involving physical contact, should now be con- sidered dangerous remains to be seen. What is certain, though, is the effects of repeated TBI last well into adulthood, and can cause serious cognitive impairment, and perhaps death, even at a relatively early age.

recall 1. Compared with younger adults, older adults are less likely to

label their feelings of sadness as_______________

2. A form of psychotherapy that focuses on people’s beliefs about the self, the world, and the future is called_______________

3. Relaxation techniques are an effective therapy for_______________

4. Early onset Alzheimer’s disease is caused by_______________

5. _______________is characterized by several characteristic motor symptoms.

Interpret ●● After reading about the symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease,

what would be the most stressful aspects of caring for a par- ent who has the disease? (You may want to refer to the sec- tion on caring for aging parents in Chapter 13.)

Apply ●● If a friend asked you the difference between dementia and the

normative increases in forgetting that occur with age, what would you tell him or her?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 14.4

SUMMARY

14.1 What Are Older Adults Like?

What are the characteristics of older adults in the population?

●● The number of older adults is growing rapidly, especially the number of people over age 85. In the future, older adults will be more ethnically diverse and better educated than they are now.

How long will most people live? What factors influence this?

●● Average longevity or life expectancy has increased dramati- cally over the past century, due mainly to improvements in health care. Maximum longevity or life expectancy is the longest time any human can live. Active life expectancy refers to the number of years that a person is free from debilitating disease; dependent life expectancy also con- siders the length of time a person experiences debilitating chronic disease.

●● Genetic factors that can influence longevity include familial longevity and a family history of certain diseases. Environ- mental factors include acquired diseases, toxins, pollutants, and lifestyle.

●● Women have a longer average life expectancy at birth than men. Ethnic group differences are complex; depending on how old people are, the patterns of differences change.

What is the distinction between the Third Age and the Fourth Age?

●● The Third Age refers to changes in research that led to cultural, medical, and economic advances for older adults (e.g., longer average longevity, increased quality of life). In contrast, the Fourth Age reflects that the oldest-old are at the limits of their functional capacity, the rates of diseases such as cancer and dementia increase dramatically, and other aspects of psychological functioning (e.g., memory) undergo significant and fairly rapid decline.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

498 PA rT F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

14.2 Physical Changes and Health

What are the major biological theories of aging?

●● There are several theories of biological aging. Metabolic the- ories focus on the rate of metabolism. Cellular theories focus on limits on cell division and the role played by telomeres, the tips of chromosomes that play a role in aging by adjust- ing the cell’s response to stress and growth stimulation and by shortening with each cell replication, and on free radicals, unstable highly reactive chemicals produced in metabolism. Genetic programming theories are based on ideas that pro- pose aging as programmed into the genetic code.

What physiological changes normally occur in later life?

●● Three important structural changes in the neurons are neu- rofibrillary tangles, dendritic changes, and neuritic plaques caused by beta-amyloid. These have important conse- quences for functioning because they reduce the effective- ness with which neurons transmit information.

●● Neuroimaging provides significant insights into aging. Techniques include structural neuroimaging (e.g., X-ray, CT, and MRI scans) and functional neuroimaging (e.g., PET, SPECT, and fMRI).

●● The risk of cardiovascular disease increases with age. Nor- mal changes in the cardiovascular system include buildup of fat deposits in the heart and arteries (atherosclerosis), a decrease in the amount of blood the heart can pump, a decline in heart muscle tissue, and stiffening of the arteries. Transient ischemic attacks and vascular dementia are pos- sible results. Most of these changes are affected by lifestyle. Stroke causes significant cognitive impairment depending on the location of the brain damage.

●● Strictly age-related changes in the respiratory system are hard to identify because of the lifetime effects of pollution. However, older adults may suffer shortness of breath and face an increased risk of chronic obstructive pulmonary disorder (COPD).

●● Age-related declines in vision and hearing are well docu- mented, and include presbyopia, glaucoma, diabetic reti- nopathy, and age-related macular degeneration. The main changes in vision concern the structure of the eye and the retina. Changes in hearing mainly involve presbycusis, reduced sensitivity to high-pitched tones.

What are the principal health issues for older adults?

●● Older adults experience higher rates of chronic diseases. Diabetes (Type 1 and Type 2) pose several risks. The risk of cancer is much higher in older adults. Several risk genes have been identified, especially for breast and prostate cancers.

●● Older adults have more sleep disturbances than do younger adults. Nutritionally, most older adults do not need vitamin or mineral supplements. Diabetes mellitus is a significant problem for many older adults. Cancer risk increases sharply with age.

14.3 Cognitive Processes

What changes occur in information processing as people age? How do these changes relate to everyday life?

●● Older adults’ speed of processing is slower than younger adults’. However, the amount of slowing is lessened if older adults have practice or expertise in the task.

●● Older adults are generally less able to perform divided attention tasks. Sensory and information-processing changes create problems for older drivers.

What changes occur in memory with age? What can be done to remediate these changes?

●● Working memory declines with age. Older adults typically do worse on tests of episodic recall; age differences are less on recognition tasks. Semantic and implicit memory are both largely unaffected by aging.

●● Distinguishing memory changes associated with aging from memory changes due to disease should be accom- plished through comprehensive evaluations. Neuroimag- ing and careful and thorough diagnosis can help make this distinction.

●● Memory training can be achieved in many ways. A useful framework is to combine explicit-implicit memory distinc- tions with external-internal types of memory aids.

What are creativity and wisdom, and how do they relate to age?

●● Research indicates that creative output peaks in late young adulthood or early middle age and declines thereafter, but the point of peak activity varies across disciplines and occupations.

●● Wisdom has more to do with being an expert in living than with age per se. Three factors that help people become wise are personal attributes, specific expertise, and facilitative life contexts.

14.4 Mental Health and Intervention

How does depression in older adults differ from depression in younger adults? How is it diagnosed and treated?

●● The key symptom of depression is persistent sadness. Other psychological and physical symptoms also occur, but the importance of these depends on the age of the person reporting them. Older and younger adults differ significantly in how they describe their symptoms and experiences.

●● Major causes of depression include imbalances in neu- rotransmitters and psychosocial forces such as loss and internal belief systems.

●● Depression can be treated with medications and through psychotherapy such as behavioral or cognitive therapy.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C H A P T e r 1 4 : T H E P E R S O N A L C O N T E x T O F   L AT E R L I F E 499

How are anxiety disorders treated in older adults?

●● Many older adults are afflicted with a variety of anxi- ety disorders. All of them can be effectively treated with either medications or psychotherapy, especially relaxation techniques.

What is Alzheimer’s disease? How is it diagnosed and man- aged? What causes it?

●● Dementia is a family of diseases that causes severe cognitive impairment. Alzheimer’s disease is the most common form of irreversible dementia.

●● Symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease include memory impair- ment, personality changes, and behavioral changes. These symptoms usually worsen gradually, with rates varying con- siderably among individuals.

●● Definitive diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease can only be made only after very careful and thorough testing,

including biomarker and genetic marker screening. The diagnostic categories of Alzheimer’s disease remain controversial.

●● Most researchers are focusing on a probable genetic cause of Alzheimer’s disease. Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is caused by single-gene mutations; later onset is likely related to various marker or risk genes.

●● Although Alzheimer’s disease is incurable, various behav- ioral interventions may improve the quality of the patient’s life.

●● Parkinson’s disease is usually marked by tremors and dif- ficulty walking, but in many cases, it also develops into a form of dementia.

●● Chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE) is of growing concern for individuals who experience repeated head trauma.

Test Yourself Recall Answers 14.1 1. 85 2. average life expectancy 3. oldest 14.2 1. telomeres 2. beta-amyloid 3. increases 14.3 1. Speed of processing 2. episodic and semantic 3. facilitative life contexts 14.4 1. depression 2. cognitive therapy 3. anxiety disorders 4. single-gene mutations 5. Parkinson’s disease

Key Terms demographers (458) population pyramid (458) average longevity (life expectancy) (461) maximum longevity (life expectancy) (461) active life expectancy (462) dependent life expectancy (462) telomeres (466) telomerase (466) free radicals (466) neurofibrillary tangles (468) beta-amyloid (468) neurotransmitters (469) structural neuroimaging (470) functional neuroimaging (470) atherosclerosis (470) strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents

(CVAs) (470) transient ischemic attacks (TIAs) (471)

vascular dementia (471) chronic obstructive pulmonary disease

(COPD) (472) presbyopia (472) cataracts (473) glaucoma (473) age-related macular degeneration (473) diabetic retinopathy (473) presbycusis (473) diabetes mellitus (475) type 1 diabetes (475) type 2 diabetes (475) circadian rhythm (476) metabolism (477) speed of processing (480) divided attention (481) working memory (482) implicit memory (482)

explicit memory (482) episodic memory (482) semantic memory (482) autobiographical memory (483) external memory aids (484) internal memory aids (484) dysphoria (488) internal belief systems (490) behavior therapy (490) cognitive therapy (490) anxiety disorders (490) dementia (492) Alzheimer’s disease (492) incontinence (492) beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis (495) Parkinson’s disease (495) chronic traumatic encephalopathy

(CTE) (496)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

H ill

S tr

ee t S

tu di

os /B

le nd

Im ag

es /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

501

What’s it really like to be an older adult? As we saw in Chapter 14, aging brings with it both physical limits (such as declines in vision and

hearing) and psychological gains (such as increased

expertise). Old age also brings social challenges.

Older adults are sometimes stereotyped as being

marginal and powerless in society, much like chil-

dren. Psychosocial issues confront older adults as

well. How do people think about their lives and

bring meaning and closure to them as they approach

death? What constitutes well-being for older people?

How do they use their time once they are no longer

working full-time? Do they like being retired? What

roles do relationships with friends and family play

in their lives? How do older people cope when their

partner is ill and requires care? What if their partner

dies? When older people need assistance, where do

they live?

These are a few of the issues we will examine in

this chapter. As in Chapter 14, our main focus will

be on the majority of older adults who are healthy

and live in the community. The distinction made in

Chapter 1 between young-old adults (60- to 80-year-olds) and old-old adults

(80-year-olds and up) is important. We know the most about young-old peo-

ple, even though the old-old reflect the majority of frail elderly and those who

live in nursing homes.

Social Aspects of Later Life Psychosocial, Retirement, Relationship, and Societal Issues

15.1 Theories of Psychosocial Aging 502 The Goal of Healthy Aging 502 Competence and Environmental Press 503 Preventive and Corrective Proactivity Model 505

REAL PEOPLE APPLYING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT: Katherine Johnson Human Computer for NASA 507

15.2 Personality, Social Cognition, and Spirituality 508 Integrity Versus Despair 508 Well-Being and Emotion 509

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: The Aging Emotional Brain 510

Spirituality in Later Life 512

15.3 I Used to Work at . . . : Living in Retirement 514 What Does Being Retired Mean? 514 Why Do People Retire? 515 Adjustment to Retirement 516 Employment and Volunteering 516

15.4 Friends and Family in Late Life 519 Friends, Siblings, and Socioemotional Selectivity 519 Marriage and Same-Sex Partnerships 521 Caring for a Partner 523 Widowhood 523 Great-Grandparenthood 524

15.5 Social Issues and Aging 525 Frail Older Adults 526 Housing Options 528 Elder Abuse and Neglect 532 Social Security and Medicare 533

WHAT DO YOU THINK? Reforming Social Security and Medicare 535

Summary 536

Key Terms 538

15

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

502 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Like other times in life, getting along in the environment is a com-

plicated issue. We begin by considering a few ideas about how to

optimize our fit with the environment. Next, we examine how we bring

the story of our lives to a culmination. After that, we consider how

interpersonal relationships and retirement provide contexts for life

satisfaction. We conclude with an examination of the social contexts

of aging.

More older adults are experiencing healthy aging by avoiding disease and engaging in life.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is social involvement and successful aging?

●● What is the competence–environmental press model, and how do docility and proactivity relate to the model?

●● What is the preventive and corrective proactivity model?

15.1 Theories of Psychosocial Aging

Since Sandy retired from her job as secretary at the local African Methodist Episcopal Church, she has hardly slowed down. She sings in the gospel choir, is involved in the Black Women’s Community Action Committee, and volunteers one day a week at a local Head Start school. Sandy’s friends say that she has to stay involved because that’s the way she’s always been. They claim you’d never know that Sandy is 81 years old.

Understanding how people grow old is not as simple as asking someone how old he or she is, as Sandy shows. As we saw in Chapter 14, aging is an individual process involving many variations in physical changes, cognitive functioning, and mental health. Psychoso- cial approaches to aging recognize these individual differences. Consider Sandy—her life reflects several key points. Her level of activity has remained constant across her adult life. This consistency fits well in continuity theory, the first framework considered in this sec- tion. Her ability to maintain this level of commitment indicates that the match between her abilities and her environment is just about right, as discussed in the competence–envi- ronmental press theory later in this section.

The Goal of Healthy Aging One of the most important aspects of research on older adults has been a rethinking of late life as one of mostly inevitable decline to one that is a more multidimensional view that also has posi- tive aspects, such as productive engagement with life and social involvement (Johnson & Mutchler, 2014). The coming demo- graphic changes in the United States and the rest of the world pres- ent a challenge for improving the kind of lives older adults live. For this reason, promoting wellness and healthy lifestyles in all living settings (community, long-term care, etc.) is seen as one of the top healthcare priorities of the 21st century (Parrella & Vormittag, 2017). Remaining healthy is important for decelerating the rate of aging (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013).kal

i9 /E

+/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 503

As researchers and theorists have approached healthy aging, they have focused on optimizing the individual outcome of the interplay of biopsychosocial forces and the indi- vidual over a lifetime (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013; Centers for Disease Control and Preven- tion, 2016e; Hostetler & Paterson, 2017; Samanta, 2017). healthy aging in this sense involves avoiding disease, being engaged with life, and maintaining high cognitive and phys- ical functioning. Healthy aging is both measurable (e.g., in terms of specific health metrics, cognitive performance, other specific behaviors) and subjective (e.g., well-being). It is reached when a person achieves his or her desired goals with dignity and as independently as possible.

The life-span perspective can be used to create a formal model for healthy aging. Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz (2010; Barlow et al., 2017) developed a theory of life-span development based on motivation and control by applying core assumptions that recog- nize aging as a complex process that involves increasing specialization and is influenced by factors unrelated to age. The basic premises of healthy aging include keeping a balance between the various gains and losses that occur over time and minimizing the influence of factors unrelated to aging. In short, these premises involve paying attention to both inter- nal and external factors impinging on the person. The antecedents include all the changes that happen to a person. The mechanisms in the model are the selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC) processes that shape the course of development, as we discussed in Chapter 1. Finally, the outcomes of the model denote that enhanced competence, qual- ity of life, and future adaptation are the visible signs of healthy aging.

Using the SOC model, enhanced by Heckhausen and colleague’s (2010; Barlow et al., 2017) notions of control and motivation, various types of interventions can be created to help people achieve healthy aging. In general, such interventions focus on the individual or on aspects of tasks and the physical and social environment that emphasize compe- tence (Aldwin & Gilmer, 2013; Lindbergh, Dishman, & Miller, 2016; Thornton, Paterson, & Yeung, 2013). When designing interventions aimed primarily at the person, it is impor- tant to understand the target person’s goals (rather than the goals of the researcher). For example, in teaching older adults how to use technology, it is essential to understand the kinds of concerns and fears older adults have and ensure the training program addresses them (Lesnoff-Caravaglia, 2010).

Performance on tests of everyday competence predicts longer term outcomes, as we saw in Chapter 14 (Allaire & Willis, 2006; Lindbergh et al., 2016). Careful monitoring of competence can be an early indicator of problems, and appropriate interventions should be undertaken as soon as possible. Because maintaining competence is an important ele- ment in determining quality of life, we now turn to a more focused consideration of what competence means in everyday contexts.

Competence and Environmental Press Understanding psychosocial aging requires attention to individuals’ needs rather than treating all older adults alike. One method focuses on the relation between the person and the environment (Aldwin, 2015; Aldwin & Igarashi, 2012). The competence–environ- mental press approach is a good example of a theory incorporating elements of the bio- psychosocial model into the person–environment relation (Lawton & Nahemow, 1973; Nahemow, 2000; Pynoos, Caraviello, & Cicero, 2010).

Competence is defined as the upper limit of a person’s ability to function in five domains: physical health, sensory-perceptual skills, motor skills, cognitive skills, and ego strength. These domains are viewed as underlying all other abilities and reflect the biologi- cal and psychological forces. environmental press refers to the physical, interpersonal, or social demands that environments put on people. Physical demands might include having to walk up three flights of stairs to your apartment. Interpersonal demands may require adjusting your behavior patterns to different types of people. Social demands involve deal- ing with laws or customs that place certain expectations on people. These aspects of the theory reflect biological, psychological, and social forces. Both competence and environ- mental press change as people move through the life span; what you are capable of doing

healthy aging Growing old by avoiding disease, being engaged with life, and maintaining high cognitive and physical functioning.

competence Upper limit of a person’s ability to function in five domains: physical health, sensory- perceptual skills, motor skills, cognitive skills, and ego strength.

environmental press Physical, interpersonal, or social demands that environments put on people.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

504 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

as a 5-year-old differs from what you are capable of doing as a 25-, 45-, 65-, or 85-year-old. Similarly, the demands put on you by the environment change as you age. Thus, the com- petence–environmental press framework also reflects life-cycle factors.

The competence and environmental press model depicted in ◗ Figure 15.1 shows how the two are related. Low to high competence is represented on the vertical axis, and weak to strong environmental press is displayed on the horizontal axis. Points in the figure rep- resent various combinations of the two. Most important, the shaded areas show adaptive behavior and positive affect can result from many different combinations of competence and environmental press levels. Adaptation level is the area where press level is average for a particular level of competence; this is where behavior and affect are normal. Slight increases in press tend to improve performance; this area on the figure is labeled the zone of maximum performance potential. Slight decreases in press create the zone of maximum comfort, in which people are able to live happily without worrying about environmental demands. Combinations of competence and environmental press that fall within either of these two zones result in adaptive behavior and positive emotion that translate into a high quality of life.

As a person moves away from these areas, behavior becomes increasingly maladaptive and affect becomes negative. Notice that these outcomes can result from several different combinations and for different reasons. For example, too many environmental demands on a person with low competence and too few demands on a person with high competence both result in maladaptive behaviors and negative emotion.

What does this mean with regard to late life? Is aging merely an equation relating certain variables? The important thing to realize about the competence–environmental press model is that each person has the potential of being happily adapted to some living situations, but not to all. Whether people function well depends on if what they are able to do fits what the environment forces them to do. When their abilities match the demands, people adapt; when there is a mismatch, they don’t. In this view, aging is more than an equation, because the best fit must be determined on an individual basis.

How do people deal with changes in their particular combinations of environmental press (such as adjusting to a new living situation) and competence (perhaps reduced abili- ties due to illness)? People respond in two basic ways (Lawton, 1989; Nahemow, 2000).

adaptation level When press level is average for a particular level of competence.

zone of maximum performance potential When press level is slightly higher, tending to improve performance.

zone of maximum comfort When press level is slightly lower, facilitat- ing a high quality of life.

●◗ Figure 15.1 The competence–environmental press model.

StrongWeak

Environmental press

Low

High Negative affect and maladaptive behavior

Negative affect and maladaptive behavior

Positive affect and adaptive behavior

M ar

gi na

l

M ar

g in

al

AB

A da

pt at

io n

le ve

l

M ax

im um

p er

fo rm

an ce

p ot

en tia

l

C o

m p

e te

n c e

From Lawton, M. P. & Nahemow, L. (1973). Ecology and the aging process. In C. Eisdorfer and M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult develop- ment and aging (pp. 619-674). Copyright © 1973 American Psychological Association.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 505

When people choose new behaviors to meet new desires or needs, they exhibit proactivity and exert control over their lives. In contrast, when people allow the situation to dictate the options they have, they demonstrate docility and have little control. Lawton (1989) argues that proactivity is more likely to occur in people with relatively high competence, and docility in people with relatively low competence.

The model has considerable research support. For example, the model accounts for why people choose the activities they do (Lawton, 1982), how well people adhere to medi- cation regimens (LeRoux & Fisher, 2006), and how they adapt to changing housing needs over time (Iwarsson, Slaug, & Fänge, 2012; Granbom et al., 2016). This model helps us understand how well people adapt to various care settings (Golant, 2012; Scharlach & Lehning, 2016). In short, there is considerable merit to the view that aging is a complex interaction between a person’s competence level and environmental press, mediated by choice. This model can be applied in many different settings.

Before leaving Lawton and Nahemow’s model, we need to note an important implica- tion for aging. To the extent people experience declines in competence (such as resulting from declines in health, sensory processes, motor skills, cognitive skills, or ego strength), they are less able to cope with environmental demands. That is why the home modifica- tions discussed later in the chapter, such as grab bars, and smartphones, are important— they functionally increase competence. Interventions based on artificial intelligence, such as robots and self-driving cars, will do even more to increase functional competence and enable people to live in the community. City planners now incorporate aspects of the competence– environmental press model in designing housing and neighborhoods (Lewis & Groh, 2016).

Additionally, the competence and environmental press model has been the basis for evaluating and optimizing living situations with people who have severe cognitive impair- ments, such as those of Alzheimer’s disease (Dalton, 2014). To manage severe cognitive impairment effectively, caregivers must identify the right level of environmental sup- port based on the patient’s level of competence. For example, people with mild cognitive impairment may be able to live independently, but as the impairment increases additional levels of support are needed. The model has provided the basis for designing special care units for people with Alzheimer’s disease. In these units, environmental supports such as color-coded room doors, help people with dementia identify where they belong.

Preventive and Corrective Proactivity Model Maintaining a high quality of life is a key goal for adults of all ages. From the competence– environmental press approach we saw proactivity, exerting control over one’s life, is cen- tral to achieving that goal. Because proactivity is so important, Kahana and Kahana (2003; Midlarsky, Kahana, & Belser, 2015) built a model of successful aging on the core concept of proactivity. The model is shown in ◗ Figure 15.2.

The Preventive and Corrective Proactivity (PCP) model explains how life stressors (such as life events, chronic illnesses) and lack of good congruence in person–environ- ment interactions (Component B), especially when the person has nothing to help buffer or protect against these things, result in poor life outcomes (Component F). The helpful buffers include external resources (Component E) such as friends or home modifications, internal resources or dispositions (Component C) such as a positive outlook on life, and specific proactive behaviors (Component D), such as physical exercise, work to lower the negative impact of the stressors and prepare people to cope better in the future. In brief, the PCP model proposes proactive adaptations and helpful external resources reduce the effect of life stressors on quality-of-life outcomes.

What kinds of actions reflect proactive adaptations? Kahana, Kahana, and Zhang (2005) described two types of proactive adaptations: preventive and corrective. Preventive adaptations are actions that avoid stressors and increase or build social resources. An example of a preventive adaptation would be increasing one’s social network by adding friends. Corrective adaptations are actions taken in response to stressors and can be facili- tated by internal and external resources. An example of a corrective adaptation is chang- ing one’s diet after having a heart attack.

proactivity When people choose new behaviors to meet new desires or needs and exert con- trol over their lives.

docility When people allow their situation to dictate the options they have.

preventive adaptations Actions that avoid stressors and increase or build social resources.

corrective adaptations Actions taken in response to stressors that can be facilitated by internal and external resources.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

506 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Older adults tend to engage in more corrective adaptations than preventive adapta- tions, at least initially. However, many actions that start as corrective adaptations turn into preventive adaptations. A great example of this is exercise. Many people begin an exercise program only after they are told to, perhaps as part of a recovery regimen after a health crisis. However, continued exercise becomes preventive by helping the person avoid future recurrences of the original health problem and avoid other problems altogether.

Research supports the importance of proactivity as described in the PCP model. Kahana, Kelley-Moore, & Kahana (2012) showed life stressors can still have a negative effect on quality-of-life outcomes four years after they occur, but proactive adaptations (such as exercise, planning ahead, and gathering support) significantly reduce this nega- tive impact. Longitudinal research in China also showed the importance of proactivity and other external and internal resources in improving quality-of-life outcomes in the oldest- old residents in the community and in long-term care facilities (Liu et al., 2012).

●◗ Figure 15.2 Model of emerging proactive options for successful aging.

(a)

(b)

(c)

(d) (f)

CUMULATIVE STRESS EXPOSURE

PROACTIVE ADAPTATIONS

QUALITY-OF-LIFE OUTCOMES

Path 4

Path 3

Path 9

Path 6b

Path 6a

Path 5

Maintenance of valued activities

and relationships

Meaning in life

Affective states

Long-term events

Recent events

Traditional Preventive Adaptations

Traditional Corrective Adaptations

• Marshalling support • Role Substitution • Environmental modifications

Emergent Adaptations Preventive and Corrective

• Technology use • Healthcare consumerism • Self-improvement

Future orientation

Hopefulness Self-Esteem

Life satisfaction

Altruism Coping

dispositions

Temporal context of history

and biography

Spatial context of demography and

community

• Chronic illness • Social losses • Person– environment • Incongruence

• Health promotion (exercise) • Planning ahead • Helping others

TEMPORAL AND SPATIAL CONTEXT

INTERNAL RESOURCES/DISPOSITIONS OF SUCCESSFUL AGING

(e)

Financial resources

Social resources

• Access to technology • Access to healthcare

Emergent resources

EXTERNAL RESOURCES

Path 8

Path 2a

Path 2b

Path 7

Path 1

From Kahana, E., Kahana, B., & Zhang, J. (2006). Motivational antecedents of preventive proactivity in late life: Linking future orientation and exercise. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 438–459 (Figure 1). doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9012-2

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 507

Katherine Johnson, Human Computer for NASA

Imagine being an African American born in 1918 and growing up in segregated West Virginia in the early part of the 20th century. Imagine also being a brilliant young woman who graduated high school at age 14. Imag- ine being that African American woman as a graduate of West Virginia State College, summa cum laude, at age 18, then being one of the first three African Americans (and only woman) to attend graduate school at West Virginia University. To top it off, her specialties were physics and math.

This was reality for Katherine Coleman Goble Johnson who, in 1953, began a career at the National Advisory Committee for Aeronau- tics (later, the National Aeronautics and Space Administration—NASA) as one of the so-called colored computers at the Guidance and Con- trol Division of Langley’s Flight Research Divi- sion in Hampton, Virginia. Because of the Jim Crow laws in the segregated South, Katherine and her other African American women col- leagues faced workplace discrimination, includ- ing separate working, eating, and bathroom facilities (located across the compound) apart from their European American counterparts. She also faced significant gender discrimination in the otherwise all-male environment.

Despite the obstacles and discrimination, Katherine was a true trailblazing star. Her job entailed, among other things, precisely

calculating rocket launch and orbital trajecto- ries by hand at a time when digital computers were in their early stages of development. She was so good, in fact, that John Glenn refused to fly his mission in 1962 unless Katherine personally verified the digital computer’s cal- culations. In 1969 she calculated the trajec- tory for the Apollo 11 mission to the moon, and in 1970 her work on backup procedures helped return the Apollo 13 crew safely after an explosion onboard forced the mission to be aborted. Katherine retired from NASA in 1986 after subsequently working on the Space Shuttle program, the Earth Resources Satel- lite, and on plans for a Mars mission. She was

consulted for the book Hidden Figures and was depicted in the 2016 film adaptation.

Katherine Johnson is a great example of the interplay between competence and environmental press. Her science and math skills competencies are unquestionably stellar. Equally strong was her competence regarding her ability to succeed despite the legal and other discriminatory barriers (environmental press) put in her way throughout her early life and career. Even in her late 90s, as various ail- ments slowed her physically, her mental com- petence was still clearly apparent. Katherine Johnson managed to maximize her adaptation level throughout her long life.

Real People Applying Human Development

Katherine Johnson

Sc ie

nc e

H is

to ry

Im ag

es /A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Understanding how people age usually entails taking a broader perspective than any single theory can offer. The Real People feature about Katherine Coleman Goble John- son, a trailblazing African American physicist and mathematician who helped make NASA’s human space flight programs successful, shows that both successful aging and competence–environmental press theory are important.

Recall 1. _______________ involves avoiding disease, being engaged

with life, and maintaining high cognitive and physical functioning.

2. A person’s ability to function in several key domains is termed _______________, whereas demands put on a person from external sources are termed _______________.

Interpret ●● How does continuity theory incorporate aspects of the bio-

psychosocial model?

Apply ●● How would a new state law requiring older adults to pass a

vision test before they could renew their driver’s license be an example of changes in environmental press?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 15.1

Think About It How does the competence– environmental press approach help explain which coping strategies might work best in a particular situation?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

508 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Fatima is a spry 88-year-old who spends more time thinking and reflecting about her past than she used to. She also tends to be less critical of decisions she made years ago. Fatima remembers her visions of the woman she wanted to become and concludes that she has come pretty close. Fatima wonders if this process of reflection is something that most older adults go through.

Think for a minute about the older adults you know. What are they like? How do they see themselves today? How do they visualize their lives a few years from now? Do they see themselves the same as or different from the way they were in the past? These ques- tions have intrigued authors since the early days of psychology. In the late 19th century, William James (1890), one of the early pioneers in psychology, wrote that a person’s per- sonality traits are set by young adulthood. Some researchers agree; as we saw in Chapter 13, some aspects of personality remain relatively stable throughout adulthood. But peo- ple also change in important ways, as Carl Jung (1960/1933) argued, by integrating such opposite tendencies as masculine and feminine traits. As we saw in Chapter 1, Erik Erik- son (1982) was convinced that personality development takes a lifetime, unfolding over a series of eight stages from infancy through late life.

In this section, we explore how people such as Fatima assemble the final pieces in the personality puzzle, and we see how important aspects of personality continue to evolve in later life. We begin with Erikson’s issue of integrity, the process by which people try to make sense of their lives. Next, we see how well-being is achieved and how personal aspirations play out. Finally, we examine how spirituality is an important aspect of many older adults’ lives.

Integrity Versus Despair As people enter late life, they begin the struggle of integrity versus despair, which involves the process by which people try to make sense of their lives. According to Erikson (1982), this struggle comes about as older adults such as Fatima try to understand their lives in terms of the future of their family and community. Thoughts of a person’s own death are balanced by the realization that they will live on through children, grandchildren, great- grandchildren, and the community as a whole. This realization produces what Erikson calls a “life-affirming involvement” in the present.

The struggle of integrity versus despair requires people to engage in a life review, the process by which people reflect on the events and experiences of their lifetimes. To achieve integrity, a person must come to terms with the choices and events that made his or her life unique. There must also be an acceptance of the fact that one’s life is drawing to a close. Looking back on one’s life may resolve some of the second-guessing of decisions made earlier in adulthood (Erikson, Erikson, & Kivnick, 1986). People who were unsure whether they made the right choices concerning their children, for example, now feel sat- isfied that things worked out well. In contrast, others feel bitter about their choices, blame themselves or others for their misfortunes, see their lives as meaningless, and greatly fear death. These people end up in despair rather than living with integrity.

Research shows a connection between engaging in a life review and achieving integ- rity; so life review forms the basis for effective mental health interventions (Villar & Serrat, 2017; Westerhof, Bohlmeijer, & Webster, 2010), especially for older individuals with depression (Hallford & Mellor, 2013). A therapeutic technique called “structured life

integrity versus despair According to Erikson, the process in late life by which people try to make sense of their lives.

life review Process by which people reflect on the events and experiences of their lifetimes.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What is integrity in late life? How do people achieve it?

●● How is well-being defined in adulthood? How do people view themselves differently as they age?

●● What role does spirituality play in late life?

15.2 Personality, Social Cognition, and Spirituality

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 509

review” (Haight & Haight, 2007, 2013) has been shown to be effective in helping people deal with stressful life events.

Who reaches integrity? Erikson (1982) emphasizes that people who demonstrate integrity come from various backgrounds and cultures and arrive there having taken dif- ferent paths. Such people have made many different choices and follow many different lifestyles; the point is that everyone has this opportunity to achieve integrity if they strive for it. Those who reach integrity become self-affirming and self-accepting; they judge their lives to have been worthwhile and good. They are glad to have lived the lives they did.

Well-Being and Emotion How is your life going? Are you reasonably content, or do you think you could be doing better? Answers to these questions provide insight into your subjective well-being, an evaluation of one’s life that is associated with positive feelings. In life-span developmental psychology, subjective well-being is usually assessed by measures of life satisfaction, hap- piness, and self-esteem (Oswald & Wu, 2010).

Overall, well-being across adulthood looks like a U-shaped function, like that shown in ◗ Figure 15.3 (Cheng, Powdthaveea, & Oswald, 2014). Young-older adults are charac- terized by improved subjective well-being compared with middle-aged adults, although the extent of the difference depends on several factors, such as hardiness, chronic illness, marital status, the quality of one’s social network, and stress (Charles & Carstensen, 2010).

These happiness-related factors and the overall shape of the function hold across cultures as well; for example, studies of Australian, German, Taiwanese, and Tanzanian older adults showed similar predictors of successful aging (Cheng et al., 2014; Hsu, 2005; Mwanyangala et al., 2010). Although gender differences in subjective well-being have been found to increase with age, they are most likely due to older women being particularly disadvantaged compared with older men with regard to chronic illness and its effect on ability to care for oneself, everyday competence, quality of social network, socioeconomic status, and widowhood (Charles & Carstensen, 2010). Such gender differences are smaller in more recent cohorts, indicating that societal changes over the past few decades have led to improvements in the way older women view themselves.

Given the findings about higher level of well-being in older adults, researchers began wondering how well-being was related to emotions. People’s feelings are clearly impor- tant, as they get expressed in daily moods and underlie mental health problems such as depression, and show a tendency to focus on more positive feelings (Cacioppo et al., 2011; Isaacowitz, 2014; Zhou et al., 2017). So how emotions are regulated in later life may pro- vide insights into people’s subjective well-being.

Emotion-focused research in neuroscience is providing answers to the question of why subjective well-being tends to increase with age (Cacioppo et al., 2011; Mather, 2016). A brain structure called the amygdala, an almond-shaped set of nuclei deep in the brain, helps regulate emotion. Evidence is grow- ing that age-related changes in how the amygdala functions may play a key role in understanding emotional regulation in older adults. Here’s how. In young adults, arousal of the amygdala is associated with negative emotional arousal. When negative emotional arousal occurs, for example, memory for events associated with the emo- tion is stronger. But the situation is different for older adults—both amygdala activation and emotional arousal are lower. That may be one reason older adults experience less negative emotion, lower rates of depression, and better well-being (Mather, 2016). But that’s not the whole story. In Chapter 13, we discovered that brain activity in the prefrontal cortex, which is associated with cognition, changes with age. Neuroimaging research shows that changes in cognitive processing in the prefrontal cortex also are associated with changes in emotional regulation in older adults. For example, research shows that the connection between certain parts of the prefrontal cortex

subjective well-being Evaluation of one’s life that is associated with positive feelings.

●◗ Figure 15.3 The pattern of a typical person’s hap- piness through life.

A ve

ra ge

l if

e sa

ti sf

ac ti

o n

s co

re

Age group

4.9

5.0

5.1

5.2

5.3

5.4

5.5

5.6

15–20 21–30 31–40 41–50 51–60 61–70

From A. Oswald “Happiness, health, and economics,” Warwick University, imechanica.org/files/andrew_oswald_presentation_071129.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

510 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

(the medial prefrontal cortex) and the amygdala are more connected and active while older adults are at rest than it is in younger adults, and such connectivity is likely the rea- son older adults remember more positive emotional content (Mather, 2016; Sakaki, Nga, & Mather, 2013). A good example of the kind of neuroimaging research that has led to these conclusions is described in the Spotlight on Research feature.

On ResearchSpotlight

The Aging Emotional Brain

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? Although much research has examined the behavioral side of emotions, very little has examined the specific underlying neural mechanisms in the brain. Winecoff and colleagues (2011) decided to examine these mechanisms and discover whether they differed with age.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? Winecoff and colleagues used a battery of tests to measure cognitive performance and emotional behavior. They tested participants’ immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition for 16 target words as measures of memory. They also administered

a response-time test to measure psychomo- tor speed (see Chapter 14), and a digit-span test to measure working memory (see Chap- ter 14). The researchers also had participants complete three questionnaires to measure various types of emotions.

After these measures were obtained, participants were given the cognitive reap- praisal task depicted in ◗ Figure 15.4. In brief, participants learned a reappraisal strategy that involved thinking of themselves as an emotionally detached and objective third party. During the training session, they told the experimenter what they were thinking about the image to ensure task compliance, but they were instructed not to speak during the scanning session. During the functional

magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session, participants completed 60 positive image trials (30 “Experience” and 30 “Reappraise”), 60 negative image trials (30 “Experience” and 30 “Reappraise”), and 30 neutral image trials (all “Experience”). Within each condition, half of the images contained people and the other half did not. The fMRI session provided images of ongoing brain activity.

Who were the participants in the study? The sample consisted of 22 younger adults (average age 235 ; range 195 to 33 years) and 20 older adults (average age 695 ; range 595 to 73 years). Participants were matched on demographic variables, including education. Participants received the cognitive/memory/ emotion tests on one day and the reappraisal

●◗ Figure 15.4 Cognitive reappraisal task. Participants were trained in the use of a reappraisal strategy for emotion regulation. (A) On “Experience” trials, participants viewed an image and then received an instruction to experience naturally the emotions evoked by that image. The image then disappeared, but participants continued to experience their emotions throughout a 6-second delay period. At the end of the trial, the participants rated the perceived emotional valence of that image using an eight-item rating scale. (B) “Reappraise” trials had similar timing, except that the cue instructed participants to decrease their emotional response to the image by reappraising the image (e.g., distancing oneself from the scene). Shown are examples of the negative (A) and positive (B) images used in the study.

N id

er la

nd er

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

N id

er la

nd er

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

1

1

very negative

very positiveneutral

experienceexperience

(a) 2s 2s 6s 4s

2s 2s 6s 4s (b)

How negative or positive was the image?

very negative

very positiveneutral

decreasedecrease

How negative or positive was the image?

iS to

ck .c

om /V

et ta

C ol

le ct

io n/

Ri cA

gu ia

r

iS to

ck .c

om /V

et ta

C ol

le ct

io n/

Ri cA

gu ia

r

From Winecoff, A., et al. (2011). Cognitive and neural contributions to emotion regulation in aging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 165–176.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 511

●◗ Figure 15.5 Modulation of prefrontal and amygdalar activation by emotion regulation.

Exp Reap

Negative

0.70

0.60

0.50

0.40

0.30

0.20

0.10

Younger adults

Older adults

0.20

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.00

– 0.05

– 0.10

– 0.15

L e ft

I F

G s

ig n

a l c h

a n

g e (

% )

A m

y g

s ig

n a l c h

a n

g e (

% )

Reap Exp

Positive

Exp

Neutral

Exp Reap

Negative

Reap Exp

Positive

Exp

Neutral

Exp Reap

Negative Negative

Reap Exp

Positive

Exp

Neutral

Exp Reap Reap Exp

Positive

Exp

Neutral

(a)

(c)

(b)

(d)

From Winecoff, A., LaBar, K. S. Madden, D. J., Cabeza, R., & Huettel, S. A. (2011). Cognitive and neural contributions to emotion regulation in aging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 165–176. By permission of Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq030

task in the fMRI session on a second day. Participants were paid $55.

What was the design of the study? The study used a cross-sectional design, with testing of two age groups over two sessions.

Were there ethical concerns with the study? All participants provided written consent under a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of Duke University Medical Center.

What were the results? Younger and older adults performed the reappraisal tasks simi- larly; that is, in the reappraisal condition, posi- tive images were reported as less positive and negative images were reported as less nega- tive. However, older adults’ reports of negative emotion were higher than those of younger adults in the negative reappraisal situation.

Examination of the fMRI results showed that reappraisals involved significant activa- tion of specific areas in the prefrontal cortex for both positive and negative emotions. For both age groups, activity in the prefrontal area increased and activity in the amygdala decreased during the reappraisal phase. These patterns are shown in ◗ Figure 15.5. As

you can see in the top figure, certain areas in the prefrontal cortex showed a pattern of acti- vation that followed participants’ self-reports of emotion regulation. Shown here are voxels activated in the contrast between “Reap- praise-Negative” and “Experience- Negative” conditions. The top graphs show that for both positive and negative stimuli and for both younger and older adults, prefrontal activation increased in “Reappraise” trials compared with “Experience” trials. In contrast, the lower graphs show that in the amygdala (Amy), there was a systematic decrease in activation during emotion regulation between “Experience-Neg- ative” and “Reappraise-Negative” conditions.

Additional analyses of the fMRI data showed that emotion regulation modulates the functional interaction between the pre- frontal cortex and the amygdala. Compared with older adults, younger adults showed more activity in the prefrontal cortex during “Reappraise” trials for negative pictures. No age difference in brain activation for positive pictures was found. Cognitive abilities were related to the degree of decrease in amygdala activation, independent of age.

What did the investigators conclude? Winecoff and colleagues concluded that the prefrontal cortex plays a major role in emotional regulation, especially for older adults. In essence, the prefrontal cortex may help suppress (regu- late) emotions in the same way that area of the brain is involved in inhibiting other behaviors. Importantly, the degree of emotional regulation was predicted by cognitive ability, with higher cognitive ability associated with higher emotional regulation. This may mean that as cognitive abili- ties decline, people are less able to regulate their emotions, a pattern typical in diseases such as dementia. Thus, besides evidence of underlying brain structures playing critical roles in emotion regulation, there may be a neurological explana- tion for the kinds of emotional outbursts that occur in dementia and related disorders.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Winecoff and colleagues studied only two age groups of healthy adults and did not include either old- old participants or adults with demonstrable cognitive impairment. It will be important to study those groups to map brain function changes and behavior more completely.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

512 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Spirituality in Later Life Humanity’s oldest surviving literary work, the Epic of Gilgamesh, tells of the title char- acter’s search for the answer to one of life’s great issues: the search for eternal life as an answer to the question “What happens next?” We continue to seek answers to life’s great existential questions: What is the meaning of life? Why do bad things happen to good peo- ple? Is there anything more than this life? Why is there so much suffering? Why am I here?

Spirituality is the context in which most people search for meaning and answers to these questions (Aldwin et al., 2014; Pargament, 2013; Park, 2013; Park, Edmondson, & Hale-Smith, 2013). Spirituality “can inform all aspects of meaning, inform- ing beliefs . . . and providing ultimate motivation and primary goals for living and guidelines for achieving those goals, along with a deep sense of purpose and mattering” (Park, 2013 p. 42).

Individuals’ sense of spirituality develops across adulthood. Thomas Merton (1949, 1955, 1962) and Richard Rohr (2011, 2013) both describe this process as the progression from the False Self to the True Self. Essentially the distinction is between the superfi- cial, external self that we mean when we use the first person singular pronoun “I,” the self that Merton considers a prison from which we must escape, and the joy of dwell- ing in union with the essence of everything in the universe in the core of our soul, the self that Merton argues we must become. Rohr writes that the False Self is rooted in a relative identity that each of us creates for ourselves, an identity that depends critically on external indicators of success (e.g., title, prestige, salary, status, etc.). The False Self is inherently fragile, tends to be dissatisfied because it depends on external definitions of achievement, and is felt as separate from others in the sense that we want to belong to the “in” group and define others selectively as members of the “out” group. Change is to be feared. Morality is defined in terms of adherence to rules. In short, the False Self is the result of building one’s ego structure based on incorporating external mes- sages regarding of “values” and “success.” There is a heavy emphasis on doing things “ correctly” according to the rules of the group or society. In Rohr’s view, achieving the True Self involves (re)discovering the universality and wholeness, an “authentic inner knowing,” that has always been within, but has gone unrecognized or unacknowledged. The True Self does not see anything with absolute certainty. The True Self is the fulfill- ment of the search for answers to the core existential questions. It entails a letting go and an acceptance of what is.

As you probably surmised, the developmental transition from False Self to True Self is a reflection of underlying cognitive (described in Chapters 10, 13, and 14) and personality development (described in this chapter and in Chapters 10 and 13). Cavanaugh (in press)

argues that it is these underlying processes that permit the shift in perspective reflected in the transition from False Self to True Self.

This change in perspective is also a key factor in how many older adults cope with life stresses. According to research, older adults in many countries and from many different backgrounds use their religious faith and spiri- tuality as the basis for coping, often more than they use family or friends (Ai et al., 2013, 2017; Ai, Wink, & Ardelt, 2010; Ardelt et al., 2013). For some older adults, especially African Americans, a strong attachment to God is what they believe helps them deal with the challenges of life (Dilworth-Anderson, Boswell, & Cohen, 2007; Harvey, Johnson, & Heath, 2013).

There is considerable evidence linking spirituality, religious-based coping, and health (Ai et al., 2010, 2017; Harvey et al., 2013; Krause, 2012; Park, 2007). In general, older adults who are more involved with and commit- ted to their faith have better physical and mental health

The spirituality of these Buddhist monks can serve as an important coping strategy.

KI M

-J A

E- H

W A

N /A

FP /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 513

than older adults who are not religious. For example, older Mexican Americans who pray to the saints and the Virgin Mary on a regular basis tend to have greater optimism and better health (Krause & Bastida, 2011). Spirituality also helps improve psychological well- being (George, Palmore, & Cohen, 2014; Hayward et al., 2016) and helps patients following cardiac surgery (Ai et al., 2010).

Researchers have increasingly focused on spiritual support—which includes seeking pastoral care, partici- pating in organized and nonorganized religious activi- ties, and expressing faith in a God who cares for people—as a key factor in understanding how older adults cope. Even when under high levels of stress, such as during critical illness or other major life trauma, people who rely on spiritual support report greater personal well-being (Ai et al., 2010, 2017; Hayward et al., 2016). Lucette and colleagues (2016; Krause, 2006) report that maintaining a sense of spirituality helps people cope with chronic health conditions.

Neuroscience research has shown a connection between certain spiritual mindfulness practices and brain activity (Dobkin & Hassed, 2016; Tang & Posner, 2012). For example, there is evidence that people who have practiced meditation show positive structural changes in areas of the brain related to attention and memory (Esch, 2014; Luders, 2014; Newberg et al., 2014). Thus, neurological evidence indicates that there may be changes in brain activity and in brain structure associated with spiritual practices that help people cope. An example of the changes that occur in brain activity between a normal resting state and a medi- tative state is shown in ◗Figure 15.6. Notice the significant increase in activity during meditation.

This kind of positive structural change has led some researchers (Luders, 2014; Luders  & Cherbuin, 2016; Newberg et al., 2014) to argue that meditation might offer promise as a way to help slow down, and perhaps even prevent, the brain changes that underlie brain diseases such as dementia. While it is still too early to tell for certain, the possibility that a practice done for centuries among all of the major world religions in one form or another, mindful meditation or contemplation, could prevent or reverse negative changes in the brain will undoubtedly be the focus of a great deal of research.

spiritual support Type of coping strategy that includes seek- ing pastoral care, participating in organized and nonorganized religious activities, and expressing faith in a God who cares for people.

Think About It How might the transition from the False Self to the True Self reflect postformal cognitive development?

Recall 1. The Eriksonian struggle that older adults face is termed

_______________.

2. An evaluation of one’s life that is associated with positive feelings is termed _______________.

3. The transition from False Self to True self is one aspect of the development of _______________.

Interpret ●● How might different spiritual traditions influence personal

well-being?

Apply ●● Given that the prefrontal cortex is activated for both positive

and negative emotions for younger and older adults, what might account for older adults’ increased reaction to negative emotions?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 15.2

●◗ Figure 15.6 SPECT scans of a subject at rest and during peak meditation showing increased cerebral blood flow (arrows) during meditation.

Attention area Attention area

Baseline Meditation

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

514 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Marcus is a 77-year-old retired construction worker who labored hard all of his life. He man- aged to save a little money, but he and his wife rely primarily on his monthly Social Security checks. Although not rich, they have enough to pay the bills. For the most part, Marcus is happy with retirement, and he stays in touch with his friends. He thinks that maybe he’s a little strange, though, because he has heard that retirees are supposed to be isolated and lonely.

Did you know that until 1934, when a railroad union sponsored a bill promoting manda- tory retirement, and 1935, when Social Security was inaugurated, retirement was not even considered a possibility by most Americans like Marcus (McClinton, 2010; Sargent et al., 2013)? Only since World War II has there been a substantial number of retired people in the United States (McClinton, 2010).

Although we take retirement for granted, economic downturns have a major disruptive effect on people’s retirement decisions and plans. New view of aging and increased health for the typical older adult has also had an effect on people’s notions about retirement. These reali- ties have, in turn, affected social policy. As more people retire and take advantage of longer lives, a significant social challenge is created regarding how to fund retiree benefits and how to support older adults who are still active (Monahan, 2017; Quinn & Cahill, 2016; Wise, 2017).

After having one or several careers across adulthood, many older adults find them- selves questioning whether they want to continue in that line of work anymore, or find themselves being forced to go through that questioning because they lost their jobs. This period of questioning and potential exploration enables people to think about their options: retiring, looking for work in the same of a different field, volunteering, or some combination of all of these.

With the movement of the baby-boom generation into old age, increasing numbers of these adults are redefining what “retirement” and “work” mean in late life. Realizing these

generational shifts reflect important changes in how people view the latter part of one’s working life, AARP created the Life Reimagined® tool that assists peo- ple in finding their path, including reawakening long-dormant interests.

As we consider retirement and other options in late life, keep in mind the world is changing, resulting in increased options and the likelihood more older adults will continue in the labor force by choice and necessity.

What Does Being Retired Mean? Retirement means different things to men and women, and to people in different ethnic groups and careers (James, Matz-Costa, & Smyer, 2016; Loretto & Vickerstaff, 2013; Silver, 2016). It has also taken on new and different meanings since the beginning of the Great Recession in 2008 because of the abrupt change in people’s planning and expectations as a result of the loss of savings or pensions (Quinn & Cahill, 2016; Sargent et al., 2013).

Part of the reason it is difficult to define retirement precisely is that the decision to retire involves the loss of occupational identity with no obvious replacement for that loss. Not having a specific job any more means we either put that aspect of our lives in the past tense—“I used to work as a manager at the Hilton”—or say nothing at all. Loss of this aspect of ourselves can be diffi- cult to face, so some look for a label other than “retired” to describe themselves.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What does being retired mean?

●● Why do people retire?

●● How do people adjust to retirement?

●● What employment and volunteer opportunities are avail- able for older adults?

15.3 I Used to Work at . . . : Living in Retirement

Retirement provides many people with the opportunity to do things they want to do rather than things they must do.

iS to

ck .c

om /X

av ie

r A rn

au

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 515

That’s why researchers view retirement as another one of many transitions people experience in life (Kojola & Moen, 2016; Moen, 2016a; Sargent et al., 2013). This view makes retirement a complex process where people withdraw from full-time participation in an occupation (Moen, 2016a; Sargent et al., 2013), recognizing there are many pathways to this end (Kojola & Moen, 2016; Moen, 2016a, b; Sargent et al., 2013).

Why Do People Retire? Provided they have good health, more workers retire by choice than for any other reason (Cohen-Mansfield & Regev, in press; Kojola & Moen, 2016), as long they feel financially secure after considering projected income from Social Security, pensions and other struc- tured retirement programs, and personal savings (Quinn & Cahill, 2016). Of course, some people are forced to retire because of health problems or because they lose their jobs. As corporations downsize during economic downturns or after corporate mergers, some older workers accept buyout packages involving supplemental payments if they retire. Others are permanently furloughed, laid off, or dismissed.

The decision to retire is influenced by one’s occupational history and goal expecta- tions (Moen, 2016a; Sargent et al., 2013). Whether people perceive they will achieve their personal goals through work or retirement influences the decision to retire and its con- nection with health and disability.

The rude awakening many people received during the Great Recession was that the best made plans are only as good as external factors allow them to be, especially when it comes to financial savings and pensions. Many people lost much of their investment sav- ings and home equity as the value of stocks plummeted, companies eliminated pension plans, and the housing market collapsed. Consequently, many people were forced to delay their retirement until they had the financial resources to do so, or to continue working part time when they had not planned to do so to supplement their income.

Income security is now at the forefront in retirement decisions (Monahan, 2017; Quinn & Cahill, 2016). Although 42% of respondents in one poll thought the ideal retire- ment age is between 60 and 65, only 29% thought they would actually be able to achieve that goal (Boschma, 2015). Indeed, 30% of respondents thought that they would be unable to retire before age 70, if ever, mainly due to pessimism regarding financial security.

How much do you need to have in savings to be comfortable in retirement? A decent rule of thumb is to save enough to generate between 70% and 80% of your current income, and to plan for about 25 years in retirement. This figure takes into account typical living and medical expenses. The bottom line is longer life expectancies have added to the amount of money you will need in retirement—and that amount is usually much greater than people think.

Gender and Ethnic Differences Women’s experience of retiring can be quite different from men’s (Loretto & Vickerstaff, 2013; Silver, 2016). Women’s employment career may have developed differently, such as having starts and stops related to dependent care responsibil- ities, or may reflect later entry into the workforce. Because of the pay gap and possible interruptions in work history, many women have fewer financial resources for retirement.

For women who were never employed outside the home, the process of retirement is especially unclear (Loretto & Vickerstaff, 2013), even when such spouses are eligible for Social Security benefits. Because they were not paid for all of their work raising children and caring for the home, it is rare for them to have their own pensions or other sources of income in retirement beyond spousal survivor benefits. Additionally, the work they have always done in caring for the home continues, often nearly uninterrupted (Ciani, 2016).

Think About It In the absence of mandatory retire- ment, is there such a concept as “early retirement?”

We know less about the experiences of older women and adults of color in retire- ment than we do about male European Americans.

ka li9

/E +/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

516 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

There has not been much research examining the process of retirement as a func- tion of ethnicity. African American and Latino older adults are likely to continue working beyond age 65, mainly due to greater financial vulnerability on average (Angel & Angel, 2015; Sullivan & Meschede, 2016; Troutman, Nies, & Mavellia, 2011).

Adjustment to Retirement How do people who go through the process of retirement adjust to it? Researchers agree on one point: New patterns of personal involvement must be developed in the context of changing roles and lifestyles in retirement (Ajrouch, Antonucci, & Webster, 2014; Potočnik, Tordera, & Peiró, 2013). People’s adjustment to retirement evolves over time as a result of complex interrelations involving physical health, financial status, to the degree  their retirement was voluntary, and feelings of personal control (Moen, 2016a; Ng et al., 2016).

How do most people fare? As long as people have financial security, health, a sup- portive network of relatives and friends, and an internally driven sense of motivation, they report feeling good about being retired (Hershey & Henkens, 2014; Moen, 2016a; Ng et al., 2016; Potočnik et al., 2013). What motivates most people, though, is finding a sense of fulfillment in ways previously unavailable to them (James et al., 2016).

Many people think being retired has negative effects on health. Research findings show the relation between health and retirement is complex. On the one hand, there is no evidence voluntary retirement has immediate negative effects on health (Hershey & Hen- kens, 2014). In contrast, there is ample evidence being forced to retire is correlated with significantly poorer physical and mental health (Hershey & Henkens, 2014).

Employment and Volunteering Retirement is an important life transition, one best understood through a life-course per- spective that takes other aspects of one’s life, such as one’s marital relationship, into account (Moen, 2016a; Wickrama, O’Neal, & Lorenz, 2013). This life change means retirees must look for ways to adapt to new routines and patterns, while maintaining social integration and being active in various ways (e.g., friendship networks, community engagement).

Employment in Late Life For an increasing number of people, especially for those whose retirement savings is insuf- ficient, “retirement” involves working at least part-time. Employment for them is a finan- cial necessity to make ends meet, especially for those whose entire income would consist only of Social Security benefits.

For others, the need to stay employed at least part-time represents a way to stay involved beyond an income supplement, and a way for employers to continue benefiting from employees’ experience. For example, even in the rapidly changing knowledge indus- try, employment of workers over age 65 has been shown to benefit both older adults and companies (Bartkowiak, 2017).

As you can see in ◗ Figure 15.7, the percentage of adults age 65 and over who are in the workforce increased dramatically between 2000 and 2016, even as the overall percent- age of Americans who were employed declined (DeSilver, 2016). Note also the trend has been consistently upward, indicating the forces keeping older adults in the labor force have been acting for many years.

Overall, labor force participation of older adults in the United States and other devel- oped countries has been increasing most rapidly among women (DeSilver, 2016; Sterns & Chang, 2010). This is due mostly to more women being in the labor force across adulthood than in decades past, more older women being single, and greater financial need. The Great Recession exacerbated the need for older adults to work at least part-time to ensure greater financial security (Quinn & Cahill, 2016).

Older workers face many challenges, not the least of which are ageism and discrimi- nation (Jackson, 2013). Employers may believe older workers are less capable, and there

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 517

is evidence this translates into lower likelihood of getting a job interview compared to younger or middle-aged workers, all other things being equal (Abrams, Swift, & Drury, 2016). As we noted earlier in this chapter, despite the fact age discrimination laws in the United States protect people over age 40, such barriers are still widespread.

The relationship between age and job performance is extremely complex (Sterns & Chang, 2010). This is because it depends a great deal on the kind of job one is considering, such as one that requires a great deal of physical exertion or one that involves a great deal of expertise and experience. In general, older workers show more reliability (e.g., showing up on time for work), organizational loyalty, and safety-related behavior.

How have companies adapted to having more older workers? One example is BMW, a company that changed a number of things in its automobile assembly plants to better meet the needs of older workers (de Pommereau, 2012). BMW provides physical trainers on the factory floor, has new, softer floors, offers chairs that rise up and down to make tasks easier, uses larger print fonts on computer screens, and provides special shoes.

The trend for companies to employ older workers, especially on a part-time basis, is likely to continue because it is a good option for companies (Beck, 2013; Coleman, 2015). Some companies find they need the expertise older workers bring, as well as the flexibility of older workers in terms of hours and the type of benefits they need (or do not need) often make it less expensive. Consequently, “retirement” is likely to continue to evolve as a concept, and is likely to include some aspect of employment well into late life.

Coleman (2015) argues that the benefits of hiring and retaining older workers could make companies stronger and more successful. However, a change in approach will require overcoming stereotypes of aging, as well as balancing the need to provide opportunities for younger workers with the need to retain the experience and knowledge of older workers.

Volunteering Healthy, active retired adults often find meaning and explore things they always wished they had had a chance to do by volunteering (Ajrouch et al., 2014; Kleiber, 2013). Older adults report they volunteer for many reasons that benefit their well-being (Ajrouch et al., 2015; Greenfield & Marks, 2005), such as to provide service to others, to maintain social interactions and improve their communities, and to keep active.

Why do so many people volunteer? Several factors are responsible (Ajrouch et al., 2015; Tang, Morrow-Howell, & Choi, 2010): changing characteristics of social networks, developing a new aspect of the self, finding a personal sense of purpose, desiring to share one’s skills and expertise, redefining the nature and merits of volunteer work, and having a

●◗ Figure 15.7 Percentage of older Americans employed, 2000–2016

65

60

55

20

15

10 2000 2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016

P e rc

e n

t o

f th

e p

o p

u la

ti o

n e

m p

lo y e d

J a n

u a ry

2 0

0 0 – M

a y 2

01 6

Year

Share of Older Americans on the Job

59.9%

18.8%

Total population

Great RecessionRecession

Population ages 65+

From DeSilver, D. (2016). More older Americans are working, and working more, than they used to. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/06/20/more-older-americans-are-working-and- working-more-than-they-used-to/.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

518 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

more highly educated and healthy population of older adults along with greatly expanded opportunities for people to become involved in volunteer work they enjoy. Research in New Zealand documents older adults who find volunteering enables them to give back to their local communities (Wiles & Jayasinha, 2013). Brown and colleagues (2011) argue volunteerism offers a way for society to tap into the vast resources older adults offer.

There is also evidence the expectations of people who volunteer in retirement are chang- ing. Seaman (2012) notes women in the leading edge of the baby-boom generation are inter- ested in volunteering for personal, rather than purely altruistic reasons and do so on their own terms. They are not as willing as volunteers were in previous generations to serve on time-consuming boards and to engage in fundraising. As a result, organizations that rely on volunteers need to be in touch with the concerns and motivations of their pool of volunteers.

The U.S. government has been tapping into this pool of talented volunteers since President John F. Kennedy’s presidency, when the Senior Corps was created. As part of the Corporation for National and Community Service, the Senior Corps consists of three major programs: Foster grandparents, Retired Senior Volunteer Program (RSVP), and Senior Companions (Corporation for National and Community Service, 2016). These national programs have chapters across the country, and interested people aged 55 and over are encouraged to explore local opportunities. These and other programs provide organized ways to get and stay active and to find meaningful forms of engagement.

Some retired adults volunteer as a way to stay active.

M ik

e G

re en

la r/

Th e

Im ag

e W

or ks

Recall 1. One useful way to view retirement is as a(n) _______________.

2. The most common reason people retire is _______________.

3. Overall, most retirees are _______________with retirement.

4. Many retirees maintain contacts in their communities by _______________.

Interpret Why does forced retirement have a negative effect on health?

Apply ●● Using the information from Chapter 12 on occupational devel-

opment, create a developmental description of occupations that incorporates retirement.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 15.3

Think About It With regard to politics, what might the opportunity for more older adults to volunteer for organizations mean?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 519

Alma was married to Charles for 46 years. Even though he died 20 years ago, Alma still speaks about him as if he only recently passed away. Alma still gets sad on special dates— their wedding anniversary, Charles’s birthday, and the date on which he died. Alma tells everyone that she and Chuck, as she called him, had a wonderful marriage and that she still misses him terribly even after all these years.

To older adults such as Alma, the most important thing in life is relationships. In this sec- tion, we consider many of the relationships that older adults have. Whether it is friendship or family ties, having relationships with others is what keeps people connected. Thus, when one’s partner is in need of care, it is not surprising to find wives and husbands devoting themselves to caregiving. Widows such as Alma also feel close to their departed partners. For a growing number of older adults, becoming a great-grandparent is an exciting time.

We have seen throughout this text how our lives are shaped by others and how we share the company of others. The term social convoy is used to suggest how a group of people journeys with us throughout our lives, providing support in good and bad times. People form the convoy, and under ideal conditions, that convoy provides a protective, secure cushion that permits people to explore and learn about the world (English & Carstensen, 2014; Luong, Charles, & Fingerman, 2011). Especially for older adults, the social convoy also provides a source of affirmation of who they are and what they mean to others, which leads to better mental health and well-being.

Several studies have shown that the size of one’s social convoy and the amount of sup- port it provides do not differ across generations. This finding strongly supports the con- clusion that friends and family are essential aspects of all adults’ lives. Social support is especially important in the African American community, as these networks provide all sorts of informal assistance for health-related and other issues (Warren-Findlow & Issel, 2010). Social networks also play a critical role in helping immigrants; for example, when settling in their new country, older Mexican American and Filipino immigrants are assisted by family members in their network (Miller-Martinez & Wallace, 2007; Molina, 2012).

Friends, Siblings, and Socioemotional Selectivity By late life, a person may have been friends with members of his or her social network for several decades. Research consistently finds that older adults have the same need for friends as do people in younger generations; it also shows that their life satisfaction is poorly correlated with the number or quality of relationships with younger family mem- bers yet is strongly correlated with the number and quality of their friendships (Blieszner & Roberto, 2012). Why? As will become clear, friends serve as confidants and sources of sup- port in ways that relatives (e.g., children or nieces and nephews) typically do not.

Friendships In Chapter 11, we explored the meanings and qualities of friendships across young adult- hood and middle age. Patterns of friendship among older adults tend to mirror those in young adulthood (Rawlins, 2004). That is, older women have more numerous and more intimate friendships than older men do (Blieszner & Roberto, 2012).

social convoy Group of people who journey with us throughout our lives, providing support in good times and bad.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● What roles do friends and family play in late life? What is

socioemotional selectivity?

●● What are older adults’ marriages and same-sex partner- ships like?

●● What is it like to provide basic care for one’s partner?

●● How do people cope with widowhood? How do men and women differ?

●● What special issues are involved in being a great-grandparent?

15.4 Friends and Family in Late Life

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

520 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

There are some important differences in later life, though. The quality and purpose of late-life friendships are particularly important (Bromell & Cagney, 2014; Schulz & Morycz, 2013). Friends are sometimes even more important to older adults in part because older adults do not want to become burdens to their families (Blieszner & Roberto, 2012; Moor- man & Greenfield, 2010). As a result, friends help each other foster independence. Having friends also provides a buffer against the loss of roles and status that accompany old age, such as retirement or the death of a loved one, and can increase people’s happiness and self-esteem (Schulz & Morycz, 2013). People who live alone especially benefit from friends in the neighborhood (Bromell & Cagney, 2014). And people who have ambivalent  feelings about their social networks have shorter telomeres than people who have strong feel- ings about theirs (Uchino et al., 2012).

In the case of online friendships (e.g., through social media), trust develops on the basis of four sources: (1) reputation; (2) performance, or what users do online; (3) pre- commitment (through personal self-disclosure); and (4) situational factors, especially the premium placed on intimacy and the relationship (Henderson & Gilding, 2004). Not surprisingly, online social network friendships develop much like face-to-face friend- ships in that the more time people spend online with friends, the more likely they are to self- disclose (Chang & Hsiao, 2014). Online environments are more conducive to people who are lonely (e.g., live alone), which make them potentially important for older adults ( Cotton, Anderson, & McCullough, 2013).

Siblings A special type of friendship exists with one’s siblings; they are the friends who people typically have the longest and with whom people share the closest bonds. The importance of these relationships varies with age (Carr & Moorman, 2011; Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). The centrality of siblings in later life depends on several things: proximity, health, and degree of relatedness (full, step-, or half-siblings), for example. No clear pattern of emotional closeness emerges when viewing sibling relationships on the basis of gender.

When sibling relationships are close and one sibling dies, the surviving sibling often reports reflecting, about or talking to the deceased sibling for advice (Vacha-Haase, Don- aldson, & Foster, 2014). What is also clear is that sibling relationships in later life are affected by events in the lives of the siblings that may have occurred decades earlier ( Knipscheer & van Tilburg, 2013). If there was no contact between the siblings over an extended period of time, it is unlikely that these relational rifts will be repaired. An important source for such

tension is hard feelings stemming from perceived parental favor- itism among siblings. This tension subsequently plays out when aging parents need care, and it may continue into the siblings’ own late life (Gilligan et al., 2013). In such situations, jealousies that have their roots in childhood fester for decades, only to resurface when siblings need to coordinate parental care.

More research into sibling relationships in late life is needed to provide a better understanding of how siblings (and step-siblings) get along and how they come to rely on each other for assistance in certain situations.

Socioemotional Selectivity Why are friends important to older adults? Some researchers believe one reason may be older adults’ not wanting to become bur- dens to their families (Adams & Taylor, 2015; Blieszner & Roberto, 2012; Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). As a result, friends help each other remain independent by providing transportation, checking on neighbors, and doing errands.

Older adults tend to have fewer relationships with people in general and develop fewer new relationships than people do in midlife and particularly in young adulthood (Adams & Taylor,

Siblings play an important role in the lives of older adults.

Br ow

ni e

H ar

ris /C

or bi

s/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Think About It What role might siblings play in an older adult’s autobiographical memory (see Chapter 14)?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 521

2015; Carr & Moorman, 2011). Carstensen and colleagues (Carstensen, 2006; Charles & Carstensen, 2010; English & Carstensen, 2016; Reed & Carstensen, 2012) have shown the changes in social behavior seen in late life reflect a more complicated and important pro- cess. They propose a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity, that argues social con- tact is motivated by a variety of goals, including information seeking, self-concept, and emotional regulation.

Each of these goals is differentially salient at different points of the adult life span and results in different social behaviors. When information seeking is the goal, such as when a person is exploring the world trying to figure out how he or she fits in, what others are like, and so forth, meeting many new people is an essential part of the process. However, when emotional regulation is the goal, people become highly selective in their choice of social partners and nearly always prefer people who are familiar to them.

Carstensen and colleagues believe information seeking is the predominant goal for young adults, emotional regulation is the major goal for older people, and both goals are in balance in midlife. Their research supports this view; people become increasingly selec- tive in whom they choose to have contact with. Additionally, evidence suggests there is an increase with age in emotional competency (e.g., Doerwald et al., 2016; Magai, 2008). Older adults appear to orient more toward emotional aspects of life and personal relation- ships as they grow older, and emotional expression and experience become more complex and nuanced. Carstensen’s theory provides a complete explanation of why older adults tend not to replace, to any great extent, the relationships they lose: Older adults are more selective and have fewer opportunities to make new friends, especially in view of the emo- tional bonds involved in friendships.

Marriage and Same-Sex Partnerships “It’s great to be 72 and still married,” said Lucia. “Yeah, it’s great to have Juan around to share old times with and to have him know how I feel even before I tell him.” Lucia and Juan are typical of most older married couples.

Marital satisfaction in long-term marriages—that is, marriages of 40 years or more—is a complex issue. In general, marital satisfaction among older couples increases shortly after retirement but then decreases with health problems and advancing age, and is directly related to the level of perceived support each partner receives (Landis et al., 2013; Proulx, 2016). The level of satisfaction in these marriages appears to be unrelated to the amount of past or present sexual interest or sexual activity, but it is positively related to the degree of social engagement such as interaction with friends (Bennett, 2005). In keeping with the married-singles concept, many older couples have simply developed detached, contented styles (Connidis, 2001; Lamanna, Riedmann, & Stewart, 2015; Proulx, 2016).

Older married couples show several specific characteristics (O’Rourke & Cappeliez, 2005). Many older couples show a selective memory regarding the occurrence of negative events and perceptions of their partner. Older couples have a reduced potential for marital conflict and greater potential for pleasure, are more likely to be similar in terms of mental and physical health, and show fewer gender differences in sources of pleasure. This is especially true if the couple have developed strong dyadic coping strategies, that is, coping strategies that rely on the interconnectedness of the couple (Berg et al., 2016). In short, older married couples developed adaptive ways to avoid conflict and grew more alike. In general, marital satisfaction among older couples remains high until health problems begin to interfere with the relationship (Connidis, 2001).

Being married in late life has several benefits. A study of 9,333 Euro- pean Americans, African Americans, and Latino Americans showed mar- riage helps people deal better with chronic illness, functional problems, and disabilities (Pienta, Hayward, & Jenkins, 2000). Although the division of

socioemotional selectivity Theory that argues that social contact is motivated by a variety of goals, including information seeking, self-concept, and emotional regulation.

Older, long-term married couples tend to emphasize their partner’s positive attributes.

Li sa

F. Y

ou ng

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

522 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

household chores becomes more egalitarian after the husband retires than it was when the husband was employed, irrespective of whether the wife was working outside the home, women still do more than half of the work (Kulik, 2011).

King (2016) argues that the experiences of older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults can- not be put into need categories, even generational ones. Rather, King emphasizes that the LGB community is at least as diverse as the heterosexual community, and needs to be understood as such.

With the advent of legalized same-sex marriage, numerous issues that heterosexual married couples have long taken for granted are being confronted in the LGBTQ com- munity, including end-of-life issues and legal matters regarding caregiving (Godfrey, 2016; Orel & Coon, 2016). Changes to state laws will continue as reforms and revisions continue to keep pace with change.

When compared to LGB individuals living with partners, LGB individuals living alone or with others (but not in a relationship with them) reported higher degrees of loneliness (Kim & Fredriksen-Goldsen, 2016). This finding parallels that of heterosexual individuals in similar living arrangements, and highlights the fact that there are many similarities in personal outcomes across various gender identity groups.

Little research has been conducted examining the development of transgender and gender nonconforming (TGNC) individuals across adulthood (Witten, 2016). One detailed examination of experiences of TGNC adults over age 50 found that many barriers to access- ing key services such as health care and social services exist, largely due to anti-TGNC prejudice, discrimination, and lack of appropriate and adequate training of professionals (Porter et al., 2016). Additionally, research indicates that transgender older adults experi- ence social isolation more than most other groups (Harley, Gassaway, & Dunkley, 2016).

So how do long-term relationships change and develop from midlife through late life? Wickrama et al. (2013) developed a model that takes into account the influences of genetic markers, personal characteristics, cumulative life experiences, and stressful events during the period from late midlife through later life. The model, shown in ◗ Figure 15.8, shows how all of these influences interact.

The key point in the model is that people bring their past with them when they enter late life, and those experiences, including their genetic makeup, work together to create the relationships that people experience as older adults. Although Wickrama et al. (2013) initially created the model to describe baby boomers’ marriages as they experience retire- ment, the researchers point out that the model is likely applicable to other forms of long- term relationships, such as same-sex relationships and cohabitation.

●◗ Figure 15.8 Marital functioning from middle to later years: A life course–stress process framework.

Middle Years

1. Cumulative chronic life experiences (adversities) • Marital functioning • Financial strain/job status • Work conditions • Parent-child relationship • Family of origin/sibling relationship

4. Stressful mid- late life events & transitions • Retirement status, form, & timing • Individual losses • Economic losses • Service losses • Social losses • Relocation

5. The level and change of marital functioning

Marital Interactions

Marital Quality

A1

B1

B2

A2

G

D

G

E

F H

C

3. Genotype

2. Personal characteristics Psychological: control, mastery Health: mental and physical

Mid-Later Transition Later Years: Posttransition

From Wickrama, K. A. S., O’Neal, C. W., & Lorenz, F. O. (2013). Marital functioning from middle to later years: A life course–stress process framework. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 5, 15–34. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12000. Retrieved from onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jftr.12000/full.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 523

Caring for a Partner When couples pledge their love to each other “in sickness and in health,” most of them envi- sion the sickness part to be no worse than an illness lasting a few weeks. That may be the case for many couples, but for others, the illness they experience severely tests their pledge.

Francine and Ron are one such couple. After 51 years of mostly good times together, Ron was diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease. When she was first contacted by research- ers, Francine had been caring for Ron for six years. “At times, it’s very hard, especially when he looks at me and doesn’t have any idea who I am. Imagine after all these years not to recognize me. But I love him, and I know that he would do the same for me. But to be perfectly honest, we’re not the same couple we once were. We’re not as close; I guess we really can’t be.”

Francine and Ron are typical of couples in which one partner cares for the other. Car- ing for a chronically ill partner presents different challenges from caring for a chronically ill parent. The partner caregiver assumes the new role after decades of shared responsi- bilities. Often without warning, the division of labor that had worked for years must be readjusted. Such change inevitably puts stress on the relationship (Haley, 2013). This is especially true when one’s spouse/partner has a debilitating chronic disease.

Studies of partner caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease show that satisfac- tion with the relationship is much lower than for healthy couples (Cavanaugh & Kinney, 1994; Haley, 2013; Kulik, 2016; Proulx, 2016). Spousal and same-sex partner caregivers report a loss of companionship and intimacy over the course of caregiving, but also receive more rewards compared with adult child caregivers (Croghan, Moone, & Olson, 2014; Kimmel, 2014; Raschick & Ingersoll-Dayton, 2004; Revenson et al., 2016). Marital satisfac- tion is also an important predictor of spousal caregivers’ reports of depressive symptoms; the better the perceived quality of the marriage, the fewer symptoms caregivers report (Kinney & Cavanaugh, 1993), a finding that holds across European American and minority spousal caregivers (Chiao, Wu, & Hsiao, 2015; Parker, 2008).

Most partner caregivers adopt the caregiver role out of necessity. Although evidence about the mediating role of caregivers’ appraisal of stressors is unclear, interventions that help improve the functional level of the ill partner generally improve the caregiving part- ner’s situation (Revenson et al., 2016; van den Wijngaart, Vernooij-Dassen, & Felling, 2007).

The importance of feeling competent as a partner caregiver fits with the docility component of the competence–environmental press model presented earlier in this chap- ter. Caregivers attempt to balance their perceived competence with the environmental demands of caregiving. Perceived competence allows them to be proactive rather than merely reactive (and docile), which gives them a better chance to optimize their situation.

Even in the best of committed relationships, providing full-time care for a partner is both stressful and rewarding in terms of the relationship (Haley, 2013; Kimmel, 2014; Revenson et al., 2016). For example, coping with a wife who may not remember her hus- band’s name, who may act strangely, and who has a chronic and fatal disease presents seri- ous challenges even to the happiest of couples. Yet even in that situation, the caregiving husband may experience no change in marital happiness despite the changes in his wife due to the disease.

Widowhood Alma, the woman in our vignette, still feels the loss of her husband, Chuck. “There are many times when I feel him around. When you’re together for so long, you take it for granted that your husband will always be there. And there are times when I just don’t want to go on without him. But I suppose I’ll get through it.”

Like Alma and Chuck, virtually all older married couples see their marriages end legally when one partner dies. For most people, the death of a partner is one of the most traumatic events they experience, causing an increased risk of death among older European Americans (but not African Americans), an effect that lasts several years (Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). An extensive study of widowed adults in Scotland showed the increased

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

524 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

likelihood of dying lasted for at least 10 years (Boyle, Feng, & Raab, 2011). Despite the stress of losing one’s partner, most widowed older adults manage to cope reasonably well (Moorman & Greenfield, 2010).

The effects are broader than just mortality, though. Lucy Kalanithi (2016) described how her marriage vows to love, to honor, and to be loyal to her husband did not end with his death at age 37 from cancer. For her, mar- riage did not end with her transition to being a widow.

Women are much more likely to be widowed than are men. More than half of all women over age 65 are widows, but only 15% of men the same age are widowers. Women have longer life expectancies and typically marry men older than themselves. Consequently, the average married woman can expect to live at least 10 years as a widow.

The impact of widowhood goes well beyond the ending of a long-term partnership (Boyle et al., 2011; Brenn & Ytterstad, 2016; McCoyd & Walter, 2016). For example, there is evidence of increased mortality, especially within the first week of widowhood (Brenn & Ytterstad, 2016).

Loneliness is a major problem. Widowed people may be left alone by family and friends who do not know how to deal with a bereaved person. As a result, widows and widowers may lose not only a partner, but also those friends and family who feel uncomfortable with including a single person rather than a couple in social functions (McCoyd & Walter, 2016).

Feelings of loss do not dissipate quickly, as the case of Alma shows clearly. Men and women react differently to widowhood. In general, those who were most dependent on their partners during the marriage report the highest increase in self-esteem in widowhood when they learn to do the tasks formerly done by their partners (Carr, 2004). Widowers may

recover more slowly unless they have strong social support systems (Bennett, 2010). Widows often suffer more financially because survivor’s benefits are usually only half of their husband’s pensions (Weaver, 2010). For many women, widowhood results in difficult financial circumstances, particularly regarding medical expenses (McGarry & Schoeni, 2005).

For many reasons, including the need for companionship and financial security, some widowed people cohabit or remarry. A newer variation on repartnering is “living alone together,” an arrangement where two older adults form a romantic relationship but main- tain separate living arrangements (Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). Repartnering in widow- hood can be difficult because of family objections (e.g., resistance from children), objective limitations (decreased mobility, poorer health, poorer finances), absence of incentives common to younger ages (desire for children), and social pressures to protect one’s estate (Moorman & Greenfield, 2010).

Great-Grandparenthood As discussed in Chapter 13, grandparenting is an important and enjoyable role for many adults. With increasing numbers of people—especially women—living to very old age, more people are experiencing great-grandparenthood. Age at first marriage and age at parenthood also play a critical role; people who reach these milestones at relatively young ages are more likely to become great-grandparents.

Although surprisingly little research has been conducted on great-grandparents, their investment in their roles as parents, grandparents, and great-grandparents forms a sin- gle family identity (Drew & Silverstein, 2004; Even-Zohar & Garby, 2016; Moorman & Greenfield, 2010). That is, great-grandparents see a true continuity of the family through the passing on of genes. However, their sources of satisfaction and meaning apparently differ from those of grandparents (Doka & Mertz, 1988; Even-Zohar & Garby, 2016; Wentkowski, 1985). Compared with grandparents, great-grandparents are more similar as a group in what they derive from the role, largely because they are less involved with the children than the grandparents are.

Widowers are less likely than widows to form new friendships, continuing a trend throughout adulthood that men have fewer close friends than do women.

Co lin

Y ou

ng -W

ol ff

/P ho

to Ed

it

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 525

Three aspects of great-grandparenthood appear to be most important (Doka & Mertz, 1988). First, being a great-grandparent provides a sense of personal and family renewal— important components for achieving integrity. Their grandchildren have produced new life, renewing their own excitement for life and reaffirming the continuance of their lin- eage. Seeing their families stretch across four generations may also provide psychological support, through feelings of symbolic immortality, to help them face death. They take pride and comfort in knowing that their families will live many years beyond their own lifetime. Keeping the family together is a major source of meaning (Even-Zohar & Garby, 2016).

Second, great-grandchildren provide new diversions in great-grandparents’ lives. There are now new people with whom they can share their experiences. Young children can learn from a person they perceive as “really old” (Mietkiewicz & Venditti, 2004).

Third, becoming a great-grandparent is a major milestone, a mark of longevity that most people never achieve. The sense that one has lived long enough to see the fourth generation is perceived very positively.

As you might expect, people with at least one living grandparent and great- grandparent interact more with their grandparent, who is also perceived as more influential (Roberto & Skoglund, 1996). Unfortunately, some great-grandparents must assume the role of pri- mary caregiver to their great-grandchildren, a role for which few great-grandparents are prepared (Bengtson, Mills, & Parrott, 1995; Burton, 1992). As more people live longer, it will be interesting to see whether the role of great-grandparents changes and becomes more prominent.

Recall 1. The longest relationship most people have is with their

_______________.

2. In general, older couples have reduced likelihood of _______________.

3. Compared to noncaregiver spouses, marital satisfaction in spousal caregivers is _______________.

4. _______________ are at special risk of experiencing a drop in living standard following the death of their spouse.

5. Three aspects of being a great-grandparent that are espe- cially important are personal and family renewal, diversion, and _______________.

Interpret ●● Why would widows and widowers want to establish a new

cohabitation relationship?

Apply ●● How do the descriptions of marital satisfaction and spou-

sal caregiving presented here fit with the descriptions of marital satisfaction in Chapter 11 and caring for aging par- ents in Chapter 13? What similarities and differences are there?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 15.4

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ●● Who are frail older adults? How common is frailty?

●● What housing options are available for older adults?

●● How do you know whether an older adult is abused, neglected, or exploited?

●● What are the key social policy issues affecting older adults?

15.5 Social Issues and Aging

Sakhra is an 82-year-old woman who still lives in the same neighborhood where she grew up. She has been in relatively good health for most of her life, but in the last year, she has needed help with tasks such as preparing meals and shopping for personal items. Sakhra very much wants to continue living in her own home. She doesn’t want to be placed in a nursing home, but her family wonders whether that might be the best option.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

526 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

Our consideration of late life thus far has focused on the experiences of the typical older adult. In this final section, we consider people such as Sakhra, who represent a sub- stantial number but still a minority of all older adults. Like Sakhra, some older adults experience problems completing common tasks such as taking care of themselves. We will consider the prevalence and kinds of problems such people face. Although most older adults live in the community, some reside in other settings; we will consider the housing options that older adults have. Unfortunately, some older adults are the victims of abuse or neglect; we will examine some of the key issues relating to how elder abuse happens. Finally, we will conclude with an overview of the most important emerging social policy issues.

All of these issues are critical when viewed from Baltes and Smith’s (2003) Fourth Age perspective, as described in Chapter 14. We will see that it is the oldest-old who make up most of the frail older adults living in both the community and long-term care facilities such as nursing homes. With the rapid increase in the number of oldest-old on the horizon (the baby-boom generation), finding ways to deal with these issues is essential.

Frail Older Adults In our discussion about aging to this point, we focused on the majority of older adults who are healthy, are cognitively competent, are reasonably financially secure, and have good family relationships. Some older adults are not as fortunate. They are the frail older adults who have physical disabilities, are very ill, may have cognitive or psychological disorders, and need assistance with everyday tasks. These frail older adults constitute a minority of the population over age 65, but the size of this group increases considerably with age.

Frail older adults are people whose competence (in terms of the competence–environmental press model presented ear- lier) is declining. They do not have one specific problem that differentiates them from their active, healthy counterparts; instead, they tend to have multiple problems (Wilhelm-Leen et al., 2014). Some researchers zero in on the age-related loss of muscle mass and strength, called sarcopenia (Afilalo, 2016). Other researchers argue that frailty is the result of accumulated deficits in several functional areas (Rockwood, 2016). In either case, to identify the areas in which people experience limited functioning, researchers have developed observational and self- report techniques to measure how well people can accomplish daily tasks.

Everyday competence assessment consists of examining how well people can complete activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living, and whether they have other physical limi- tations (Verbrugge, Brown, & Zajacova, 2017). Activities of daily living (ADLs) include basic self-care tasks such as eating, bathing, toileting, walking, or dressing. A person can be considered frail if he or she needs help with one or more of these tasks. Instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) are actions that entail some intel- lectual competence and planning. Which activities constitute IADLs varies widely across cultures. For example, for most adults in Western culture, IADLs would include shopping for personal items, paying bills, making telephone calls, taking medications appropriately, and keeping appointments. In other cultures, IADLs might include caring for animal herds, making bread, threshing grain, and tending crops. A third way of assessing competence is to focus on physical limitations (PLIMs), activities that reflect func- tional limitations such as walking a block or sitting for about two hours.

frail older adults Older adults who have physical disabilities, are very ill, may have cognitive or psycho- logical disorders, and need assistance with everyday tasks.

activities of daily living (ADLs) Basic self-care tasks such as eating, bathing, toileting, walking, and dressing.

instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) Actions that require some intellectual com- petence and planning.

physical limitations (PLIMs) Activities that reflect functional limitations such as walking a block or sitting for about two hours.

Older adults over age 85 are more likely to be frail and to need help with basic daily tasks.

Jo hn

B ird

sa ll/

Th e

Im ag

e W

or ks

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 527

As you can see in ◗ Figure 15.9, the number of difficulties with ADLs, IADLs, and PLIMs increases as men and women age (Verbrugge et al., 2017). Such limitations increase rapidly when the person nears death. People who live longer show both fewer disabilities on average as well as slower increases in disability over time. By paying close attention to the number and extent of disabilities, healthcare professionals and family members may get advance warning of serious underlying problems.

What Causes Functional Limitations and Disability in Older Adults? If you and your classmates created a list of all the conditions you believe cause functional limitations and disabilities in older adults, the list undoubtedly would be long. (Try it and see for yourself.) But by strategically combining a large representative sample of condi- tions with sophisticated statistical analyses, this list can be shortened greatly. If these steps are taken, what conditions best predict future problems in functioning?

In a classic longitudinal study conducted over three decades, Strawbridge and col- leagues (1998) found that smoking, heavy drinking, physical inactivity, depression, social isolation, and fair or poor perceived health predicted who would become disabled in some way. As predicted by Verbrugge and Jette (1994), lack of physical activity is a powerful pre- dictor of later disability and with higher rates of cancer, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and obesity, all of which result in higher rates of disability and premature death (Afilalo, 2016; Gretebeck et al., 2012; Rockwood, 2016).

How Important Are Socioeconomic Factors? Once we have identified the specific conditions that are highly predictive of future func- tional limitations, an important question is whether the appropriate intervention and pre- vention programs should be targeted at particular groups of people. That is, would people who are well educated and have high incomes have the same rate of key chronic conditions

●◗ Figure 15.9 Mean disability of the AHEAD living cohort by gender, 1993–2010. Mean disability of the AHEAD liv- ing cohort by gender, 1993–2010. Waves 1–9 are years 1993, 1995, 1998, 2000, 2002, 2004, 2006, 2008, 2010.

5.0

4.5

4.0

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

0

Women: ADL Women: IADL Women: PLIM Men: ADL Men: IADL Men: PLIM

M e a n

D is

a b

il it

y

Wave 1

Wave 2

Wave 3

Wave 4

Wave 5

Wave 6

Wave 7

Wave 8

Wave 9 Interview Wave

From Verbrugge, L. M., Brown, D. C., & Zajacova, A. (in press). Disability rises gradually for a cohort of older Americans, Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw002

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

528 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

as people in lower socioeconomic groups? If not, then people with different socioeco- nomic backgrounds have different needs.

Research indicates a fairly strong and consistent relationship between socioeconomic status and health-related quality of life. Across all racial and ethnic groups, more affluent older adults have lower levels of disability and higher health-related quality of life than individuals in lower socioeconomic groups (Gardiner, Mishra, & Dobson, 2016; National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). A Canadian study showed that this difference appears to be set in early adulthood and maintained into late life (Ross et al., 2012).

How Does Disability in Older Adults Differ Globally? Throughout this and previous chapters, we have encountered important differences between men and women and between various ethnic/racial and socioeconomic groups. Do these patterns hold globally?

Not surprisingly, the answer is “yes” (World Health Organization, 2012). As the num- ber of older adults rises around the world, the number of people with disabilities or func- tional limitations does, too. Also, the rates of disabilities are higher in low-income countries and among women. Early detection and treatment of chronic disease can lower these rates.

Looked at more closely, some interesting patterns emerge. The United States, for exam- ple, has higher rates of most chronic diseases and functional impairment than England or the rest of Europe (National Institute on Aging, 2012). An important difference is access to health care, in terms of whether everyone is guaranteed access by the government or not.

Housing Options Where you live, and how you feel about it, is a critical aspect of personal identity. We expe- rience this every time someone asks us, “Where do you live?” For most people, it is the sense of place that makes the difference. A sense of place refers to the cognitive and emo- tional attachments that a person puts on his or her place of residence, by which a “house” is made into a “home.”

Throughout adulthood, people adapt to changes in the places where they live, some- times severing connections with past settings (Rowles & Watkins, 2003; Wahl, 2015). Making a change in where people live, and having to psychologically disconnect with a place where they may have lived for many decades, can be difficult and traumatic. There is no question people develop attachments to place, develop a sense of place, and derive a major portion of their identity from it.

Rowles (2006) discusses the process of how a place becomes a home. Because of the psychological connections, the sense that one is “at home” becomes a major concern in relocation, especially if the relocation involves giving up one’s home. This attachment to place appears to be cross-cultural phenomenon (Felix et al., 2015).

Aging in Place Feeling one is “at home” is a major aspect of aging in place. Providing older adults a place to call their own that supports the development of the psychological attachments neces- sary to convert the place to a home is key for successful aging in place (Scheidt & Schwarz, 2010; Wahl, 2015). Aging in place provides a way for older adults to continue finding aspects of self-identity in where they live, and to take advantage of support systems that are established and familiar by balancing environmental press and competence through selection and compensation. Being able to maintain one’s independence in the community is often important for people, especially in terms of their self-esteem and ability to con- tinue engaging in meaningful ways with friends, family, and others.

Whether a person can age in place usually depends on their functional health. Functional health refers to the ability to perform the activities of daily living (ADLs), instru- mental activities of daily living (IADLs), and physical limitations (PLIMs) discussed earlier in this section. As a person’s functional health diminishes, the level of support needed from the environment increases and the optimal housing situation changes.

sense of place Cognitive and emotional attachments that a person puts on his or her place of residence, by which a “house” is made into a “home.”.

functional health Ability to perform the activities of daily liv- ing (ADLs), instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs), and physical limitations (PLIMs).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 529

The competence–environmental press model provides two options for people who experience difficulties dealing with the tasks of daily life. On one hand, people can increase their competency and develop better or new skills for handling. To better remember where you put your car keys, you can learn a new memory strategy. On the other hand, people can lower the environmental press by modifying the environment to make the task easier; putting a hook for the car keys next to the door you exit so you see them on your way out.

These two options represent applications of theory to real-world settings that also apply to helping people deal with the challenges they face in handling tasks of daily living in their homes. When it comes to these kinds of issues, the most frequent solution involves modifying one’s home (i.e., changing the environment) in order to create a new optimal balance or better “fit” between competence and environmen- tal press (Scharlach & Lehning, 2016; Scheidt & Schwarz, 2010).

Many strategies are available for modifying a home to help a per- son accommodate changing competencies. Minor structural changes, such as installing assistive devices (e.g., hand rails in bathrooms and door handles that are easier to grip), are common strategies. In other cases, more extensive modifications may be needed to make a home fully accessible, such as widening doorways, lowering countertops, adding power stairlifts, and constructing ramps.

Although minor alterations can often be done at low cost, more extensive modifications needed by people with greater limitations may be unaffordable for low-income individuals. Even though the cost of such interventions is significantly lower than placement in long-term care facilities or assisted living, many people simply cannot afford them. As a result, many older adults with functional impair- ments experience a mismatch between their competency and their environment (Granbom et al., 2016; Iwarsson et al., 2012; Wahl, 2015).

Research indicates home modifications done to address difficulties with accomplish- ing activities of daily living (ADLs) typically reduce disability-related outcomes ( Iwarsson et al., 2012; Wahl, 2015; Yuen & Vogtle, 2016). Although home modification can help reduce falls in older adults (Ripp, Jones, & Zhang, 2016), understanding the role of self- efficacy beliefs in falling is also an important factor to address in successful interventions to reduce falling (Dadgari et al., 2015).

Assisted Living Bessie lived in the same home for 57 years. Her familiar surroundings enabled her to com- pensate for and manage the challenges of failing eyesight and worsening arthritis that made it hard to walk. Eventually, though, her health problems interfered with her ability to live alone. Finally, her children convinced her that she needed to relocate to an assisted living facility that would provide the necessary support for her to continue to have her own space.

Bessie is fairly typical of people who move to assisted living facilities. Assisted living facilities provide a supportive living arrangement for people who need assistance with ADLs or IADLs but who are not so impaired physically or cognitively that they need 24-hour care. Estimates are that nearly two-thirds of residents of assisted living facilities over age 65 have an ADL or IADL limitation (AgingStats.gov, 2016) and that about half have some degree of memory impairment (Pynoos et al., 2010). Assisted living facilities usually provide support for activities of daily living (e.g., assistance with bathing) as well as meals and other services. Healthcare personnel are available to assist with medications and certain other procedures.

Assisted living facilities provide a range of services and care, but they are not designed to provide intensive, around-the-clock medical care. As residents become frailer, their needs may go beyond those the facility can provide. Policies governing discharge for this reason, usually to a long-term care facility, may result in competing interests between providing

assisted living facilities Supportive living arrangement for people who need assistance with ADLs or IADLs but who are not so impaired physically or cognitively that they need 24-hour care.

One way that older adults can age in place is to renovate their home

Su sa

nH Sm

ith /E

+/ G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

530 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

for the residents’ increasing medical needs and providing a familiar environment. To help families choose the best option, the National Center for Assisted Living (2016) provides a Consumer’s Guide that leads families through a series of issues such as the level of care needs, now and in the future, location, and various key metrics regarding staffing. Unfor- tunately, some older adults eventually need a higher level of care that is available at assisted living facilities, so they must turn to long-term care facilities such as nursing homes.

Nursing Homes The last place Sadie thought she would end up was in a bed in a nursing home. “That’s a place where old people go to die,” she would tell her friends. “It’s not gonna be for me.” But here she is. Sadie fell a few weeks ago and broke her hip. Because she lives alone, she needs to stay in the facility for rehabilitation until she recovers. She detests the food; “tasteless,” she calls it. Doris, who is 87 and has dementia, lives a few doors down. Doris is likely to live there for the remainder of her life.

Sadie and Doris are representative of the people who live in nursing homes—some temporarily, some permanently. If given the choice, the vast majority of older adults do not want to live there; they and their families would prefer that they age in place. Sometimes, though, placement in a nursing home is necessary because of the older person’s needs or the family’s circumstances.

A nursing home is a type of long-term care facility that provides medical care 24 hours a day, 7 days a week using a team of healthcare professionals that includes physicians (who must be on call at all times), nurses, therapists (e.g., physical, occupational), and others. Misconceptions about nursing homes are common. Contrary to what some people believe, only about 5% of older adults live in nursing homes on any given day. As you can see in ◗ Figure 15.10, the percentage of older adults enrolled in Medicare who live in a long-term care facility at any given point in time increases from 1% in those aged 65 to74 to 15% of

nursing home Type of long-term care facility that provide medical care 24 hours a day, 7 days a week using a team of healthcare professionals that includes physicians (who must be on call at all times), nurses, therapists (e.g., physical, occupational), and others.

●◗ Figure 15-10 Percentage distribution of Medicare benefi- ciaries age 65 and over residing in selected residential settings, by age group, 2013.

0

20

40

60

80

100 3 15

8

77

9393 98

4 1

1

P er

ce n

t

65 and over

NOTE: Community housing with services applies to respondents who reported they lived in retirement communities or apartments, senior citizen housing, continuing care retirement facilities, assisted living facilities, staged living communities, board and care facilities/homes, and similar situations and who reported they had access to one or more of the following services through their place of residence: meal preparation, cleaning or housekeeping services, laundry services, or help with medications. Respondents were asked about access to these services, but not whether they actually used the services. A residence (or unit) is considered a long-term care facility if it is certif ied by Medicare or Medicaid; or has 3 or more beds, is licensed as a nursing home or other long-term care facility, and provides at least one personal care service; or provides 24-hour, 7-day-a-week supervision by a non-family, paid caregiver. Reference population: These data refer to Medicare benef iciaries.

65–74 75–84 85 and over

Long-term care facilities Community housing with services

Traditional community

4 3

From Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. (2016). 2013 Medicare current beneficiary access to care PUF. Retrieved from www.cms.gov/Research-Statistics-Data-and-Systems/Downloadable-Public- Use-Files/MCBS-Public-Use-File/index.html.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 531

adults over age 85 (AgingStats.gov, 2016). Someone who turns 65 today has a nearly 70% chance of spending some time in a long-term care facility, with women requiring longer care (3.7 years on average) than men (2.2 years on average) (LongTermCare.gov, 2016). The gender difference is due to the fact that older women take care of their husbands at home, but then they, in turn, need to relocate to long-term care facilities for their own care because their husbands are, on average, deceased.

The decision to place a family member in a nursing home is a difficult one (Caron, Ducharme, & Griffith, 2006; Krull, 2013). Placement decisions can occur in reaction to a crisis, such as a person’s impending discharge from a hospital or another health emergency, or can be the outcome of many months or even years of conversation among family members. Because the level of impairment of nursing home residents is typi- cally severe (with an average of four out of a possible five areas of impairment with ADLs and 60% with moderate to severe cognitive impairment; Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 2016b), the placement decision tends to be made by partners or adult children, often in consultation with medical and spiritual advisers, a finding that generalizes across ethnic groups (Koss & Ekerdt, in press; Krull, 2013; Lord et al., 2016; Ruiz et al., 2016).

Selecting a nursing home should be done carefully. The Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (2016a) provides a detailed Nursing Home Quality Initiative website that is a guide for choosing a nursing home based on several key quality factors. Among the most important things to consider are quality of life for residents (e.g., whether resi- dents are well groomed, the food is tasty, and rooms contain comfortable furniture); qual- ity of care (whether staff members respond quickly to calls, whether staff and family are involved in care decisions); safety (whether there are enough staff members, whether hall- ways are free of clutter); and other issues (whether outdoor areas are available for residents to use). These aspects of nursing homes reflect the dimensions that states consider in their inspections and licensing process.

Emerging Housing Arrangements In response to the need to provide support for older adults who require assistance with ADLs and IADLs and their desire to age in place, new approaches to housing options have emerged that provide both. These movements include programs that infuse a different culture into nursing homes as well as those approaches that create small-scale living (usu- ally six to ten residents) in a community-based setting with an emphasis on living well rather than on receiving care (Pynoos et al., 2010).

The Eden Alternative (Eden Alternative, 2017) seeks to eliminate loneliness, helpless- ness, and boredom from the lives of those living in long-term care facilities and to create a community in which life is worth living. This can be achieved by rethinking how care is provided in the older person’s own home or in long-term care facilities through training.

The Green House Project (Jenkins, 2016) creates small neighborhood-integrated homes for six to ten residents in which older adults receive a high level of personal and professional care. The Green House Project takes the principles of the Eden Alternative and creates a different culture of care in the community.

The Pioneer Network (Pioneer Network, 2017) focuses on changing the culture of aging in America irrespective of where older adults live. Like the Eden Alternative, this approach focuses on respecting older adults and providing maximally supportive environments for them. The Pioneer Network, as part of the larger cultural change in caring for older adults, advocates for a major emphasis on making nursing homes more like a home.

Various cohousing and cluster housing options provide additional alternative approaches. Cohousing is a planned community that is modest in size and is built around an open, walkable space designed to foster social interaction among neighbors (Klimczuk, 2017; Pynoos et al., 2010). Neighbors provide care for each other when it is needed as a way to foster personal autonomy. Cluster housing combines the aging-in-place philosophy with supportive services (de Jong et al., 2012). A key feature is that services are provided

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

532 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

to the residents by staff hired by the owner or by a service provider under contract. The aging-in-place philosophy in these settings emphasizes individual choice on the part of residents in terms of what services to use. This approach is also being adopted in other countries, such as the Netherlands and Spain (de Jong et al., 2012; Fernández-Carro, 2016).

These alternatives to traditional housing options for older adults indicate that the choices for how one spends late life are becoming more varied and that appropriate sup- port systems are in place. Such alternatives will be important as the baby-boom generation enters the years in which support services will be needed even more. Researchers need to focus their attention on documenting the types of advantages these alternatives offer and exploring their relative effectiveness.

Elder Abuse and Neglect Arletta, an 82-year-old woman in relatively poor health, has been living with her 60-year- old daughter, Sally, for the past two years. Recently, neighbors became concerned because they had not seen Arletta very often for the last several months. When they did, she looked rather worn, thin, and unkempt. Finally, the neighbors decided that they should do some- thing; so they called the local office of the Department of Human Services. Upon hearing the details of the situation, a caseworker immediately investigated. The caseworker found that Arletta was severely malnourished, had not bathed in weeks, and appeared disori- ented. Based on these findings, the agency concluded that Arletta was a victim of neglect. She was moved temporarily to a county nursing home.

Although elder abuse, neglect, and exploitation are difficult to define precisely, the following categories are commonly used (National Center for State Courts, 2016):

●● Physical abuse: use of physical force that may result in bodily injury, physical pain, or impairment

●● Sexual abuse: nonconsensual sexual contact of any kind ●● Emotional or psychological abuse: infliction of anguish, pain, or distress ●● Financial or material exploitation: illegal or improper use of an older adult’s funds,

property, or assets ●● Abandonment: desertion of an older adult by an individual who had physical custody or

otherwise had assumed responsibility for providing care for the older adult ●● Neglect: refusal or failure to fulfill any part of a person’s obligation or duties to an

older adult ●● Self-neglect: behaviors of an older person that threaten his or her health or safety,

excluding those conscious and voluntary decisions made by a mentally competent and healthy adult

Researchers estimate that perhaps one in four vulnerable older adults are at risk for some type of abuse, neglect, or exploitation (Nerenberg, 2010; Roberto, 2016; Wong & Rothenhaus, 2014). Unfortunately, only a small proportion of these cases are reported to authorities; of those that are, neglect is the most common type. Elder abuse is often noted by physicians first, either through routine examinations or in emergency depart- ments in hospitals (Linden & Olshaker, 2014). Financial exploitation, particularly through electronic means, costs older adults billions of dollars annually (DeLiema, Yon, & Wilber, 2016). Researchers and policy makers argue that prevention of financial exploitation can be greatly assisted by banks and other financial institutions, who can monitor account activity and question unusual withdrawals (Lichtenberg, 2016).

Current approaches to elder abuse, especially by adult children, conceptualize it not as an outcome of a caregiving situation, but as a development from the longer ongoing relationship between parent and child (Pickering & Phillips, 2014). Similarly, abuse by inti- mate partners may also be tied to former existing relationship issues even if abuse had not been present previously (Bows, in press; Roberto, McPherson, & Brossoie, 2013). Under- standing, detecting, and addressing elder abuse requires culturally sensitive approaches that reflect the values of older adults in different societies (Teaster, Harley, & Kettaneh,

Think About It How are housing options related to cog- nitive ability and to the competence–envi- ronmental press model?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 533

2014). Although certain characteristics, such as a correlation with disability, occur across cultures, how those characteristics are manifest differ. Likewise, prevention and interven- tion strategies must take cultural differences into account.

As with abuse at other points in the life span, identifying and reporting it is everyone’s concern. If you suspect that an older adult is a victim of elder abuse, neglect, or exploita- tion, the best thing you can do is contact your local adult protective services office and report it.

Social Security and Medicare Without doubt, the most important societal changes in the United States regarding older adults during the 20th century were the creation of retirement income plans such as Social Security and other pension plans as well as universal basic healthcare plans such as Medi- care (Polivka, 2010). Similar programs also were initiated in other developed countries. Even though the costs of such programs continue to rise dramatically, Social Security and Medicare are so strongly supported that elected officials are often afraid to discuss and implement much-needed reforms.

Such programs have reduced the number of older adults who live below the pov- erty line (AgingStats.gov, 2016). However, projections indicate that baby boomers and Generation X members will have lower incomes in old age due to fundamental changes in pension availability and lower savings rates.

In analyzing the financial well-being of older adults, economists argue that older adults should be evaluated by different standards compared with younger or middle-aged adults (AgingStats.gov, 2016). Economists often argue that older adults need 200% of the federal poverty limit to make ends meet, especially with respect to healthcare costs. By this measure, women and minorities are especially at risk financially.

To capture these issues, the Wider Opportunities for Women and the Gerontol- ogy Institute at the University of Massachusetts Boston developed the Elder Economic Security Standard™ Index. The Elder Index is a measure of the income that older adults need to meet their basic housing, health care, transportation, food, and other essential costs and to age in place. Separate indices are available for cities and counties in the United States.

The aging of the baby boomers presents difficult and expensive problems. In fiscal year 2017, federal spending on the various parts of Social Security and Medicare alone was expected to be over $1.5 trillion in a total budget of roughly $4 trillion (Congressional Budget Office, 2017). Note that this expenditure for just these two programs is more than 37% of the federal budget. Without major reforms in these programs, such growth will force extremely difficult choices in how to pay for them.

Social Security Social Security began in 1935 as an initiative by President Franklin D. Roosevelt to “frame a law which will give some measure of protection to the average citizen and to his family against the loss of a job and against poverty-ridden old age” (Roosevelt, 1935). Thus, Social Security was originally intended to provide a supplement to savings and other means of financial support.

Two key things have changed since then. First, the proportion of people who reach age 65 has increased significantly. In 1940, only about 54% of men and 61% of women reached age 65. Today, that’s increased to about 75% of men and 85% of women. Since 1940, men collect payments about 3 years longer, and women collect about 5 years longer. Both trends increase the cost of the program.

Second, revisions to the original law have changed Social Security so it now represents the primary source of financial support after retirement for most U.S. citizens, and the only source for many (Polivka, 2010). Since the 1970s, more workers have been included in employer-sponsored defined contribution plans such as 401(k), 403(b), 457 plans, and

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

534 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

mutual funds, as well as various types of individual retirement accounts (IRAs), but fewer defined benefit traditional pension plans (Polivka, 2010; Polivka & Luo, 2015). A key dif- ference in these plans is defined contribution plans rely a great deal on employee partici- pation (i.e., workers saving money for retirement) whereas traditional pension programs did not require employee participation as they provided a monthly income for life paid completely by the company.

The primary challenge facing Social Security is the aging of the baby boomers and the much smaller Generation X that immediately follows (National Academy of Social Insur- ance, 2016). Because Social Security is funded by payroll taxes, the amount of money each worker must pay depends to a large extent on the ratio of the number of people paying Social Security taxes to the number of people collecting benefits. By 2030, this ratio will drop nearly in half; that is, by the time baby boomers have largely retired, there will only be two workers paying into Social Security to support every person collecting benefits, down from 3-to-1 today.

One point that confuses many people is the benefits received do not come from an “account” that reflects what you actually contributed over your employed career. Because Social Security is a revenue-in/payments-out model, people do not build up Social Secu- rity “savings.” Rather, the money they pay in taxes today goes out as payments to those who are collecting benefits today. So the payments current workers receive in the future will actually be from the taxes paid by workers in the labor force at that future time. That’s why the payee/recipient ratio matters—Social Security tax rates must inevitably go up or the benefits received must go down if there are fewer people paying to support an increasing number of recipients.

Over most of the history of Social Security, revenues were greater than payments because of the large number of people paying in compared to the lower number of people collecting benefits. Excess revenue was saved for the future. But that changed in 2010, when benefit payments first exceeded revenues, a situation that remains true today. The interest from those previous saved revenues in the Social Security Trust Fund is now being spent to make up the difference, and will keep the gap closed until around 2020. After that, the Trust Fund itself gets spent. By 2034, all of the Trust Fund assets will be gone. At that point, revenue from Social Security taxes would only cover about 75% of the benefits promised (Social Security and Medicare Boards of Trustees, 2016).

Despite knowing the fiscal realities for decades, Congress has not yet taken the actions necessary to ensure the long-term financial stability of Social Security.

Medicare Over 55 million U.S. citizens depend on Medicare for their medical insurance (Kaiser Family Foundation, 2016). To be eligible, a person must meet one of the following criteria: be over age 65, be disabled, or have permanent kidney failure. Medicare consists of three parts (Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 2016d): Part A, that covers inpatient hospital services, skilled nursing facilities, home health services, and hospice care; Part B, covers the cost of physician services, outpatient hospital services, medical equipment and supplies, and other health services and supplies; and Part D, that provides some cov- erage for prescription medications. Expenses relating to most long-term care needs are funded by Medicaid, another major healthcare program funded by the U.S. government and aimed at people who are poor. Out-of-pocket expenses associated with copayments and other charges are often paid by supplemental insurance policies, sometimes referred to as “Medigap” policies (Medicare.gov, 2016).

Like Social Security, Medicare is funded by a payroll tax. However, whereas the Social Security payroll tax has an upper limit on the salary used to compute how much people pay (the upper limit of salary increases each year), the Medicare payroll tax is applied to one’s entire salary. Still, the funding problems facing Medicare are similar to those facing Social Security and are greatly exacerbated by the aging of the baby boomers. In addition, Medicare costs have increased dramatically as a result of the rapidly increasing costs of health care (Social Security and Medicare Boards of Trustees, 2016).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 535

Cost containment of health care remains a major political concern, and the issue has been caught in the overall debate about healthcare reform in the United States. Also, because Medicare is a government-run healthcare program, it continues to be controver- sial among those who oppose such approaches to health care. But unlike Social Security, Medicare has already been subjected to significant cuts in expenditures, typically through reduced payouts to healthcare providers. President Obama included reductions to the Medicare Advantage (Part C) program in his Affordable Care Act as part of his overall strategy to control increase in healthcare costs.

Taken together, the challenges facing society concerning older adults’ financial secu- rity and health insurance coverage will continue to be major political and economic issues throughout the first half of the 21st century at least. There are no easy answers and many political challenges, but open discussion of the various arguments and the stark budget realities will be essential for creating the optimal solution. Public misperceptions of how the programs actually work, and the tough choices that must be faced are discussed in more detail in the What Do You Think? feature.

As pointed out earlier in the chapter, the amount of benefits people collect in Social Security and Medicare is not directly connected to the amount they paid in taxes over their working careers. That’s a point many people misunderstand—they think they get out what they have put in their personal account. As explained in the text, this misperception makes it very difficult to make changes in the benefits structure (the cost) of these programs.

The fact is, the average person collects substantially more in lifetime Medicare benefits than he or she paid in total taxes during his or her working career (Steuerle & Quakenbush, 2015). This imbalance (and misperception) is a major reason why restructuring the benefits or raising copayments to control costs is so

difficult to do, and why the current benefit structure is unsustainable.

There’s another aspect to the problem. To keep Medicare Part B and Part D premiums affordable, the typical older American only pays about 25% of the actual cost through premiums, deductibles, and co-insurance (higher income people pay a larger share, up to 80%). Where does the rest of the money come from? General revenue in the national budget pays for the rest, and is why there is increasing pressure to cut reimbursements to healthcare providers under Medicare.

Similar analyses can be done for Social Security, but with a different outcome. Whereas nearly all Medicare recipients get much more out of the program than they

paid in taxes, that’s not true for many people regarding Social Security. Most people will pay more in taxes to support current beneficiaries (Steuerle & Quakenbush, 2015).

The biggest challenge is controlling the costs of Medicare given the arrival of the baby boomers as beneficiaries. But explaining to those who are already receiving or are about to receive the benefit is a daunting task. That’s why most proposals under discussion target individuals who are about 10 years away from receiving Medicare benefits.

The political debates around Social Security and Medicare will not end soon. What is clear, though, is the current financial model is unsus- tainable over the long run, and action, espe- cially with respect to Medicare, is needed now.

What to Do About Social Security and Medicare?What Do You Think?

Recall 1. Activities of daily living (ADLs) include functioning in

the areas of bathing, toileting, walking, dressing, and _______________.

2. A type of long-term care facility that provides medical care 24 hours per day, 7 days per week using a variety of health- care professionals is a _______________.

3. Refusal or failure to fulfill any part of a person’s obligation or duties to an older adult is officially termed _______________.

4. The two most important public policy issues in the United States that are being affected by the aging baby-boom gen- eration are Social Security and _______________.

Interpret ●● How might the large generation now graduating from high

school affect Social Security in the future?

Apply ●● How would the competence–environmental press framework,

presented earlier in this chapter, apply specifically to the vari- ous types of housing and nursing homes discussed in this section?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 15.5

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

536 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

15.1 Theories of Psychosocial Aging

What is social involvement and healthy aging?

●● Healthy aging involves avoiding disease, being engaged with life, and maintaining high cognitive and physical function- ing. The basic premises of healthy aging include keeping a balance between the various gains and losses that occur over time and minimizing the influence of factors unrelated to aging.

What is the competence–environmental press model, and how do docility and proactivity relate to the model?

●● According to the competence–environmental press theory, people’s optimal adaptation occurs when there is a balance between their ability to cope and the level of environmental demands placed on them. When balance is not achieved, behavior becomes maladaptive. The model has wide appli- cability in aging.

●● People exhibit proactivity when they choose new behaviors to meet new desires and exert control over their lives. In contrast, people demonstrate docility when they allow the situation to dictate the options they have.

What is the proactivity and corrective proactivity model?

●● The preventive and corrective proactivity model explains how life stressors and lack of good congruence in person- environment interactions result in poor life outcomes. Key buffers (external and internal resources) and proactive behaviors lower the negative impact of stressors and help people cope.

●● Preventive adaptations are actions that avoid stressors and increase or build social resources. Corrective adaptations are actions taken in response to stressors and can be facili- tated by internal and external resources.

15.2 Personality, Social Cognition, and Spirituality

What is integrity in late life? how can people achieve it?

●● Older adults face the Eriksonian struggle of integrity versus despair primarily through a life review. Integrity involves accepting one’s life for what it is; despair involves bitterness about one’s past. People who reach integrity become self- affirming and self-accepting, and they judge their lives to have been worthwhile and good.

how is well-being defined in adulthood? how do people view themselves differently as they age?

●● Subjective well-being is an evaluation of one’s life that is associated with positive feelings. In life-span developmental

psychology, subjective well-being is usually assessed by measures of life satisfaction, happiness, and self-esteem.

●● Neuroscience research shows a developmental connec- tion with brain activity in the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala.

What role does spirituality play in late life?

●● By later life, most people have made the transition from False Self (the superficial, external self ) to True Self (the experience of dwelling in union with the essence of every- thing in the universe, a sense of “inner knowing” that does not see anything with absolute certainty).

●● Spiritual support is a key factor in older adults’ coping. People who are more involved and committed to their faith have better physical and mental health.

15.3 I Used to Work at . . . : Living in Retirement

What does being retired mean?

●● Retirement is a complex process by which people with- draw from full-time employment. No single definition is adequate for all ethnic groups; self-definition involves several factors, including eligibility for certain social programs.

Why do people retire?

●● People generally retire because they choose to, although some people are forced to retire or do so because of serious health problems, such as cardiovascular disease or cancer. However, there are important gender and ethnic differences in why people retire and how they label themselves after retirement.

how do people adjust to retirement?

●● Retirement is an important life transition. Most people are satisfied with retirement. Most retired people maintain their health, friendship networks, and activity levels—at least in the years immediately following retirement. For men, personal life priorities are all-important; little is known about women’s retirement satisfaction.

What employment and volunteer opportunities are available for older adults?

●● Increasingly, people continue some level of participation in the labor force during retirement, usually for financial reasons. Labor force participation among older adults continues to increase. Volunteer work is another way of achieving personal fulfillment and staying active in retirement.

SUMMARY

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h A P T e R 1 5 : S O C I A L A S P E C T S O F L AT E R L I F E 537

15.4 Friends and Family in Late Life

What role do friends and family play in late life?

●● A person’s social convoy is an important source of satisfaction in late life. Patterns of friend- ships among older adults are similar to those among young adults, but older adults are more selective. Sibling relationships are especially important in old age. Because people cannot choose their siblings as they do their friends, compared with friendships, these relationships often take more work. Although they tend to have smaller social net- works, older adults’ social contact is motivated by a variety of goals, including information seeking, self-concept, and emotional regulation, explained in the theory of socioemo- tional selectivity.

What are older adults’ marriages and same-sex partnerships like?

●● Long-term marriages tend to be happy until one partner develops serious health problems. Older married couples show a lower potential for marital conflict and greater potential for pleasure. Long-term gay and lesbian relation- ships tend to be similar in characteristics to long-term heterosexual marriages; issues pertaining to legal rights continue to present challenges.

What is it like to provide basic care for one’s partner?

●● Caring for a partner puts considerable strain on the rela- tionship. The degree of marital satisfaction strongly affects how spousal caregivers perceive stress. Although caught off guard initially, most spousal caregivers are able to provide adequate care. Perceptions of competence among spousal caregivers at the outset of caregiving may be especially important.

how do people cope with widowhood? how do men and women differ?

●● Widowhood is a difficult transition for most people. Feel- ings of loneliness are hard to cope with. Men generally have problems in social relationships and in household tasks; women tend to have more severe financial problems. Some widowed people remarry, partly to solve loneliness and financial problems.

What special issues are involved in being a great-grandparent?

●● Becoming a great-grandparent is an important source of personal satisfaction for many older adults. Great-grand- parents as a group are more similar to each other than grandparents are. Three aspects of great-grandparenthood

are important: It offers a sense of personal and family renewal, provides new diversions in life, and is a major life milestone.

15.5 Social Issues and Aging

Who are frail older adults? how common is frailty?

●● The number of frail older adults is growing. Frailty is defined in terms of impairment in activities of daily liv- ing (basic self-care skills) and instrumental activities of daily living (actions that require intellectual competence or planning). As many as half of those over age 85 may need assistance with ADLs, IADLs, or PLIMs. Supportive environments are useful in optimizing the balance between competence and environmental press.

What housing options are available for older adults?

●● Most older adults prefer to age in place; home modification offers one option to achieve that. Assisted living facilities offer support for ADLs and IADLs while providing a significant degree of independence. Nursing homes provide 24/7 medical care for those who need continual assistance. Newer alternatives include the Eden Alternative, the Green House Project, and cohousing options that focus on providing greater support for people to remain in the community.

how do you know whether an older adult is abused, neglected, or exploited?

●● Abuse and neglect of older adults is an increasing problem. However, abuse and neglect are difficult to define precisely. Several categories are used, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional or psychological abuse, financial or mate- rial exploitation, abandonment, neglect, and self-neglect. Most perpetrators are family members, usually partners or adult children of the victims. Research indicates that abuse results from a complex interaction of characteristics of the caregiver and care recipient.

What are the key social policy issues affecting older adults?

●● Although initially designed as an income supplement, Social Security has become the primary source of retire- ment income for most U.S. citizens. The aging of the baby-boom generation will place considerable stress on the system’s financing.

●● Medicare is the principal health insurance program for adults in the United States over age 65. Cost containment is a major concern, and there are no easy answers for ensuring the system’s future viability.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

538 PA RT F O U R : L AT E A D U LT H O O D

15.1 1. Healthy aging 2. competence, environmental press 15.2 1. integrity versus despair 2. subjective well-being 3. spirituality 15.3 1. complex process by which people gradually withdraw from employment 2. because they choose to do so 3. satisfied 4. volunteering 15.4 1. siblings 2. marital conflict 3. lower 4. Widows 5. the fact that it is a milestone 15.5 1. eating 2. nursing home 3. neglect 4. Medicare

Test Yourself Recall Answers

healthy aging (503) competence (503) environmental press (503) adaptation level (504) zone of maximum performance

potential (504) zone of maximum comfort (504) proactivity (505) docility (505)

preventive adaptations (505) corrective adaptations (505) integrity versus despair (508) life review (508) subjective well-being (509) spiritual support (513) social convoy (519) socioemotional selectivity (521) frail older adults (526)

activities of daily living (ADLs) (526) instrumental activities of daily living

(IADLs) (526) physical limitations (PLIMs) (526) sense of place (528) functional health (528) assisted living facilities (529) nursing home (530)

Key Terms

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

540 pa rt O N E : PA R T T I T L E Kar en

M as

si er

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

541

When famous people such as Prince or Amy Winehouse die unexpectedly, people are confronted with the reality that death happens to

everyone. Many of us die from the same conditions;

the most likely ways people die across the life span

are shown in ◗ ­Figure 16.1. But the plain truth is that

each of us makes the transition from life to death.

We have a paradoxical relationship with death.

Sometimes we are fascinated by it. As tourists, we

visit places where famous people died or are buried.

We watch as television newscasts show scenes of

devastation in natural disasters and war. But when it

comes to pondering our own death or that of people

close to us, we experience difficulty. As French writer

and reformer La Rochefoucauld wrote over 300 years

Dying and Bereavement

16.1 Definitions and Ethical Issues 542 Sociocultural Definitions of Death 543 Legal and Medical Definitions 544 Ethical Issues 545

WHAT DO YOU THINK? The Brittany Maynard Case 547

16.2 thinking about Death: personal aspects 548 A Life-Course Approach to Dying 549 REAL PEOPLE: Randy Pausch’s Last Lecture 547 Dealing with One’s Own Death 550 Death Anxiety 551

16.3 End-of-Life Issues 554 Creating a Final Scenario 554 The Hospice Option 556 Making Your End-of-Life Intentions Known 558

16.4 Surviving the Loss: the Grieving process 560 The Grief Process 561 Typical Grief Reactions 563 Coping with Grief 564 Ambiguous Loss 566 Complicated or Prolonged Grief Disorder 567

SPOTLIGHT ON RESEARCH: The Costs of Holding in Grief for the Sake of One’s Partner 568

Disenfranchised Grief 568

16.5 Dying and Bereavement Experiences across the Life Span 569 Childhood 570 Adolescence 571 Adulthood 572 Late Adulthood 574 Conclusion 575

Summary 576

Key terms 578

16

Amy Winehouse

A P

Im ag

es /M

at t D

un ha

m

Prince

D on

al d

M ira

lle /G

et ty

Im ag

es S

po rt

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

542 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

­◗ Figure 16.1 Age-adjusted death rates for selected causes of death for all ages, by sex: 2004–2014.

Male

Year Year

Female

2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014

All causes Heart disease Cancer Unintentional injuries CLRD Alzheimer’s disease Diabetes Stroke

10,000

1,000

100

10

D ea

th s

p er

1 00

,0 00

p o

p u

la ti

o n

( lo

g sc

al e)

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From National Center for Health Statistics. (2016). Health, United States, 2015. Figure 2. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/hus/ hus15.pdf.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ­● How is death defined?

­● What legal and medical criteria are used to determine when death occurs?

­● What are the ethical dilemmas surrounding euthanasia?

­● What issues surround the costs of life-sustaining care?

16.1 Definitions and Ethical Issues

ago, “looking into the sun is easier than contemplating our death.”

When death is personal, we become uneasy. Looking at the sun is

hard indeed.

In this chapter we delve into thanatology. thanatology is the study

of death, dying, grief, bereavement, and social attitudes toward these issues. We

first consider definitional and ethical issues surrounding death. Next,

we look specifically at the process of dying. Dealing with grief is impor-

tant for survivors, so we consider this topic in the third section. Finally,

we examine how people view death at different points in the life span.

thanatology Study of death, dying, grief, bereavement, and social attitudes toward these issues.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 543

Ernesto and Paulina had been married 48 years when Ernesto developed terminal pancreatic cancer. Ernesto was suffering terrible pain and begged Paulina to make it stop. He said she would not let their pet suffer this way, so why let him? Paulina heard about “mercy killing” that involved administering high dosages of certain medications, but she believed this was the same as murder. Yet, she could hardly bear to watch her beloved husband suffer. Paulina wondered what she should do.

When one first thinks about it, death seems a simple concept to define: It is the point when a person is no longer alive. Similarly, dying appears to be simply the process of making the transition from being alive to being dead. It all seems clear enough, doesn’t it? But death and dying are actually far more complicated concepts.

As we will see, there are many cultural and religious differences in the definition of death and the customs surrounding it. The meaning of death depends on the observer’s perspective as well as the specific medical and biological criteria one uses.

Sociocultural Definitions of Death Although death is one of the few truly universal experiences, each culture has its own ways of thinking about, defining, and ritualizing it (Bustos, 2007; Gire, 2014; Penson, 2004). All cultures have their own views. Some cultures ritually pull their hair as a sign of grief (Lewis, 2013). Melanesians have a term, mate, that includes the extremely sick, the very old, and the dead; the term toa refers to all other living people (Counts & Counts, 1985). Other cultures believe the life force leaves the body during sleep or illness, or involves reaching a certain age (Gire, 2014). Still other cultures view death as a transition to a dif- ferent type of existence that still allows interaction with the living, and some believe there is a circular pattern of multiple deaths and rebirths (Gire, 2014). In Ghana people are said to have a “peaceful” or “good” death if the dying person finished all business and made peace with others before death, and implies being at peace with his or her own death (van der Geest, 2004).

Mourning rituals, expressions of grief, and states of bereavement also vary in different cultures (Lee, 2010; Norton & Gino, 2014). In the United States and many other countries, the death of a dignitary is marked by lowering flags to half-staff and declaring a formal period of mourning. Some cultures have formalized periods of time during which certain prayers or rituals are performed. After the death of a close relative, Orthodox Jews recite ritual prayers and cover all the mirrors in the house. The men slash their ties as a symbol of loss. In Papua New Guinea, there are accepted time periods for phases of grief (Hemer, 2010). The Muscogee Creek tribe’s rituals include dig- ging the grave by hand and giving a “farewell handshake” by throwing a handful of dirt into the grave before covering it (Walker & Balk, 2007). Ancestor worship, a deep respectful feeling toward individuals from whom a family is descended or who are important to them, is an important part of customs of death in many Asian cultures (Roszko, 2010). We must keep in mind the experiences of our culture or par- ticular group may not generalize to other cultures or groups.

Death can be a truly cross-cultural experience. The interna- tional outpouring of grief over the death of world leaders such Nelson Mandela in 2013, the thousands killed in various terrorist attacks against the United States and other countries, and the hundreds of thousands killed in such natural disasters as the earthquake in Haiti in 2010 drew much attention to the ways the deaths of people we do not know personally can still affect us. It is at these times we realize death happens to us all and death can simultaneously be personal and public.

The symbols we use when people die, such as these elaborate caskets from Ghana, provide insights into how cultures think about death.

M ax

M ill

ig an

/A W

L Im

ag es

/G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

544 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

The many ways of viewing death can be seen in various funeral customs. You may have experienced a range of different types of funeral customs, from small, private services to elaborate rituals. Variations in the customs surrounding death are reflected in some of the most iconic structures on earth, such as the pyramids in Egypt, and some of the most beautiful, such as the Taj Mahal in India.

Legal and Medical Definitions Sociocultural approaches help us understand the different ways people conceptualize and understand death, but they do not address a fundamental question: How do we determine someone has died? The medical and legal communities grappled with this question for centuries and continue to do so today. Let’s see what the current answers are.

Determining when death occurs has always been subjective. For hundreds of years, people accepted and applied the criteria that now define clinical death: lack of heartbeat and respiration. Today, however, the definition used in most countries is whole-brain death. In 2010, the American Academy of Neurology proposed new guidelines for determining brain death (Wijdicks et al., 2010). The goal in this revision of the criteria was to provide guidelines that were based on research. According to the guidelines, there are three signs that a person's brain has permanently stopped functioning. First, the person is in a coma, and the cause of the coma is known. Second, all brainstem reflexes have permanently stopped working. Third, breathing has permanently stopped, so that a ventilator, or breathing machine, must be used to keep the body functioning.

The guidelines describe several complex steps physicians must follow to diagnose brain death. In some cases, this involves more than 25 specific tests. For example, the guidelines describe the best way to demonstrate absence of breathing. In contrast to pre- vious guidelines, the current revision states that laboratory tests such as EEG or cerebral flow studies are not needed to determine brain death. The guidelines also make clear that this complex determination of brain death must be completed by a physician who has been trained in diagnosing brain death.

Although these criteria marked a significant advance over earlier definitions, they have still not been adopted at all hospitals in the United States (Greer et al., 2016). This means that somewhat different criteria for determining brain death are applied at differ- ent hospitals. This can result in misdiagnosis, delays in organ transplants, and emotionally wrenching decisions for families (Kaplan, 2015).

Brain death is also controversial from some religious perspectives. For example, there is no general consensus whether brain death is accepted as true death under Islamic law (Al-Bar & Chamsi-Pasha, 2015; Miller, 2016). Roman Catholics focus on what they term “natural death” (Kassim & Alias, 2016). When these perspectives are considered, the deter- mination of death can be difficult.

It is possible for a person’s cortical functioning to cease while brainstem activity contin- ues; this is a persistent vegetative state, from which the person does not recover. This condition can occur following disruption of the blood flow to the brain, a severe head injury, or a drug overdose. Persistent vegetative state allows for spontaneous heartbeat and respiration but not for consciousness. The whole-brain standard does not permit a decla- ration of death for someone who is in a persistent vegetative state. Because of conditions like persistent vegetative state, family members sometimes face difficult ethical decisions concerning care for the individual. These issues are the focus of the next section.

Ethical Issues An ambulance screeches to a halt and emergency personnel rush a woman into the emer- gency room. As a result of an accident at a swimming pool, she has no pulse and no respi- ration. Working rapidly, the trauma team reestablishes a heartbeat through electric shock. A respirator is connected. An EEG and other tests reveal extensive and irreversible brain damage—she is in a persistent vegetative state. What should be done?

clinical death Lack of heartbeat and respiration.

whole-brain death Declared only when the deceased meets eight criteria, which were established in 1981.

persistent vegetative state Situation in which a person’s cortical func- tioning ceases while brainstem activity continues.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 545

This is an example of the kinds of problems faced in the field of bioethics, the study of the interface between human values and technological advances in health and life sciences. Bioethics grew from two bases: respect for individual freedom and the impossibility of establishing any single version of morality by rational argument or common sense. Both of these factors are increasingly based on empirical evidence and cultural contexts ( Priaulx, 2013; Sherwin, 2011). In practice, bioethics emphasizes the importance of individual choice and the minimization of harm over the maximization of good. That is, bioethics requires people to weigh how much the patient will benefit from a treatment relative to the amount of suffering he or she will endure as a result of the treatment. Examples of the tough choices required are those facing cancer patients about aggressive treatment for cancer that is quite likely to be fatal in any case and those facing family members about whether to turn off a life-support machine attached to their loved one.

In the arena of death and dying, the most important bioethical issue is euthanasia— the practice of ending life for reasons of mercy. The moral dilemma posed by euthanasia becomes apparent when trying to decide the circumstances a person’s life should be ended, that implicitly forces one to place a value on the life of another (Bedir & Aksoy, 2011; Kassim & Alias, 2016; Munoz & Fox, 2013). It also makes us think about the difference between “killing” and “letting die” at the end of life (Dickens, Boyle, & Ganzini, 2008). In our society, this dilemma occurs most often when a person is being kept alive by machines or when someone is suffering from a terminal illness. This is the situation confronting Ernesto and Paulina in the opening vignette.

Euthanasia Euthanasia can be carried out in two different ways: actively and passively (Moeller, Lewis, & Werth, 2010). active euthanasia involves the deliberate ending of someone’s life, that may be based on a clear statement of the person’s wishes or be a decision made by someone else who has the legal authority to do so. Usually, this involves situations when people are in a persistent vegetative state or suffer from the end stages of a terminal disease. Exam- ples of active euthanasia would be administering a drug overdose or ending a person’s life through so-called mercy killing.

A second form of euthanasia, passive euthanasia, involves allowing a person to die by withholding available treatment. A ventilator might be disconnected, chemotherapy might be withheld from a patient with terminal cancer, a surgical procedure might not be per- formed, or food could be withdrawn.

There is debate regarding the need to differentiate active and passive euthanasia (e.g., European Association of Palliative Care, 2011; Garrard & Wilkinson, 2005; Strinic, 2015). Some argue the single label “euthanasia” suffices; others prefer to distinguish between the traditional labels. The main point is that a decision to continue or terminate a person’s life is made.

Most Americans favor such actions as disconnecting life support in situations involv- ing patients in a persistent vegetative state, withholding treatment if the person agrees or is in the later stages of a terminal illness, and the concept of assisted death. But feelings also run strongly against such actions for religious or other reasons (Meilaender, 2013; Strinic, 2015). Even political debates incorporate the issue, as demonstrated in the sum- mer of 2009 in the United States when opponents of President Obama’s healthcare reform falsely claimed “death panels” would make decisions about terminating life support if the reform measure passed.

Globally, opinions about euthanasia vary (Bosshard & Materstvedt, 2011; Strinic, 2015). Western Europeans tend to view active euthanasia more positively based on less influence of religion and more social welfare services than residents of Eastern European and Islamic countries, who are more influenced by religious beliefs arguing against such practices (Bau- mann et al., 2011; Hains & Hulbert-Williams, 2013; Miller, 2016; Nayernouri, 2011).

Disconnecting a life-support system is one thing; withholding nourishment from a terminally ill person is quite another issue for many people. Indeed, such cases often end up in court. The first high-profile legal case involving passive euthanasia in the United

bioethics Study of the interface between human values and technological advances in health and life sciences.

euthanasia Practice of ending life for reasons of mercy.

active euthanasia Deliberate ending of someone’s life.

passive euthanasia Practice of allowing a person to die by with- holding available treatment.

Think About It How do sociocultural forces shape attitudes about euthanasia?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

546 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

States was brought to the courts in 1990; the U.S. Supreme Court took up the case of Nancy Cruzan, whose family wanted to end her forced feeding. The court ruled that unless clear and incontrovertible evidence is presented to indicate that an individual desires to have nourishment stopped, such as through a healthcare power of attorney or living will, a third party (such as a parent or partner) cannot decide to terminate nourishment. We will consider later in this chapter how to ensure that one’s wishes about these matters are expressed clearly.

Physician-Assisted Suicide Taking one’s own life through suicide has never been popular in the United States because of religious and other prohibitions. In other cultures, such as Japan, suicide is viewed as an honorable way to die under certain circumstances (Joiner, 2010).

Attitudes regarding suicide in certain situations are changing. Much of this change concerns the topic of physician-assisted suicide, in which physicians provide dying patients with a fatal dose of medication that the patient self-administers. Most Americans, as do people in other countries, favor having a choice regarding assisted suicide if they should ever be diagnosed with a terminal disease. However, many oppose it on moral or religious grounds, irrespective of the wishes of the dying person.

In some cases this option has been put into law. Several countries—including Switzerland, Belgium, the Netherlands, and Colombia—have legalized physician-assisted suicide. Each of these laws sets clear guidelines for when this option is permitted. For example, in the Netherlands, five criteria must be met before a terminally ill person can request physician-assisted suicide as an option:

1. The patient’s condition is intolerable with no hope for improvement. 2. No relief is available. 3. The patient is competent. 4. The patient makes a request repeatedly over time. 5. Two physicians review the case and agree with the patient’s request.

In the United States, voters in Oregon passed the Death with Dignity Act in 1994, the first physician-assisted suicide law in the country. Although the U.S. Supreme Court ruled in two cases in 1997 (Vacco v. Quill and Washington v. Glucksberg) there is no right to assisted suicide, the Court decided in 1998 not to overturn the Oregon law. As of 2016, physician-assisted suicide was also legal by state law in California, Colorado, District of Columbia, Vermont, and Washington, and by court decision in Montana. Several other states are considering legislation or have cases under court review.

In general, all of these laws permit people to obtain and use prescriptions for self- administered lethal doses of medication. The laws often require a physician to inform the person he or she is terminally ill and describe alternative options (e.g., hospice care, pain control). The person must be mentally competent and make multiple requests. Such pro- visions are included to ensure people making the request fully understand the issues and the request is not made hastily.

Available data indicate laws such as Oregon’s have psychological benefits for patients, who value having autonomy in death as in life, especially in situations involving unbear- able suffering (Hendry et al., 2013). Still, the idea of physicians helping people end their lives is a difficult topic for many.

A controversial case of assisted suicide involved Brittany Maynard, a 29-year-old woman who had terminal brain cancer and committed suicide in 2014. As discussed in the What Do You Think? feature, such cases reveal the difficult legal, medical, and ethical issues as well as the high degree of emotion surrounding the topic of euthanasia and death with dignity.

There is no question the debate over physician-assisted suicide will continue. As the technology to keep people alive continues to improve, the ethical issues about active euthanasia in general and physician-assisted suicide in particular will continue to become more complex and will likely focus increasingly on quality of life and death with dignity (Gostin & Roberts, 2016).

physician-assisted suicide Process in which physicians provide dying patients with a fatal dose of medication that the patient self-administers.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 547

On November 2, 2014, Brittany Maynard took a lethal dose of medication pre- scribed by her physician and ended her life. She was 29 years old, and had been suffering from terminal brain cancer, a diagnosis she received on New Year’s Day 2014. She died in her bedroom, surrounded by loved ones (Bever, 2014a, b, c).

Those are the bare facts of Brittany’s case. The broader context and debate that her case created is more complicated. Rather than keep her situation private, Brittany went public, conducting several interviews with media and making her thoughts widely known. From there, debate raged.

Brittany’s disease progressed rapidly. Nine days after her diagnosis, surgeons removed part of her brain and performed a partial resection of her temporal lobe to stop the tumor from growing. By April 2014, it was clear that this procedure did not work—the tumor was back, and it was more aggressive. Brittany was given about 6 months to live.

Because of her diagnosis, Brittany quali- fied for physician-assisted suicide under the Death with Dignity Act discussed earlier in the chapter. At the time, Brittany and her hus- band, Dan Diaz, lived in San Francisco. After carefully considering the various treatment options, which would not cure her, as well as hospice options, she made her decision. Because California does not have a death with dignity law, Brittany and her family moved to Portland, Oregon.

Brittany spent much of her remaining life working on behalf of the death with dignity movement, volunteering for Compassion & Choices. Because she was public and open about her dying process, her case has been a focal point for the right-to-die debate. Critics of death-with-dignity laws, such as National Right to Life, argue that terminal illness does not carry with it the “right” to be “assisted” in dying.

The Brittany Maynard case raises many serious personal, ethical, and moral issues. It also forces us to confront our anxieties about death, how we confront death as a society, and what we truly believe. Each of us must take the time to think through these issues and make our desires known. What do you think about what Brittany chose?

The Brittany Maynard CaseWhat Do You Think?

Brittany Maynard

A P

Im ag

es /P

R N

EW SW

IR E/

Co m

pa ss

io n

& C

ho ic

es

The Price of Life-Sustaining Care A growing debate in the United States, particularly in the aftermath of the Affordable Care Act passed in 2010, concerns the financial, personal, and moral costs of keeping people alive on life-support machines and continuing aggressive care when people have termi- nal conditions. Debate continues on whether secondary health conditions in terminally ill people should be treated. The argument is such care is expensive, these people will die soon anyway, and needlessly prolonging life is a burden on society.

However, many others argue all means possible should be used, whether for a pre- mature infant or an older adult, to keep them alive despite the high cost and possible risk of negative side effects of the treatment or intervention. They argue life is precious, and humans should not “play God” and decide when it should end.

There is no question extraordinary interventions are expensive. Healthcare costs can soar during the last year of a person’s life. Data indicate less than 7% of people who receive hospital care die each year, but account for nearly 25% of all Medicare expenditures (Adamy & McGinty, 2012). One example involves the continued use of chemotherapy for end-stage cancer patients; the higher costs of care given the evidence of limited benefit and potential harm of continued treatment are coming under increased scrutiny (Garrido et al., 2016). Expenditures for end-of-life care are typically less for those having advance directives (discussed later in this chapter) who receive palliative care.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

548 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

The biggest challenge in confronting these differences in approach and cost is the difficulty in deciding when to treat or not treat a disease a person has. There are no easy answers. Witness the loud criticism when research evidence indicated various types of cancer screening (e.g., breast, prostate) should not be provided to everyone as early or as often as initially thought. Despite the lack of evidence to support and the cost of continuing traditional approaches, many patients and physicians do so anyway. Failure to base care on evidence has a price. Whether that is affordable in the long run seems unlikely.

Think About It How should the biopsychosocial model influence political debates about dying and death?

recall 1. Flying a flag at half-staff is an example of a(n)

_______________definition of death.

2. The difference between brain death and a persistent vegeta- tive state is _______________.

3. Withholding an antibiotic from a person who dies as a result is an example of _______________.

Interpret ­● What is the difference between the Oregon Death with Dignity

Act and active euthanasia?

apply ­● Describe how people at each level of Kohlberg’s theory of

moral reasoning (described in Chapter 8) would deal with the issue of euthanasia.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 16.1

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ­● How do feelings about death change over adulthood?

­● How do people deal with their own death?

­● What is death anxiety, and how do people show it?

Jean is a 49-year-old woman whose parents have both died in the past three years. She now realizes she is the oldest living member of her family (she has two younger siblings). She started thinking about the fact that someday she too will die. Jean gets anxious when she thinks about her death and tries to block it out of her mind.

Like Jean, most people are uncomfortable thinking about their own death, especially if they think it will be unpleasant. As one research participant put it, “You are nuts if you aren’t afraid of death” (Kalish & Reynolds, 1976). Still, death is a paradox, as we noted at the beginning of the chapter. That is, we are afraid of or anxious about death but we are drawn to it, sometimes in public ways. We examine this paradox at the personal level in this section. Specifically, we focus on two questions: How do people’s feelings about death differ with age? What is it about death we fear or that makes us anxious?

Before we begin, let’s take a moment and reflect on how people are remembered after death. One common way to remember people immediately after they die is through an obituary, an experience we may have with hundreds of people we know but never our own. Here’s a chance to think about one’s own death from that perspective. Take a few minutes and do the following exercise:

­● In 200 words or less, write your own obituary. Be sure to include your age and cause of death. List your lifetime accomplishments. Don’t forget to list your survivors.

16.2 Thinking About Death: Personal Aspects

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 549

­● Think about all the things you will have done that are not listed in your obituary. List some of them.

­● Think of all the friends you will have made and how you will have affected them. ­● Would you make any changes in your obituary now?

A Life-Course Approach to Dying Suppose you learned today you had only a few months to live. How would you feel about dying? That’s what Randy Pausch, a professor at Carnegie Mellon University, faced when he was told he had 3 to 6 months to live after his pancreatic cancer came back. What happened next, describe in the Real People feature, touched millions of people around the world.

It probably doesn’t surprise you to learn feelings about dying vary with age and cul- tures (Gire, 2014). Each person comes to terms with death in an individual and a family- based way, and together they cocreate ways the patient meets his or her goals (Bergdahl et al., 2013; Carlander et al., 2011; Milligan et al., 2016).

Although not specifically addressed in research, the shift from formal operational thinking to post-formal thinking (see Chapter 7) could be important in young adults’ con- templation of death. Presumably, this shift in cognitive development is accompanied by a lessening of the feeling of immortality in adolescence to one that integrates personal feel- ings and emotions with their thinking.

Midlife is the time when most people in developed countries confront the death of their parents. Until that point, people tend not to think much about their own death; the fact their parents are still alive buffers them from reality. After all, in the normal course of events, our parents are supposed to die before we do.

Once their parents die, people realize they are now the oldest generation of their family—the next in line to die. Reading the obituary pages, they are reminded of this, as the ages of many of the people who died get closer and closer to their own.

Randy Pausch was a famous computer scientist on the faculty at Carnegie Mellon university. He cofounded the Entertainment Technology Center there and invented a highly innovative way to teach computer program- ming, called Alice. But that’s not what made him world famous. He was a pioneer of virtual reality.

At the age of 46, Randy was told that his pancreatic cancer had recurred and he had between 3 and 6 months to live. So instead of just getting depressed about it, he decided to give a lecture a month later about achiev- ing one’s childhood dreams. His lecture is both moving and funny. Rather than talking about dying, Randy focused on overcoming obstacles and seizing every moment of one’s life because, as he put it, “Time is all you have . . . and you may find one day that you have less than you think.” He spoke of his love for his wife and three children. He had a birth- day cake brought onto the stage for his wife. Randy lived several more months after his lec- ture, dying in July 2008 at age 47.

You can see Randy’s lecture by searching YouTube. It was also published as a book. In a strange twist, the coauthor of the book, Jeff Zaslow, was himself killed in an automobile accident at age 53.

One never knows when one’s life will end; it is said that the end comes like a thief in the night. But people such as Randy Pausch help us put our own death into perspective by reminding us what is important.

Real People Randy Pausch’s Last Lecture

Randy Pausch

Pi tt

sb ur

gh P

os t-

G az

et te

/Z U

M A

P re

ss /N

ew sc

om

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

550 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Probably as a result of this growing realization of their own mortality, middle-aged adults’ sense of time undergoes a subtle yet profound change. It changes from an emphasis on how long they have already lived to how long they have left to live, a shift that increases into late life (Cicirelli, 2006; Maxfield et al., 2010). This may lead to occupational change or other redirection such as improving relationships that deteriorated over the years. It is also the case that certain strategies are used to deflect attention from or buffer the real- ity of death anxiety. For example, Yaakobi (2015) found that the desire to work serves as a death anxiety buffer for adults.

In general, older adults are less anxious about death and more accepting of it than any other age group. Still, because the discrepancy between desired and expected number of years left to live is greater for young-old than for mid-old adults, anxiety is higher for young-old adults (Cicirelli, 2006). In part, the greater overall acceptance of death results from the achievement of ego integrity, as described in Chapter 15. For other older adults, the joy of living is diminishing. More than any other group, they experienced loss of fam- ily and friends and have come to terms with their own mortality. Older adults have more chronic diseases that are not likely to go away. They may feel their most important life tasks have been completed (Kastenbaum, 1999).

Understanding how adults deal with death, end-of-life issues, and grief can be approached from the perspective of attachment theory (Hales, 2016; Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2005). In this view, a person’s reactions are a natural consequence of forming attachments and then losing them. We consider adult grief a bit later in the chapter.

Dealing with One’s Own Death Many authors have tried to describe the dying process, often using the metaphor of a tra- jectory that captures the duration of time between the onset of dying (e.g., from the diag- nosis of a fatal disease) as well as death and the course of the dying process (D’Angelo et al., 2015; Field & Cassel, 2010). These dying trajectories vary a great deal across diseases, as illustrated in ◗ ­Figure 16.2. Some diseases, such as lung cancer, have a clear and rapid period of decline; this “terminal phase” is often used to determine eligibility for certain services (e.g., hospice, discussed later). Other diseases, such as congestive heart failure, have no clear terminal phase. The two approaches of describing the dying process we con- sider will try to account for both types of trajectories.

­◗ Figure 16.2 Some fatal diseases, such as lung cancer, have a clear decline phase, whereas others, such as congestive heart failure, do not.

Time

Congestive heart failure

Lung cancer

Clear phase of decline — allows hospice referral

Death unpredictable (no clear decline phase)

F u

n c ti

o n

a l a b

il it

y o

r s e v e ri

ty o

f il ln

e s s

Worst Death

Best

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Skolnick, A. A. (1998). MediCaring project to demonstrate and evaluate innovative end-of-life program for chronically ill. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 1511–1512. Reprinted with permission of the American Medical Association.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 551

Kübler-Ross’s Work Elisabeth Kübler-Ross changed the way we approach dying. When she began her investi- gations into the dying process in the 1960s, such research was controversial; her physician colleagues initially were outraged and some even denied their patients were terminally ill. Still, she persisted. More than 200 interviews with terminally ill people convinced her most people experienced several emotional reactions. Using her experiences, she described five reactions that represented the ways people dealt with death: denial, anger, bargaining, depression, and acceptance (Kübler-Ross, 1969). Although they were first presented as a sequence, it was subsequently realized the emotions can overlap and be experienced in different order.

Although she believed these five stages represent the typical range of emotional devel- opment in the dying, Kübler-Ross (1974) cautioned not everyone experiences all of them or progresses through them at the same rate or in the same order. Research supports the view her “stages” should not be viewed as a sequence (Charlton & Verghese, 2010; Parkes, 2013). In fact, we could actually harm dying people by considering these stages as fixed and universal. Individual differences are great. Emotional responses may vary in intensity throughout the dying process. Thus, the goal in applying Kübler-Ross’s ideas to real-world settings would be to help people achieve an appropriate death: one that meets the needs of the dying person, allowing him or her to work out each problem as it comes.

A Contextual Theory of Dying Describing the process of dying is difficult. One reason for these problems is the realiza- tion there is no one right way to die, although there may be better or worse ways of coping (Corr, 2010a,b; Corr & Corr, 2013; Corr, Corr, & Nabe, 2008; Pope, 2017). Corr identified four dimensions of the issues or tasks a dying person faces from their perspective: bodily needs, psychological security, interpersonal attachments, and spiritual energy and hope. This holistic approach acknowledges individual differences and rejects broad generaliza- tions. Corr’s task work approach also recognizes the importance of the coping efforts of family members, friends, and caregivers as well as those of the dying person.

Kastenbaum and Thuell (1995) argue what is needed is an even broader contextual approach that takes a more inclusive view of the dying process. They point out theories must be able to handle people who have a wide variety of terminal illnesses and be sensitive to dying people’s own perspectives and values related to death. The socio- environmental context where dying occurs often changes over time and must be recognized. A person may begin the dying process living independently but end up in a long-term care facil- ity. Such moves may have profound implications for how the person copes with dying. A contextual approach provides guidance for healthcare professionals and families for dis- cussing how to protect the quality of life, provide better care, and prepare caregivers for dealing with the end of life. Such an approach would also provide research questions such as how does one’s acceptance of dying change across various stages?

Although we do not yet have a comprehensive theory of dying, we examine people’s experiences as a narrative that can be written from many points of view (e.g., the patient, family members, caregivers). What emerges would be a rich description of a dynamically changing, individual process.

Death Anxiety We have seen how people view death varies with age. In the process, we encountered the notion of feeling anxious about death. Death anxiety refers to people’s anxiety or even fear of death and dying. Death anxiety is tough to pin down; indeed, it is the ethereal, unknown nature of death, rather than something about it in particular, that makes us feel so uncom- fortable. Because of this, we must look for indirect behavioral evidence to document death  anxiety. Research findings suggest death anxiety is a complex, multidimensional construct.

death anxiety Feeling of anxiety or even fear of death and dying.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

552 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

For nearly three decades, researchers have applied terror management theory as a framework to study death anxiety (Burke, Martens, & Faucher, 2010; Park & Pyszczynski, 2016). terror management theory addresses the issue of why people engage in certain behaviors to achieve particular psychological states based on their deeply rooted concerns about mortality. The theory proposes that ensuring the continuation of one’s life is the primary motive underlying behavior and that all other motives can be traced to this basic one. An overview of the theory is shown in ◗ ­Figure 16.3 (Arndt & Goldenberg, 2017).

Essentially, terror management theory explains how health conditions affect how we think about death. Basically, when mortality concerns are in our conscious thought, our health-related decisions are determined by our short-term goal of reducing our perceived vulnerability to the health threat and its related concerns about mortality.

Additionally, some suggest older adults present an existential threat for the younger and middle-aged adults because they remind us all that death is inescapable, the body is fallible, and the bases that we may secure self-esteem (and manage death anxiety) are tran- sitory (Martens, Goldenberg, & Greenberg, 2005). That may be why some people seek cosmetic surgery as a way to deal with their death anxiety (Tam, 2013). Thus, death anxiety is a reflection of one’s concern over dying, an outcome that would violate the prime motive.

Neuroimaging research shows terror management theory provides a useful framework for studying brain activity related to death anxiety. Quirin and colleagues (2012) found brain activity in the right amygdala, left rostral anterior cingulate cortex, and right caudate nucleus was greater when male participants were answering questions about fear of death and dying than when they were answering questions about dental pain. Similarly, electrical

terror management theory Theory that addresses the issue of why people engage in certain behaviors to achieve particular psychological states based on their deeply rooted concerns about mortality.

­◗ Figure 16.3 The Terror Management Health Model

Health Scenarios/Threats

Conscious Death-Thought Activation

Threat-Avoidance Outcomes

Motivation: Reduce Vulnerability/ Awareness of Death

Motivation: Bolster Meaning and

Symbolic Self-Conception

Health-Behavior- Oriented Outcomes

Nonconscious Death- Thought Activation

Health-Defeating Outcomes

Health-Facilitating Outcomes

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2017). Where health and death intersect: Insights from a Terror Management Health Model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 126–131. Figure 1, p. 127. doi:10.1177/0963721416689563

Think About It Why does death anxiety have so many components?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 553

activity in the brain indicates people defend themselves against emotions related to death (Klackl, Jonas, & Kronbichler, 2013). And younger adults show greater brain responses to death-related terms than do older adults, indicating a fundamental shift in how adults pro- cess death-related concepts with age (Bluntschli et al., in press). There is neurophysiologi- cal evidence that shows Jean’s attempts to block thoughts of her own death in the opening vignette are common across people.

On the basis of several diverse studies using many different measures, researchers now conclude death anxiety consists of several components. Each of these components is most easily described with terms that reflect areas of great concern (anxiety) but that cannot be tied to any one specific focus. These components of death anxiety include pain, body malfunction, humiliation, rejection, nonbeing, punishment, interruption of goals, being destroyed, and negative impact on survivors (Power & Smith, 2008). To complicate matters further, each of these components can be assessed at any of three levels: pub- lic, private, and nonconscious. What we admit feeling about death in public may differ greatly from what we feel when we are alone with our own thoughts from what we may be unaware of that still influences our behavior. In short, the measurement of death anxiety is complex, and researchers need to specify what aspects they are assessing.

Much research has been conducted to learn what demographic and personality vari- ables are related to death anxiety. Although the results often are ambiguous, some patterns have emerged. Older adults tend to have lower death anxiety than younger adults, perhaps because of their tendency to engage in life review, have a different perspective about time, and their higher level of religious motivation (Henrie, 2010). Men show greater fear of the unknown than women, but women report more specific fear of the dying process (Cici- relli, 2001). Death anxiety varies across cultures in how it is (or is not) expressed (Park & Pyszczynski, 2016). In Taiwan, higher death anxiety among patients with cancer is associ- ated with not having a purpose in life and level of fear of disease relapse (Tang et al., 2011).

Strange as it may seem, death anxiety may have a beneficial side. For one thing, being afraid to die means we often go to great lengths to make sure we stay alive, as argued by terror management theory (Burke et al., 2010; Park & Pyszczynski, 2016). Because staying alive ensures the continuation and socialization of the species, fear of death serves as a motivation to have children and raise them properly.

Learning to Deal with Death Anxiety Although some degree of death anxiety may be appropriate, we must guard against letting it become powerful enough to interfere with normal daily routines. Several ways exist to help us in this endeavor. Perhaps the one most often used is to live life to the fullest. Kalish (1984, 1987) argues people who do this enjoy what they have; although they may still fear death and feel cheated, they have few regrets.

Koestenbaum (1976) proposes several exercises and questions to increase one’s death awareness. Some of these are to write your own obituary (like you did earlier in this chapter) and to plan your own death and funeral ser- vices. You can also ask yourself: “What circum- stances would help make my death acceptable?” “Is death the sort of thing that could happen to me right now?”

These questions serve as a basis for an increasingly popular way to reduce anxiety: death education. Most death education pro- grams combine factual information about death with issues aimed at reducing anxiety and fear to increase sensitivity to others’ feelings. These programs vary widely in orientation; they

Facing death on a regular basis often forces people to confront their death anxiety.

ka ni

ns tu

di o/

12 3R

F

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

554 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

include such topics as philosophy, ethics, psychology, drama, religion, medicine, art, and many others. Additionally, they focus on death, the process of dying, grief and bereave- ment, or any combination of those. In general, death education programs help primarily by increasing our awareness of the complex emotions felt and expressed by dying people and their families. It is important to make education programs reflect the diverse backgrounds of the participants (Fowler, 2008).

Research shows participating in experiential workshops about death significantly low- ers death anxiety in younger, middle-aged, and older adults and raises awareness about the importance of advance directives (Moeller et al., 2010).

recall 1. _______________ are most likely to face the death of their

parents.

2. The _______________ acknowledges individual differences and rejects broad generalizations.

3. The primary framework for studying death anxiety is _______________.

Interpret ­● Why is it important to confront the issue of death as a part of

human development?

apply ­● using Erikson’s theory as a framework, explain how death

anxiety changes from adolescence to late life.

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 16.2

Think About It How might different approaches to low- ering death anxiety be useful to you as a healthcare worker?

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ­● What are end-of-life issues? What is a final scenario?

­● What is hospice? How does hospice relate to end-of-life issues?

­● How does one make end-of-life desires and decisions known?

16.3 End-of-Life Issues

Jean is a 72-year-old woman recently diagnosed with advanced colon cancer. She has vivid memories of her father dying a long, protracted death in great pain. Jean is afraid she will suffer the same fate. She heard the hospice in town emphasizes pain management and provides a lot of support for families. Jean wonders whether that is something she should explore in the time she has left.

When people think about how they would like to die, no one chooses a slow, painful pro- cess where medical intervention continues well beyond the point of increasing quality of life over quantity of life. However, medical intervention such as life support or cardio- pulmonary resuscitation (CPR), are common, often required by law, even in situations in which people would prefer them not to be used. How can people make their wishes known about how they want to experience the end of their life?

Creating a Final Scenario When given the chance, many adults would like to discuss a variety of issues, collectively called end-of-life issues: management of the final phase of life, after-death disposition of their body, memorial services, and distribution of assets (Moeller et al., 2010). We are expe- riencing a major shift in how people handle end-of-life issues. Prior to the current genera- tion of older adults, people rarely planned ahead for or made their wishes known about

end-of-life issues Issues pertaining to management of the final phase of life, after-death disposition of the body and memorial services, and distribution of assets.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 555

medical care they did or did not want. Now, people want to manage the final part of their lives by thinking through the choices between traditional care (e.g., provided by hospitals and nursing homes) and alternatives (such as hospices, that we discuss in the next sec- tion), completing advance directives (e.g., healthcare power of attorney, living will), resolv- ing key personal relationships, and perhaps choosing the alternative of ending one’s life prematurely through euthanasia.

Consider the issue of rituals surrounding the time immediately following death. What happens to one’s body and how one is memorialized matters to most people. But decisions about these have to be made. Is a traditional burial preferred over cremation? A traditional funeral over a memorial service? Such choices often are based in people’s religious beliefs and their desire for privacy for their families after they have died.

Making sure one’s estate and personal effects are passed on appropriately often is overlooked. Making a will is especially important to ensure one’s wishes are carried out. Providing for the informal distribution of personal effects also helps prevent disputes between family members.

Whether people choose to address these issues formally or informally, it is important they be given the opportunity to do so. In many cases, family members are reluctant to discuss these matters with the dying relative because of their own anxiety about death. Making such choices known about how they do and do not want their lives to end consti- tutes a final scenario.

One of the most difficult and important parts of a final scenario for most people is the process of separation from family and friends (Corr & Corr, 2013; Wanzer & Glenmul- len, 2007). The final days, weeks, and months of life provide opportunities to affirm love, resolve conflicts, and provide peace to dying people. The failure to complete this process often leaves survivors feeling they did not achieve closure in the relationship, and can result in bitterness toward the deceased.

Healthcare workers realize the importance of giving dying patients the chance to cre- ate a final scenario and recognize the uniqueness of each person’s final passage. A key part of their role is to ease this process through good communication with the family (Curtis et al., 2016; Wanzer & Glenmullen, 2007). Any given final scenario reflects the individual’s personal past, that is the unique combination of the development forces the person expe- rienced. Primary attention is paid to how people’s total life experiences prepared them to face end-of-life issues (Curtis et al., 2016).

One’s final scenario helps family and friends interpret one’s death, especially when the sce- nario is constructed jointly, such as between spouses, and when communication is open and honest. The different perspectives of everyone involved are unlikely to converge without clear communication and discussion. Respecting each person’s perspective is basic and greatly helps in creating a good final scenario.

Encouraging people to decide for them- selves how the end of their lives should be han- dled helps people take control of their dying (Hains & Hulbert-Williams, 2013) and think through issues such as euthanasia are processed at the individual level (Feltz, 2015). Taking per- sonal control over one’s dying process is a trend occurring even in cultures such as Japan that tra- ditionally defer to physician’s opinions (Alden, Merz, & Akashi, 2012). The emergence of final scenarios as an important consideration fits well with the emphasis on addressing pain through palliative care, an approach underlying hospice.

final scenario Way for people to make their choices known about how they do and do not want their lives to end.

Deciding whether to have a traditional funeral is part of the creation of one’s final scenario.

Em ip

re ss

/S hu

tt er

st oc

k. co

m

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

556 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

The Hospice Option As we have seen, most people would like to die at home among family and friends. An important barrier to this choice is the availability of support systems when the person has a terminal disease. Most people believe they have no choice but to go to a hospital or nurs- ing home. However, another alternative exists. hospice is an approach to assist dying peo- ple emphasizing pain management, or palliative care, and death with dignity (Knee, 2010; Winslow & Meldrum, 2013). The emphasis in a hospice is on the dying person’s quality of life. This approach grows out of an important distinction between the prolongation of life and the prolongation of death, a distinction important to Jean, the woman we met in the vignette. In a hospice the concern is to make the person as peaceful and as comfortable as possible, not to delay an inevitable death. An approach to care based on an ethic of control- ling and relieving pain or other symptoms and not on attempting to cure disease is called palliative care (Prince-Paul & Daly, 2016). Hospice is the leading provider of such care, but palliative care is also adopted in other settings.

Modern hospices are modeled after St. Christopher’s Hospice in England, founded in 1967 by Dr. Cicely Saunders. Hospice services are requested only after the person or physician believes no treatment or cure is possible, making the hospice program markedly different from hospital or home care. The differences are evident in the principles that underlie hospice care (Knee, 2010):

­● Clients and their families are viewed as a unit, clients should be kept free of pain, emo- tional and social impoverishment must be minimal;

­● Clients must be encouraged to maintain competencies, conflict resolution and fulfill- ment of realistic desires must be assisted; and

­● Clients must be free to begin or end relationships, an interdisciplinary team approach is used, and staff members must seek to alleviate pain and fear.

Two types of hospices exist: inpatient and outpatient. Inpatient hospices provide all care for clients; outpatient hospices provide services to clients who remain in their own homes. The outpatient variation, when a hospice nurse visits clients in their home, is becoming increasingly popular, largely because more clients can be served at a lower cost. Having hospice services available to people at home is a viable option for many people, especially in helping home-based caregivers cope with loss, but should be provided by specially trained professionals (Newman, Thompson, & Chandler, 2013).

Hospices do not follow a hospital model of care. The role of the staff in a hospice is not so much to treat the client as it is just to be with the client. A client’s dignity is always maintained; often more attention is paid to appearance and personal grooming than to medical tests. Hospice staff members also provide a great deal of support to the client’s family.

Increasingly, this support includes different ways of being present to and with the person who is dying. Many hospices have the option for death doulas who help ease the  passage through death. For some dying individuals, the doula may simply hold their hand; for others it may be through playing special music and sitting or meditating with them. The main role is to ensure that dying people are not alone, and that families, if pres- ent, have personal support.

Hospice and hospital patients differ in important ways (Knee, 2010). Hospice clients are more mobile, less anxious, and less depressed; spouses visit hospice clients more often and participate more in their care; and hospice staff members are perceived as more  accessible. Research consistently shows significant improvements in clients’ quality of life occur after hospice placement or beginning palliative care (Blackhall et al., 2016; Rocque & Cleary, 2013).

Although the hospice is a valuable alternative for many people, it may not be appro- priate for everyone. Those who trust their physician regarding medical care options are more likely to select hospice than those who do not trust their physician, especially among African Americans, who as a group prefer more aggressive treatment options (Ludke & Smucker, 2007; Smith-Howell et al., 2016). Most people who select hospice suffer from

hospice Approach to assisting dying people that emphasizes pain management, or palliative care, and death with dignity.

palliative care Care that is focused on providing relief from pain and other symptoms of disease at any point during the disease process.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 557

cancer, AIDS, cardiovascular disease, pulmonary disease, or a progressive neurological condition such as dementia; two-thirds are over age 65; and most are in the last six months of life (Hospice Foundation of America, 2016a).

Needs expressed by staff, family, and clients differ (Hiatt et al., 2007). Staff and family members tend to emphasize pain management, whereas many clients want more attention paid to personal issues, such as spirituality and the process of dying. This difference means the staff and family members may need to ask clients more often what they need instead of making assumptions about what they need.

How do people decide among various care options, such as hospice, home health, or skilled care? Families should ask several key questions (Hospice Foundation of America, 2016b; Karp & Wood, 2012; Knee, 2010):

­● Is the person completely informed about the nature and prognosis of his or her condi- tion? Full knowledge and the ability to communicate with healthcare personnel are essential to understanding what hospice has to offer.

­● What options are available at this point in the progress of the person’s disease? Know- ing about all available treatment options is critical. Exploring treatment options also requires healthcare professionals to be aware of the latest approaches and be willing to disclose them.

­● What are the person’s expectations, fears, and hopes? Some older adults, like Jean, remember or have heard stories about people who suffered greatly at the end of their lives. This can produce anxiety about one’s own death. Similarly, fears of becoming dependent play an important role in a person’s decision making. Discovering and dis- cussing these anxieties help clarify options.

­● How well do people in the person’s social network communicate with each other? Talking about death is taboo in many families. In others, intergenerational communication is difficult or impossible. Even in families with good communication, the pending death of a loved relative is difficult. As a result, the dying person may have difficulty express- ing his or her wishes. The decision to explore the hospice option is best made when it is discussed openly.

­● Are family members available to participate actively in terminal care? Hospice relies on family members to provide much of the care that is supplemented by professionals and volunteers. We saw in Chapters 11 and 13 being a primary caregiver can be highly stressful. Having a family member who is willing to accept this responsibility is essential for the hospice option to work.

Hospice outpatient healthcare workers provide help for people with terminal diseases who choose to die at home.ZU

M A

P re

ss In

c/ A

la m

y St

oc k

Ph ot

o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

558 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

­● Is a high-quality hospice care program available? Hospice programs are not uniformly good. As with any healthcare provider, patients and family members must investigate the quality of local hospice programs before making a choice. The Hospice Foundation of America provides excellent material for evaluating a hospice.

­● Is hospice covered by insurance? Hospice services are reimbursable under Medicare in most cases, but any additional expenses may or may not be covered under other forms of insurance.

Hospice provides an important end-of-life option for many terminally ill people and their families. Moreover, the supportive follow-up services they provide are often used by surviving family and friends. Most important, the success of the hospice option has had important influences on traditional health care. For example, the American Academy of  Pain Medicine (2016) publishes official position papers regarding the appropriate use  of medical and behavioral interventions to provide pain management. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2016c) also published guidelines for the appropriate use of opioids for pain management due to the risk of addiction from these medications.

Despite the importance of the hospice option for end-of-life decisions, terminally ill persons face the barriers of family reluctance to face the reality of terminal illness and participate in the decision-making process, and healthcare providers who hinder access to hospice care (Knee, 2010; Moon, 2017; Torres et al., 2016).

As the end of life approaches, the most important thing to keep in mind is that the dying person has the right to state-of-the-art approaches to treatment and pain management. Irrespective of the choice of traditional health care or hospice, the wishes of the dying per- son should be honored, and family members and primary care providers must participate.

Making Your End-of-Life Intentions Known End-of-life realities raise complex legal, political, and ethical issues. In most jurisdictions, ending life through such means as euthanasia or assisted suicide is legal only when a person has made known his or her wishes concerning medical or other intervention. Unfortunately, many people fail to explicitly state their wishes, perhaps because it is difficult to think about such situations or because they do not know the options available to them. Without clear directions, though, medical personnel may be unable to take a patient’s preferences into account. For instance, many states have laws requiring CPR or other attempts at resuscita- tion be used in the absence of clear evidence that the person does not want them used.

There are two ways to make one’s intentions known. In a living will, a person simply states his or her wishes about life support and other treatments. In a healthcare power of attorney, an individual appoints someone to act as his or her agent for healthcare decisions (see ◗ ­Figure 16.4). A major purpose of both is to make one’s wishes known about the use of life-support interventions in the event the person is unconscious or otherwise incapable of expressing them, along with other related end-of-life issues such as organ transplantation and other healthcare options. Without them, the ethical considerations of life- sustaining intervention for terminally ill patients is fraught with ethical dilemmas (Awadi & Mrayyan, 2016; Portnoy et al., 2015). A durable power of attorney for health care has an additional advantage: It names an individual who has the legal authority to speak and make decisions for the person if necessary.

A living will or a durable power of attorney for health care can be the basis for a “do not resuscitate” medical order. A do not resuscitate (DNr) order means cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR) is not started should one’s heart and breathing stop. In the normal course of events, a medical team will immediately try to restore normal heartbeat and respiration. With a DNR order, this treatment is not done. As with living wills and health- care power of attorney, it is extremely important to let all appropriate medical personnel know a DNR order is in effect.

Although there is considerable support for both living wills and healthcare power of attorney, there are several challenges as well (Alspach, 2016; Izumi & Son, 2016). States vary in their laws relating to advance directives. Many people fail to inform their relatives and physicians about their healthcare decisions. Others do not tell the person named in a durable

living will Document in which a person states his or her wishes about life support and other treatments.

healthcare power of attorney Document in which an individual appoints someone to act as his or her agent for healthcare decisions.

do not resuscitate (DNr) order Medical order that means that cardiopul- monary resuscitation (CPR) is not started should one’s heart and breathing stop.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 559

power of attorney where the document is kept. Obviously, this puts relatives at a serious disadvantage if decisions concerning the use of life-support systems need to be made.

Fortunately, since 2016 Medicare covers advance care planning as a separate service provided by physicians and other healthcare professionals such as nurse practitioners (Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2016). This makes it easier for patients to discuss their wishes with a healthcare professional and to learn about options. Ideally, these discussions result in the actual documentation of a person’s specific wishes.

Patient Self-Determination and Competency Evaluation Making your decisions about health care known presumes that you are competent and able to make those decisions for yourself. To aid healthcare providers in this process, the Patient Self-Determination Act, passed in 1990, requires most healthcare facilities to pro- vide information to patients in writing that they have the right to:

­● Make their own healthcare decisions. ­● Accept or refuse medical treatment. ­● Make an advance healthcare directive.

­◗ Figure 16.4 Example of a healthcare power of attorney document.

3. General statement of authority granted.

Except as indicated in section 4 below, I hereby grant to my health care agent named above full power and authority to make health care decisions on my behalf, including, but not limited to, the following:

A. To request, review, and receive any information, verbal or written, regarding my physical or mental health, including, but not limited to, medical and hospital records, and to consent to the disclosure of this information;

B. To employ or discharge my health care providers;

C. To consent to and authorize my admission to and discharge from a hospital, nursing or convalescent home, or other institution;

D. To give consent for, to withdraw consent for, or to withhold consent for, X ray, anesthesia, medication, surgery, and all other diagnostic and treatment procedures ordered by or under the authorization of a licensed physician, dentist, or podiatrist. This authorization speci�cally includes the power to consent to measures for relief of pain.

E. To authorize the withholding or withdrawal of life-sustaining procedures when and if my physician determines that I am terminally ill, permanently in a coma, suffer severe dementia, or am in a persistent vegetative state. Lifesustaining procedures are those forms of medical care that only serve to arti�cially prolong the dying process and may include mechanical ventilation, dialysis, antibiotics, arti�cial nutrition and hydration, and other forms of medical treatment which sustain, restore or supplant vital bodily functions. Life-sustaining procedures do not include care necessary to provide comfort or alleviate pain.

I DESIRE THAT MY LIFE NOT BE PROLONGED BY LIFE-SUSTAINING PROCEDURES IF I AM TERMINALLY ILL, PERMANENTLY IN A COMA, SUFFER SEVERE DEMENTIA, OR AM IN A PERSISTENT VEGETATIVE STATE.

F. To exercise any right I may have to make a disposition of any part or all of my body for medical purposes, to donate my organs, to authorize an autopsy, and to direct the disposition of my remains.

G. To take any lawful actions that may be necessary to carry out these decisions, including the granting of releases of liability to medical providers.

4. Special provisions and limitations.

(Notice: The above grant of power is intended to be as broad as possible so that your health care agent will have authority to make any decisions you could make to obtain or terminate any type of health care. If you wish to limit the scope of your health care agent’s powers, you may do so in this section.)

In exercising the authority to make health care decisions on my behalf, the authority of my health care agent is subject to the following special provisions and limitations (Here you may include any specific limitations you deem appropriate such as: your own definition of when life-sustaining treatment should be withheld or discontinued, or instructions to refuse any specific types of treatment that are inconsistent with your religious beliefs, or unacceptable to you for any other reason.) :

5. Guardianship provision.

If it becomes necessary for a court to appoint a guardian of my person, I nominate my health care agent acting under this document to be the guardian of my person, to serve without bond or security.

6. Reliance of third parties on health care agent.

A. No person who relies in good faith upon the authority of or any representations by my health care agent shall be liable to me, my estate, my heirs, successors, assigns, or personal representatives, for actions or omissions by my health care agent.

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From North Carolina State University, A&T State University Cooperative Extension.

Think About It Have you made your end-of-life intentions known?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

560 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Patients must be asked if they have an advance directive, and, if so, it must be included in the medical record. Staff at the healthcare facility must receive training about advance directives, and cannot make admissions or treatment decisions based on whether those directives exist.

One major concern regarding the appropriate implementation of the Patient Self- Determination Act is whether the person is cognitively or legally able to make the deci- sions about end-of-life care (Moye, Sabatino, & Brendel, 2013). There are two types of determination: the capacity to make decisions, that is a clinical determination, and a com- petency decision, made legally by the court (Wettstein, 2013). With capacity determina- tions, the issue is whether the individual is able to make a decision about specific tasks, and the abilities necessary are subject to measurement. With competency determinations, the individual is being judged either with respect to a specific task or in general, and the determination can be made subjectively by the court.

At this point, the case law is limited regarding whether a person who lacks the capac- ity to make healthcare decisions can still designate a surrogate to make them on their behalf. This situation is rather common, though, given the tendency for families to not discuss these issues, individual’s reluctance to face the potential need, and the politiciza- tion of the conversation in the healthcare arena. Guidelines for professionals regarding the assessment of competence are available, and they provide insight into both the psychologi- cal and legal issues surrounding such evaluations (Moye et al., 2013; Wettstein, 2013).

Research indicates family members and other surrogate decision-makers are often wrong about what loved ones, even spouses or partners, really want (Moorman & Inoue, 2013). This further emphasizes the critical need, especially for couples, to discuss end-of- life issues ahead of time and ensure the appropriate advance directives are in place and key individuals are aware of them (Queen, Berg, & Lowrance, 2015).

Think About It What steps are necessary to ensure that your advance directives about health care are followed?

recall 1. Making choices known about what people do and do not

want their lives to end constitutes a(n) _______________.

2. _______________ is an approach to assisting dying people that emphasizes pain management, or palliative care, and death with dignity.

3. In a(n) _______________, a person states his or her wishes about life support and other treatments.

Interpret ­● Why is there a difference in treatment approach between hos-

pitals and hospices?

apply ­● How does creating a final scenario incorporate the biopsycho-

social forces of development?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 16.3

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ­● How do people experience the grief process?

­● What feelings do grieving people have?

­● How do people cope with grief?

­● What are the types of ambiguous loss?

­● What is the difference between normal and complicated grief?

­● What is disenfranchised grief?

16.4 Surviving the Loss: The Grieving Process

After 67 years of marriage, Bertha recently lost her husband. At 90, Bertha knew neither she nor her husband was likely to live much longer, but the death was a shock just the same. Bertha thinks about him much of the time and often finds herself making decisions on the basis of “what John would have done” in the same situation.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 561

Each of us suffers many losses over a lifetime. Whenever we lose someone close to us through death or other separation, like Bertha we experience bereavement, grief, and mourning. Bereavement is the state or condition caused by loss through death. Grief is the sorrow, hurt, anger, guilt, confusion, and other feelings that arise after suffering a loss. Mourning concerns the ways we express our grief. You can tell people in some cultures are bereaved and in mourning because of the clothing they wear. Mourning is highly influ- enced by culture. For some, mourning may involve wearing black, attending funerals, and observing an official period of grief; for others, it means drinking, wearing white, and mar- rying the deceased spouse’s sibling. Grief corresponds to the emotional reactions follow- ing loss, whereas mourning is the culturally approved behavioral manifestations of those feelings. Even though mourning rituals may be fairly standard within a culture, how peo- ple grieve varies, as we see next. We will also see how Bertha’s reactions are fairly typical of most people.

The Grief Process How do people grieve? What do they experience? Perhaps you already have a good idea about the answers to these questions from your own experience. If so, you already know the process of grieving is a complicated and personal one. Just as there is no right way to die, there is no right way to grieve. Recognizing there are plenty of individual differences, we consider these patterns in this section.

The grieving process is often described as reflecting many themes and issues people confront that may be expressed through rituals, both in-person and digital (Gamba, 2015; Norton & Gino, 2014). Like the process of dying, grieving does not have clearly demar- cated stages through which we pass in a neat sequence, although there are certain issues people must face similar to those faced by dying people. When someone close to us dies, we must reorganize our lives, establish new patterns of behavior, and redefine relation- ships with family and friends. Indeed, Attig (1996) provided one of the best descriptions of grief when he wrote grief is the process by which we relearn the world.

Unlike bereavement, over which we have no control, grief is a process that involves choices in coping, from confronting the reality and emotions to using religion to ease one’s pain (Cummings, 2015; Norton & Gino, 2014). From this perspective, grief is an active process when a person must do several things (Worden, 1991):

­● Acknowledge the reality of the loss. We must overcome the temptation to deny the reality of our loss; we must fully and openly acknowledge it and realize it affects every aspect of our life.

­● Work through the emotional turmoil. We must find effective ways to confront and express the complete range of emotions we feel after the loss and must not avoid or repress them.

­● Adjust to the environment where the deceased is absent. We must define new pat- terns of living that adjust appropriately and meaningfully to the fact the deceased is not present.

­● Loosen ties to the deceased. We must free ourselves from the bonds of the deceased in order to reengage with our social network. This means finding effective ways to say good-bye.

Grief is an active coping process (Bagbey Darian, 2014). In processing grief, survivors must come to terms with and integrate the

bereavement State or condition caused by loss through death.

grief Sorrow, hurt, anger, guilt, confusion, and other feelings that arise after suffering a loss.

mourning Ways in which people express their grief.

Dealing with the death of friends is often especially difficult for young adults.

D re

am Pi

ct ur

es /B

le nd

Im ag

es /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

562 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

physical world of things, places, and events as well as their spiritual place in the world; the interpersonal world of interactions with family and friends, the dead, and, in some cases, God; and aspects of our inner selves and our personal experiences. Bertha, the woman in  the vignette, is in the middle of this process. Even the matter of deciding what to do with the deceased’s personal effects can be part of this active coping process (Attig, 1996).

To make sense of grief, we need to keep several things in mind. First, grieving is a highly individual experience (Bagbey Darian, 2014; Cummings, 2015; Steck & Steck, 2016). A process that works well for one person may not be the best for someone else. Second, we must not underestimate the amount of time people need to deal with the various issues. To a casual observer, it may appear a survivor is “back to normal” after a few weeks ( Harris, 2016). Actually, what may look like a return to normal activities may reflect bereaved peo- ple feeling social pressure to “get on with things.” It takes most people much longer to resolve the complex emotional issues faced during bereavement. Researchers and thera- pists alike agree a person needs at least a year following the loss to begin recovery, and two years is not uncommon.

Finally, “recovery” may be a misleading term. It is probably more accurate to say we learn to live with our loss rather than we recover from it (Attig, 1996). The impact of the loss of a loved one lasts a long time, perhaps for the rest of one’s life. Still, most people reach a point of moving on with their lives (Bagbey Darian, 2014; Bonanno, 2009; Harris, 2016).

Recognizing these aspects of grief makes it easier to know what to say and do for bereaved people. Among the most useful things are simply to let the person know you are sorry for his or her loss, you are there for support, and mean what you say.

Risk Factors in Grief Bereavement is a life experience most people have many times, and most people eventu- ally handle it, often better than we might suspect (Bagbey Darian, 2014; Bonanno, 2009; Bonanno, Westphal, & Mancini, 2011; Cummings, 2015; Steck & Steck, 2016). However, there are some risk factors that make bereavement more difficult. Several of the more important are the mode of death, personal factors (e.g., personality, religiosity, age, gender, income), and interpersonal context (social support, kinship relationship).

Most people believe the circumstances of death affects the grief process. A person whose family member was killed in an automobile accident has a different situation to deal with than a person whose family member died after a long period of suffering with Alzheimer’s disease. It is believed when death is anticipated, people go through a period of anticipatory grief before the death that supposedly serves to buffer the impact of the loss when it does come and to facilitate recovery (Shore et al., 2016). Indicators of anticipatory grief may appear as pre-loss grief, and as depression, anxiety, or pain.

The research evidence for whether anticipatory grief helps people cope with loss better is mixed. Anticipating the loss of a loved one from cancer or other terminal disease can provide a framework for understanding family members’ reactions (Coombs, 2010; Shore et al., 2016). However, anticipatory grief does not appear to alleviate the outcome of the bereavement; in fact, it may even make it more difficult to reach a positive outcome (Nielsen et al., 2016).

The strength of attachment to the deceased person does make a difference in dealing with a sudden as opposed to an expected death. Attachment theory provides a framework for understanding different reactions (Hales, 2016; Stroebe & Archer, 2013). When the deceased person was one for whom the survivor had a strong and close attachment and the loss was sudden, the grief is greater. However, such secure attachment styles tend to result in less depression after the loss because of less guilt over unresolved issues (because there are fewer of them), things not provided (because more were likely provided), and so on.

Few studies of personal risk factors have been done, and few firm conclusions can be drawn. To date there are no consistent findings regarding personality traits that either help buffer people from the effects of bereavement or exacerbate them (Haley, 2013;

anticipatory grief Grief that is experienced during the period before an expected death occurs that sup- posedly serves to buffer the impact of the loss when it does come and to facilitate recovery.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 563

Stroebe & Archer, 2013). Some evidence suggests church attendance or spirituality in gen- eral helps people to deal with bereavement and subsequent grief through the post-grief period (Gordon, 2013). There are, however, consistent findings regarding gender. Men and women differ in the ways they express emotions related to grief (Kersting & Nagl, 2016). Men have higher mortality rates following bereavement than women, who have higher rates of depression and complicated grief (discussed later in this section) than men, but the reasons for these differences are unclear (Kersting et al., 2011). Research also consis- tently shows older adults suffer the least health consequences following bereavement, with the impact perhaps being strongest for middle-aged adults, but strong social support net- works, including virtual ones, lessen these effects to varying degrees (Chang et al., 2016; Papa & Litz, 2011).

Typical Grief Reactions The feelings experienced during grieving are intense; these feelings not only make it dif- ficult to cope but can also make a person question her or his own reactions. The feelings involved usually include sadness, denial, anger, loneliness, and guilt.

Many authors refer to the psychological side of coming to terms with bereavement as grief work. Whether the loss is ambiguous and lacking closure (e.g., waiting to learn the fate of a missing loved one) or certain (e.g., verification of death through a dead body), people need space and time to grieve (Berns, 2011; Harris, 2016). However, a major chal- lenge in American society is that, as noted earlier, people feel pressured to “move on” quickly after a loss, especially if that loss is not of a spouse or child. That is not how people really feel or want to deal with their grief; they want the opportunity to work through their feelings on their own terms and timeline.

Muller and Thompson (2003) examined people’s experience of grief in a detailed inter- view study and found five themes. Coping concerns what people do to deal with their loss in terms of what helps them. Affect refers to people’s emotional reactions to the death of their loved one, such as certain topics that serve as emotional triggers for memories of their loved one. Change involves the ways survivors’ lives change as a result of the loss; personal growth (e.g., “I didn’t think I could deal with something that painful, but I did”) is a common expe- rience. Narrative relates to the stories survivors tell about their deceased loved one, that sometimes includes details about the process of the death. Finally, relationship reflects who the deceased person was and the nature of the ties between that person and the survivor. Collectively, these themes indicate the experience of grief is complex and involves dealing with one’s feelings as a survivor as well as memories of the deceased person.

How people show their feelings of grief var- ies across ethnic and cultural groups ( Bordere, 2016; Gire, 2014). For example, families in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa, have a strong desire for closure and need for dealing with the “ loneliness of grief ” (Brysiewicz, 2008). In many cultures the bereaved construct a relationship with the person who died, but how this hap- pens differs widely, from “ghosts” to appearances in dreams to connection through prayer and ancestor worship (Cacciatore & DeFrain, 2015). Differences in dealing with grief and bereave- ment can also be observed across different subgroups within ethnic groups. For instance, various Hispanic groups (e.g., Mexican, Puerto Rican, Central American) differ somewhat from each other in ritual practices (Schoulte, 2011).

In addition to psychological grief reactions, there are also physiological ones (McCoyd &

grief work Psychological side of coming to terms with bereavement.

Think About It How is grief influenced by sociocultural factors?

How openly grief is expressed varies considerably across cultures.

Ku rs

at B

ay ha

n/ G

et ty

Im ag

es N

ew s/

G et

ty Im

ag es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

564 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Walter, 2016). Physical health may decline, illness may result, and use of healthcare ser- vices may increase. Some people report sleep disturbances as well as neurological and cir- culatory problems (Kowalski & Bondmass, 2008; Naef et al., 2013). Widowers in general report major disruptions in their daily routines (Naef et al., 2013).

In the time following the death of a loved one, dates having personal significance may reintroduce feelings of grief. Holidays such as Thanksgiving or birthdays that were spent with the deceased person may be difficult times. The actual anniversary of the death can be especially troublesome. The term anniversary reaction refers to changes in behavior related to feelings of sadness on this date. Personal experience and research show recurring feelings of sadness or other examples of the anniversary reaction are common in normal grief (DiBello, 2015; Holland & Neimeyer, 2010; Rostila et al., 2015). Such feelings also accompany remembrances of major catastrophes across cultures, such as Thais remem- bering the victims of a tsunami and major flood (Assanangkornchai et al., 2007).

Most research on how people react to the death of a loved one is cross-sectional. This work shows grief tends to peak within the first six months following the death of a loved one (Maciejewski et al., 2007) but may never fully go away, with effects on cognitive well- being greater than for emotional well-being in general (Luhmann et al., 2012).

Research has been done to examine longitudinal effects of grief. Some widows show no sign of lessening of grief after 5 years (Kowalski & Bondmass, 2008). Rosenblatt (1996) reported people still felt the effects of the deaths of family members 50 years after the event. The depth of the emotions over the loss of loved ones sometimes never totally goes away, as people still cry and feel sad when discussing the loss despite the length of time that had passed. In general, though, people move on with their lives and deal with their feelings reasonably well (Bagbey Darian, 2014; Bonanno, 2009; Bonnano et al., 2011; Cum- mings, 2015; Steck & Steck, 2016).

Coping With Grief Thus far, we considered the behaviors people show when they are dealing with grief. We have also seen these behaviors change over time. How does this happen? How can we explain the grieving process?

Numerous theories have been proposed to account for the grieving process, such as general life-event theories, psychodynamic, attachment, and cognitive process theories (Stroebe & Archer, 2013; Stroebe, Schut, & Boerner, 2010). All of these approaches to grief are based on more general theories that result in none of them providing an adequate explanation of the grieving process. Three integrative approaches have been proposed specific to the grief process: the four-component model, the dual-process model of coping with bereavement, and the model of adaptive grieving dynamics.

The Four-Component Model The four-component model proposes understanding grief is based on four things: (1) the context of the loss, referring to the risk factors such as whether the death was expected; (2)  continuation of subjective meaning associated with loss, ranging from evaluations of everyday concerns to major questions about the meaning of life; (3) changing representa- tions of the lost relationship over time; and (4) the role of coping and emotion regulation processes that cover all coping strategies used to deal with grief (Bonanno, 2009; Bonanno et al., 2011). The four-component model relies heavily on emotion theory, has much in common with the transactional model of stress, and has empirical support. According to the four-component model, dealing with grief is a complicated process only understood as a complex outcome that unfolds over time.

There are several important implications of this integrative approach. One of the most important in helping a grieving person involves helping her or him make meaning from the loss (Bratkovich, 2010; Wong, 2015). Second, this model implies encouraging people to express their grief may actually not be helpful. An alternative view, called the grief work as rumination hypothesis, not only rejects the necessity of grief processing for recovery from loss

anniversary reaction Changes in behavior related to feelings of sadness on the anniversary date of a loss.

four-component model Model for understanding grief that is based on (1) the context of the loss, (2) continua- tion of subjective meaning associated with loss, (3) changing representations of the lost relationship over time, and (4) the role of coping and emotion-regulation processes.

grief work as rumination hypothesis Approach that not only rejects the necessity of grief processing for recovery from loss but also views extensive grief processing as a form of rumination that may increase distress.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 565

but views extensive grief processing as a form of rumination that may actually increase dis- tress (Bonanno, Papa, & O’Neill, 2001). Although it may seem people who think obsessively about their loss or who ruminate about it are confronting the loss, rumination is actually considered a form of avoidance because the person is not dealing with his or her real feel- ings and moving on (Bui et al., 2015; Eisma et al., 2015; Robinaugh & McNally, 2013).

One prospective study shows, for instance, bereaved individuals who were not depressed prior to their spouse’s death but then evidenced chronically elevated depression through the first year and a half of bereavement (i.e., a chronic grief pattern) also tended to report more frequently thinking about and talking about their recent loss at the 6-month point in bereavement (Bonanno, Wortman, & Neese, 2004). Thus, some bereaved indi- viduals engage in minimal grief processing whereas others are predisposed toward more extensive grief processing. Furthermore, the individuals who engage in minimal grief pro- cessing will show a relatively favorable grief outcome, whereas those who are predisposed to more extensive grief processing tend toward ruminative preoccupation and, conse- quently, to a more prolonged grief course (Bonanno, 2009; Bonanno et al., 2011; Bui et al., 2015; Robinaugh & McNally, 2013).

In contrast to the traditional perspective that equates the absence of grief processing with grief avoidance, the grief work as rumination framework assumes resilient individu- als are able to minimize processing of a loss through relatively automated processes, such as distraction or shifting attention toward more positive emotional experiences (Bonanno, 2009; Bonanno et al., 2011; Eisma et al., 2015). The grief work as rumination framework argues the deliberate avoidance or suppression of grief actually makes the experience of grief worse (Bonanno, 2009; Bonanno et al., 2011).

The Dual Process Model The dual process model (DpM) of coping with bereavement integrates existing ideas regarding stressors (Stroebe & Archer, 2013; Utz & Pascoe, 2016). As shown in ◗ ­Figure 16.5, the DPM defines two broad types of stressors. Loss-oriented stressors concern the loss itself, such as the grief work that needs to be done. Restoration-oriented stressors are those that involve adapting to the survivor’s new life situation, such as building new relation- ships and finding new activities. The DPM proposes dealing with these stressors is a dynamic process, as indicated by the lines connecting them in the figure. This is a distin- guishing feature of DPM. It shows how bereaved people cycle back and forth between dealing mostly with grief and trying to move on with life. At times the emphasis will be on grief; at other times on moving forward.

The DPM captures well the process bereaved people themselves report—at times they are nearly overcome with grief, while at other times they handle life well. The DPM also helps us understand how, over time, people come to a balance between the long-term effects of bereavement and the need to live life. Understanding how people handle grief

dual process model (DpM) View of coping with bereavement that integrates loss-oriented stressors and restoration-oriented stressors.

­◗ Figure 16.5 The dual process model of coping with bereavement shows the relation between dealing with the stresses of the loss itself (loss-oriented) and moving on with one's life (restoration-oriented).

• Restoration-oriented • Doing new things • Distraction from grief • Denial/avoidance of

grief • New roles/identities/

relationships

Everyday Life Experience

• Loss-oriented • Grief work • Intrusion of grief • Breaking bonds/ties

relocation of the deceased person

• Denial/avoidance of restoration changes

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (2001). Models of coping with bereavement: A review. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 375–403). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

566 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

requires understanding the various contexts in which people live and interact with others (Sandler, Wolchik, & Ayers, 2007; Sandler et al., 2013).

The Model of Adaptive Grieving Dynamics For anyone who has experienced the loss of a loved one, grieving is an intense, personal, complicated process that does not follow a straight path through predictable stages (Bagbey Darian, 2014). Consequently, to understand grief is to understand that multiple responses to loss may each prove adaptive in different ways. Only when they are consid- ered together and in their unique combination for each grieving person is a more com- plete understanding of grief possible.

The basic structure of the model of adaptive grieving dynamics is shown in ◗ ­Figure 16.6. As can be seen, the model of adaptive grieving dynamics (MaGD) is based on two sets of pairs of adaptive grieving dynamics. One pair consists of lamenting and heartening responses to grief; the other pair consists of integrating and tempering responses to grief. These four interrelated dynamics are defined as follows (Bagbey Darian, 2014):

­● Lamenting: experiencing and/or expressing grieving responses that are distressful, dis- heartening, and/or painful;

­● Heartening: experiencing and/or expressing grieving responses that are gratifying, uplifting, and/or pleasurable;

­● Integrating: assimilating internal and external changes catalyzed by a grief-inducing loss, and reconciling differences in past, present, and future realities in light of these changes; and

­● Tempering: avoiding chronic attempts to integrate changed realities impacted by a grief- inducing loss that overwhelm a griever’s and/or community’s resources and capacities to integrate such changes.

Bagbey Darian (2014) argues that although the pairs of dynamics appear to be con- tradictory, in processing grief they actually work together. For instance, grieving people often experience both joy and sorrow simultaneously when remembering a loved one. This

simultaneity of experience is a key difference between MAGD and the dual process model, as the dual process model argues that grieving peo- ple oscillate between loss-oriented tasks and restoration-oriented tasks.

According to the MAGD, the outcome of grief is not “working things through,” or necessarily finding meaning in the loss. Rather, it aims at understanding how people continually negotiate and renegoti- ate their personal and interpersonal equilibrium over time. Grieving never really ends; how the person continues finding balance given that reality is the issue.

Ambiguous Loss To this point we have been considering grief reactions to loss in which there is the possibility of closure. In these situations, there is proof of death, usually a physical dead body, and the likelihood of a funeral or some other ritual for support of the survivors. But this is not always the case. Consider major natural disasters, such as tsunamis, or major explosions, accidents, or repressive governments who kidnap or other- wise make people “disappear.” The tsunami in Japan in 2011 left thou- sands unaccounted for.

Boss (2015) coined the term ambiguous loss for these circum- stances. ambiguous loss refers to situations of loss in which there is no resolution or closure. Boss (2010, 2015) describes two types of ambigu- ous loss. The first type refers to a missing person who is physically absent but still very present psychologically to family and friends. Examples of this type include people missing after disasters, victims of kidnapping, and those never recovered from accidents.

model of adaptive grieving dynamics (MaGD) Model of grieving based on two sets of pairs of adaptive grieving dynamics: lamenting and heartening responses to grief; integrat- ing and tempering responses to grief.

ambiguous loss Situations of loss in which there is no resolution or closure to a loss.

­◗ Figure 16.6 Model of adaptive grieving dynamics

Tempering Griever Integrating

Lamenting

Heartening

Griever’s expanding

life experiences

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Bagbey Darian, C. D. (2014). A new mourning: Synthesizing an interactive model of adaptive grieving dynamics. Illness, Crisis & Loss, 22(3), 195–235. doi: 10.2190/IL.22.3.c p. 203.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 567

The primary challenge for the families and friends of the missing is the unending pain of not knowing for certain what happened, where their loved one is at present, and their specific fate. It is this pain that terrorists rely on to inflict constant pain and suffering. Typ- ical grief reactions are postponed indefinitely, making it essentially impossible for people to move on with their lives, sometimes for generations (Boss, 2015). What motivates these families and friends is hope for an eventual return of the loved one (Boss, 2015; Wayland et al., 2016).

A second type of ambiguous loss involves a loved one who is psychologically absent but who is still physically present. They are what people term “here but gone,” as is the case in certain diseases such as dementia. Families caring for loved ones with Alzheimer’s dis- ease, for instance, report going through grieving as their loved one loses more and more of what makes them who they are, ultimately coming to a point at which they think of their loved one as “dead” even though they are physically alive (Boss, 2010).

The common aspect of both types of ambiguous loss is that as long as certainty is not reached, closure is not possible in the usual sense. Families and friends report feeling pressure to stop holding out hope, on one hand, or are accused of being cold on the other. Either way, ambiguous grief is especially difficult to deal with.

Complicated or Prolonged Grief Disorder Not everyone is able to cope with grief well and begin rebuilding a life. Sometimes the feel- ings of hurt, loneliness, and guilt are so overwhelming they become the focus of the survi- vor’s life to such an extent there is never any closure and the grief continues to interfere indefinitely with one’s ability to function. When this occurs, individuals are viewed as hav- ing complicated grief, which is characterized by persistent and intrusive feelings of grief lasting beyond the expected period of adaptation to loss, and is associated with separation distress and traumatic distress (Arizmendi, Kaszniak, & O’Connor, 2016). Symptoms of separation distress include preoccupation with the deceased to the point it interferes with everyday functioning, upsetting memories of the deceased, longing and searching for the deceased, and isolation following the loss. Symptoms of traumatic distress include feeling disbelief about the death, mistrust, anger, and detachment from others as a result of the death, feeling shocked by the death, and the experience of physical presence of the deceased.

Complicated grief forms a separate set of symptoms from depression (Stroebe, Abakoumkin, & Stroebe, 2010). Individuals experiencing complicated grief report high levels of separation distress (such as yearning, pining, or longing for the deceased per- son), along with specific cognitive, emotional, or behavioral indicators (such as avoiding reminders of the deceased, diminished sense of self, difficulty in accepting the loss, feeling bitter or angry), as well as increased morbidity, increased smoking and substance abuse, and difficulties with family and other social relationships. Similar distinctions have been made between complicated or prolonged grief disorder and anxiety disorders.

Arizmendi and colleagues (2016) report that people who experience complicated grief process grief emotions differently. Specifically, they do not engage those areas of the brain that are typically involved in regulating emotions, supporting the idea that complicated grief involves avoidance and a disruption of emotion regulation.

The presence of complicated grief transcends culture. For example, Li & Prigerson (2016) validated a measure of complicated grief in a sample of Chinese adults. Likewise, complicated grief was validated in a sample of Spanish adults in terms of how they respond to emotion (Fernández-Alcántara et al., 2016).

Some researchers believe that complicated grief may be more likely with certain types of loss (such as the death of a child). In addition, some also speculate that whether a per- son has a supportive social network may make a difference. However, too little research has been conducted for clear conclusions to be drawn.

The Spotlight on Research feature explores grief work regarding a common but under-researched topic: the degree to which partners influence each other’s grieving pro- cess (Stroebe, Finkenauer et al., 2013). As you read it, note how grief, which is typically thought of in an individual context, is experienced in a social context.

complicated grief or prolonged grief disorder Expression of grief that is distinguished from depression and from normal grief in terms of separation distress and traumatic distress.

separation distress Expression of complicated or prolonged grief disorder that includes being preoc- cupied with the deceased to the point that it interferes with everyday functioning, having upsetting memories of the deceased, longing and searching for the deceased, and feeling isolated following the loss.

traumatic distress Expression of complicated or prolonged grief disorder that includes disbelief about the death; mistrusting others, feeling anger, and being detachment from others as a result of the death; feeling shocked by the death; and experiencing the physical pres- ence of the deceased.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

568 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Disenfranchised Grief As we have noted repeatedly throughout this chapter, the experience of loss and the sub- sequent grief we feel is a highly personal matter. Sometimes, a loss that appears insignifi- cant to others is highly consequential to the person who suffers the loss; such situations give rise to disenfranchised grief. A good example of disenfranchised grief is the loss of a pet. To most of the world, the loss is not a big deal—an animal has died. But to the person whose pet died, the loss may be very traumatic and result in social isolation in grief.

Disenfranchised grief stems from the social expectations we place on people to “move on” after loss (Harris, 2016). However, those expectations can result in failure to under- stand the personal impact that every loss has on someone and a failure to be empathetic to that person’s experience. Such failure may also reflect certain stereotypes or bias regarding the value of various people’s lives, such as drug addicts or very old people. In this con- text, it is good to keep in mind John Donne’s passage from his Devotions upon Emergent Occasions, written in December 1623 as he recovered from a very serious illness:

No man is an island, Entire of itself, Every man is a piece of the continent, A part of the main. If a clod be washed away by the sea, Europe is the less. As well as if a promontory were. As well as if a manor of thy friend's Or of thine own were:

disenfranchised grief A loss that appears insignificant to others is highly consequential to the person who suffers the loss.

On ResearchSpotlight

The Costs of Holding in Grief for the Sake of One’s Partner

Who were the investigators, and what was the aim of the study? When par- ents experience the loss of a child, they must cope with two main things: how to deal with the worst thing imaginable with the death of their child and how to deal with each other. Surprisingly, little research has been done to examine how partners work through the grieving process alone and together. Stroebe and colleagues wanted to know how each partner regulates or tries to protect the other from the pain of the loss and from his or her own grief.

How did the investigators measure the topic of interest? The researchers admin- istered the Dutch version of the Inventory of Complicated Grief, items that measured Partner-Oriented Self-Regulation (POSR, such as “I stay strong for my partner”) and items that assessed expressions of concern for one’s partner.

Who were the participants in the study? Participants were 463 Dutch couples who had lost a child and were invited to partici- pate. Parents who also were grandparents and single parents were excluded. A total of 219 couples agreed to participate. Their age range was 26–68 years, the causes of death of the children were varied, and roughly two- thirds of the children were males.

What was the design of the study? The study used a longitudinal design with three times of measurement (6, 13, and 20 months after the death of the child).

Were there ethical concerns in the study? There were no ethical concerns, as partici- pants were carefully screened, were provided detailed information about the nature of the project, and were given the opportunity to stop their participation at any time.

What were the results? Several findings are noteworthy. First, grief lessened over time for both partners, with a recognition that grief was always greater for women than for men. Higher levels of POSR mattered;

individuals whose partner reported more POSR experienced more grief themselves, as did their partner. These relations held over time. Expressions of concern for the partner showed the opposite—more expressions of concern were related to lower levels of grief in the partner being targeted by those expressions.

What did the investigators conclude? The researchers argued that behaviors intended to show that one is strong for one’s partner are actually unhelpful in low- ering the experience of grief. On the other hand, one partner who expresses grief and concern about his or her partner can be helpful in the couples’ and the individuals’ coping with grief.

What converging evidence would strengthen these conclusions? Because the study included only Dutch couples, more diverse samples would be beneficial. Also, more analyses on the cause of death and the age of the child at death may provide addi- tional insightful information.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 569

Any man's death diminishes me, Because I am involved in mankind, And therefore never send to know for whom the bell tolls;

It tolls for thee.

Think About It How would you use the biopsychosocial model to create a support group for bereaved people?

recall 1. Feeling sad on the date when your grandmother died the

previous year is an example of a(n) _______________.

2. Compared with other age groups,_______________ show the most negative effects following bereavement.

3. Separation distress and _______________ are two characteristics of complicated or prolonged grief disorder.

Interpret ­● What connections might there be between bereavement and

stress?

apply ­● If you were to create a brochure listing the five most impor-

tant things to do and not to do in reacting to someone who just lost a close family member or friend through death, what would you include? Why?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 16.4

Donna and Carl have a 6-year-old daughter, Jennie, whose grandmother just died. Jennie and her grandmother were very close, as the two saw each other almost every day. Other adults have told her parents not to take Jennie to the funeral. Donna and Carl aren’t sure what to do. They wonder whether Jennie will understand what happened to her grand- mother, and they worry about how she will react.

Coming to grips with the reality of death may be one of the hardest things we have to do in life. American society does not help much either, as it tends to distance itself from death through euphemisms, such as “passed away” or “dearly departed,” and by eliminating many rituals from the home (for example, viewings no longer take place there, no more official mourning visits to the bereaved’s house).

These trends make it difficult for people such as Donna, Carl, and Jennie to learn about death in its natural context. Dying itself has been moved from the home to hospitals and other institutions such as nursing homes. The closest most people get to death is a quick glance inside a nicely lined casket at a corpse that has been made to look as if the person were still alive.

What do people, especially children such as Jennie, understand about death? How do Donna and Carl feel? How do the friends of Jennie’s grandmother feel? In this section, we consider how our understanding of death changes throughout the life span.

L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S ­● What do children understand about death? How should

adults help them deal with it?

­● How do adolescents deal with death?

­● How do adults deal with death? What special issues do they face concerning the death of a child or parent?

­● How do older adults face the loss of a child, grandchild, or partner?

16.5 Dying and Bereavement Experiences Across the Life Span

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

570 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Childhood Parents often take their children to funerals of relatives and close friends. But many adults, such as Donna and Carl in the vignette, wonder whether young children really know what death means. Children’s understanding of death must be understood in terms of their cultural background and the fact that it changes with their development ( Rosengren, Gutiérrez, & Schein, 2014a). Preschoolers tend to believe that death is temporary and magical, something dramatic that comes to get you in the middle of the night like a burglar or a ghost. Not until children are 5 to 7 years of age do they realize that death is perma- nent, that it eventually happens to everyone, and that dead people no longer have any biological functions.

Why does this shift occur? There are three major areas of developmental change in children that affect their understanding of death and grief (Rosengren et al., 2014a; Webb, 2010a): cognitive-language ability, emotional/psychosocial development, and coping skills. In terms of cognitive-language ability, think back to Chapters 4 and 6, especially to the discussion of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. Consider Jennie, the 6-year-old daughter of Donna and Carl in the vignette. Where would she be in Piaget’s terms? In this perspective, the ages 5 to 7 include the transition from preoperational to concrete- operational thinking. Concrete-operational thinking permits children to know that death is final and permanent. Therefore, Jennie is likely to understand what happened to her grandmother. With this more mature understanding of death comes a lower fear of death, too (Gutiérrez et al., 2014; Rosengren et al., 2014b).

Children’s expressions of grief at the loss of a loved one vary with age, too (Halliwell & Franken, 2016; Webb, 2010b). Several common manifestations of grief among children are shown in ◗ ­Figure 16.7. Typical reactions in early childhood include regression, guilt for causing the death, denial, displacement, repression, and wishful thinking that the deceased will return. In later childhood, common behaviors include problems at school, anger, and physical ailments. As children mature, they acquire more coping skills that permit a shift to problem-focused coping, which provides a better sense of personal control. Children will often flip between grief and normal activity, a pattern they may learn from adults (Stroebe et al., 2005), and may be more likely to disclose their true feelings online ( Halliwell & Franken, 2016). Sensitivity to these feelings and how they get expressed is essential so that the child can understand what happened and that he or she did not cause the death.

Research shows that bereavement per se during childhood typically does not have long-lasting effects such as depression (Miller, Rosengren, & Gutiérrez, 2014; Webb, 2010b). Problems are more likely to occur if the child does not receive adequate care and attention following the death.

Understanding death can be particularly difficult for children when adults are not open and honest with them, especially about the meaning of death (Miller & Rosengren, 2014). Most adults believe that it is best to shield their children, especially younger chil- dren, from death. The use of euphemisms such as “Grandma has gone away” and “Mommy is only sleeping” reflects this belief, but is unwise. First, young children do not understand the deeper level of meaning in such statements and are likely to take them literally. Second, and more important, adults cannot fully shield children from the reality of death.

When explaining death to children, it is best to deal with them on their terms. Keep explanations simple, at a level they can understand. Try to allay their fears and reassure them that whatever reaction they have is okay. Providing loving support for the child will maximize the potential for a successful (albeit painful) introduction to one of life’s reali- ties. One male college student recalled how, when he was 9, his father helped him deal with his feelings after his grandfather’s death:

The day of my grandfather’s death my dad came over to my aunt and uncle’s house where my brother and I were staying. He took us into one of the bedrooms and sat us down. He told us Granddaddy Doc had died. He explained to us that it was okay if we needed to cry. He told us that he had cried, and that if we did cry we wouldn’t be babies, but would just be men showing our emotions. (Dickinson, 1992, pp. 175–176)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 571

It is important for children to know that it is okay for them to feel sad, to cry, or to show their feelings in whatever way they want. Reassuring children that it’s okay to feel this way helps them deal with their confusion at some adults’ explanations of death. Young adults remember feeling uncomfortable as children around dead bodies, often fearing that the deceased person would come after them. Still, researchers believe it is very impor- tant for children to attend the funeral of a relative or to have a private viewing (Miller & Rosengren, 2014; Webb, 2010b). They will process the experience in age-appropriate ways, especially if they have understanding adults available to answer questions.

Adolescence Adolescents have more personal experience with death and grief than many people realize (Balk, 2014). Surveys of college students indicate that between 40 and 70% of traditional- aged college students will experience the death of someone close to them during their col- lege years, such as a parent, sibling, or friend.

­◗ Figure 16.7 Children show their grief in many ways, including physiological (somatic), emotional (intrapsychic), and behavioral.

D is

ab lin

g gr

ief

Disabling grief

Normal grief Disabling grief

Somatic Sleep di�culties Refusing to eat Bedwetting Headaches Stomach aches (Serious illness)

Intrapsychic Emotional distress Separation anxiety Fear that others will also die Death fantasies Learning di�culties Guilt (School phobia) (Severe depression)

Behavioral Regression Explosive emotions Acting out Temper tantrums Extreme shyness Disinterest in play Overdependence Demand for attention (Delinquent activity)

© 2

01 9

Ce ng

ag e

From Oltjenbruns, K. A. (2001). Developmental context of childhood: Grief and regrief phenomena. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut, (Eds.), Handbook of Bereavement Research: Consequences, Coping, and Care (Fig. 8-1, p. 177). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Copyright © 2001 by the American Psychological Association.

Think About It How could parents use knowledge of child development to help children understand death?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

572 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Adolescence is a time of personal and physical change, when one is trying to develop a theory of self. When teenagers experience the death of someone close to them, they may have considerable trouble making sense of the event, especially if this is their first experi- ence (Oltjenbruns & Balk, 2007). The effects of bereavement in adolescence can be quite severe, especially when the death was unexpected, and can be expressed in many ways, such as chronic illness, enduring guilt, low self-esteem, poorer performance in school and on the job, substance abuse, problems in interpersonal relationships, and suicidal thinking (Malberg, 2014; Morgan & Roberts, 2010).

An important aspect of adolescent development is wanting to be perceived as part of the peer group. As a result, younger adolescents are particularly reluctant to discuss their grief (for instance, over the loss of a sibling), mainly because they do not want to appear different from their peers (Balk, 2014). This reluctance leaves them particularly vulnerable to psychosomatic symptoms such as headaches and stomach pains that signal underlying problems.

Adolescents often do not demonstrate a clear end point to their grief over the loss of a sibling or parent; they continue to miss and to love their dead loved ones and try to find ways to keep them in their lives (Barrera et al., 2013; Collins-Colosi, 2016). However, few nonbereaved peers were willing to talk with the bereaved students about their experience or even felt comfortable being with them (Balk, 2014).

However, grief does not interfere with normative developmental processes. Bereaved adolescent siblings experience continued personal growth following the death of a loved one in much the same way as adolescents who did not experience such a loss (Balk, 2014; Malberg, 2014).

Adulthood Because young adults are just beginning to pursue the family, career, and personal goals they have set, they tend to be more intense in their feelings toward death. When asked how they feel about death, young adults report a strong sense those who die at this point in their lives would be cheated out of their future (Attig, 1996). Complicated grief and mental health problems are relatively common (Bolton et al., 2016; Mash, Fullerton, & Ursano, 2013).

Wrenn (1999) relates one of the challenges faced by bereaved college students is learn- ing “how to respond to people who ignore their grief, or who tell them they need to get on with life and it’s not good for them to continue to grieve” (p. 134). College students have a need to express their grief like other bereaved people do, so providing them the oppor- tunity to do so is crucial (Fajgenbaum, Chesson, & Lanzl, 2012; McCoyd & Walter, 2016).

Experiencing the loss of one’s partner in young adulthood can be traumatic, not only because of the loss itself but also because such loss is unexpected. As Trish Straine, a 32-year-old widow whose husband was killed in the World Trade Center attack put it: “I suddenly thought, ‘I’m a widow.’ Then I said to myself, ‘A widow? That’s an older woman, who’s dressed in black. It’s certainly not a 32-year-old like me’” (Lieber, 2001). One of the most difficult aspects for young widows and widowers is they must deal with both their own and their young children’s grief and provide the support their children need, and that can be extremely difficult. “Every time I look at my children, I’m reminded of Mark,” said Stacey, a 35-year-old widow whose husband died of bone cancer. “And people don’t want to hear you say you don’t feel like moving on, even though there is great pressure from them to do that.”

Becoming a widow as a young adult can be especially traumatic.

M on

as he

e Fr

an tz

/O JO

Im ag

es /G

et ty

Im ag

es

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 573

The death of a girlfriend or boyfriend in young adulthood is often unacknowledged as bereavement. Leichtentritt and colleagues (2013) found that girlfriends of soldiers killed in battle felt socially isolated and lonely in dealing with their loss. Clearly, this is a missed opportunity to be sensitive to people’s loss and to provide support.

Death of One’s Child The death of one’s child, for most parents, brings unimaginable grief (McCoyd & Walter, 2016; Stroebe et al., 2013). Because children are not supposed to die before their par- ents, it is as if the natural order of things has been violated, shaking parents to their core (Rubin  & Malkinson, 2001). Mourning and relationship stress are always intense; some parents never recover or reconcile themselves to the death of their child and may ter- minate their relationship with each other, while others find solace and strength in each other ( Albuquerque, Pereira, & Narciso, 2016). The intensity of feelings is due to the strong parent–child bond that begins before birth and lasts a lifetime (Maple et al., 2010; Rosenbaum, Smith, & Zollfrank, 2011).

Young parents who lose a child unexpectedly report high anxiety, a more negative view of the world, and much guilt, that results in a devastating experience (Seyda & Fitzsimons, 2010). The most overlooked losses of a child are those that happen through stillbirth, mis- carriage, abortion, or neonatal death (Earle, Komaromy, & Layne, 2012; McCoyd & Walter, 2016). Attachment to the child begins before birth, especially for mothers, so the loss hurts deeply. For this reason, ritual is extremely important to acknowledge the death and vali- date parents’ feelings of grief (Nuzum et al., 2015).

Yet parents who experience this type of loss are expected to recover quickly. The lived experience of parents tells a different story (Seyda & Fitzsimons, 2010). These par- ents talk about a life-changing event and report a deep sense of loss and hurt, especially when others do not understand their feelings. Worst of all, if societal expectations for quick recovery are not met, the parents may be subjected to unfeeling comments. As one mother notes, parents often just wish somebody would acknowledge the loss (Okonski, 1996).

The loss of a young adult child for a middle-aged parent is experienced differently but is equally devastating (Maple et al., 2013; Schneider, 2013). Complicated grief is more common among bereaved parents than in other groups, most likely due to the very differ- ent nature of the parent–child relationship (Zetumer et al., 2015).

Death of One’s Parent Most parents die after their children are grown. But whenever parental death occurs, it hurts. Losing a parent in adulthood is a rite of passage as one is transformed from being a “son” or “daughter” to being “without parents” (Abrams, 2013; McCoyd & Walter, 2016). We, the children, are now next in line.

The loss of a parent is significant. For young adult women transitioning to mother- hood, losing their own mother during adolescence raises many feelings, such as deep loss at not being able to share their pregnancies with their mothers and fear of dying young themselves (Franceschi, 2005). Death of one’s parent in adulthood has understandable effects on adult children, largely depending on the quality of the relationship with the deceased parent (Stokes, 2016).

The feelings accompanying the loss of an older parent reflect a sense of letting go, loss of a buffer against death, better acceptance of one’s own eventual death, and a sense of relief the parent’s suffering is over (Abrams, 2013; Igarashi et al., 2013; McCoyd & Walter, 2016). Yet, if the parent died from a cause such as Alzheimer’s disease, which involves the loss of the parent–child relationship along the way, then bodily death can feel like the second time the parent died (Shaw, 2007). Whether the adult child now tries to separate from the deceased parent’s expectations or finds comfort in the memories, the impact of the loss is great.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

574 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

Late Adulthood In general, older adults are less anxious about death and more accepting of it than any other age group (Kastenbaum, 1999). They may believe that their most important life tasks have been completed. However, that is not to say that older adults are unaffected by loss. Far from it.

Death of One’s Child or Grandchild in Late Life The loss of a child can happen at any point over the adult life span. Older bereaved parents tend to reevaluate their grief as experienced shortly after the loss and years and decades later. Even more than 30 years after the death of a child, older adults still feel a keen sense of loss and have continued difficulty coming to terms with it (Malkinson & Bar-Tur, 2004– 2005). The long-lasting effects of the loss of a child are often accompanied by a sense of guilt that the pain affected the parents’ relationships with the surviving children. Loss of a child in young adulthood may also result in lower cognitive functioning in late life (Greene et al., 2014).

The loss of a grandchild results in similar feelings: intense emotional upset, survivor guilt, regrets about the relationship with the deceased grandchild, and a need to restruc- ture relationships with the surviving family. However, bereaved grandparents tend to con- trol and hide their grief behavior in an attempt to shield their child (the bereaved parent) from the level of pain being felt. In cases in which older adults were the primary caregivers for grandchildren, feelings can be especially difficult. For example, custodial grandparents in South Africa whose grandchildren in their care died from AIDS go through emotionally difficult times due to the loss and to the social stigma regarding the disease (Boon et al., 2010).

Death of One’s Partner Experiencing the loss of one’s partner is the type of loss in late life we know most about. The death of a partner differs from other losses. It clearly represents a deep personal loss, especially when the couple has had a long and close relationship (Lee, 2014). In a very real way, when one’s partner dies, a part of oneself dies, too.

There is pressure from society to mourn the loss of one’s partner for a period of time and then to “move on” (Jenkins, 2003). Typically, this pressure is manifested if the survi- vor begins to show interest in finding another partner before an “acceptable” period of mourning has passed. Although Americans no longer specify the length of the mourning period, many believe that about a year is appropriate. The fact that such pressure and neg-

ative commentary usually do not accompany other losses is another indication of the seriousness with which most people take the death of a partner.

Older bereaved spouses may grieve a great deal for a long time (Lee, 2014); research has shown that grief can sometimes last for years (Naef et al., 2013). Given that, you might wonder whether having a supportive social network can help people cope. Research findings on this topic are mixed, however. Some studies find that social support plays a significant role in the outcome of the grieving process. For example, some data show that during the first two years after the death of a partner, the quality of the support system—rather than simply the number of friends—is especially important for the grieving partner. Survivors who have confidants are better off than survi- vors who  have many acquaintances (Hansson & Stroebe, 2007). In contrast, other studies find that

The loss of a spouse or partner can be especially traumatic after a long relationship.

PH O

VO IR

/A la

m y

St oc

k Ph

ot o

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 575

having a supportive social network plays little role in helping people cope ( DiGiacomo et al., 2013). Issues may include whether there is a complex relationship involving the bereaved person, whether he or she wants to have contact with others, who in the social network is willing to provide support, and whether that support is of high quality.

When one’s partner dies, how he or she felt about the relationship can play a role in coping with bereavement. For instance, widowed older adults who felt worsening regret about unfinished aspects of their marriages over time had the hardest time dealing with grief (Holland et al., 2014).

Several studies of widows document a tendency for some older widows to “sanctify” their husbands (Lopata, 1996). Sanctification involves describing a deceased spouse in idealized terms, and it serves several functions: validating that the widow had a strong marriage, is a good and worthy person, and is capable of rebuilding her life. European American women who view being a wife as above all other roles a woman can perform are somewhat more likely to sanctify their husbands (Lopata, 1996). In fact, the higher the quality of the relationship, the more bereaved spouses yearn for their lost spouse (Stroebe, Abakoumkin, & Stroebe, 2010).

Older bereaved spouses who can talk about their feelings concerning their loss exhibit reduced levels of depression and functional impairment (Pfoff, Zarotney, & Monk, 2014). Cognitive-behavioral therapy is one especially effective intervention to help bereaved peo- ple make sense of the loss and deal with their other feelings and thoughts (Lichtenthal & Sweeney, 2014). A key to this process is helping people make meaning from the death (Neimeyer & Wogrin, 2008).

Gay and lesbian couples may experience other feelings and reactions in addition to typical feelings of grief (Clarke et al., 2010). For example, a partner may feel disenfran- chised by family members of the deceased at the funeral, making it hard for the partner to bring closure to the relationship (McNutt & Yakushko, 2013). For gay partners who were also caregivers, the loss affects one’s sense of identity in much the same way as the death of a spouse, and making sense of the death becomes the primary issue (Cadell & Marshall, 2007). Lesbian widows report similar feelings (Bent & Magilvy, 2006). As same-sex mar- riage becomes more accepted, it will be important for researchers to document the experi- ence of same-sex widows and widowers.

Conclusion Death is not as pleasant a topic as children’s play or occupational development. It’s not something we can go to college to master. What it represents to many people is the end of their existence, and that is a scary prospect. But because we all share in this fear at some level, each of us is equipped to provide support and comfort for grieving survivors.

Death is the last life-cycle force we encounter, the ultimate triumph of the biological forces that limit the length of life. Yet the same psychological and social forces so influen- tial throughout life help us deal with death, either our own or someone else’s. As we come to the end of our life journey, we understand death through an interaction of psychological forces—such as coping skills and intellectual and emotional understanding of death—and the sociocultural forces expressed in a particular society’s traditions and rituals.

Learning about and dealing with death is clearly a developmental process across the life span that fits well in the biopsychosocial framework. Most apparent is that biological forces are essential to understanding death. The definition of death is based on whether certain biological functions are present; these same definitions create numerous ethical dilemmas that must be dealt with psychologically and socioculturally. Life-cycle forces also play a key role. We have seen, depending on a person’s age, the concept of death has varied meanings beyond the mere cessation of life.

How a person’s understanding of death develops is also the result of psychological forces. As the ability to think and reflect undergoes fundamental change, the view of death changes from a mostly magical approach to one that can be transcendent and transform- ing. As we have seen, people who face their own imminent death experience certain feel- ings. Having gained experience through the deaths of friends and relatives, a person’s level

Think About It Why do many older adults continue to “talk” with their deceased partners?

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

576 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

SUMMARY

of comfort with his or her own death may increase. Such personal experience may also come about by sharing the rituals defined through sociocultural forces. People observe how others deal with death and how the culture sets the tone and prescribes behavior for survivors. The combined action of forces also determines how they cope with the grief that accompanies the loss of someone close. Psychologically, confronting grief depends on many things, including the quality of the support system we have.

Thus, just as the beginning of life represents a complex interaction of biological, psy- chological, sociocultural, and life-cycle factors, so does death. What people believe about what follows after death is also an interaction of these factors. So, as we bring our study of human development to a close, we end where we began: What we experience in our lives cannot be understood from only a single perspective.

recall 1. In general, adults should be _______________ when

discussing death with children.

2. Compared with other age groups, _______________ are especially reluctant to talk about their grief experiences.

3. The most devastating type of loss for an adult is the loss of a(n) _______________.

4. _______________ involves describing a deceased spouse in idealized terms.

Interpret ­● What similarities and differences would you expect to find

between the survivors in heterosexual marriages and in gay or lesbian relationships when the spouse or partner dies?

apply ­● How do the different ways that adults view death relate to the

stages of Erikson’s theory discussed in Chapters 10, 13, and 15?

Check your answers to the Recall Questions at the end of the chapter.

TEST YOURSELF 16.5

13.1 Definitions and Ethical Issues

how is death defined?

­● Death is a difficult concept to define precisely. Different cultures have different meanings and rituals surrounding death.

What legal and medical criteria are used to determine when death occurs?

­● For many centuries, a clinical definition of death was used: the absence of a heartbeat and respiration. Currently, whole-brain death is the most widely used definition. It is based on several highly specific criteria that are assessed by trained healthcare professionals. The condition of persis- tent vegetative state often creates a complicated situation for determination of death.

What are the ethical dilemmas surrounding euthanasia?

­● Two types of euthanasia are distinguished. Active euthanasia consists of deliberately ending someone’s life, such as turning off a life-support system. Pas- sive euthanasia is ending someone’s life by withhold- ing some type of intervention or treatment (e.g., by

stopping nutrition). Physician-assisted suicide is a controversial issue and is being addressed in some states through laws. It is essential people make their wishes known through either a healthcare power of attorney or a living will.

­● The personal and financial costs of prolonging life when the patient would have preferred another option are significant, as are the ethical issues regarding prolonging life without considering quality of life.

13.2 Thinking About Death: Personal Aspects

how do feelings about death change over adulthood?

­● Cognitive developmental level is important for understand- ing how young adults view death and the lessening of feel- ings of immortality.

­● Middle-aged adults usually experience the death of their parents and begin to confront their own mortality, under- going a change in their sense of time lived and time until death.

­● Older adults are les anxious about and more accepting of death.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

C h a p t E r 1 6 : D Y I N G A N D B E R E Av E M E N T 577

how do people deal with their own death?

­● Kübler-Ross’s approach includes five stages: denial, anger, bargaining, depression, and acceptance. People may be in more than one stage at a time and do not necessarily go through them in order.

­● A contextual theory of dying emphasizes the tasks a dying person must face. Four dimensions of these tasks have been identified: bodily needs, psychological security, interper- sonal attachments, and spiritual energy and hope. A con- textual theory incorporates differences in reasons people die and the places people die.

What is death anxiety, and how do people show it?

­● Most people exhibit some degree of anxiety about death, even though it is difficult to define and measure. Terror management theory is a common frame for understanding death anxiety. Young adults show greater brain activity when exposed to death-related concepts than do older adults.

­● The main ways death anxiety is shown are by avoiding death (e.g., refusing to go to funerals) and deliberately chal- lenging it (e.g., engaging in dangerous sports). The many components of death anxiety affect many different aspects of behavior.

­● Death education has been shown to be extremely effective in helping people deal with death anxiety.

13.3 End-of-Life Issues

how do people deal with end-of-life issues and create a final scenario?

­● Managing the final aspects of life, after-death disposition of the body, memorial services, and distribution of assets are important end-of-life issues. Making choices about what people want and do not want done constitute making a final scenario. One of the most difficult parts of a final sce- nario is separation from family and friends. Jointly creating a final scenario among the dying person, family, and health- care professionals is optimal.

What is hospice?

­● The goal of a hospice is to maintain the quality of life and manage the pain of terminally ill patients. Hospice clients typically have cancer, AIDS, or a progressive neurological disorder. Family members tend to stay involved in the care of hospice clients. Pain management can also be achieved in ways other than through hospice.

how does one make one’s end-of-life desires and decisions known?

­● End-of-life decisions are made known most often through a living will, healthcare power of attorney, and specific medical requests such as a do not resuscitate order. It is important family and healthcare professionals are aware of these deci- sions. The Patient Self-Determination Act requires health- care facilities to inform patients of these rights but must take the patient’s competence to make decisions into account.

13.4 Surviving the Loss: The Grieving Process

how do people experience the grief process?

­● Grief is an active process of coping with loss. Four aspects of grieving must be confronted: the reality of the loss, the emotional turmoil, adjusting to the environment, and loos- ening the ties with the deceased. When death is expected, survivors go through anticipatory grief; unexpected death is usually more difficult for people to handle.

­● “Recovery” may be a misleading term, as the process of adjusting and readjusting to life following a loss never ends.

­● The mode of death, personal factors, and extent of social support make a difference in dealing with grief.

­● Anticipatory grief does not appear to make the grieving process any easier. However, the degree of attachment to the person who died does.

What feelings do grieving people have?

­● Dealing with grief, called grief work, usually takes more time than society wants to allot. Grief is equally intense for both expected and unexpected death. Normal grief reac- tions include sorrow, sadness, denial, disbelief, guilt, and anniversary reactions.

­● How people demonstrate grief varies across cultures.

how do people cope with grief?

­● Three general approaches help explain how people cope with grief.

­● The four-component model is based on the processes of: the context of the loss; continuation of subjective meaning of the loss; changing representations of the lost relationship over time; and the role of coping and emotion regulation processes. However, the grief work as rumination hypoth- esis rejects the need for grief as a basis for recovering form loss.

­● The dual process model integrates existing ideas about stress into the context of loss and restoration. These stress- ors operate in a dynamic relation.

What are the types of ambiguous loss?

­● Ambiguous loss refers to situations of loss in which there is no resolution or closure. There are two types of ambiguous loss. The first type refers to a missing person who is physi- cally absent but still very present psychologically to family and friends. The second type refers to situations in which the loved one is psychologically absent but still physically present.

What is the difference between normal and complicated grief?

­● Complicated grief involves symptoms of separation distress and traumatic distress. Separation distress is preoccupation with the deceased to the extent that it interferes with every- day functioning. Excessive guilt and self-blame are common manifestations of traumatic grief.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

578 pa rt F O U r : L AT E A D u LT H O O D

What is disenfranchised grief?

­● Disenfranchised grief, in which a loss is downplayed by oth- ers in a bereaved person’s social network, is an important consideration in understanding grief.

16.5 Dying and Bereavement Experiences Across the Life Span

What do children understand about death? how should adults help them deal with it?

­● The cognitive and psychosocial developmental levels of children determine their understanding of and ability to cope with death. This is especially evident in the behaviors children use to display their grief.

­● Research indicates that there are few long-lasting effects of bereavement in childhood.

how do adolescents deal with death?

­● Adolescents may have difficulty making sense of death and are often severely affected by bereavement. Adolescents may be reluctant to discuss their feelings of loss, and peers often provide little support.

how do adults deal with death? What special issues do they face concerning the death of a child or parent?

­● Young and middle-aged adults usually have intense feelings about death. Attachment theory provides a useful frame- work for understanding these feelings.

­● Midlife is a time when people usually deal with the death of their parents and confront their own mortality.

­● The death of one’s child is especially difficult to cope with. ­● The death of one’s parent deprives an adult of many impor-

tant things, and the feelings accompanying it are often complex.

how do older adults face the loss of a child, grandchild, or partner?

­● Older adults are usually less anxious about death and deal with it better than any other age group.

­● The death of a grandchild can be traumatic for older adults, and the feelings of loss may never go away.

­● The death of one’s partner represents a deep personal loss, especially when the couple had a long and close relation- ship. The history of the relationship influences the grief process.

Test Yourself: Recall Answers 16.1 1. sociocultural 2. the brainstem still functions in a persistent vegetative state 3. passive euthanasia 16.2 1. Middle-aged adults 2. contextual theory of dying 3. terror management theory 16.3 1. final scenario 2. Hospice 3. living will 16.4 1. anniversary reaction 2. middle-aged adults 3. traumatic distress 16.5 1. honest 2. adolescents 3. child 4. Sanctification

Key Terms thanatology (542) clinical death (544) whole-brain death (544) persistent vegetative state (544) bioethics (545) euthanasia (545) active euthanasia (545) passive euthanasia (545) physician-assisted suicide (546) death anxiety (551) terror management theory (552) end-of-life issues (554)

final scenario (555) hospice (556) palliative care (556) living will (558) healthcare power of attorney (558) do not resuscitate (DNR) order (558) bereavement (561) grief (561) mourning (561) anticipatory grief (562) grief work (563) anniversary reaction (564)

four-component model (564) grief work as rumination hypothesis (564) dual process model (DPM) (565) model of adaptive grieving dynamics

(MAGD) (566) ambiguous loss (566) complicated or prolonged grief

disorder (567) separation distress (567) traumatic distress (567) disenfranchised grief (568)

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

580 G l o s s a r y

Note: Many familiar words have specialized meanings and usage in psychology. Several of these, used in the text, are defined here. When a glossary term is used to define another glos- sary term, it appears in bold.

abusive relationship relationships in which one person becomes aggressive toward the partner.

accommodation according to Piaget, changing existing knowledge based on new knowledge.

active euthanasia deliberate ending of some- one’s life.

active life expectancy Number of years that a person is free from debilitating chronic disease and impairment.

activities of daily living (ADLs) basic self-care tasks such as eating, bathing, toileting, walk- ing, and dressing.

adaptation level When press level is average for a particular level of competence.

adolescent egocentrism Self-absorption that is characteristic of teenagers as they search for identity.

adole scent-limite d anti so ci al b ehav ior behavior of youth who engage in relatively minor criminal acts but aren’t consistently antisocial.

aerobic exercise exercise that places moderate stress on the heart by maintaining a pulse rate between 60% and 90% of a person’s maximum heart rate.

age discrimination involves denying a job or promotion to someone solely on the basis of age.

age of viability age at which a fetus can survive outside the womb because most of its bodily systems function adequately; typically at seven months after conception.

age-related macular degeneration Progres- sive and irreversible destruction of receptors in the retina from any of a number of causes.

agreeableness dimension of personality asso- ciated with being accepting, willing to work with others, and caring.

alcohol use disorder drinking pattern that results in significant and recurrent conse- quences that reflect loss of reliable control over alcohol use.

alert inactivity State in which a baby is calm  with eyes open and attentive; the baby seems to be deliberately inspecting the environment.

alienation When workers believe that what they  are doing is meaningless and that their efforts are devalued or when they do not see the connection between what they do and the final product.

alleles variations of genes.

altruism prosocial behavior such as helping and sharing in which the individual does not benefit directly from his or her behavior.

Alzheimer ’s disease disease marked by gradual declines in memory, attention, and judgment; confusion as to time and place; dif- ficulties in communicating; decline in self-care skills; inappropriate behavior; and personality changes.

ambiguous loss Situations of loss in which there is no resolution or closure.

amniocentesis Prenatal diagnostic technique that uses a syringe to withdraw a sample of amniotic fluid through the mother’s abdomen.

amnion inner sac in which the developing child rests.

amniotic fluid fluid that surrounds the fetus. analytic ability in Sternberg’s theory of intel-

ligence, the ability to analyze problems and generate different solutions.

animism Phenomenon of crediting inanimate objects with life and lifelike properties such as feelings.

anniversary reaction changes in behavior related to feelings of sadness on the anniver- sary date of a loss.

anorexia nervosa Persistent refusal to eat accompanied by an irrational fear of being overweight.

anticipatory grief grief that is experienced during the period before an expected death occurs that supposedly serves to buffer the impact of the loss when it does come and to facilitate recovery.

anxiety disorders Problems such as feelings of severe anxiety, phobias, and obsessive- compulsive behaviors.

assimilation according to Piaget, taking in information that is compatible with what one already knows.

assisted living facilities Supportive living arrangement for people who need assistance with ADLs or IADLs but who are not so impaired physically or cognitively that they need 24-hour care.

assortative mating theory stating that people find partners based on their similarity to each other.

attachment enduring socioemotional relation- ship between infants and their caregivers.

attention Processes that determine which information will be processed further by an individual.

authoritarian parenting Style of parenting in which parents show high levels of control and low levels of warmth toward their children.

authoritative parenting Style of parenting in which parents use a moderate amount of control and are warm and responsive to their children.

autobiographical memory Memory for events that occur during one’s life.

autosomes first 22 pairs of chromosomes. average children as applied to children’s popu-

larity, children who are liked and disliked by different classmates, but with relatively little intensity.

average longevity (life expectancy) age at which half of the people born in a particular year will have died.

avoidant attachment relationship in which infants turn away from their mothers when they are reunited following a brief separation.

axon tubelike structure that emerges from the cell body and transmits information to other neurons.

babbling Speechlike sounds that consist of vowel–consonant combinations; common at about 6 months.

backup care emergency care for dependent children or adults so that an employee does not need to lose a day of work.

basic cry cry that starts softly and gradu- ally becomes more intense; often heard when babies are hungry or tired.

basic emotions emotions experienced by humankind that consist of three elements: a subjective feeling, a physiological change, and an overt behavior.

battered woman syndrome Situation occur- ring when a woman believes that she cannot leave the abusive situation and may even go so far as to kill her abuser.

behavior therapy type of therapy based on the notion that depressed people experience too few rewards or reinforcements from their environment.

behavioral genetics the branch of genetics that studies the inheritance of behavioral and psychological traits.

bereavement State or condition caused by loss through death.

beta amyloid Protein that is produced in abnor- mally high levels in persons with Alzheimer’s disease.

beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis theory of Alzheimer’s disease in which beta-amyloid deposits create neuritic plaques, that in turn lead to neurofibrillary tangles, that cause neuronal death and Alzheimer’s disease.

binge drinking type of drinking defined for men as consuming five or more drinks in a row and for women as consuming four or more drinks in a row within the past two weeks.

binocular disparity the condition in which the left and right eyes often see slightly differ- ent versions of the same scene.

Glossary

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G l o s s a r y 581

bioethics Study of the interface between human values and technological advances in health and life sciences.

biological forces all genetic and health-related factors that affect development.

biopsychosocial framework a useful way to organize the biological, psychological, and sociocultural forces on human development.

blended family family consisting of a biologi- cal parent, a stepparent, and children.

body mass index (BMI) ratio of body weight and height related to total body fat.

boomerang employees individuals who ter- minate employment at one point in time but return to work in the same organization at a future time.

bulimia nervosa disease in which people— most often adolescent girls—alternate between binge eating—periods when they eat uncontrollably—and purging with laxatives or self-induced vomiting.

burnout depletion of a person’s energy and motivation, the loss of occupational idealism, and the feeling that one is being exploited.

cardinality principle counting principle that states that the last number name denotes the number of objects being counted.

career construction theory theory that pos- its that people build careers through their own actions that result from the interface of their personal characteristics and the social context.

career plateauing Situation that occurs when there is a lack of challenge in one’s job or promotional opportunity in the organiza- tion or when a person decides not to seek advancement.

cataracts opaque spots on the lens of the eye that limit the amount of light transmitted.

cell body center of the neuron that keeps the neuron alive.

centration according to Piaget, narrowly focused type of thought characteristic of pre- operational children.

cephalocaudal principle a principle of physi- cal growth that states that structures nearest the head develop first.

cerebral cortex Wrinkled surface of the brain that regulates many functions that are dis- tinctly human.

cesarean section (C-section) Surgical removal of an infant from the uterus through an inci- sion made in the mother’s abdomen.

chorionic villus sampling (CVS) Prenatal diagnostic technique that involves taking a sample of tissue from the placenta.

chromosomes threadlike structures in the nuclei of cells that contain genetic material.

chronic obstructive pulmonar y disease (COpD) Most common form of incapacitat- ing respiratory disease among older adults.

chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CtE) form of dementia caused by repeated head trauma such as concussions.

circadian rhythm Sleep–wake cycle. classical conditioning a form of learning

that involves pairing a neutral stimulus and

a response originally produced by another stimulus.

climacteric biological process during which women pass from their reproductive to non- reproductive years.

clinic al de ath l ack of he ar tb e at and respiration.

clique Small group of friends who are similar in age, sex, and race.

co-rumination conversations about one’s per- sonal problems, common among adolescent girls.

cognitive self-regulation Skill at identifying goals, selecting effective strategies, and moni- toring accurately; a characteristic of successful students.

cognitive therapy type of therapy based on the idea that maladaptive beliefs or cognitions about oneself are responsible for depression.

cohabitation People in committed, intimate, sexual relationships who live together but are not married.

cohort effects Problem with cross-sectional designs in which differences between age groups (cohorts) may result as much from environmental events as from developmental processes.

collaborative divorce voluntary, contractually based alternative dispute resolution process for couples who want to negotiate a resolu- tion of their situation rather than have a ruling imposed on them by a court or an arbitrator.

competence upper limit of a person’s ability to function in five domains: physical health, sensory-perceptual skills, motor skills, cogni- tive skills, and ego strength.

complicated or prolonged grief disorder expression of grief that is distinguished from depression and from normal grief in terms of separation distress and traumatic distress.

comprehension the process of extracting meaning from a sequence of words.

cones Specialized neurons in the back of the eye that sense color.

conscientiousness dimension of personality in  which people tend to be hard-work- ing, ambitious, energetic, scrupulous, and persevering.

constricting actions interaction in which one partner tries to emerge as the victor by threat- ening or contradicting the other.

continuity–discontinuity issue Whether a particular developmental phenomenon rep- resents a smooth progression throughout the life span (continuity) or a series of abrupt shifts (discontinuity).

controversial children as applied to children’s popularity, children who are intensely liked or disliked by classmates.

conventional level Second level of reasoning in Kohlberg’s theory, where moral reasoning is based on society’s norms.

cooing early vowel-like sounds that babies produce.

cooperative play Play that is organized around a theme, with each child taking on a different role; begins at about 2 years of age.

coping attempt to deal with stressful events. core knowledge hypothesis infants are born

with rudimentary knowledge of the world, which is elaborated based on experiences.

corpus callosum thick bundle of neurons that connects the two hemispheres.

corrective adaptations actions taken in response to stressors that can be facilitated by internal and external resources.

correlation coefficient an expression of the strength and direction of a relation between two variables.

correlational study investigation looking at relations between variables as they exist natu- rally in the world.

counterimitation learning what should not be done by observing the behavior.

covenant marriage expands the marriage con- tract to a lifelong commitment between the partners within a supportive community.

creative ability in Sternberg’s theory of intel- ligence, the ability to deal adaptively with novel situations and problems.

cross-sectional study Study in which devel- opmental differences are identified by testing people of different ages.

crowd large group including many cliques that have similar attitudes and values.

crowning appearance of the top of the baby’s head during labor.

crying State in which a baby cries vigorously, usually accompanied by agitated but uncoor- dinated movement.

crystallization first phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which adolescents use their emerging identities to form ideas about careers.

crystallized intelligence the knowledge you have acquired through life experience and edu- cation in a particular culture.

culture-fair intelligence tests intelligence tests devised using items common to many cultures.

cyberbullying using social media to hurt other people by repeatedly insulting them, excluding them, or spreading rumors about them.

death anxiety feeling of anxiety or even fear of death and dying.

deductive reasoning drawing conclusions from facts; characteristic of formal-operational thought.

dementia family of diseases involving serious impairment of behavioral and cognitive func- tioning and some form of permanent damage to the brain.

demographers People who study population trends.

dendrite end of the neuron that receives infor- mation; it looks like a tree with many branches.

deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) Molecule com- posed of four nucleotide bases that is the bio- chemical basis of heredity.

dependent life expectancy Number of years a person lives with a debilitating chronic disease in which the person depends on others for care.

dependent variable the behavior being observed in an experiment.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

582 G l o s s a r y

depression disorder characterized by perva- sive feelings of sadness, irritability, and low self-esteem.

developmental coach individual who helps a person focus on their goals, motivations, and aspirations to help them achieve focus and apply them appropriately.

diabetes mellitus disease that occurs when the pancreas produces insufficient insulin.

diabetic retinopathy eye disease that is a result of diabetes and can involve fluid retention in the macula, detachment of the retina, hemor- rhage, and aneurysms.

differentiation distinguishing and mastering individual motions.

direct instruction telling a child what to do, when, and why.

disenfranchised grief a loss that appears insignificant to others is highly consequential to the person who suffers the loss.

disorganized (disoriented) attachment rela- tionship in which infants don’t seem to under- stand what’s happening when they are separated and later reunited with their mothers.

divergent thinking thinking in novel and unusual directions.

divided attention the ability of people to per- form more than one task simultaneously.

dizygotic twins the result of two separate eggs fertilized by two sperm; also called fraternal twins.

do not resuscitate (DNR) order Medical order that means that cardiopulmonary resuscitation (cPr) is not started should one’s heart and breathing stop.

docility When people allow their situation to dictate the options they have.

dominance hierarchy ordering of individuals within a group in which group members with lower status defer to those with greater status.

dominant form of an allele whose chemical instructions are followed.

dual process model (DpM) view of coping with bereavement that integrates loss-oriented stressors and restoration-oriented stressors.

dual-energy X-ray absorptiometr y (DXA) test test of bone mineral density (bMd) at the hip and spine.

dynamic systems theory theory that views motor development as involving many distinct skills that are organized and reorganized over time to meet specific needs.

dysphoria feeling sad or down.

ecological theory theory based on idea that human development is inseparable from the environmental contexts in which a person develops.

ectoderm outer layer of the embryo, which becomes the hair, outer layer of skin, and ner- vous system.

edgework the desire to live life more on the edge through physically and emotion- ally threatening situations on the boundary between life and death.

ego resilience Person’s ability to respond adap- tively and resourcefully to new situations.

egocentrism difficulty in seeing the world from another’s point of view; typical of chil- dren in the preoperational period.

elaboration Memor y strateg y in which information is embellished to make it more memorable.

electroencephalography the study of brain waves recorded from electrodes that are placed on the scalp.

embryo term given to the zygote once it is completely embedded in the uterine wall.

emerging adulthood Period between late teens and mid- to late twenties when individ- uals are not adolescents but are not yet fully adults.

emotional intelligence (EI) ability to use one’s own and others’ emotions effectively for solv- ing problems and living happily.

emotion-focused coping dealing with one’s feelings about a stressful event.

empathy act of experiencing another person’s feelings.

enabling actions individuals’ actions and remarks that tend to support others and sus- tain the interaction.

end-of-life issues issues pertaining to man- agement of the final phase of life, after-death disposition of the body and memorial services, and distribution of assets.

endoderm inner layer of the embryo, which becomes the lungs and digestive system.

environmental press Physical, interpersonal, or social demands that environments put on people.

epigenetic principle in erikson’s theory, the idea that each psychosocial strength has its own special period of particular importance.

episodic memory general class of memory having to do with the conscious recollection of information from a specific time or event.

equilibration according to Piaget, the process by which children reorganize their schemes to return to a state of equilibrium when disequi- librium occurs.

essentialism children’s belief that all living things have an essence that can’t be seen but gives a living thing its identity.

ethnic identity feeling that one belongs to a specific ethnic group.

eugenics effort to improve the human species by letting only people whose characteristics are valued by a society mate and pass along their genes.

euthanasia Practice of ending life for reasons of mercy.

evolutionary psychology theoretical view that many human behaviors represent successful adaptations to the environment.

exchange theory relationship, such as mar- riage, based on each partner contributing something to the relationship that the other would be hard-pressed to provide.

exosystem Social settings that a person may not experience firsthand but that still influence development.

experience-dependent growth Process by which an individual’s unique experiences

over a lifetime affect brain structures and organization.

experience-expectant growth Process by which the wiring of the brain is organized by experiences that are common to most humans.

experiment a systematic way of manipulating the key factor(s) that the investigator thinks causes a particular behavior.

explicit memory deliberate and conscious remembering of information that is learned and remembered at a specific time.

expressive style language-learning style of children whose vocabularies include many social phrases that are used like one word.

extended family Most common form of fam- ily around the world; one in which grandpar- ents and other relatives live with parents and children.

external memory aids Memory aids that rely on environmental resources, such as note- books and calendars.

extraversion Personality trait dimension associ- ated with the tendency to thrive on social inter- action, to like to talk, to take charge easily, to readily express their opinions and feelings, to keep busy, to have boundless energy, and to pre- fer stimulating and challenging environments.

extremely low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 1,000 grams (2.2 pounds).

factor the interrelated abilities measured by two tests if the performance on one test is highly related to the performance on another.

familism idea that the family’s well-being takes precedence over the concerns of individual family members.

fast mapping a child’s connections between words and referents that are made so quickly that he or she cannot consider all possible meanings of the word.

fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD) dis- order affecting babies whose mothers con- sumed large amounts of alcohol while they were pregnant.

fetal therapy field of medicine concerned with treating prenatal problems before birth.

filial obligation Sense of obligation to care for one’s parents if necessary.

final scenario Way for people to make their choices known about how they do and do not want their lives to end.

fine motor skills Motor skills associated with grasping, holding, and manipulating objects.

fluid intelligence abilities that make you a flexible and adaptive thinker, allow you to make inferences, and enable you to understand the relations among concepts.

four-component model Model for under- standing grief that is based on (1) the con- text of the loss, (2) continuation of subjective meaning associated with loss, (3) changing representations of the lost relationship over time, and (4) the role of coping and emotion- regulation processes.

frail older adults older adults who have physi- cal disabilities, are very ill, and may have cogni- tive or psychological disorders.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G l o s s a r y 583

free radicals highly reactive chemicals pro- duced randomly in normal metabolism.

friendship voluntary relationship between two people involving mutual liking.

frontal cortex brain region that regulates per- sonality and goal-directed behavior.

functional health ability to perform the activ- ities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs).

functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Method of studying brain activity by using magnetic fields to track blood flow in the brain.

functional neuroimaging Neuroimaging technique that provides an indication of brain activity but not high anatomical detail.

gender discrimination act of denying a job to someone solely on the basis of whether the person is a man or a woman.

gender identity Sense of oneself as male or a female.

gender stereotypes beliefs and images about males and females that are not necessarily true.

gender-schema theory theory that states that children want to learn more about an activity only after first deciding whether it is masculine or feminine.

gene group of nucleotide bases that provides a specific set of biochemical instructions.

generativity in erikson’s theory, being produc- tive by helping others to ensure the continua- tion of society by guiding the next generation.

genotype Person’s hereditary makeup. germ disc Small cluster of cells near the center

of the zygote that eventually develop into the baby.

glass ceiling level to which women may rise in an organization but beyond which they may not go.

glass cliff Situation in which a woman’s leader- ship position is precarious.

glaucoma disease in which the fluid in the eye does not drain properly, causing very high internal pressure that can damage the eye and cause loss of vision.

grammatical morphemes Words or endings of words that make a sentence grammatical.

grief Sorrow, hurt, anger, guilt, confusion, and other feelings that arise after suffering a loss.

g r ief work a s r u mi nation hy p othe si s approach that not only rejects the necessity of grief processing for recovery from loss but also views extensive grief processing as a form of rumination that may increase distress.

grief work Psychological side of coming to terms with bereavement.

guided participation children’s involvement in structured activities with others who are more skilled, typically producing cognitive growth.

habituation act of becoming unresponsive to a stimulus that is presented repeatedly.

healthcare power of attorney document in which an individual appoints someone to act as his or her agent for health care decisions.

healthy aging growing old by avoiding disease, being engaged with life, and maintaining high cognitive and physical functioning.

hemispheres right and left halves of the cortex.

heritability coefficient a measure (derived from a correlation coefficient) of the extent to which a trait or characteristic is inherited.

heterozygous When the alleles in a pair of chromosomes differ from each other.

high-density lipoproteins (HDLs) chemicals that help keep arteries clear and break down LDLs.

homogamy Similarity of values and interests. homozygous When the alleles in a pair of

chromosomes are the same. hope according to erikson, openness to new

experience tempered by wariness that occurs when trust and mistrust are in balance.

hospice approach to assisting dying people that emphasizes pain management, or pallia- tive care, and death with dignity.

hostile aggression unprovoked aggression that seems to have the sole goal of intimidating, harassing, or humiliating another child.

human development the multidisciplinary study of how people change and how they remain the same over time.

Huntington’s disease Progressive and fatal type of dementia caused by dominant alleles.

hypoxia a birth complication in which umbili- cal blood flow is disrupted and the infant does not receive adequate oxygen.

illusion of invulnerability adolescents’ belief that misfortunes cannot happen to them.

imaginary audience adolescents’ feeling that their behavior is constantly being watched by their peers.

imitation or observational learning learn- ing that occurs by simply watching how others behave.

implantation Step in which the zygote bur- rows into the uterine wall and establishes con- nections with the woman’s blood vessels.

implementation third phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which individuals enter the workforce.

implicit memory unconscious remembering of information learned at some earlier time.

impression formation the way we form and revise first impressions about others.

in vitro fertilization Process by which sperm and an egg are mixed in a petri dish to create a zygote, which is then placed in a woman’s uterus.

incomplete dominance Situation in which one allele does not dominate another completely.

incontinence loss of bladder or bowel control. independent variable the factor being manip-

ulated in an experiment. infant mortality the number of infants out of

1,000 births who die before their first birthday. infant-directed speech Speech that adults use

with infants that is slow and has exaggerated changes in pitch and volume; it helps children master language.

information-pro cessing the or y the or y proposing that human cognition consists of mental hardware and mental software.

instr umental activities of daily living (IADLs) actions that require some intellec- tual competence and planning.

instrumental aggression aggression used to achieve an explicit goal.

instrumental orientation characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 2, in which moral reasoning is based on the aim of looking out for one’s own needs.

integration linking individual motions into a coherent, coordinated whole.

integrity versus despair according to erikson, the process in late life by which people try to make sense of their lives.

intellectual disability Substantially below- average intelligence and problems adapting to  an environment that emerge before the age of 18.

intelligence quotient (IQ) Mathematical rep- resentation of how a person scores on an intel- ligence test in relation to how other people of the same age score.

interindividual variability Patterns of change that vary from one person to another.

internal belief systems view of a cause of depression of what one tells oneself about why certain things are happening.

internal memory aids Memory aids that rely on mental processes, such as imagery.

internal working model infant’s understand- ing of how responsive and dependable the mother is; thought to influence close relation- ships throughout the child’s life.

interpersonal norms characteristic of Kohl- berg’s stage 3, in which moral reasoning is based on winning the approval of others.

intersensory redundancy infants’ sensory systems are attuned to information presented simultaneously to different sensory modes.

intersubjectivity Mutual, shared understand- ing among participants in an activity.

intimacy versus isolation Sixth stage in erikson’s theory and the major psychosocial task for young adults.

irregular or rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep irregular sleep in which an infant’s eyes dart rapidly beneath the eyelids while the body is quite active.

job satisfaction Positive feeling that results from an appraisal of one’s work.

joint custody custody agreement in which both parents retain legal custody of their chil- dren following divorce.

kinetic cues cues to depth perception in which motion is used to estimate depth.

kinkeeper Person who gathers family mem- bers together for celebrations and keeps them in touch with each other, usually a middle-aged mother.

knowledge-telling strategy Writing down information as it is retrieved from memory, a common practice for young writers.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

584 G l o s s a r y

knowledge-transforming strategy deciding what information to include in writing and how best to organize it to convey a point.

learning disability When a child with normal intelligence has difficulty mastering at least one academic subject.

leisure discretionary activity that includes simple relaxation, activities for enjoyment, and creative pursuits.

life review Process by which people reflect on the events and experiences of their lifetimes.

life story a personal narrative that organizes past events into a coherent sequence.

life-course persistent antisocial behavior antisocial behavior that emerges at an early age and continues throughout life.

life-course perspective description of how various generations experience the biologi- cal, psychological, and sociocultural forces of development in their respective historical contexts.

life-cycle forces differences in how the same event affects people of different ages.

life-span construct unified sense of the past, present, and future based on personal experi- ence and input from other people.

life-span perspective view that human devel- opment is multiply determined and cannot be understood within the scope of a single framework.

linear perspective a cue to depth percep- tion based on the fact that parallel lines come together at a single point in the distance.

living will document in which a person states his or her wishes about life support and other treatments.

locomote to move around in the world. longevity Number of years a person can expect

to live. longitudinal study a research design in which

the same individuals are observed or tested repeatedly at different points in their lives.

long-term memory Permanent storehouse for memories that has unlimited capacity.

low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 2,500 grams (5.5 pounds).

low-density lipoproteins (LDLs) chemicals that cause fatty deposits to accumulate in arteries, impeding blood flow.

macrosystem the cultures and subcultures in which the microsystem, mesosystem, and exosystem are embedded.

mad cry More intense version of a basic cry. malnourished being small for one’s age

because of inadequate nutrition. marital adjustment degree to which a hus-

band and wife accommodate to each other over a certain period of time.

marital quality Subjective evaluation of the couple’s relationship on a number of different dimensions.

marital satisfaction global assessment of one’s marriage.

marital success umbrella term referring to any marital outcome.

maximum life expectancy oldest age to which any person lives.

meaning-mission fit alignment between an executive’s personal intentions and his or her firm’s mission.

mechanics of intelligence those aspects of intelligence comprising fluid intelligence.

menarche onset of menstruation. menopausal hormone therapy (MHt) ther-

apy in which women take low doses of estro- gen, which is often combined with progestin (a synthetic form of progesterone).

menopause Point at which menstruation stops. mental age (MA) in intelligence testing, a mea-

sure of children’s performance corresponding to the chronological age of those whose perfor- mance equals the child’s.

mental hardware Mental and neural structures that are built in and allow the mind to operate.

mental operations cognitive actions that can be performed on objects or ideas.

mental software Mental “programs” that are the basis for performing particular tasks.

mentor Person who is part teacher, part spon- sor, part model, and part counselor who facili- tates on-the-job learning to help a new hire do the work required in his or her present role and to prepare for future roles.

mesoderm Middle layer of the embryo, which becomes the muscles, bones, and circulatory system.

mesosystem Provides connections across microsystems.

meta-analysis a tool that enables researchers to synthesize the results of many studies to estimate relations between variables.

metabolism how much energy the body needs. metacognitive knowledge a person’s knowl-

edge and awareness of cognitive processes. metamemory Person’s informal understand-

ing of memory; includes the ability to diagnose memory problems accurately and to monitor the effectiveness of memory strategies.

microsystem the people and objects in an individual’s immediate environment.

midlife correction reevaluating one’s roles and dreams and making the necessary corrections.

mindfulness-based stress reduction Stress reduction technique based on being aware and nonjudgmental of whatever is happening at that moment.

mo del of ad aptive g rie v ing dy namic s (MAgD) Model of grieving based on two sets of pairs of adaptive grieving dynamics: lamenting and heartening responses to grief; integrating and tempering responses to grief.

monozygotic twins the result of a single fertil- ized egg splitting to form two new individuals; also called identical twins.

motion parallax Kinetic cue to depth percep- tion based on the fact that nearby moving objects move across our visual field faster than do distant objects.

motor skills coordinated movements of the muscles and limbs.

mourning Ways in which people express their grief.

multidimensional characteristic of theories of intelligence that identify several types of intel- lectual abilities.

multidirectionality developmental pattern in which some aspects of intelligence improve and other aspects decline during adulthood.

myelin fatty sheath that wraps around neurons and enables them to transmit information more rapidly.

narrative Way in which a person derives per- sonal meaning from being generative by con- structing a life story, which helps create the person’s identity.

naturalistic observation technique in which people are observed as they behave spontane- ously in a real-life situation.

nature–nurture issue the degree to which genetic or hereditary influences (nature) and experiential or environmental influences (nur- ture) determine the kind of person you are.

negative reinforcement trap unwittingly rein- forcing a behavior you want to discourage.

neglected children as applied to children’s popularity, children who are ignored—neither liked nor disliked—by their classmates.

neural efficiency hypothesis States intelligent people process information more efficiently, showing weaker neural activations in a smaller  number of areas than less intelligent people.

neural plate flat group of cells present in pre- natal development that becomes the brain and spinal cord.

neurofibrillary tangles Spiral-shaped masses formed when fibers that compose the axon become twisted together.

neuron basic cellular unit of the brain and ner- vous system that specializes in receiving and transmitting information.

neuroscience the study of the brain and ner- vous system, especially in terms of brain– behavior relationships.

neuroticism Personality trait dimension asso- ciated with the tendency to be anxious, hos- tile, self-conscious, depressed, impulsive, and vulnerable.

neurotransmitters chemicals released by the terminal buttons that allow neurons to com- municate with each other.

niche-picking Process of deliberately seeking environments that are compatible with one’s genetic makeup.

non-invasive prenatal testing (NIpt ) a prenatal diagnostic technique that analyzes genetic material released from the placenta that circulates in a pregnant woman’s blood stream.

nonshared environmental influences forces within a family that make siblings different from one another.

nuclear family Most common form of family in Western societies, consisting only of parent(s) and child(ren).

nursing home type of long-term care facility that provide medical care 24 hours a day 7 days a week using a team of health care professionals

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G l o s s a r y 585

that includes physicians (who must be on call at all times), nurses, therapists (e.g., physical, occupational), and others.

obedience orientation characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 1, in which moral reasoning is based on the belief that adults know what is right and wrong.

object permanence understanding, acquired in infancy, that objects exist independently of oneself.

one-to-one principle counting principle that states that there must be one and only one number name for each object counted.

open adoption an adoption in which adopted children (and their adoptive families) commu- nicate with the children’s birth family.

openness to experience Personality dimension that reflects a tendency to have a vivid imagi- nation and dream life, an appreciation of art, and a strong desire to try anything once.

operant conditioning learning paradigm proposed by b. f. Skinner in which the con- sequences of a behavior determine whether a behavior is repeated.

organization as applied to children’s memory, a strategy in which information to be remem- bered is structured so that related information is placed together.

orienting response an individual views a strong or unfamiliar stimulus, and changes in heart rate and brain-wave activity occur.

osteoarthritis Most common form of arthri- tis, a disease marked by gradual onset of bone damage with progression of pain and disabil- ity together with minor signs of inflammation from wear and tear.

osteoporosis disease in which bones become porous and extremely easy to break.

overextension When children define words more broadly than adults do overregular- ization grammatical usage that results from applying rules to words that are exceptions to the rule.

pain cry cry that begins with a sudden long burst, followed by a long pause and gasping.

palliative care care that is focused on provid- ing relief from pain and other symptoms of disease at any point during the disease process.

parallel play When children play alone but are aware of and interested in what another child is doing.

parieto-frontal integration theory (p-FIt) theory that proposes that intelligence comes from a distributed and integrated network of neurons in the parietal and frontal lobes of the brain.

parkinson’s disease brain disease known pri- marily for its characteristic motor symptoms: very slow walking, difficulty getting into and out of chairs, and a slow hand tremor, but it can develop into a form of dementia.

passion Strong inclination toward an activity that individuals like (or even love), that they value (and thus find important), and in which they invest time and energy.

passive euthanasia Practice of allowing a per- son to die by withholding available treatment.

perception Processes by which the brain receives, selects, modifies, and organizes incoming nerve impulses that are the result of physical stimulation.

perimenopause individually varying time of transition from regular menstruation to menopause.

period of the fetus longest period of prenatal development, extending from the 9th until the 38th week after conception.

permissive parenting Style of parenting in which parents offer warmth and caring but little control over their children.

persistent vegetative state Situation in which a person’s cortical functioning ceases while brainstem activity continues.

personal control beliefs the degree to which you believe your performance in a situation depends on something you do.

personal fable attitude of many adolescents that their feelings and experiences are unique and have never been experienced by anyone else.

p er sonalit y adju stment d e velopmental changes in terms of their adaptive value and functionality such as whether one can func- tion effectively within society and how per- sonality  contributes to everyday life running smoothly.

personality growth form of personality that refers to ideal end states such as increased self- transcendence, wisdom, and integrity.

personality-type theory view proposed by holland that people find their work fulfilling when the important features of a job or profes- sion fit their personality.

phenotype Physical, behavioral, and psycho- logical features that result from the interaction between one’s genes and the environment.

phenylketonuria (pKU) inherited disorder in which the infant lacks a liver enzyme.

phonological awareness the ability to hear the distinctive sounds of letters.

phonological memory ability to remember speech sounds briefly; an important skill in acquiring vocabulary.

physical limitations (pLIMs) activities that reflect functional limitations such as walking the equivalent of about a city block or sitting for about two hours.

physician-assisted suicide Process in which phy- sicians provide dying patients with a fatal dose of medication that the patient self-administers.

pictorial cues cues to depth perception that are used to convey depth in drawings and paintings.

placenta Structure through which nutrients and wastes are exchanged between the mother and the developing child.

plasticity concept that intellectual abilities are not fixed, but can be modified under the right conditions at just about any point in adulthood.

polygenic inheritance When phenotypes are the result of the combined activity of many separate genes.

popular children children who are liked by many classmates.

population pyramid graphic technique for illustrating population trends.

populations broad groups of people that are of interest to researchers.

possible selves representations of what we could become, what we would like to become, and what we are afraid of becoming.

post-traumatic stress disorder (ptSD) anxi- ety disorder that can develop after exposure to a terrifying event or ordeal in which grave physical harm occurred or was threatened.

postconventional level third level of reason- ing in Kohlberg’s theory, in which morality is based on a personal moral code.

postformal thought thinking characterized by recognizing that the correct answer varies from one situation to another, that solutions should be realistic, that ambiguity and contra- diction are typical, and that subjective factors play a role in thinking.

practical ability in Sternberg’s theory of intel- ligence, the ability to know which problem solutions are likely to work.

practical intelligence broad range of skills related to how individuals shape, select, or adapt to their physical and social environments.

pragmatics of intelligence those aspects of intelligence ref le cting cr ystallize d intelligence.

preconventional level first level of reasoning in Kohlberg’s theory, where moral reasoning is based on external forces.

preimplantation genetic screening (pgS) a procedure used to test the heredity of an egg fertilized with assisted reproductive technol- ogy, typically to determine the presence of genetic disorders.

prenatal development the many changes that turn a fertilized egg into a newborn human.

presbycusis reduced sensitivity to high- pitched tones.

presbyopia difficulty seeing close objects clearly.

preterm or premature babies born before the 36th week after conception.

preventive adaptations actions that avoid stressors and increase or build social resources.

primary appraisal Process that categorizes events into three groups based on the signifi- cance they have for one’s well-being: irrelevant, benign or positive, and stressful.

primary control behavior aimed at affecting the individual’s external world.

primary mental abilities groups of related intellectual skills (such as memory or spatial ability).

primary sex characteristics Physical signs of maturity that are directly linked to the repro- ductive organs.

private speech a child’s comments that are not intended for others but are designed to help regulate the child’s own behavior.

proactivity When people choose new behav- iors to meet new desires or needs and exert control over their lives.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

586 G l o s s a r y

problem-focused coping attempts to tackle a problem head-on.

prosocial behavior any behavior that benefits another person.

proximodistal principle Principle of physical growth that states that structures nearest the center of the body develop first.

psychodynamic theories theories proposing that development is largely determined by how well people resolve conflicts they face at dif- ferent ages.

psychological capital theory Notion that hav- ing a positive outlook improves processes and outcomes.

psychometricians Psychologists who special- ize in measuring psychological traits such as intelligence and personality.

psychosocial theory erikson’s proposal that personality development is determined by the interaction of an internal maturational plan and external societal demands.

puberty collection of physical changes that marks the onset of adolescence, including a growth spurt and the growth of breasts or testes.

punishment a consequence that decreases the likelihood of the behavior that it follows.

purpose according to erikson, balance between individual initiative and the willing- ness to cooperate with others.

qualitative research Method that involves gaining in-depth understanding of human behavior and what governs it.

reality shock Situation in which what you learn in the classroom does not always transfer directly to the real world and does not repre- sent all that you need to know.

reappraisal Process of making a new pri- mary or secondary appraisal resulting from changes in the situation.

recessive Allele whose instructions are ignored in the presence of a dominant allele.

recursive thinking thoughts that focus on what another person is thinking.

referential style language-learning style of children whose vocabularies are dominated by names of objects, persons, or actions.

reflective judgment Way in which adults rea- son through real-life dilemmas.

reflexes unlearned responses triggered by spe- cific stimulation.

regular or nonREM sleep Sleep in which heart rate, breathing, and brain activity are steady.

reinforcement consequence that increases the likelihood that a behavior will be repeated in the future.

rejected children as applied to children’s popularity, children who are disliked by many classmates.

relational aggression aggression used to hurt others by undermining their social relationships.

reliability extent to which a measure provides a consistent index of a characteristic.

resistant attachment relationship in which, after a brief separation, infants want to be held but are difficult to console.

returning adult students college students over age 25.

rheumatoid arthritis More destructive disease of the joints that develops slowly; it typically affects different joints and causes different types of pain than osteoarthritis does.

rites of passage rituals marking initiation into adulthood.

role transitions Movement into the next stage of development marked by assumption of new responsibilities and duties.

sample a subset of the population. sandwich generation Middle-aged adults who

are caught between the competing demands of two generations: their parents and their children.

scaffolding a style in which teachers gauge the amount of assistance they offer to match the learner’s needs.

scenario Manifestation of the life-span con- struct through expectations about the future.

schemes according to Piaget, mental struc- tures that organize information and regulate behavior.

secondary appraisal Process that evaluates one’s perceived ability to cope with harm, threat, or challenge.

secondar y control behavior or cognition aimed at affecting the individual’s internal world.

secondary mental abilities broader intellec- tual skills that subsume and organize the pri- mary abilities.

secondary sex characteristics Physical signs of maturity that are not directly linked to repro- ductive organs.

secure attachment relationship in which infants have come to trust and depend on their mothers.

selective estrogen receptor modulators (SERMs) compounds that are not estrogens, but have estrogen-like effects on some tissues and estrogen-blocking effects on other tissues.

selective optimization with compensation (SOC) Model in which three processes (selection, optimization, and compensation) form a system of behavioral action that gener- ates and regulates development and aging.

self-efficacy People’s beliefs about their own abilities and talents.

self-reports People’s answers to questions about the topic of interest.

semantic memory general class of memory concerning the remembering of meanings of words or concepts not tied to a specific time or event.

sense of place cognitive and emotional attach- ments that a person puts on his or her place of residence, by which a “house” is made into a “home”.

sensorimotor period first of Piaget’s four stages of cognitive development, which lasts from birth to approximately 2 years.

separation distress expression of compli- cated or prolonged grief disorder that includes being preoccupied with the deceased to the point that it interferes with everyday

functioning, having upsetting memories of the deceased, longing and searching for the deceased, and feeling isolated following the loss.

sequential design developmental research design based on cross-sectional and longitu- dinal designs.

sex chromosomes 23rd pair of chromosomes; these determine the sex of the child.

sickle-cell trait disorder in which individuals show signs of mild anemia only when they are seriously deprived of oxygen; occurs in individ- uals who have one dominant allele for normal blood cells and one recessive sickle-cell allele.

simple social play Play that begins at about 15 to 18 months; toddlers engage in similar activities as well as talk and smile at each other.

skeletal maturity Point at which bone mass is greatest and the skeleton is at peak develop- ment, around 18 for women and 20 in men.

sleeping State in which a baby alternates between being still and breathing regularly to moving gently and breathing irregularly; the eyes are closed throughout.

social clock tagging future events with a par- ticular time or age by which they are to be completed.

social cognitive career theory (SCCt) theory that proposes that career choice is a result of  the application of bandura’s social cog- nitive  theor y, especially the concept of self-efficacy.

social contract characteristic of Kohlberg’s Stage 5, in which moral reasoning is based on the belief that laws are for the good of all mem- bers of society.

social convoy group of people who journey with us throughout our lives, providing sup- port in good times and bad.

social referencing behavior in which infants in unfamiliar or ambiguous environments look at an adult for cues to help them interpret the situation.

social role Set of cultural guidelines about how one should behave, especially with other people.

social smiles Smile that infants produce when they see a human face.

social system morality characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 4, in which moral reasoning is based on maintenance of order in society.

socialization teaching children the values, roles, and behaviors of their culture.

socioemotional selectivity theory that argues that social contact is motivated by a variety of goals, including information seeking, self- concept, and emotional regulation.

specification Second phase in Super’s theory of career development, in which adolescents learn more about specific lines of work and begin training.

speed of processing how quickly and effi- ciently the early steps in information process- ing are completed.

spermarche first spontaneous ejaculation of sperm.

spina bifida disorder in which the embryo’s neural tube does not close properly.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

G l o s s a r y 587

spiritual support type of coping strategy that includes seeking pastoral care, participating in organized and nonorganized religious activi- ties, and expressing faith in a god who cares for people.

stable-order principle counting principle that states that number names must always be counted in the same order.

stagnation in erikson’s theory, the state in which people are unable to deal with the needs of their children or to provide mentoring to younger adults.

stereotype threat an evoked fear of being judged in accordance with a negative stereo- type about a group to which you belong.

stranger wariness first distinct signs of fear that emerge around 6 months of age when infants become wary in the presence of unfa- miliar adults.

stress Physical and psychological responses to threatening or challenging conditions.

stress and coping paradigm framework that views stress not as an environmental stimu- lus or as a response, but as the interaction of a thinking person and an event.

strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents (CVAs) interruption of the blood flow in the brain due to blockage or a hemorrhage in a cerebral artery.

structural neuroimaging Neuroimaging tech- nique such as ct and Mri scans that provides highly detailed images of anatomical features in the brain.

structure of intelligence the organization of interrelated intellectual abilities.

structured observations technique in which a researcher creates a setting that is likely to elicit the behavior of interest.

subjective well-being evaluation of one’s life that is associated with positive feelings.

successful aging Pathway through late life that focuses on positive outcomes through health and social engagement to achieve well-being.

sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) When a healthy baby dies suddenly for no apparent reason.

synaptic pruning gradual reduction in the number of synapses, beginning in infancy and continuing until early adolescence.

systematic observation Watching people and carefully recording what they do or say.

telegraphic speech Speech used by young chil- dren that contains only the words necessary to convey a message.

teleological explanations children’s belief that living things and parts of living things exist for a purpose.

telomerase enzyme needed in DNA replica- tion to fully reproduce the telomeres when cells divide.

telomeres tips of chromosomes that play a major role in aging by adjusting the cell’s

response to stress and growth stimulation based on cell divisions and DNA damage.

temperament consistent style or pattern of behavior.

teratogen an agent that causes abnormal pre- natal development.

terminal buttons Small knobs at the end of the axon that release neurotransmitters.

terror management theor y theor y that addresses the issue of why people engage in certain behaviors to achieve particular psy- chological states based on their deeply rooted concerns about mortality.

texture gradient Perceptual cue to depth based on the fact that the texture of objects changes from coarse and distinct for nearby objects to finer and less distinct for distant objects.

thanatology Study of death, dying, grief, bereavement, and social attitudes toward these issues.

theor y an organized set of ideas that is designed to explain development.

theor y of mind ideas about connections between thoughts, beliefs, intentions, and behavior that create an intuitive understanding of the link between mind and behavior.

time-out punishment that involves removing children who are misbehaving from a situation to a quiet, unstimulating environment.

toddler young children who have just learned to walk.

toddling early, unsteady form of walking done by infants.

transient ischemic attacks (tIAs) interrup- tion of blood flow to the brain; often an early warning sign of stroke.

traumatic distress expression of complicated or prolonged grief disorder that includes dis- belief about the death; mistrusting others, feel- ing anger, and being detachment from others as a result of the death; feeling shocked by the death; and experiencing the physical presence of the deceased.

type 1 diabetes type of diabetes that usually develops earlier in life and requires the use of insulin; sometimes called insulin-dependent diabetes.

type 2 diabetes type of diabetes that typically develops in adulthood and is often effectively managed through diet.

ultrasound Prenatal diagnostic technique that uses sound waves to generate an image of the fetus.

umbilical cord Structure containing veins and arteries that connects the developing child to the placenta.

underextension When children define words more narrowly than adults do.

uninvolved parenting Style of parenting in which parents provide neither warmth nor control and minimize the time they spend with their children.

universal and context-specific development issue Whether there is one path of develop- ment or several paths.

universal ethical principles characteristic of Kohlberg’s stage 6, in which moral reasoning is based on moral principles that apply to all.

validity extent to which a measure actually assesses what researchers think it does.

vascular dementia disease caused by numer- ous small cerebral vascular accidents.

vernix Substance that protects the fetus’s skin during development.

very low birth weight Newborns who weigh less than 1,500 grams (3.3 pounds).

visual acuity Smallest pattern that one can dis- tinguish reliably.

visual cliff glass-covered platform that appears to have a “shallow” and a “deep” side; used to study infants’ depth perception.

visual expansion Kinetic cue to depth percep- tion that is based on the fact that an object fills an ever-greater proportion of the retina as it moves closer.

vocational maturity degree of congruence between people’s occupational behavior and what is expected of them at different ages.

v ul ner abil it y – stre ss –ad apt ation mo del Model that proposes that marital quality is a dynamic process resulting from the couple’s ability to handle stressful events in the context of their particular vulnerabilities and resources.

waking activity State in which a baby’s eyes are open but seem unfocused while the arms or legs move in bursts of uncoordinated motion.

wear-and-tear disease degenerative disease caused by injury or overuse.

whole-brain death declared only when the deceased meets eight criteria, which were established in 1981.

will according to erikson, a young child’s understanding that he or she can act on the world intentionally; this occurs when auton- omy, shame, and doubt are in balance.

word recognition the process of identifying a unique pattern of letters.

work–family conflict feeling of being pulled in multiple directions by incompatible demands from one’s job and one’s family.

working memory Processes and structures involved in holding information in mind and simultaneously using it for other functions.

zone of maximum comfort When press level is  slightly lower, facilitating a high quality of life.

zone of maximum performance potential When press level is slightly higher, tending to improve performance.

zone of proximal development difference between what children can do with assistance and what they can do alone.

zygote fertilized egg.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

References

A Better Balance. (2016). The need for paid family leave. Retrieved from www.abetterbalance.org/ web/ourissues/familyleave.

AAIDD Ad Hoc Committee on Terminology and Classification. (2010). Intellectual disability (11th ed.). Washington, D C: American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities.

AARP. (1999). AARP/Modern maturity sexuality sur vey. Retrieved from assets .aarp.org/ rgcenter/health/mmsexsurvey.pdf.

AARP. (2005). Sexuality at midlife and beyond: 2004 update of attitudes and behaviors. Retrieved from assets.aarp.org/rgcenter/ general/2004_sexuality.pdf.

AARP. (2010). Sex, romance, and relationships: AARP survey of midlife and older adults. Retrieved from assets.aarp.org/rgcenter/ general/srr_09.pdf.

AARP. (2016). About GrandFacts. Retrieved from www.aarp.org/relationships/friends-family/ grandfacts-sheets/.

Aasland, O. G., Rosta, J., & Nylenna, M. (2010). Healthcare reforms and job satisfaction among doctors in Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 38, 253–258. doi:10.1177/1403494810364559

Abbott, G., Gilbert, K., & Rosinski, P. (2013). Cross-cultural working in coaching and mentoring. In J. Passmore, D. B. Peterson, & T. Freire (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of coaching and mentoring (pp. 483–500). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Abela, A., & Walker, J. (2014). Global changes in marriage, parenting, and family life: An overview. In A. Abela & J. Walker (Eds.), Contemporary issues in family studies: Global perspectives on partnerships, parenting and support in a changing world (pp. 5–15). New York: Wiley.

Aberson, C. L., Shoemaker, C., & Tomolillo, C. (2004). Implicit bias and contact: The role of interethnic friendships. Journal of Social Psycholog y, 144, 335–347. doi:10.3200/ SOCP.144.3.335–347

Aboud, F. E., & Yousafzai, A. K. (2015). Global health and development in early childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 433–457.

Abraham, A ., Beudt, S., Ott, D. V. M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: Dissociations between frontal, parietal-temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Research, 1482, 55–70. doi:10.1016/ j.bbr.2011.03.031

Abrams, D., Swift, H. J., & Drury, L. (2016). Old and unemployable? How age-based stereotypes affect willingness to hire job candidates. Journal of Social Issues, 72, 105–121. doi:10.1111/ josi.12158

Abrams, R. (2013). When parents die: Learning to live with the loss of a parent (3rd ed.). New York: Routledge.

Achor, S. (2010). The happiness advantage: The seven principles of positive psychology that fuel success and performance at work. New  York: Random House.

Ackerman, B. P. (1993). Children’s understanding of the speaker’s meaning in referential communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 56–86.

Adams, J. (1999). On neurodevelopmental disorders: Perspectives from neurobehavioral teratology. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 451–468). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Adams, M. J., Treiman, R., & Pressley, M. (1998). Reading, writing, and literacy. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 4 (pp. 275–356). New York: Wiley.

Adams, R. B., Jr., Nelson, A. J., Soto, J. A., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Emotion in the neutral face: A mechanism for impression formation? Cognition and Emotion, 26, 431–441. doi:10.1 080/02699931.2012.666502

Adams, R. E., Santo, J. B., & Bukowski, W. M. (2011). The presence of a best friend buffers the effects of negative experiences. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1786–1791.

Adams, R. G., & Taylor, E, M. (2015). Friendship and happiness in the third age. In M. Demir (Ed.), Friendship and happiness (pp. 155–169). New York: Springer.

Adamy, J., & McGinty, T. (2012). The crushing cost of care. Retrieved from online.wsj.com/article/ SB100014240527023044414045774830509767 66184.html.

Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19, 33–41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x

Addo, F. R. (2017). Financial integration and relationship transitions of young adult cohabiters. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 38, 84–99. doi:10.1007/s10834-016- 9490-7

Adelman, L. (2013). “Dear Prudence” columnist publishes rape denial manifesto advising women to “stop getting drunk.” Retrieved from feministing.com/2013/10/16/emily-yoffe-aka- dear-prudence-publishes-rape-denialism- manifesto-tells-women-point-blank-to-stop- getting-drunk-to-avoid-rape/.

Adhikari, K., Fontanil, T., Cal, S., Mendoza-Revilla, J., Fuentes-Guajardo, M., Chacón-Duque, J.-C. et al. (2016). A genome-wide association scan in admixed Latin Americans identifies loci influencing facial and scalp hair features. Nature Communications, 7(Article 10815). Retrieved

from www.nature.com/ncomms/2016/160301/ ncomms10815/full/ncomms10815.html.

Adler, L. L. (2001). Women and gender roles. In L. L. Adler & U. P. Gielen (Eds.), Cross-cultural topics on psychology (2nd ed., pp. 103–114). Westport, CT: Praeger/Greenwood.

Adler, R. A., Fuleihan, G. E.-H., Bauer, D. C., Camacho, P. M., Clarke, B. L., Clines, G. A., et al. (2016). Managing osteoporosis in patients on long-term bisphosphonate treatment: Report of a task force of the American Society for Bone and Mineral Research. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research, 31, 16–35. doi:10.1002/jbmr.2708

Adolph, K. (2002). Learning to keep balance. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 30, pp. 1–40). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Adolph, K. E. (2000). Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a veritable cliff. Psychological Science, 11, 290–295.

Adolph, K. E., & Berger, S. E. (2015). Physical and motor development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp. 261–333). New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.

Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2013). The road to walking: What learning to walk tells us about development. In P. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp.  403–443). New York: Oxford University Press.

Adolph, K . E., Cole, W. G., Komati, M., Garciaguirre, J. S., Badaly, D., Lingeman, J. M., et al. (2012). How do you learn to walk? Thousands of steps and dozens of falls per day. Psychological Science, 23, 1387–1394.

Adolphus, K., Lawton, C. L., & Dye, L. (2013). The effects of breakfast on behavior and academic performance in children and adolescents. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience, 7, 425.

Afilalo, J. (2016). Conceptual models of frailty: The sarcopenia phenotype. Canadian Journal of Cardiology, 32, 1051–1055. doi:10.1016/ j.cjca.2016.05.017

Agabio, R., Trincas, G., Floris, F., Mura, G., Sancassiani, F., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2017). A systematic review of school-based alcohol and other drug prevention programs. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology, 13, 102–112.

Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. (2016). 2015 national healthcare quality and disparities report and 5th anniversary update on the national quality strategy. Retrieved from www.ahrq.gov/sites/default/ files/w ysiw yg/research/findings/nhqrdr/ nhqdr15/2015nhqdr.pdf.

AgingStats.gov. (2016). Older Americans 2016: Key indicators of well-being. Retrieved from agingstats .gov/docs/LatestReport/Older-Americans-2016- Key-Indicators-of-WellBeing.pdf.

588 R e f e R e n c e s

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 589

Ahmad, F. (2012). Graduating toward marriage? Attitudes towards marriage and relationships among university-educated British Muslim women. Culture and Religion: An Interdisci- plinary Journal, 13, 193–210. doi:10.1080/147 55610.2012.674953

Ahmad, I., Smetana, J. G., & Klimstra, T. (2015). Maternal monitoring, adolescent disclosure, and adolescent adjustment among Palestinian refugee youth in Jordan. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25, 403–411.

Ai, A. L., Richardson, R., Plummer, C., Ellison, C. G., Lemieux, C., Tice, T. N., et al. (2013). Character strength and deep connections following Hur- ricanes Katrina and Rita: Spiritual and secu- lar pathways to resistance among volunteers. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 52, 537–556. doi:10.1111/jssr.12043

Ai, A. L., Wink, P., & Ardelt, M. (2010). Spiri- tuality and aging: A journey for meaning through deep interconnection in humanity. In J. C.  Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 222–246). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Ai, A. L., Wink, P., Gall, T. L., Dillon, M., & Tice, T. N. (2017). Assessing reverence in contexts: A positive emotion related to psychological functioning. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 57, 64–97. doi:10.1177/0022167815586657

Ainsworth, M. S. (1978). The development of infant–mother attachment. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Ricciuti (Eds .), Review of child development research (Vol. 3, pp. 1–94). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Ainsworth, M. S. (1993). Attachment as related to mother–infant interaction. Advances in Infancy Research, 8, 1–50.

Ajrouch, K. J., Antonucci, T. C., & Webster, N. J. (2014). Volunteerism: Social network dynamics and education. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 71, 309–319. doi:10.1093/geronb/ gbu166

Al-Bar, M. A., & Chamsi-Pasha, H. (2015). Brain death. In M. A. Al-Bar & H. Chamsi- Pa sha (Eds .), C ontemporar y bioethic s (pp. 227–242). Ne w  York : Spr inger. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-18428-9_14

Alatupa, S., Pulkki-Råback, L., Hintsanen, M., Elovainio, M., Mullola, S., & Keltikangas- Järvinen, L. (2013). Disruptive behavior in child- hood and socioeconomic position in adulthood: A prospective study over 27 years. International Journal of Public Health, 58, 247–256.

Alavi, M. V. (2016). Aging and vision. In C. B. Rickman, M. M. LaVail, R. E. Anderson, C. Grimm, J. Hollyfield, & J. Ash (Eds.), Retinal degenerative diseases (pp. 393–399). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-17121-0_52

Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Judgment and decision making in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 211–224.

Albert, D., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2013). Peer influences on adolescent decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 114–120.

Alberts, A . E. (2005). Neonatal behavioral assessment scale. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner

(Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 1, pp. 111–115). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Albuquerque, S., Pereira, M., & Narciso, I. (2016). Couple’s relationship after the death of a child: A systematic review. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 30–53. doi:10.1007/ s10826-015-0219-2

Alden, D. L., Merz, M. Y., & Akashi, J. (2012). Young adult preference for physician decision- making style in Japan and the United States. Asia-Pacific Journal of Public Health, 24, 173–184. doi:10.1177/1010539510365098

Aldridge, V., Dovey, T. M., & Halford, J. C. G. (2009). The role of familiarity in dietary development. Developmental Review, 29, 32–44.

Aldwin, C. M. (2015). How can developmental systems theories cope with free will? The importance of stress-related growth and mindfulness. Research in Human Development, 12, 189–195. doi:10.1080/15427609.2015 .1068042

Aldwin, C. M., & Gilmer, D. F. (2013). Health, illness, and optimal aging: Biological and psychosocial perspectives (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

Aldwin, C. M., Park, C. L., Jeong, Y.-J., & Nath,  R. (2014). Differing pathways between religiousness, spirituality, and health: A self- regulation perspective. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 6, 9–21. doi: 10.1037/ a0034416

Aldwin, C., & Igarashi, H. (2012). An ecological model of resilience in late life. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 32, 115–130. doi:10.1891/0198-8794.32.115

Alegría-Flores, K., Raker, K., Pleasants, R. K., Weaver, M. A., & Weinberger, M. (2017). Preventing interpersonal violence on college campuses: The effect of One Act training on bystander intervention. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32, 1103–1126.

Alexander, G. M., & Wilcox, T. (2012). Sex differences in early infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 400–406.

Allaire, J. C., & Willis, S. L. (2006). Competence in ever yday activities as a predictor of cog nitive r isk and morbidity. Ag ing , Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 207–224. doi:10.1080/13825580490904228

Allemand, M., Zimprich, D., & Hendriks, A. A. J. (2008). Age differences in five personality domains across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 44, 758–770. doi:10.1037/0012- 1649.44.3.758

Allen, J. P., Chango, J., Szwedo, D., Schad, M., & Marston, E. (2012). Predictors of susceptibility to peer influence regarding substance use in adolescence. Child Development, 83, 337–350.

Allen, J., Gregory, A., Mikami, A., Lun, J., Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. (2013). Observations of effective teacher–student interactions in secondary school classrooms: Predicting student achievement with the Classroom Assessment Scoring System—Secondar y. School Psychology Review, 42, 76–98.

Allen, K. R., & Roberto, K. A. (2016). Family relationships of older LGBT adults. In D. A Harley & P. B. Teaster (Eds.),  Handbook of LGBT elders (pp. 43–64). New York: Springer.

Allexandre, D., Bernstein, A. M., Walker, E., Hunter, J., Roizen, M. F., & Morledge,  T.  J. (2016). A web-based mindfulness stress management program in a cor p orate call center. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine , 58, 254–264. doi:10.1097/JOM.0000000000000680

Alley, J. L. (2004). The potential meaning of the grandparent-grandchild relationship as perceived by young adults: An exploratory study. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 65(3-B), 1536.

Almeida, J., Molnar, B. E., Kawachi, I., & Subramanian, S. V. (2009). Ethnicity and nativity status as determinants of perceived social support: Testing the concept of familism. Social Science and Medicine, 68, 1852–1858 doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.029

Alspach, J. G. (2016). When it’s your time, will it be your way? Critical Care Nurse, 36, 10–13. doi:10.4037/ccn2016452

Altschul, I., Lee, S. J., & Gershoff, E. T. (2016). Hugs, not hits: Warmth and spanking as predictors of child social competence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 78, 695–714.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2016a). Quick facts. Retrieved from www.alz.org/facts/overview.asp.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2016b). Alzheimer’s and dementia basics. Retrieved from www.alz.org/ alzheimers_disease_what_is_alzheimers.asp.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2016c). 2016 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 12, 459–509. Retrieved from www .alz.org/documents_custom/2016-facts-and- figures.pdf.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2016d). 10 early signs and symptoms of Alzheimer’s. Retrieved from a l z . o rg / 1 0 - s i g n s - s y mp to m s - a l z h e i m e r s - dementia.asp.

Alzheimer’s Association. (2016e). Stages of Alzheimer’s. Retrieved from www.alz.org/ alzheimers_disease_stages_of_alzheimers.asp.

Amato, P. R . (2010). Research on divorce: Continuing trends and new developments. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 650–666.

Amato, P. R., & Boyd, L. M. (2014). Children and divorce in world perspective. In A. Abela & J. Walker (Eds.), Contemporary issues in family studies: Global perspectives on partnerships, parenting, and support in a changing world (pp. 227–244). New York: Wiley.

Amato, P. R., & Cheadle, J. (2005). The long reach of divorce: Divorce and child well- being across three generations. Journal of Mar r i age & Family, 67, 191–206. doi:10.1111/j.0022-2445.2005.00014.x

Amato, P. R., & Fowler, F. (2002). Parenting practices, child adjustment, and family diversity. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 703–716.

Amato, P. R., King, V., & Thorsen, M. L. (2016). Parent-child relationships in stepfather families and adolescent adjustment: A latent class analy- sis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 78, 482–497.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

590 R e f e R e n c e s

American Academy of Pain Medicine. (2016) Position statements to support your pain practice. Retrieved from www.painmed.org/ practicemanagement/position-statements/.

American Academy of Pediatrics. (2011). Caring for our children: National health and safety performance standards; guidelines for early child care and education programs (3rd ed.). Elk Grove Village, IL: Author.

American Cancer Society. (2012). Prostate cancer. Retrieved from www.cancer.org/cancer/ prostatecancer/index.

American Cancer Society. (2016). Benefits of quitting smoking over time. Retrieved from www .cancer.org/healthy/stayawayfromtobacco/ benefits-of-quitting-smoking-over-time.

American Cancer Society. (2016a). American C ancer Society skin cancer prevention activities. Retrieved from www.cancer.org/ h e a l t hy / m o r e w ay s a c s h e l p s y o u s t ay w e l l / acs-skin-cancer-prevention-activities.

American Cancer Society. (2016b). Lifetime risk of developing or dying from cancer. Retrieved from www.cancer.org/cancer/cancerbasics/ lifetime-probability-of-developing-or-dying- from-cancer.

American Cancer Society. (2016c). Stay healthy. Retrieved from www.cancer.org/healthy/index.

American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. (2011a). Frequently asked questions: Cesarean birth. Washington DC: Author.

American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. (2011b). Frequently asked questions: Pain relief during labor and delivery. Washington DC: Author.

American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. (2015). Cell-free DNA prenatal screening test. Washington, DC: Author.

American Diabetes Association. (2016a). Diabetes basics. Retrieved from www.diabetes .org/diabetes-basics/?loc=db-slabnav.

American Diabetes Association. (2016b). Living with diabetes. Retrieved from www.diabetes .org/living-with-diabetes/?loc=lwd-slabnav.

American Heart Association. (2016a). The American Heart Association’s diet and lifestyle recommendations. Retrieved from www.heart.org/HEARTORG/HealthyLiving/ HealthyEating/Nutrition/The-American- H e a r t- A s s o c i at i o n s - D i e t- a n d - L i f e s t y l e - Recommendations_UCM_305855_Article .jsp#.V_jurPkrLIU.

A m e r i c a n He a r t A s s o c i at i o n . ( 2 0 1 6 b ) . Seeds of native health: A campaign for indigenous nutrition. Retrieved from http:// seedsofnativehealth.org/fertile-ground-ii/.

American Lung Association. (2016). COPD. R e t r i e v e d f r o m w w w. l u n g . o r g / l u n g - health-and-diseases/lung-disease-lookup/ copd/.

American Lung Association. (2016). Smoking fact s . Re tr ie ve d f rom w w w.lung .org/ stop-smoking/smoking-facts/.

American Psychiatric Association. (2004). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

A m e r i c a n S t r o k e A s s o c i at i o n . ( 2 0 1 6 ) . Stroke treatments. Retrieved from www . s t r o k e a s s o c i a t i o n . o r g / S T R O K E O R G / AboutStroke/Treatment/Stroke-Treatments_ UCM_310892_Article.jsp#.VzEmpWP7048.

Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2006). Learning by selection: Visual search and object perception in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1236–1245.

Andersen, G. J. (2012). Aging and vision: Changes in function and performance from optics to perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 403–410. doi:10.1002/ wcs.1167

Anderson, E. R., & Greene, S. M. (2013). Beyond divorce: Research on children in repartnered and remarried families. Family Court Review, 51, 119–130. doi:10.1111/fcre.12013

Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281–296.

Andreassi, J. K. (2011). What the person brings to the table: Personality, coping, and work- family conflict. Journal of Family Issues, 32, 1474–1499. doi:10.1177/0192513X11401815

Angel, R. J., & Angel, J. L. (2015). Latinos in an aging world: Social, psychological, and economic perspectives. New York: Routledge.

Anisfeld, M. (1996). Only tongue protrusion m o d e l i n g i s m at c h e d b y n e o n at e s . Developmental Review, 16, 149–161.

Antheunis, M. L. (2016). Friendships and the internet. In C. R. Berger, M. E. Roloff, S. R. Wilson, J. P. Dillard, J. Caughlin, & D. Solomon (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of interpersonal communication. New  York: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic261

Anticevic, A., Repovs, G., & Barch, D. M. (2012). Emotion effects on attention, amygdala activation, and functional capacity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 967–980. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq168

Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2013). Developmental origins of the other-race effect. Current Direc- tions in Psychological Science, 22, 173–178.

Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Heron-Delaney, M., et al. (2012). Brief daily exposures to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 484–495.

Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Learning (not) to talk about race: When older children underperform in social categorization. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1513–1518.

Apgar, V. (1953). A proposal for a new method of evaluation of the newborn infant. Current Researches in Anesthesia and Analgesia, 32, 260–267.

Ap onte, M. (2007). Mentor ing: C are er advancement of Hispanic army nurses. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(4-A), 1609.

Arbeev, K. G., Ukraintseva, S. V., & Yashin, A. I. (2016). Dynamics of biomarkers in relation to aging and mortality. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 156, 42–54. doi:10.1016/ j.mad.2016.04.010

Archer, J. (2013). Can evolutionary principles expl ain p atter ns of f amily v iolence? Psychological Bulletin, 139, 403–440.

Ardelt, M. (2010). Age, experience, and the beginning of wisdom. In D. Dannefer & C.  Phillipson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of social gerontology (pp. 306–316). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Ardelt, M., Landes, S. D., Gerlach, K. R., & Fox,  L.  P. (2013). Rediscovering internal strengths of the aged: The beneficial impact of wisdom, mastery, purpose in life, and spiritual- ity on aging well. In J. D. Sinnott (Ed.), Positive psychology (pp. 97–119). New York: Springer.

Arizmendi, B., Kaszniak, A. W., & O’Connor, M.-F. (2016). Disrupted prefrontal activity during emotion processing in complicated grief : An fMRI investigation. NeuroImage, 124 (Part A), 968–976. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage .2015.09.054

Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2017). Where health and death intersect: Insights from a Terror Management Health Model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 126–131. doi:10.1177/0963721416689563

Arnett, J. J. (2013). Adolescence and emerging adulthood: A cultural approach (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

Arnett, J. J. (2016). Introduction: Emerging adulthood theory and research: Where we are and where we should go. In J. J. Arnett (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of emerging adulthood (pp. 1–7). New York: Oxford University Press.

Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong,  G., Li,  H., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337. doi:10.1152/ jn.00838.2004

Arseneault, L., Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, B., & Saucier, J. F. (2002). Obstetrical complications and violent delinquenc y : Testing two developmental pathways. Child Development, 73, 496–508.

Årseth, A. K., Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Marcia, J. E. (2009). Meta-analytic studies of identity status and the relational issues of attachment and intimacy. Identity, 9, 1–32. doi:10.1080/15283480802579532

Artazcoz, L., Benach, J., Borrell, C., & Cortès, I. (2004). Unemployment and mental health: Understanding the interactions among gender, family roles, and social class. American Journal of Public Health, 94, 82–88. doi:10.2105/ AJPH.94.1.82

Arunachalam, S., Escovar, E., Hansen, M. A., & Waxman, S. R. (2013). Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 591

information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 417–425.

Aryee, S., Chu, C. W. L., Kim, T.-Y., & Ryu, S. (2013). Family-supportive work environment and employee work behaviors: An investigation of mediating mechanisms. Journal of Management, 39, 792–813. doi:10.1177/0149206311435103

Asarnow, J. R., & Miranda, J. (2014). Improving care for depression and suicide risk in adolescents: Innovative strategies for bringing treatments to community settings. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 275–303.

Asbridge, M., Brubacher, J. R., & Chan, H. (2013). Cell phone use and traffic crash risk: A culpability analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 259–267.

Asendorpf, J. B., Denissen, J. J. A ., & van  Aken,  M.  A.  G. (2008). Inhibited and aggressive preschool children at 23 years of age: Personality and social transitions into adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 44, 997–1011.

Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach. Developmental Review, 2, 212–236.

Aslin, R. (2017). Statistical learning: a powerful mechanism that operates by mere exposure. WIREs Cognitive Science, 8, e1373.

Asoodeh, M. H., Khalili, S., Daneshpour, N., & Lavasani, M. G. (2010). Factors of successful marriage: Accounts from self-described happy couples. Procedia Social and Behavioral S cience s , 5, 2042–2046. doi:10.1016/ j.sbspro.2010.07.410

Assanangkornchai, S., Tangboonngam, S., Samangsri, N., & Edwards, J. G. (2007). A Thai community’s anniversary reaction to a major catastrophe. Stress and Health, 23, 43–50. doi:10.1002/smi.1118

Attia, E. Anorexia nervosa. In K. D. Brownell & B. T. Walsh (Eds.), Eating disorders and obesity (3rd ed., pp. 176–181). New York: Guilford.

Attig, T. (1996). How we grieve: Relearning the world. New York: Oxford University Press.

Au, T. K., & Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: To honor or not to honor? Child Development, 61, 1474–1490.

Aunola, K., Stattin, H., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2000). Parenting styles and adolescents’ achievement strategies. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 205–222.

Awa, W. L., Plaumann, M., & Walter, U. (2010). Burnout prevention: A review of intervention programs. Patient Education and Counseling, 78, 184–190. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2009.04.008

Awadi, M. A., & Mrayyan, M. T. (2016). Opponents and proponents views regarding palliative sedation at end of life. Journal of Palliative Care & Medicine, 6, 242. doi:10.4172/2165-7386 .1000242. Retrieved from www.omicsgroup .org/journals/opponents-and-proponents- views-regarding-palliative-sedation-at-end- oflife-2165-7386-1000242.php?aid=66887.

Awong, T., Grusec, J. E., & Sorenson, A . (2008). Respect-based control and anger as determinants of children’s socio-emotional

de velopment . Social D e velopment , 17, 941–959.

Bachman, B. (2017). Ethical leadership in organizations. New York: Springer.

Bachman, J. G., Staff, J., O’Malley, P. M., & Freedman-Doan, P. (2013). Adolescent work intensity, school performance, and substance use: Links vary by race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2125–2134.

Bachman, J. G., Staff, J., O’Malley, P.M., Freedman-Doan, P. (2014). What do teenagers do with their earnings, and does it matter for their academic achievement and development? (Monitoring the Future Occasional Paper No. 78). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.

Bade, M. K. (2012). Personal growth in the midst of negative life experiences: The role of religious coping strategies and appraisals. Dissertation submitted to Texas Tech University. Retrieved f ro m ttu - i r. td l . o rg / ttu - i r / h a n d l e / 2 3 4 6 / 10475/.

Baer, D. M., & Wolf, M. M. (1968). The reinforcement contingency in preschool and remedial education. In R. D. Hess & R. M Baer (Eds.), Early education: Theory, research and practice (pp. 119–130). Chicago: Aldine.

Bagbey Darian, C. D. (2014). A new mourning: Synthesizing an interactive model of adaptive grieving dynamics. Illness, Crisis & Loss, 22, 195–235. doi:10.2190/IL.22.3.c

Bagnall, A.-M., Jones, R., Akter, H., & Woodall, J. (2016). Inter ventions to prevent burnout in high risk individuals: Evidence review. Retrieved from www.gov.uk/government/ uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/ f i l e / 5 0 6 7 7 7 / 2 5 0 2 2 0 1 6 _ B u r n o u t _ R a p i d _ Review_2015709.pdf.

Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 30, pp. 153–177). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2012). The role of intersensory redundancy in early perceptual, cognitive, and social development. In A.  Bremner, D. J. Lewkowicz, and C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 183–205). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R., Castellanos, I., & Todd, J. T. (2015). Intrasensory redundancy facilitates infant detection of tempo: Extending predictions of the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Infancy, 20, 377–404.

Bailey, J. A., Hill, K. G., Oesterle, S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2009). Parenting practices and problem behavior across three generations: Monitoring, harsh discipline, and drug use in the intergenerational transmission of externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1214–1226.

Bailey, J. M., Bechtold, K. T., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2002). Who are tomboys and why should we study them? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 333–341.

Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½- and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–664.

Baillargeon, R. (1994). How do infants learn about the physical world? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 133–140.

Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 159–186.

Baker, L . (1994). Fostering metacognitive development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 25, pp. 201–239). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Bakolis, I., Kelly, R., Fecht, D., Best, N. Millet, C., Garwood, K., et al. (2016). Protective effects of smoke-free legislation on birth outcomes in England: A regression discontinuity design. Epidemiology, 27, 810–818.

Balk, D. E. (2014). Dealing with dying, death, and grief during adolescence. New York: Routledge.

Ballard, R. H., Rudd, B. N., Applegate, A. G., & Holtzworth-Munroe, A. (2014). Hearing the voice of the child in divorce. In M. K. Miller, J. Chamberlain, & T. Wingrove (Eds.), Psychology, law, and the wellbeing of children (pp. 121–137). New York: Oxford University Press.

Baltes , B. B., & He ydens-Gahir, H. A . (2003). Reduction of work-family conflict through the use of selection, optimization, and comp ensation b ehav iors . Jour nal of Applied Psycholog y, 88, 1005–1018. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.1005

Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1999). Social- psychological theories and their applications to aging: From individual to collective. In V. L. Bengtson & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 209–226). New York: Springer.

Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. The Gerontologist, 33, 580–594. doi:10.1093/geront/33.5.580

Baltes , P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, o p t i m i z a t i o n , a n d c o m p e n s a t i o n a s foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380. doi:10.1037/0003- 066X .52.4.366

Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135. doi:10.1159/000067946

Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. Amer ic an P sy cholog i st, 55, 122–136. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122

Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 569–664). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Bambra, C. (2010). Yesterday once more? Unem- ployment and health in the 21st century. Jour- nal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 64, 213–215. doi:10.1136/jech.2009.090621

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

592 R e f e R e n c e s

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A . (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A., & Bussey, K. (2004). On broadening the cognitive, motivational, and sociostructural scope of theorizing about gender development and functioning: Comment on Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 130, 691–701.

Banerjee, K., & Bloom, P. (2015). “Everything happens for a reason”: Children’s beliefs about purpose in life events. Child Development, 86, 503–518.

Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four- word combinations. Psychological Science, 19, 241–248.

Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83, 413–422.

Barber, B. K., & Olsen, J. A. (1997). Socialization in context: Connection, regulation, and autonomy in the family, school, and neighborhood, and with peers. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12, 287–315.

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., & Grafman, J. (2014). Distributed neural system for emotional intelligence revealed by lesion mapping. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 265–272. doi:10.1093/scan/nss124

Barca, L., Ellis, A. W., & Burani, C. (2007). Context-sensitive rules and word naming in Italian children. Reading & Writing, 20, 495–509.

Barenboim, C. (1981). The development of person perception in childhood and adolescence: From behavioral comparisons to psychological constructs to psychological comparisons. Child Development, 52, 129–144.

Barker, E. D., Copeland, W., Maughan, B., Jaffee, S. R., & Uher, R. (2012). Relative impact of maternal depression and associated risk factors on offspring psychopathology. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 200, 124–129.

Barkley, R. A. (2004). Adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An overview of empirically based treatments. Journal of Psychiatric Review, 10, 39–56.

Barlow, M., Wrosch, C., Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (2017). Control strategies for managing physical health problems in old age: Evidence for the motivational theory of lifespan development. In F. J. Infurna & J. W. Reich (Eds.), Perceived control: Theory, research, and practice in the first 50 years (pp. ). New  York: Oxford University Press.

Barnett, A. E. (2013). Pathways of adult children providing care to older parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 75, 178–190. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01022.x

Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The Empathizing System: A revision of the 1994 model of the Mind Reading System. In B. J. Ellis &

D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 468–492). New York: Guilford.

Baron, A . S. (2015). Constraints on the development of implicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 50–54.

B aron, R ., Manniën, J., de Jonge, A ., Heymans,  M.  W., Klomp, T., Hutton, E. K., et al. (2013). Socio-demographic and lifestyle- related characteristics associated with self- reported any, daily and occasional smoking during pregnancy. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74197.

Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 1067–1081.

Barr, R., & Linebarger, D. N. (2017). The new blooming, buzzing confusion: Introduction to media exposure during infancy and early childhood. In R. Barr & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood (pp. xi–xv). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

Barrera, M., Alam, R ., D’Agostino, N. M., Nicholas, D. B., & Schneiderman, G. (2013). Parental perceptions of siblings’ grieving after a childhood cancer death: A longitudinal study. Death Studies, 37, 25–46. doi:10.1080/074811 87.2012.678262

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Li, N. (2013). The theory of purposeful work behavior: The role of personality, higher order goals, and job characteristics. Academy of Management Review, 38, 132–153. doi:10.5465/amr.2010.0479

Bartkowiak, G. (2017). Best practices in the employment of knowledge workers 65 and over and the benefits of employing them (An empirical approach). In M. H. Bilgin, H. Danis, E. Demir, & U. Can (Eds.), Financial environment and business development (pp. 463–471). New York: Springer.

Bartl, R ., Bartl, C., & Frisch, B. (2015). Osteoporosis: Diagnosis, prevention, therapy (3rd ed.). New York: Springer.

Bartlett, D. B., & Huffman, K. M. (2017). Lifetime interventions to improve immunesenescence. In V. Bueno, J. M. Lord, & T. A. Jackson (Eds.), The ageing immune system and health (pp. 161–176). New York: Springer.

Barton, C., Ketelle, R., Merrilees, J., & Miller, B. (2016). Non-pharmacological management of behavioral symptoms in frontotemporal and other dementias. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 16, 14. doi:10.1007/ s11910-015-0618-1

Barton, M. E., & Tomasello, M. (1991). Joint attention and conversation in mother-infant- sibling triads. Child Development, 62, 517–529.

Barzilai, N., Huffman, D. M., Muzumdar, R. H., & Bartke, A. (2012). The critical role of metabolic pathways in aging. Diabetes, 61, 1315–1322. doi:10.2337/db11-1300

Basso, K. H. (1970). The Cibecue Apache. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Baste, V., Oftedal, G., Møllerløkken, O. J., Hansson Mild, K., & Moen, B. E. (2015). Prospective study of pregnancy outcomes after parental cell phone exposure: The Norwegian

Mother and Child Cohort study. Epidemiology, 26, 613–621.

Basten, U., Hilger, K., & Fiebach, C. J. (2015). Where smart brains are different: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence, 51, 10–27. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.009

Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New  York: Cambridge University Press.

Bates, J. S. (2009). Generative grandfathering, commitment, and contact: How grandfathers nurture relationships with grandchildren and the relational and mental health benefits for aging men. Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the doctor of philosophy degree at Syracuse University.

Batt, M. E., Tanji, J., & Börjesson, M. (2013). Exercise at 65 and beyond. Sports Medicine, 43, 525–530. doi:10.1007/s40279-013-0033-1

Battista, R. N., Blancquaert, I., Laberge, A.-M., van Schendel, N., & Leduc, N. (2012). Genetics in health care: An overview of current and emerging models. Public Health Genomics, 15, 34–45. doi:10.1159/000328846

Baucom, B. R., Dickenson, J. A., Atkins, D. C., Baucom, D. H., Fischer, M. S., Weusthoff, S. et al. (2015). The interpersonal process model of demand/withdraw behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 29, 80–90. doi:10.1037/ fam0000044

Baude, A., Pearson, J., & Drapeau, S. (2016). Child adjustment in joint physical custody versus sole custody: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 57, 338–360.

Bauer, P. J. (2015). Development of episodic and autobiographical memory: The importance of remembering forgetting. Developmental Review, 38, 146–166.

Bauer, P. J., & Lukowski, A. F. (2010). The memory is in the details: Relations between memory for the specific features of events and long-term recall during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 1–14.

Bauer, P. J., San Souci, P., & Pathman, T. (2010). Infant memory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 267–277.

Baum, C. L., II, & Ruhm, C. J. (2016). The effects of paid family leave in California on labor market outcomes. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 35, 333–356. doi:10.1002/ pam.21894

Baumann, A., Claudot, F., Audibert, G., Mertes, P.-M., & Puybasset, M. (2011). The ethical and legal aspects of palliative sedation in severely brain-injured patients: A French perspective. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine, 6. Retrieved from preview.peh-med. com/content/pdf/1747-5341-6-4.pdf.

Baumeister, R. F. (2010). The self. In R. F. Baumeister & E. J. Finkel (Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science (pp. 139–175). New York: Oxford University Press.

Baumrind, D. (1975). Early socialization and the discipline controversy. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 593

Baumrind, D. (1991). Parenting styles and adolescent development. In R. M. Lerner, A. C. Petersen, & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (pp. 746–758). New York: Garland.

Baxter, A. J., Brugha, T. S., Erskine, H. E., Scheurer, R. W., Vos, T., & Scott, J. G. (2015). The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine, 45, 601–613.

Bayl-Smith, P. H., & Griffin, B. (2014). Age discrimination in the workplace: Identifying as a late career worker and its relationship with engagement and intended retirement age.  Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44, 588–599. doi:10.1111/jasp.12251

Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development: Manual. New York, New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Beal, C. R ., & Belgrad, S. L. (1990). The development of message evaluation skills in young children. Child Development, 61, 705–712.

Beauchamp, G. K., & Mennella, J. A. (2011). Flavor perception in human infants: Development and functional significance. Digestion, 83, 1–6.

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experi- mental, and theoretical aspects. New  York: Harper & Row.

Beck, A. T., Rush, J., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New  York: Guilford.

Beck, E., Burnet, K. L., & Vosper, J. (2006). Birth-order effects on facets of extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 953–959.

Beck, V. (2013). Employers’ use of older workers in the recession. Employee Relations, 35, 257–271. doi:10.1108/01425451311320468

Becker, B. J. (1986). Influence again: An exami- nation of reviews and studies of gender dif- ferences in social influence. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender differences. Advances through meta-analysis. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Becker, C. B., Perez, M., Kilpela, L. S., Diedrichs, P. C., Trujillo, E., & Stice, E. (in press). Engaging stakeholder communities as body image intervention partners: The Body Project as a case example. Eating Behaviors.

Beckes, L., & Coan, J. A . (2013). Voodoo versus me-you correlations in relationship ne uro s c ience. Jour nal of S o ci al and Per sonal Rel ation ship s, 30, 189–197. doi:10.1177/0265407512454768

Beckes, L., Coan, J. A., & Hasselmo, K. (2013). Familiarity promotes the blurring of self and other in the neural perception of threat. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 670–677. doi:10.1093/scan/nss046

Bedir, A., & Aksoy, S. (2011). Brain death revisited: It is not “complete death” according to Islamic sources. Journal of Medical Ethics, 37, 290–294. doi:10.1136/jme.2010.040238

Behl, C., & Ziegler, C. (2014). Cell aging : Molecular mechanisms and implications for disease. New York: Springer.

Behnke, A. O., Plunkett, S. W., Sands, T., & Bámaca-Colbert, M. Y. (2011). The relationship between Latino adolescents’ perceptions of discrimination, neighborhood risk, and parenting on self-esteem and depressive s y mptoms. Jour nal of Cro ss - Cultural Psychology, 42, 1179–1197.

Behnke, M., & Eyler, F. D. (1993). The consequences of prenatal substance use for the developing fetus, newborn, and young child. International Journal of the Addictions, 28, 1341–1391.

Belsky, G. (2007). Over the hill and between the sheets: Sex, love and lust in middle age. New York: Springboard Press.

Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304.

Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., Houts, R. M., Halpern- Felsher, B., and the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2010). The development of reproductive strategy in females: Early maternal harshness → earlier menarche → increased sexual risk taking. Developmental Psychology, 46, 120–128.

beneden health. (2013). Study reveals changing attitudes to middle age. Retrieved from www .benenden.co.uk/newsroom/research/research- archive/study-reveals-changing-attitudes-to- middle-age/.

Bengtson, V. L., Mills, T. L., & Parrott, T. M. (1995). Ageing in the United States at the end of the century. Korea Journal of Population and Development, 24, 215–244.

Bennett, K. M. (2005). Psychological wellbeing in later life: The longitudinal effects of marriage, widowhood and marital status change. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 280–284. doi:10.1002/ gps.1280

Bennett, K. M. (2010). How to achieve resilience as an older widower: Turning points or gradual change? Ageing and Society, 30, 369–382. doi:10.1017/S0144686X09990572

Bent, K. N., & Magilvy, J. K. (2006). When a partner dies: Lesbian widows. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 27, 447–459. doi:10.1080/01612840600599960

Benton, S. L., Corkill, A. J., Sharp, J. M., Downey, R. G., et al. (1995). Knowledge, interest, and narrative writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 66–79.

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Berg, C. A., Sewell, K. K., Hughes Lansing, A. E., Wilson, S. J., & Brewer, C. (2016). A developmental perspective to dyadic coping across adulthood. In J. Bookwala (Ed.), Couple relationships in the middle and later years: Their nature, complexity, and role in health and illness (pp. 259–280). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Bergdahl, E., Benzein, E., Ternestedt, B.-M., Elmberger, E., & Andershed, B. (2013). Co-creating possibilities for patients in

palliative care to reach vital goals—A multiple case study of home-care nursing encounters. Nursing Inquiry, 20, 341–351. doi:10.1111/ nin.12022

Berger, R. G., Lunkenbein, S., Ströhle, A., & Hahn, A. (2012). Antioxidants in food: Mere myth or magic medicine? Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 52, 162–171. doi:10.10 80/10408398.2010.499481

Berger, S. E., Adolph, K. E., & Lobo, S. A. (2005). Out of the toolbox: Toddlers differentiate wobbly and wo o den handrails . Child Development, 76, 1294–1307.

Bergman, K. N., Cummings, E. M., & Warmuth, K. A. (2016). The benefits of marital conflict: constructiveness and resolution as predictors of child positive outcomes. In D. Narvaez et al. (Eds.), Contexts for young children flourishing: Evolution, family, and society (pp. 233–244). New York: Oxford University Press.

Bergsma, A., & Ardelt, M. (2012). Self-reported wisdom and happiness : An empirical investigation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 481–499. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9275-5

Berk, L. E. (2003). Vygotsky, Lev. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (Vol. 6). London: Macmillan.

Berk, R. A. (2010). Where’s the chemistry in mentor-mentee academic relationships? Try speed mentoring. The International Journal of Mentoring and Coaching, 8, 85–92. Retrieved from www.ronberk.com/articles/2010_mentor .pdf.

Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.

Berkowitz, M. W., Sherblom, S., Bier, M., & Battistich, V. (2006). Educating for posi- tive youth development. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral devel- opment (pp. 683–702). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

B er mejo-Toro, L ., Pr ieto-Ursúa , M., & Hernández, V. (2016). Towards a model of teacher well-being: Personal and job resources involved in teacher burnout and engagement. Educational Psychology, 36, 481–501. doi:10.1 080/01443410.2015.1005006

Bernadotte, A., Mikhelson, V. M., & Spivak, I. M. (2016). Markers of cellular senescence. Telomere shortening as a marker of cellular senescence. Aging, 8, 3–11. www.ncbi.nlm.nih .gov/pmc/articles/PMC4761709/.

Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, S., & Christophe, A. (2010). Two-year-olds compute syntactic structure on-line. Developmental Science, 13, 69–76.

Berndt, T. J., & Murphy, L. M. (2002). Influences of friends and friendships: Myths, truths, and research recommendations. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30, 275–310.

Berndt, T. J., & Perry, T. B. (1990). Distinctive features and effects of adolescent friendships. In R. Montemayer, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), From childhood to adolescence: A transition period? London: Sage.

Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

594 R e f e R e n c e s

executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 326–339.

Berns, N. (2011). Closure: The rush to end grief and what it costs. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

Berry, J., Hastings, E., West, R., Lee, C., & Cavanaugh, J. C. (2010). Memory aging: Deficits , beliefs , and inter ventions . In J.  C.  Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 1: Psychological Aspects (pp. 255–299). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.

Bertenthal, B. H., & Clifton, R. K. (1998). Perception and action. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology ( Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

Best, J. R. (2013). Exergaming in youth: Effects on physical and cognitive health. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 221, 72–78.

Beuker, K. T., Rommelse, N. N. J., Donders, R., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2013). Development of early communication skills in the first two years of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 71–83.

Bever, L. (2014a). Cancer patient Brittany Maynard, 29, has scheduled her death for Nov. 1. Washington, Post. Retrieved from http:// wapo.st/10O22rQ.

Bever, L. (2014b). Brittany Maynard, as promised, ends her life at 29. Washington Post. Retrieved from http://wapo.st/1yPi6F0.

Bever, L. (2014c). How Brittany Maynard may change the right-do-die debate. Washington Post. Retrieved from http://wapo.st/1A3F9QR.

Bhatt, R. S., Hock, A., White, H., Jubran, R., & Galati, A . (2016). The development of body structure knowledge in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 45–52.

Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560–567.

Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93–105.

Bian, F., Blachford, D., & Durst, D. (2015). The color purple: Perspectives of Canadian parents of adopted children from China. Journal of Comparative Social Work, 10(2). Retrieved from journal.uia.no/index.php/JCSW/article/ view/345/289.

Biblarz, T. J., & Savci, E. (2010). Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 480–497. doi:10.1111/j.1741 -3737.2010.00714.x

Bidwell, M. (2013). What happened to long- term employment? The role of worker power and environmental turbulence in explaining declines in worker tenure. Organization Science, 24, 1061–1082. doi:10.1287/orsc.1120.0816

Biggs , S., McGann, M., Bowman, D., & Kimberley, H. (2017). Work, health and the commodification of life’s time: Reframing work-life balance and the promise of a long life. Ageing and Society, 37, 1458–1483. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X16000404

Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing

children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166.

Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 68, 530–543.

Bingenheimer, J. B., Brennan, R. T., & Earls, F. J. (2005). Firearm violence exposure and serious violent behavior. Science, 308, 1323–1326.

Bird, E. R., Gilmore, A. K., George, W. H., & Lewis, M. A . (2016). The role of social drinking factors in the relationship between incapacitated sexual assault and drinking before sexual activity. Addictive Behaviors, 52, 28–33. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.08.001

Bird, S., Burke, L., Goebel, C., & Greaves, R. (2015). Doping in sport and exercise: Anabolic, ergogenic, health and clinical issues. Annals of Clinical Biochemistry, 53, 196–221.

Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., Mizrahi, M., Kanat- Maymon, Y., Sass, O., Granovski-Milner, C. (2016). Intimately connected: The importance of partner responsiveness for experiencing sexual desire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 530–546. doi:10.1037/ pspi0000069

Bjerkedal, T., Kristensen, P., Skjeret, G. A., & Brevik, J. I. (2007). Intelligence test scores and birth order among young Norwegian men (conscripts) analyzed within and between families. Intelligence, 35, 503–514.

Bjork, J. M., & Gilman, J. M. (2014). The effects of acute alcohol administration on the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Neurophar- macology, 84, 101–110. doi:10.1016/j.neuro pharm.2013.07.039

Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Children’s thinking : Cog nitive de velopment and individual differences (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Bjorklund, D. F., & Jordan, A. C. (2013). Human parenting from an evolutionary perspective. In W. B. Wilcox & K. K. Kline (Eds.), Gender and parenthood: Biological and social scientific perspectives (pp. 61–90). New York: Columbia University Press.

Bjorklund, D. F., Yunger, J. L., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The evolution of parenting and evolutionary approaches to childrearing. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol.  2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 3–30). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Blachnio, A., Przepiorka, A., Balakier, E., & Boruch, W. (2016). Who discloses the most on Facebook? Computers in Human Behavior, 55 Part B, 664–667. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.007

Black, J. E. (2003). Environment and development of the nervous system. In I. B. Weiner, M. Gallagher, & R. J. Nelson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 3: Biological psychology (pp. 655–668). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Blackhall, L. J., Read, P., Stukenborg, G., Dillon, P., Barclay, J., Romano, A. et al. (2016). CARE Track for advanced cancer: Impact and timing of an outpatient palliative clinic. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 19, 57–63. doi:10.1089/ jpm.2015.0272

Blackstone, A., & Stewart, M. D. (2016). “There’s more thinking to decide”: How the childfree decide not to parent. The Family Journal, 24, 296–303. doi:10.1177/1066480716648676

Blair, B. L., Perry, N. B., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2014). The indirect effects of maternal emotion socialization on friendship quality in middle childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50, 566–576.

Blair, C. (2016). Executive function and early childhood education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 102–107.

Blakemore, J. E. O. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys. Sex Roles, 48, 411–419.

Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychologi- cal Science, 16, 26–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721 .2007.00469.x

Blanchard-Fields, F., Baldi, R. A., & Constantin, L. P. (2004). Interrole conflict across the adult lifespan: The role of parenting stage, career stages and quality of experiences. Unpublished manuscript, School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology.

Blieszner, R. (2014). The worth of friendship: Can friends keep us happy and healthy? Generations, 38, 24–30.

Blieszner, R., & Roberto, K. A. (2012). Partners and friends in adulthood. In S. K. Whitbourne & M. J. Sliwinski (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell h a n d b o o k o f a d u l t h o o d a n d a g i n g (pp. 381–398). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Bligh, M. C. (2017). Leadership and trust. In J.  Marques & S. Dhiman (Eds.), Leadership today (pp. 21–42). New York: Springer.

Bloom, L., & Tinker, E. (2001). The intentionality model and language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66 (Serial No. 267).

Bloom, L., Margulis, C., Tinker, E., & Fujita, N. (1996). Early conversations and word learning: Contributions from child and adult. Child Development, 67, 3154–3175.

Bloom, L., Rocissano, L., & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child di s cours e: D e velopment al interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 521–552.

Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Blume-Peytavi, U., Kottner, J., Sterry, W., Hodin, M. W., Griffiths, T. W., Watson, R. E. B., et al. (2016). Age-associated skin conditions and diseases: Current perspectives and future options. The Gerontologist, 56, S230–S242. doi:10.1093/geront/gnw003

Blümel, J. E., Chedraui, P., Baron, G., Belzares, E., Bencosme, A ., Calle, A . et al. (2012). Menopausal symptoms appear before the menopause and persist 5 years beyond: A detailed analysis of a multinational study. Climacteric, 15, 542–551. doi:10.3109/136971 37.2012.658462

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 595

Bluntschli, J. R., Maxfield, M. M., Grasso, R. L., & Kisley, M. A. (in press). The last word: A comparison of younger and older adults’ brain responses to reminders of death. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbv115

Boals, A., Hayslip, B., & Banks, J. B. (2014). Age differences in autobiographical memories of negative events. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 78, 47–65. doi:10.2190/AG.78.1.d

Böckerman, P., Ilmakunnas, P. Jokisaari, M., & Yuori, J. (2013). Who stays unwillingly in a job? A study based on a representative random sample of employees. Economic and Industrial Democracy, 34, 25–43. doi:10.1177/0143831X11429374

Boehm, S. A., Dwertmann, D. J. G., Kunze, F., Michaelis, B., Parks, K. M., & McDonald, D. P. (2014). Expanding insights on the diversity climate-performance link : The role of workgroup discrimination and group size. Human Resource Management, 53, 379–402. doi:10.1002/hrm.21589ER

Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012). The construc- tion of emotion in interactions, relationships, and cultures. Emotion Review, 4, 221–229.

Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Gagnon, C. (1992). A reassessment of the self-perception profile for children: Factor structure, reliability, and con- vergent validity of a French version among sec- ond through sixth grade children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 275–290.

Boldt, L. J., Kochanska, G., Grekin, R ., & Brock,  R.  L. (2016). Attachment in middle childho o d: Pre dictors , correlates , and implications for adaptation. Attachment and Human Development, 18, 115–140.

Bolton, J. M., Au, W., Chateau, D., Walld, R., Leslie, W. D., Enns, J. et al. (2016). Bereavement after sibling death: A population-based longitudinal case-control study. World Psychiatry, 15, 59–66. doi:10.1002/wps.20293

Bonanno, G. A. (2009). The other side of sadness: What the new science of bereavement tells us about life after loss. New York: Basic Books.

Bonanno, G. A ., Papa, A ., & O’Neill, K . (2001). Loss and human resilience. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 10, 193–206. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(01)80014-7

Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Zhang, N., & Noll, J. G. (2005). Grief processing and deliberate grief avoidance: A prospective comparison of b ereave d sp ouses and parents in the United States and the People’s Republic of China. Journal of Consulting and C linic al P sy cholog y, 73, 86–98. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.86

Bonanno, G. A., Westphal, M., & Mancini, A. D. (2011). Resilience to loss and trauma. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 511–535. doi:10.1146/annurevclinpsy-032210-104526

Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., & Neese, R. M. (2004). Prospective patterns of resilience and maladjustment during widowhood. P sy cholog y and Ag ing, 19, 260–271. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.260

Booker, J. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2016). Profiles of wisdom among emerging adults: Associations with empathy, gratitude, and forgiveness. Journal of Positive Psychology, 11, 315–325. doi :10.1080/17439760.2015.1081970

Booker, J. A ., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2017). Affective social competence in adolescence: Current findings and future directions. Social Development, 26, 3–20.

Boon, H., Ruiter, R. U. C., James, S., van den Borne,  B., Williams, E., & Reddy, P. (2010). Correlates of grief among older adults caring for children and grandchildren as a consequence of HIV and AIDS in South Africa. Journal of Aging and Health, 22, 48–67. doi:10.1177/0898264309349165

Booth, J. N., Leary, S. D., Joinson, C., Ness, A. R., Tomporowski, P. D. Boyle, J. M. et al. (2014). Associations between objectively measured physical activity and academic attainment in adolescents from a UK cohort. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48, 265–270.

Bordere, T. C. (2016). Social justice conceptual- izations in grief and loss. In D. L. Harris & T. C. Bordere (Eds.), Handbook of social justice in loss and grief: Exploring diversity, equity, and inclusion (pp. 9–20). New York: Routledge.

B ornstein, M. H., & Arterberr y, M. E . (2003). Recognition, discrimination, and categorization of smiling by 5-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 6, 585–599.

Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Gartstein, M. A., Hahn, C-S., Auestad, N., & O’Connor, D. L. (2015). Infant temperament: Stability by age, gender, birth order, term status, and SES. Child Development, 86, 844–863.

Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Suwalsky, J. T., & Gini, M. (2006). Maternal chronological age, prenatal and perinatal history, social support, and parenting of infants. Child Development, 77, 875–892.

Bortfeld, H., & Morgan, J. L. (2010). Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 241–266.

Bos, H. M., Knox, J. R., van Rijn-van Gelderen, L., & Gartrell, N. K. (2016). Same-sex and different-sex parent households and child health outcomes: Findings from the National Sur vey of Children’s Health. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 37, 179–187. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000288

Boschma, J. (2015, June 23). When do Americans think they’ll actually retire? The Atlantic. Retrieved from www.theatlantic.com/business/ archive/2015/06/ideal-retirement-age-work/ 396464/.

Bosco, A., di Masi, M. N., & Manuti, A. (2013). Burnout internal factors—self-esteem and nega- tive affectivity in the workplace: The mediation role of organizational identification in times of job uncertainty. In S. Bährer-Kohler (Ed.), Burn- out for experts (pp. 145–158). New York: Springer.

Boseovski, J. J. (2010). Evidence for “rose-colored glasses”: An examination of the positivity bias in young children’s personality judgments. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 212–218.

Bosma , H. A ., & Kunnen, E . S. (2001). Determinants and mechanisms in ego identity development: A review and synthesis. Developmental Review, 21, 39–66.

Boss, P. (2010). The trauma and complicated grief of ambiguous loss. Pastoral Psychology, 59, 137–145. doi:10.1007/s11089-009-0264-0

Boss, P. (2015). Coping with the suffering of ambiguous loss. In R . E. Anderson (Ed.), World suffering and quality of life (pp. 125–134). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9670-5_10

Bosshard, G., & Materstvedt, L. J. (2011). Medical and societal issues in euthanasia and assisted suicide. In R. Chadwick, H. Ten Have, & E.  M.  Meslin (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of health care ethics (pp. 202–218). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Boswell, J., Thai, J., & Brown, C. (2015). Older adults’ sleep. In A. Green & C. Brown (Eds.), An occupational therapist’s guide to sleep and sleep problems (pp. 185–206). Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Bouchard, T. J. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148–151.

Bouchard, T. J. (2009). Genetic influence on human intelligence (Spearman’s g): How much? Annals of Human Biology, 36, 527–544.

Bowker, A. (2006). The relationship between sports participation and self-esteem during early adolescence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 38, 214–229.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.

B owlby, J. (1991) Etholog ic al light on psychoanalytical problems. In P. Bateson (Ed.), The development and integration of behaviour: Essays in honour of Robert Hinde (pp. 301–313). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bows, H. (in press). Sexual violence against older people: A review of the empirical literature. Trauma , Violence , & Abuse. Retrieved from journals.sagepub.com/doi/ full/10.1177/1524838016683455. doi:10.1177/ 1524838016683455

Boyce, W. T., & Kobor, M. S. (2015). Development and the epigenome: The “synapse” of gene- environment interplay. Developmental Science, 18, 1–23.

Boyczuk, A. M., & Fletcher, P. C. (2016). The ebbs and flows: Stresses of sandwich generation caregivers. Journal of Adult Development, 23, 51–61. doi:10.1007/s10804-015-9221-6

Boyle, P. J., Feng, Z., & Raab, G. M. (2011). Does widowhood increase mortality risk? Testing for selection effects by comparing causes of spousal death. Epidemiology, 22, 1–5. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181fdcc0b

Bradshaw, C. P. (2015). Translating research to practice in bullying prevention. American Psychologist, 70, 322–332.

Brainerd, C. J. (1996). Piaget: A centennial celebration. Psychological Science, 7, 191–203.

Brancucci, A . (2012). Neural correlates of cognitive ability. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 90, 1299–1309. doi:10.1002/jnr.23045

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

596 R e f e R e n c e s

Brandtstädter, J. (1989). Personal self-regulation of development: Cross-sequential analyses of development-related control beliefs and emotions. Developmental Psychology, 25, 96–108. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.96

Brandtstädter, J. (1997). Action culture and de velopment : Point s of convergence. Culture and P sycholog y, 3, 335–352. doi:10.1177/1354067X9733007

Brandtstädter, J. (1999). Sources of resilience in the aging self. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard- Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (pp. 123–141). San Diego: Academic Press.

Bratkovich, K. L. (2010). The relationship of attachment and spirituality with posttraumatic growth following a death for college students. Doctoral dissertation submitted to the Department of Psychology at Oklahoma State University.

Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791–804.

Braungart, J. M., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Genetic influence on tester-rated infant temperament as assessed by Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record: Nonadoptive and adoptive siblings and twins. Developmental Psychology, 28, 40–47.

Braver, S. L., & Lamb, M. E. (2013). Marital dissolution. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 487–516). New York: Springer.

Braza, P., Carreras, R., Muñoz, J. M., Braza, F., Azurmendi, A., Pascual-Sagastizábal, E., et al. (2015). Negative maternal and paternal parenting styles as predictors of children’s behavioral problems: Moderating effects of the child’s sex. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 847–856.

Brazelton, T. B., & Nugent, J. K. (1995). Neonatal behavioral assessment scale (3rd ed.). London: Mac Keith.

Brechwald, W. A., & Prinstein, M. J. (2011). Beyond homophily: A decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 166–179.

Brehmer, Y., Shing, Y. L., Heekeren, H. R., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2016). Training-induced changes in subsequent- memory effects: No major differences among children, younger adults, and older adults. NeuroImage, 131, 214–225. doi:10.1016/ j.neuroimage.2015.11.074

Bremner, J. G., Slater, A. M., & Johnson, S. P. (2015). Perception of object persistence: The origins of object permanence in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 7–13.

Bremner, J. G., Slater, A. M., Mason, U. C., Spring, J., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Trajectory perception and object continuity: Effects of shape and color change on 4-month-olds’ perception of trajectory identity. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1021–1026.

Brendgen, M., Lamarche, V., Wanner, B., & Vitaro,  F. (2010). Links between friendship relations and early adolescents’ trajectories of depressed mood. Developmental Psychology, 46, 491–501.

Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionea, G., & Perusse, D. (2006). Examining genetic and environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1299–1312.

Brenn, T., & Ytterstad, E. (2016). Increased risk of death immediately after losing a spouse: Cause-specific mortality following widowhood in Norway. Preventive Medicine, 89, 251–256. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.019

Brenner, J. S. (2016). Sports specialization and intensive training in young athletes. Pediatrics, 138, e20162148.

Bretherton, I. (2010). Fathers in attachment theory and research: a review. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 9–23.

Brissette, I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of optimism in social network development, coping, and psychological adjustment during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 102–111. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.102

Brockington, I. (1996). Motherhood and mental health. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Brody, G. H. (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and consequences. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 1–24.

Brody, G. H., Kim, S., Murry, V. M., & Brown, A. C. (2003). Longitudinal direct and indirect pathways linking older sibling competence to the development of younger sibling competence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 618–628.

Brody, G. H., Stoneman, A., & McCoy, J. K. (1994). Forecasting sibling relationships in early adolescence from child temperament and family processes in middle childhood. Child Development, 65, 771–784.

Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Barton, A. W., & Miller, G. E. (2017). Youth temperament, harsh parenting, and variation in the oxytocin receptor gene forecast allostatic load during emerging adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 29, in press.

Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Bromell, L., & Cagney, K. A. (2014). Compan- ionship in the neighborhood context: Older adults’ living arrangements and perceptions of social cohesion. Research on Aging, 36, 228–243. doi:10.1177/0164027512475096

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (2006). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 793–828). New York: Wiley.

Brooker, R. J., Buss, K. A., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Aksan, N., Davidson, R. J., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2013). The development of stranger fear in infancy and toddlerhood: normative d e v e l o p m e nt , i n d i v i d u a l d i f f e r e n c e s , antecedents, and outcomes. Developmental Science, 16, 864–878.

Brooks-Gunn, J., & Paikoff, R. (1993). “Sex is a gamble, kissing is a game”: Adolescent sexuality, contraception, and sexuality. In S. P. Millstein, A. C. Petersen, & E. O. Nightingale (Eds.), Promoting the health behavior of adolescents (pp. 180–208). New York: Oxford University Press.

Brooks-Gunn, J., & Ruble, D. N. (1982). The development of menstrual-related beliefs and behaviors during early adolescence. Child Development, 53, 1567–1577.

Brown, B. B. (2014). An autobiographical journey through adolescents’ social world: Peer groups, peer influence, and the effects of electronic media on social adjustment in college. In R.  M.  Lerner et al. (Eds.), The developmental science of adolescence: History through biography (pp. 45–54). New York: Psychology Press.

Brown, B. B., Bakken, J. P., Ameringer, S. W., & Mahon, S. D. (2008). A comprehensive con- ceptualization of the peer influence process in adolescence. In M. J. Prinstein & K. Dodge (Eds.), Understanding peer influences in chil- dren and adolescents (pp. 17–44). New York: Guilford Publications.

Brown, D. A., & Lamb, M. E. (2015). Can children be useful witnesses? It depends how they are questioned. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 250–255.

Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding from three to six years. Child Development, 67, 789–802.

Brown, J. W., Chen, S-L., Mefford, L., Brown, A., Callen, B., & McArthur, P. (2011). Becoming an older volunteer: A grounded theor y study. Nursing Research and Practice, 2011. doi:10.1155/2011/361250

Brown, J., & Dunn, J. (1992). Talk with your mother or your sibling? Developmental changes in early family conversations about feelings. Child Development, 63, 336–349.

Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (2016). Vocational psychology: Agency, equity, and well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 541–565. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033237

Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Orobio de Casto, B., Overbeek, G., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). “That’s not just beautiful—that’s incredibly beautiful!” The adverse impact of inflated praise on children with low self-esteem. Psychological Science, 25, 728–735.

Brymer, E., & Mackenzie, S. H. (2017). Psychology and the extreme sport experience. In F. Feletti (Ed.), Extreme sports medicine (pp. 3–13). New York: Springer.

Brysiewicz, P. (2008). The lived experience of losing a loved one to a sudden death in KwaZulu- Natal, South Africa. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 17, 224–231. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2702 .2007.01972.x

Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J. S., & Becker, J. B. (1992). Are adolescents the victims of raging hormones? Evidence for activational effects of hormones on moods and behavior at adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 62–107.

Buckingham-Howes, S., Berger, S. S., Scaletti, L. A., & Black, M. M. (2013). Systematic review of

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 597

prenatal cocaine exposure and adolescent development. Pediatrics, 131, e1917-d1936.

Budgeon, S. (2016). The “problem” with single women: Choice, accountability and social change. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 33, 401–418. doi:10.1177/026540 7515607647

Buhl, H. (2008). Development of a model describing individuated adult child-parent relationships . International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 381–389. doi:10.1177/0165025408093656

Bui, E., Mauro, C., Robinaugh, D. J., Skritskaya, N. A., Wang, Y., Gribbin, C. et al. (2015). The Structured Clinical Interview for Complicated Grief: Reliability, validity, and explanatory factor analysis. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 485–492. doi:10.1002/da.22385

Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L. K., & Hoza, B. (1999). Same and other: Interdependency between participation in same- and other-sex friendships. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 28, 439–459.

Bullard, L., Wachlarowicz, M., DeLeeuw, J., Snyder, J., Low, S., Forgatch, M., et al. (2010). Effects of the Oregon model of Parent Management Training (PMTO) on marital adjustment in new stepfamilies: A randomized trial. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 485–496.

Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1990). Who am I? The development of self-understanding in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217–238.

B u r e a u o f L a b o r S t a t i s t i c s . ( 2 0 1 6 a ) . Unemployment rates by age, sex, race, and Hispanic or Latino ethnicity. Retrieved from www.bls.gov/web/empsit/cpsee_e16.htm.

Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2016b). Families by presence and relationship of employed members and family type, 2014–2015 annual averages. Retrieved from www.bls.gov/news.release/ famee.t02.htm.

Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2016c). Employment characteristics of families—2015. Retrieved from www.bls.gov/news.release/pdf/famee.pdf.

Burk, W. J., & Laursen, B. (2005). Adolescent perceptions of friendship and their associations with individual adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 156–164.

Burke, B. L., Martens, A., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 155–195. doi:10.1177/1088868309352321

Burke, R., & Kao, G. (2013). Bearing the burden of whiteness: The implications of racial self- identification for multiracial adolescents’ school belonging and academic achievement. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 36, 747–773.

Burnette, D. (1999). Social relationships of Latino grandparent caregivers: A role theory perspective. The Gerontologist, 39, 49–58. doi:10.1093/ geront/39.1.49

Burns, S. T. (2005). The transition to parenthood: The effects of gender role attitudes and level of culture change in Chinese-American couples.

Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 66(6-B), 3461.

Burton, L. M. (1992). Black grandparents rearing children of drug-addicted parents: Stressors, outcomes, and social service needs. The Gerontologist, 32, 744–751. doi:10.1093/geront/ 32.6.744

Buss, D. (2016). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (5th ed.). New  York: Routledge.

Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A ., Asherian, A., Biaggio, A., Blanco-Villasenor, A., et al. (1990). International preferences in selecting mates: A study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 5–47. doi:10.1177/0022022190211001

Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359–374.

Buss, K. A., & Kiel, E. J. (2004). Comparison of sadness, anger, and fear facial expressions when toddlers look at their mothers. Child Development, 75, 1761–1773.

Bustos, M. L. C. (2007). La muerte en la cultura occidental: Antropología de la muerte [Death in Western culture: Anthropology of death]. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría, 36, 332–339. Retrieved from www.redalyc.org/ articulo.oa?id=80636212.

Buttelmann, D., & Böhm, R. (2014). The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science, 25, 921–927.

Byock, S. D. (2010, August). The quarterlife crisis and the path to individuation in the first half of life. Master’s thesis completed at the Pacifica Graduate Institute, Carpinteria, CA.

Byrnes, J. P., & Dunbar, K. N. (2014). The nature and development of critical-analytic thinking. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 477–493.

Bywaters, P., Bunting, L., Davidson, G., Hanratty, J., Mason, W., McCartan, C., et al. (2016). The relationship between poverty, child abuse and neglect: An evidence review. York, UK: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.

Cabeza de Baca, T., & Ellis, B. J. (2017). Early stress, parental motivation, and reproductive decision-making: applications of life history theory to parental behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 1–6.

Cable, N., & Sacker, A. (2008). Typologies of alcohol consumption in adolescence: Predictors and adult outcomes. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 43, 81–90.

Cacciatore, J., & DeFrain, J. (2015). The world of bereavement. New York: Springer.

Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Hawkley, H. C. (2011). Could an aging brain contribute to subjective well-being? The value added by a social neuroscience perspective. In A. Todorov, S. Fiske, & D. Prentice (Eds.), Social neuroscience: toward understanding the underpinnings of the social mind (pp. 249–262). New York: Oxford University Press.

Cacioppo, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2013). Lust for life. Scientific American Mind, 24(5), 56–63.

Cadell, S., & Marshall, S. (2007). The (re) construction of self after the death of a partner to HIV/AIDS. Death Studies, 31, 537–548. doi:10.1080 /07481180701356886

Cahill, E., Lewis, L. M., Barg, F. K., & Bogner, H. R. (2009). “You don’t want to burden them”: Older adults’ views on family involvement in care. Journal of Family Nursing, 27, 295–317. doi:10.1177/1074840709337247

Cain, K . (1999). Ways of reading: How knowledge and use of strategies are related to reading comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 293–312.

Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T. et al. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, Serial No. 299.

Callahan, C. M. (2000). Intelligence and giftedness. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 159–175). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Calvert, S. L. (2015). Children and digital media. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol. 4, pp. 375–415). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Calvert, S. L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2013). The influence of television, video games, and the Internet on children’s creativity. In M. Taylor (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination (pp. 438–450). New York: Oxford.

Calvert, S. L., Appelbaum, M., Dodge, K. A., Graham, S. Nagayama Hall, G. C., Hamby, S. et al. (2017). The American Psychological Association Task Force assessment of violent video games: Science in the service of public interest. American Psychologist, 72, 126–143.

Calvete, E., Fernández-González, L., Orue, I., & Little, T. D. (2017). Exposure to family violence and dating violence perpetration in adolescents: Potential cognitive and emotional mechanisms. Psychology of Violence, in press.

Camberis, A.L., McMahon, C. A., Gibson, F. L., & Bolvin, J. (2016). Maternal age, psychological maturity, parenting cognitions, and mother- infant interaction. Infancy, 21, 396–422. doi:10.1111/infa.1211

Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Brown, R., & Douch, R. (2006). Changing children’s intergroup attitudes toward refugees: Testing different models of extended contact. Child Development, 77, 1208–1219.

Camp, C. J. (1999a). Memory interventions for normal and pathological older adults. In R. Schulz, M. P. Lawton, & G. Maddox (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 18, pp. 155–189). New York: Springer.

Camp, C. J. (2005). Spaced retrieval: A model for dissemination of a cognitive intervention for persons with dementia. In D. K. Attix & K. A . Velsh-Bohmer (Eds.). Geriatric neuropsychology: Assessment and intervention (pp. 275–292). New York: Guilford Press.

Camp, C. J. (Ed.). (1999b). Montessori-based activities for persons with dementia (Vol. 1). Beachwood, OH: Menorah Park Center for Senior Living.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

598 R e f e R e n c e s

Camp, C. J., Foss, J. W., Stevens, A. B., Reichard, C. C., McKitrick, L. A., & O’Hanlon,  A. M. (1993). Memory training in normal and demented elderly populations: The E-I-E- I-O model. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 277–290. doi: 10.1080/03610739308253938

Camp, C. J., Skrajner, M. J., & Kelly, M. (2005). Early stage dementia client as group leader. Clinical Gerontologist, 28, 81–85. doi:10.1300/ J018v28n04_06

Campbell, J., & Ehlert, U. (2012). Acute psychoso- cial stress: Does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 1111–1134. doi: 10.1016/j. psyneuen.2011.12.010

Campbell, K., & Kaufman, J. (2015). Do you pursue your heart or your art? Creativity, personality, and love. Journal of Family Issues, 38:3, 287-311. doi:10.1177/0192513X15570318

Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 61–68.

Campinha-Bacote, J. (2010). A culturally conscious model of mentoring. Nurse Educator, 35, 130–135. doi:10.1097/NNE.0b013e3181d950bf

Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.

Campos, J. J., Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svedja, M. (1978). The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The origins of affect. New York: Plenum.

Camras, L. A., & Shuster, M. (2013). Children’s expressive behavior in different cultural contexts . In D. Hermans , B. Rimé, & B.  Mesquita (Eds.), Changing emotions (pp. 24–30). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Candel, I., Hayne, H., Strange, D., & Prevoo, E. (2009). The effect of suggestion on children’s recognition memory for seen and unseen details. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 15, 29–39.

Cappell, K. A., Gmeindl, L., & Reuter-Lorenz, P.A. (2010). Age differences in prefrontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex, 46, 462–473. doi:10.1016/j.cortex .2009.11.009

Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., et al. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678–686.

Cardoso-Leite, P., & Bavelier, D. (2014). Video game play, attention, and learning: how to shape the development of attention and influence learning? Current Opinion in Neurology, 27, 185–191.

Carlander, I., Ternestedt, B.-M., Sahlberg-Blom, E., Hellström, I., & Sandberg, J. (2011). Being me and being us in a family living close to death at home. Qualitative Health Research, 5, 683–695. doi: 10.1177/1049732310396102

Carlo, G., Koller, S., Raffaelli, M., & de Guzman, M. R. T. (2007). Culture-related strengths among Latin American families: A case study

of Brazil. Marriage & Family Review, 41, 335–360. doi:10.1300/ J002v41n03_06

Carlo, G., McGinley, M., Hayes, R., Batenhorst, C., & Wilkinson, J. (2007). Parenting styles or practices? Parenting, sympathy, and prosocial behaviors among adolescents. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 168, 147–176.

Carlson Jones, D. (2004). Body image among adolescent girls and boys: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 823–835.

Carlson, M. D., Mendle, J., & Harden, K. P. (2014). Early adverse environments and genetic influences on age at first sex: Evidence for gene × environment interaction. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1532–1542.

Carnelley, K., & Ruscher, J. B. (2000). Adult attachment and exploratory behavior in leisure. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 153–165. Retrieved from crawl.prod. proquest.com.s3.amazonaws.com/fpcache/ cdd5594c53a7864881fb71e54a7422f1.pdf?A WSAccessKeyId=AKIAJF7V7KNV2KKY2NU Q&Expires=1476725696&Signature=EiFM03 6jVPsv4iNCNcf8At5HlAY%3D.

Carnevale, A. P., Jayasundera, T., & Gulish, A. (2016). America’s divided recovery: College haves and have-nots. Retrieved from cew.georgetown. edu/cew-reports/americas-divided-recovery/.

Caron, C. D., Ducharme, F., & Griffith, J. (2006). Deciding on institutionalization for a relative with dementia: The most difficult decision for caregivers. Canadian Journal on Aging, 25, 193–205. doi:10.1353/cja.2006.0033

Carpenter, P. A., & Daneman, M. (1981). Lexical retrieval and error recovery in reading: A model based on eye fixations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 137–160.

Carpenter, R., McGarvey, C., Mitchell, E. A., Tappin, D. M., Vennemann, M. M., Smuk, M., et al. (2013). Bedsharing when parents do not smoke: Is there a risk of SIDS? An individual level analysis of five major case-control studies. British Medical Journal Open, 3:e002299.

Carr, A., & McNulty, M. (2016). Cognitive behaviour therapy. In A. Carr & M. McNulty (Eds .), The handbook of adult clinical psychology (2nd ed.). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Carr, D. (2004). Gender, preloss marital depen- dence, and older adults’ adjustment to wid- owhood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 220–235. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00016.x

Carr, D., & Moorman, S. M. (2011). Social relations and aging. In R. A. Settersten & J.  L.  Angel (Eds.), Handbook of sociology of aging (pp. 145–160). New York: Springer.

Carré, J. M., McCormick, C. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2011). The social neuroendocrinology of human aggression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 935–944.

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Carroll, J. B. (1996). A three-stratum theory of intelligence: Spearman’s contribution. In I. Dennis & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Their nature and measurement (pp. 1–18). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Carroll, J. L., & Loughlin, G. M. (1994). Sudden infant death syndrome. In F. A. Oski, C. D. DeAngelis, R. D. Feigin, J. A. McMillan, & J. B. Warshaw (Eds.), Principles and practice of pediatrics. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312, 1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488

Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 17–39.

Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2003). National estimates of adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior : Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 23–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V. (2012). Early action and gesture “vocabulary” and its relation with word comprehension and production. Child Development, 83, 526–542.

Casey, B. J. (2015). Beyond simple models of self- control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 295–319.

Casey, B. J., & Caudle, K. (2013). The teenage brain: Self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 82–87.

Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 104–110.

Cash, S. J., & Bridge, J. A. (2009). Epidemiology of youth suicide and suicidal behavior. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 21, 613–619.

Casper, W. J., Marquardt, D. J., Roberto, K. J., & Buss, C. (2016). The hidden family lives of single adults without dependent children. In T. D. Allen & L. T. Eby (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of work and family (pp. 182–195). New York: Oxford University Press.

Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. (2005). Personality development. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484. doi:10.1146/annurev .psych.55.090902.141913

Castro, A. S. (2010). The rite to womanhood: An inter- disciplinary study of female circumcision among the Gikuyu of Kenya. Bachelor’s degree thesis completed at Wesleyan University. Retrieved from wesscholar.wesleyan.edu/cgi/viewcontent. cgi?article=1516&context=etd_hon_theses.

Cattell, R. B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality. Baltimore, MD: Penguin.

Cauvet, E., Limissuri, R., Millotte, S., Skoruppa, K., Cabrol, D., & Christophe, A. (2014). Function words constrain on-line recognition of verbs and nouns in French 18-month-olds. Language Learning and Development, 10, 1–18.

Cavanaugh, J. C. (1999). Caregiving to adults: A life event challenge. In I. H. Nordhus, G. R. VandenBos, S. Berg, & P. Fromholt (Eds.), Clinical geropsychology (pp. 131–135). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Cavanaugh, J. C. (in press). Spirituality as a framework for confronting life’s existential questions in later adulthood. In J. D. Sinnott

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 599

(Ed.), Identity flexibility during adulthood: Perspectives on adult development. New York: Springer.

Cavanaugh, J. C., & Kinney, J. M. (1994, July). Marital satisfaction as an important contextual factor in spousal caregiving. Paper presented at the 7th International Conference on Personal Relationships, Groningen, The Netherlands.

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1998). Children’s testimony: Applied and basic issues. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.

Census Bureau. (2013). Valentine’s Day 2013: Feb.  14. Retrieved from www.census.gov/ n e w s r o o m / r e l e a s e s / a r c h i v e s / f a c t s _ f o r _ features_special_editions/cb13-ff06.html.

Census Bureau. (2014). Educational attainment. Retrie ve d from w w w.census .gov/hhes/ socdemo/education/index.html.

Census Bureau. (2015). New Census Bureau report analyzes U.S. population projections. Retrieved from www.census.gov/newsroom/ press-releases/2015/cb15-tps16.html.

Census Bureau. (2016). Median age at first marriage: 1890 to present. Retrieved from www.census.gov/hhes/families/files/graphics/ MS-2.pdf.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Youth risk behavior surveillance— United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 61. Atlanta GA: Author.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). 2015 Assisted Reproductive Technology National Summary Report. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). Suicide: Facts at a glance, 2015. Atlanta, GA: Author.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016). Diagnoses of HIV infection in the United States and dependent areas. HIV surveillance report, Vol. 27. Atlanta, GA: Author.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016a). Economic facts about U.S. tobacco production and use. Retrieved from www .cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/fact_sheets/ economics/econ_facts/.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016b). Health behaviors of adults: United States 2011–2014. Table ALC-2a. Retrieved from ftp.cdc .gov/pub/Health_Statistics/ NCHS/NHIS/SHS/2011-2014_AHB_Table_ ALC-2.pdf.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016c) Guideline for prescribing opioids for chronic pain. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/ drugoverdose/pdf/guidelines_factsheet-a.pdf.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016c). Early release of selected estimates based on data from the National Health Interview Survey, 2015: Data for Figure 9.2. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhis/ earlyrelease/earlyrelease201605.pdf.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016d). About adult BMI. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/ adult_bmi/index.html.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016e). Adult obesity facts. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/obesity/data/adult.html.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016e). Health, United States, 2015: With special feature on racial and ethnic health disparities. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/ data/hus/hus15.pdf.

C e nt e r s f o r D i s e a s e C o nt r o l . ( 2 0 1 6 ) . Understanding teen dating violence: Fact sheet 2016. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/ violenceprevention/intimatepartnerviolence/ teen_dating_violence.html.

Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. (2016a). Nursing Home Quality Initiative. Retrieved from www.cms.gov/Medicare/ Q u ality -Initi ative s -Patient-A ss e ssment- Instruments/NursingHomeQualityInits/index .html?redirect=/NursinghomeQualityinits/.

Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. (2016b). Nursing home data compendium 2015. Retrieved from www.cms.gov/Medicare/ Prov ider-Enrollment-and-Cer tif ic ation/ Cer tif ic ationandComplianc/D ow nlo ads/ nursinghomedatacompendium_508-2015.pdf.

Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. (2016c). Five-Star Quality Rating System. Retrieved from www.cms.gov/medicare/ p r o v i d e r- e n r o l l m e n t- a n d - c e r t i f i c a t i o n / certificationandcomplianc/fsqrs.html.

Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. ( 2 0 1 6 d ) . Me di c ar e p r o g ram — G e n e ral information. Retrieved from www.cms.gov/ Medicare/Medicare-General-Information/ MedicareGenInfo/index.html.

Central Intelligence Agenc y. (2013). The World  Factbook 2013–14. Washington, DC: Author.

Champion, T. B. (2003). A “matter of vocabulary”: Performance of low-income African-American Head Start children on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 24, 121–127.

Chandler, M. J., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (1998). Inching toward a mature theory of mind. In M. D. Ferrari & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Self-awareness: Its nature and development (pp. 148–190). New York: Guilford Press.

Chang, J. E. Sequeira, A., McCord, C. E., & Garney, W. R. (2016). Videoconference grief group counseling in rural Texas: Outcomes, challenges, and lessons learned. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 41, 140–160. doi:10 .1080/01933922.2016.114376

Chang, T.-S., & Hsiao, W.-H. (2014). Time spent on social networking sites: Understanding user behavior and social capital. Systems Research and Behavioral Science, 31, 102–114. doi:10.1002/sres.2169

Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Development, 72, 1832–1843.

Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 735–765.

Chapman, P. D. (1988). Schools as sorters: Lewis M. Terman, applied psychology, and the intelligence testing movement, 1890–1930. New York: NYU Press.

Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383–409. doi:10.1146/annurev. psych.093008.100448

Charlton, B., & Verghese, A. (2010). Caring for Ivan Ilyich. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 25, 93–95. doi:10.1007/s11606-009-1177-4

Charness, N., & Bosman, E. A. (1990). Expertise and aging: Life in the lab. In T. M. Hess (Ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 343–385). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Chaudhari, A., Gupta, R., Makwana, K., & Kondratov, R. (2016). Circadian clocks, diets, and aging. Nutrition and Healthy Aging. Retrieved from content.iospress.com/articles/ nutrition-and-healthy-aging/nha160006.

Chen-Gaddini, M. (2012). Chinese mothers and adolescents’ views of authority and autonomy: A study of parent-adolescent conflict in urban and rural China. Child Development, 83, 1846–1852.

Chen, J., & Gardner, H. (2005). Assessment based on multiple intelligences theory. In D. P.  Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contempo- rary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 77–102). New York: Guilford Press.

Chen, X., Wang, L., & Cao, R. (2011). Shyness- sensitivity and unsociability in rural Chinese children: Relations with social, school, and psychological adjustment. Child Development, 82, 1531–1543.

Chen, Y., Norton, D. J., McBain, R., Gold, J., Frazier, J. A., & Coyle, J. T. (2012). Enhanced local processing of dynamic visual information in autism: Evidence from speed discrimination. Neuropsychologia, 50, 733–739.

Chen, Y., Peng, Y., & Fang, P. (2016). Emotional intelligence mediates the relationship between age and subjective well-being. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 83, 91–107. doi:10.1177/0091415016648705

Cheng, E., & Pegg, S. (2016). “If I’m not gardening, I’m not at my happiest”: Exploring the positive subjective experiences derived from serious leisure gardening by older adults. World Leisure Journal, 58, 285–297. doi:10.1080/160 78055.2016.1228219

Cheng, S., & Powell, B. (2007). Under and beyond constraints: Resource allocation to young children from biracial families. American Journal of Sociology, 112, 1044–1094. doi:10.1086/508793

Cheng, T. C., Powdthaveea, N., & Oswald, A. J. (2014) Longitudinal evidence for a midlife nadir in human well-being: Results from four data sets. Working Paper. Coventry, UK: University of War wick , Department of Economics. Retrieved from www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/soc/ economics/research/workingpapers/2014/ twerp_1037_oswald.pdf.

Cherko, M., Hickson, L., & Bhutta, M. (2016). Auditor y deprivation and health in the

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

600 R e f e R e n c e s

elderly. Maturitas, 88, 52–57. doi:10.1016/ j.marturitas.2016.03.008

Cheung, C. K., Kam, P. K., & Ngan, R. M. H. (2011). Age discrimination in the labour market from the perspectives of employers and older workers. International Social Work, 54, 118–136. doi:10.1177/0020872810372368

Cheung, W. W., & Mao, P. (2012). Recent advances in obesity: Genetics and beyond. ISRN Endocrinology, Article ID 536905.

Chew-Graham, C. A., & Ray, M. (Eds.). (2016). Mental health and older people: A guide for primary care practitioners. New York: Springer.

Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Laboratory methods for assessing experts’ and novices’ knowledge. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 167– 184). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Chiao, C.-Y., Wu, H.-S., & Hsiao, C.-Y. (2015). Caregiver burden for informal caregivers of patients with dementia: A systematic review. International Nursing Review, 62, 340–350. doi:10.1111/inr.12194

Child Trends. (2016). Youth employment. Bethesda, MD: Author.

ChildStats.gov. (2016). America’s children in brief: Key national indicators of well-being, 2016. Retrieved from www.childstats.gov/ americaschildren/index.asp.

Choe, D. E., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2013). The interplay of externalizing problems and physical and inductive discipline during childhood. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2029–2039.

Choi, H. S., Johnson, B., & Kriewitz, K. (2013). Benefits of inclusion and segregation for individuals with disabilities in leisure. International Journal on Disability and Human Development, 12, 15–23. doi:10.1515/ ijdhd-2012-0120

Choi, M., Sprang, G., & Eslinger, J. G. (2016). Grandparents raising grandchildren: A synthetic review and theoretical model for interventions. Family & Community Health, 39, 120–128. doi:10.1097/0000000000000097

Choi, Y., Kim, T. Y., Pekelnicky, D. D., Kim, K., & Kim, Y. S. (2017). Impact of youth cultural orientation on perception of family process and development among Korean Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23, 244–257.

Chomitz, V. R., Cheung, L. W. Y., & Lieberman, E. (1995). The role of lifestyle in preventing low birth weight. The Future of Children, 5, 121–138.

Chow, B. W., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Chow, C. S. (2008). Dialogic reading and morphology training in Chinese children: Effects on language and literacy. Developmental Psychology, 44, 233–244.

Christ, C. C., Carlo, G., & Stoltenberg, S. F. (2016). Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) single nucleotide polymorphisms indirectly predict prosocial behavior through perspective yaking and empathic concern. Journal of Personality, 84, 204–213.

Christensen, A . (1990). Gender and social structure in the demand/ withdrawal pattern of marital conf lict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 73–81. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.73

Chronaki, G., Hadwin, J. A ., Garner, M., Maurage, P., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2015). The development of emotion recognition from facial expressions and non-linguistic vocalizations during childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–236.

Chun, S., Heo, J., Lee, S., & Kim, J. (2016). Leisure- related predictors on a sense of purpose in life among older adults with cancer. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 40, 266–280. doi:10.108 0/01924788.2016.1199517

Church, A. T. (2016). Personality traits across cultures. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 22–30. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.014

Ciani, E. (2016). Retirement, pension eligibility and home production. Labour Economics, 38, 106–120. doi:10.1016/j.labeco.2016.01.004

Cicchetti, D. (2013). Annual research review: Resilient functioning in maltreated children: Past, present, and future perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 402–422.

Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 187–211.

Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Developmental psychopathology and preventive intervention. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 497–547). New York: Wiley.

Cicirelli, V. G. (2001). Personal meaning of death in older adults and young adults in relation to their fears of death. Death Studies, 25, 663–683. doi:10.1080/713769896

Cicirelli, V. G. (2006). Fear of death in mid-old age. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 61B, P75–P81. Retrieved from psychsocgerontolog y.oxfordjournals .org/ content/61/2/P75.full.

Ciscato, E., Galichon, A., & Gousse, M. (2015). Like attract like? A structural comparison of monogamy across same-sex and different-sex households. Social Science Research Network. doi:10.2139/ssm.2530724 Retrieved from ssrn .com/abstract=2530724.

Clarke, P. J., Truelove, E., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2014). Developing reading comprehension. Chichester West Sussex, UK: Wiley.

Clarke, V., Ellis, S. J., Peel, E., & Riggs, D. W. (2010). Lesbian, gay, bisexual, trans and queer psychology: An introduction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Cleary, M. N., Wozniak, K., Marienau, C., Wilbur, G., Tolliver, D. E., & Meyer, P. (2017). Learning, adults, and competency-based education. In K. Rasmussen, P. Northrup, & R. Colson (Eds.), Handbook of research on competency-based education in university settings (pp. 210–231). Hershey, PA: IGI Global.

Clements, D. H. (1995). Teaching creativity with computers. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 141–161.

Cleveland, K. C., Quas, J. A., & Lyon, T. D. (2016). Valence, implicated actor, and children’s acquiescence to false suggestions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 43, 1–7.

Coan, J. A. (2008). Toward a neuroscience of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications (2nd ed., pp. 241–265). New York: Guilford.

Coan, J. A., & Sbarra, D. A. (2015). Social baseline theory: The social regulation of risk and effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 87–91. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.021

Coates, B. A. (2017). Divorce with decency (5th ed.). Honolulu, HI: Latitude 20.

Coelho, J. S., Jansen, A., Roefs, A., & Nederkoom, C. (2009). Eating behavior in response to food- cue exposure: Examining the cue-reactivity and counteractive-control models. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 131–139.

Cohen Kadosh, K., Johnson, M. H., Dick, F., Cohen Kadosh, R., & Blakemore, S. J. (2013). Effects of age, task performance, and structural brain development on face processing. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1630–1642.

Cohen-Mansfield, J., & Regev, I. (in press). Retirement preparation programs : An examination of retirement perceptions, self- mastery, and well-being. Research on Social Work Practice. doi:10.1177/1049731516645194

Cohen, A. O., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., Bonnie, R. J., Scott, E. S., Taylor-Thompson,  K.  A. et al. (2016). When is an adolescent an adult? Assessing cognitive control in emotional and nonemotional contexts. Psychological Science, 27, 549–562. doi:10.1177/0956797615627625

Cohen, F., Kemeny, M. E., Zegans, L., Johnson, P., Kearney, K. A., & Sites, D. P. (2007). Immune function declines with unemployment and recovers after stressor termination. Psycho- somatic Medicine, 69,  225–234. doi:10.1097/ PSY.0b013e31803139a6.

Cohen, R. W., Martinez, M. E., & Ward, B. W. (2010). Health insurance coverage: Early release of estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, 2009. Hyattsville MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. M. (1991). Stress and infectious disease in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 5–24.

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Doyle, W. J., Miller, G. E., Frank, E., Rabin, B. S., et al. (2012). Chronic stress, glucocorticoid receptor resistance, inflammation, and disease risk. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 5995–5999. doi:10.1073/pnas.1118355109

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Terry, R., & Wright, V. (1991). The role of aggression in peer relations: An analysis of aggression episodes in boys’ play groups. Child Development, 62, 812–826.

Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J. C., & Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral develop- ment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48 (Serial No. 200).

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 601

Cole, D. A., & Jordan, A. E. (1995). Competence and memory: Integrating psychosocial and cognitive correlates of child depression. Child Development, 66, 459–473.

Cole, M. (2006). Culture and cognitive devel- opment in phylogenetic, historical, and ontogenetic perspective. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychol- ogy (6th ed., Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

Cole, P. M., Tamang, B. L., & Shrestha, S. (2006). Cultural variations in the socialization of young children’s anger and shame. Child Development, 77, 1237–1251.

Coleman-Jensen, A., Rabbitt, M. P., Gregory, C. A., & Singh, A. (2016). Household Food Security in the United States in 2015. Washington DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service.

Coleman, J. (2015). Unfinished work : The struggle to build an aging American workforce. New York: Oxford University Press.

Coleman, M., Ganong, L., & Russell, L. T. (2013). Resilience in stepfamilies. In D. S. Becvar (Ed.), Handbook of family resilience (pp. 85–103). New York: Springer.

Coles, R. L. (2016). Race and family: A structural approach (2nd ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Collardeau, F., & Ehrenberg, M. (2016). Parental divorce and attitudes and feelings toward marriage and divorce in emerging adulthood: New insights from a multiway-frequency analysis. Journal of European Psychology Students. Retrieved from jeps.efpsa.org/ articles/10.5334/jeps.341/.

Collins-Colosi, K. (2016). Young adult narratives of sibling loss and bereavement during adolescence. Unpublished dissertation, Health Psychology, Walden University.

Collins, R. L., Martino, S. C., Elliot, M. N., & Miu,  A. (2011). Relationships between ado- lescent sexual outcomes and exposure to sex in media: Robustness to propensity-based analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47, 585–591.

Collins, W. A., & van Dulmen, M. (2006). “The course of true love(s)…”: Origins and pathways in the development of romantic relationships. In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Risks and opportunities (pp. 63–86). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Collins, W. A., Welsh, D. P., & Furman, W. (2009). Adolescent romantic relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 631–652.

Commons, M. L. (2016). The fundamental issues with behavioral development. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 21, 1–12. doi:10.1037/ bdb0000022

Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2015). Impact of early inter vention on psychopathology, crime, and well-being at age 25. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 59–70.

Congressional Budget Office. (2017). Budget. Retrieved from www.cbo.gov/topics/budget.

Connidis, I. A. (2001). Family ties and aging. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Conradt, E. (2017). Using behavioral epigenetic principles to advance the field of early life stress research. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 107–112.

Conry-Murray, C., & Turiel, E. (2012). Jimmy’s baby doll and Jenny’s truck: Young children’s reasoning about gender norms . Child Development, 83, 146–158.

Conway, A. M., Tugade, M. M., Catalino, L. I., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). The broaden-and- build theory of positive emotions: form, function, and mechanisms. In S. A. David, I. Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), Oxford handbook of happiness (pp. 17–34). Oxford UK: Oxford University Press.

Cook , D. A ., Bahn, R . S., & Menaker, R . (2010). Speed mentoring: An innovative m e t h o d t o f a c i l i t a t e m e n t o r i n g relationships. Medical Teacher, 32, 692–694. doi:10.3109/01421591003686278

Cook, E. C., Buehler, C., & Henson, R. (2009). Parents and peers as social influences to deter antisocial behavior. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 38, 1204–1252.

Cooke, L. J., Chambers, L. C., Añez, E. V., Croker, H. A. Boniface, D., Yeomans, M. R., et al. (2011). Eating for pleasure or profit: The effects of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. Psychological Science, 22, 190–196.

Cooklin, A. R., Westrupp, E. M., Strazdins,  L., Giallo, R., Martin, A., & Nicholson, J. M. (2016). Fathers at work: Work-family conflict, work-family enrichment, and parenting in an Australian cohort. Journal of Family Issues, 37, 1611–1635.

Coombs, M. A. (2010). The mourning that comes before: Can anticipatory grief theory inform family care in adult intensive care? International Journal of Palliative Nursing, 16, 580–584. doi:10.12968/ijpn.2010.16.12.580

Cooney, T. M., & Uhlenberg, P. (1990). The role of divorce in men’s relations with their adult children after mid-life. Journal of Marriage and Family, 52, 677–688.

Cooney, T. M., Smyer, M. A., Hagestad, G. O., & Klock, R. (1986). Parental divorce in young adult- hood: Some preliminary findings. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 470–477. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.1986.tb03478.x

Copen, C. E., Daniels, K., Mosher, W. D., & Division of Vital Statistics. (2013). First premarital cohabitation in the United States: 2006-2010 National Survey of Family Growth. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhsr/ nhsr064.pdf.

Coplan, R. J., Ooi, L. L., & Nocita, G. (2015). When one is company and two is a crowd: Why some children prefer solitude. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 133–137.

Copley, S. J. (2016). Morphology of the aging lung on computed tomography. Journal of Thoracic Imaging, 31, 140–150. doi:10.1097/ RTI.0000000000000211

Coppus, A. M. W. (2013). People with intellectual disability: What do we know about adulthood and life expectancy. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 18, 6–16.

Corbett, B. A., & Hilty, D. M. (2006). Managing your time. In L. W. Roberts & D. M. Hilty (Eds.), Handbook of career development in academic psychiatry and behavioral sciences (pp. 83–91). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Cornelius, M., Taylor, P., Geva, D., & Day,  N. (1995). Prenatal tobacco exposure and marijuana use among adolescents: Effects on offspring gestational age, growth, and morphology. Pediatrics, 95, 738–743.

Cornwall, A. (2016). Women’s empowerment: What works? Journal of International Development, 28, 342–359. doi:10.1002/ jid.3210

Corporation for National and Community Service. (2016). Senior Corps. Retrieved from www .nationalservice.gov/programs/senior-corps.

Corr, C. A. (2010a). Children, development, and encounters with death, bereavement, and coping. In C. A. Corr & D. E. Balk (Eds.), Children’s encounters with death, bereavement, and coping (pp. 3–19). New York: Springer.

Corr, C. A . (2010b). Children’s emerging awareness and understandings of loss and death. In C. A. Corr & D. E. Balk (Eds.), Children’s encounters with death, bereavement, and coping (pp. 21–37). New York: Springer.

Corr, C. A., & Corr, D. M. (2013). Death and dying: Life and living (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Corr, C. A., Corr, D. M., & Nabe, C. M. (2008). Death and dying: Life and living. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Corriveau, K. H., Kinzler, K. D., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Accuracy trumps accent in children’s endorsement of object labels. Developmental Psychology, 49, 470–479.

Cortiella, C., & Horowitz, S. H. (2014). The state of learning disabilities: Facts, trends and emerging issues. New York: National Center for Learning Disabilities.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J.  L.  Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40). Washington, D C: Academic Psychological Association.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2011). Five- factor theory, and interpersonal psychology. In L. M. Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (pp. 91–104). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Costin, S. E., & Jones, D. C. (1992). Friendship as a facilitator of emotional responsiveness and prosocial interventions among young children. Developmental Psychology, 28, 941–947.

Cotton, S. R., Anderson, W. A., & McCullough, B. M. (2013). Impact of Internet use on loneliness and contact with others among older adults: Cross- sectional analysis. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 15, e39. doi:10.2196/jmir.2306

Coulton, C. J., Crampton, D. S., Irwin, M., Spilsbury, J. C., & Korbin, J. E. (2007). How neighborhoods influence child maltreatment: A review of the literature and alternative

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

602 R e f e R e n c e s

pathways. Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 1117–1142.

Council for Adult and Experiential Learning. (2016). Higher education. Retrieved from www .cael.org/higher-education.

Council on Contemporary Families. (2015). Remarriage in the United States: If at first they don’t succeed, do most Americans “try, try again”? Retrieved from contemporaryfamilies. org/remarriage-brief-report/.

Counts, D., & Counts, D. (Eds.). (1985). Aging and its transformations: Moving toward death in Pacific societies. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

Cousminer, D. L., Berry, D. J., Timpson, N. J., Ang, W., Thiering, E., Byrne, E.M., et al. (2013). Genome-wide association and longitudinal analyses reveal genetic loci linking pubertal height growth, pubertal timing, and childhood adiposity. Human Molecular Genetics, 22, 2735–2747.

Cox, C. B. (2007). Grandparent-headed families: Needs and implications for social work interventions and advocacy. Families in Society, 88, 561–566. doi:10.1606/1044-3894.3678

Coyne, C. A., Långström, N., Rickert, M. E., Lichtenstein, P., & D’Onofrio, B. M. (2013). Maternal age at first birth and offspring criminality: Using the children of twins design to test causal hypotheses. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 17–35.

Cram, B., Alkadry, M. G., & Tower, L. E. (2016). Social costs: The career-family tradeoff. In M. L. Connerley & J. Wu (Eds.), Handbook of well-being of working women (pp. 473–487). New York: Springer.

Crawford-Williams, F. M., Roberts, R. M., & Watts, D. (2016). Alcohol consumption and protective behavioural strategy use among Australian young adults. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 21, 119–123. doi:10. 1080/02673843.2013.831363

Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social-information processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.

Cristia, A., Seidl, A., Singh, L., & Houston, D. (2016). Test-retest reliability in infant speech perception tasks. Infancy, 21, 684–667.

Crocetti, E. (2017). Identity formation in adolescence: The dynamic of forming and consolidating identity commitments. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 145-150.

Crocetti, E., Rubini, M., Luyckx, K., & Meeus, W. (2008). Identity formation in early and middle adolescence from various ethnic groups: From three dimensions to five statuses. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 37, 983–996.

Croghan, C. F., Moone, R. P., & Olson, A. M. (2014). Friends, family, and caregiving among midlife and older lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender adults. Journal of Homosexuality, 61, 79–102. doi:10.1080/00918369.2013 .835238

Cross, S., & Markus, H. (1991). Possible selves across the lifespan. Human Development, 34, 230–255. doi:10.1159/000277058

Crouter, A. C., & Bumpus, M. F. (2001). Linking parents’ work stress to children’s and adoles- cents’ psychological adjustment. Current Direc- tions in Psychological Science, 10, 156–159.

Crouter, A. C., Whiteman, S. D., McHale, S. M., & Osgood, D. (2007). Development of gender attitude traditionality across middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 78, 911–926.

Csizmadia, A., & Ispa, J. M. (2014). Black-White biracial children’s social development from kindergarten to fifth grade: Links with racial identification, gender, and socioeconomic status. Social Development, 23, 157–177.

Csizmadia, A., Brunsma, D. L., & Cooney, T. M. (2012). Racial identification and developmental outcomes among Black-White multiracial youth: A review from a life course perspective. Advances in Life Course Research, 17, 34–44.

Cukier, H. N., Kunkle, B. W., Vardarajan,  B.  N., Rolati, S., Hamilton-Nelson, K. L., Kohli, M. A., et al. (2016). ABC A7 frameshift deletion associated with Alzheimer disease in African Americans. Neurology Genetics, 2, e79. doi:10 .1212/NXG.0000000000000079. Retrieved from ng.neurology.org/content/2/3/e79.full.

Cummings, E. M., George, M. R. W., McCoy, K., P., & Davies, P. T. (2012). Interparental conflict in kindergarten and adolescent adjustment: Prospective investigation of emotional security as an explanatory mechanism. Child Development, 83, 1703–1715.

Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Papp, L. M. (2016). Couple conflict, children, and families. In A. Booth et al., (Eds.), Couples in conflict (pp. 117–147). New York: Routledge.

Cummings, E., Schermerhorn, A. C., Davies, P. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, J. S. (2006). Interparental discord and child adjustment: Prospective investigations of emotional security as an explanatory mechanism. Child Development, 77, 132–152.

Cummings, K. (2015). Coming to grips with loss: Normalizing the grief process. Boston: Sense Publishers.

Cundiff, N. L., & Stockdale, M. S. (2013). Social psychological perspectives on discrimination against women leaders. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.), Women and management: Global issues and promising solutions (pp. 155–174). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self- teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185–199.

Cunningham, W. R. (1987). Intellectual abilities and age. In K. W. Schaie (Ed.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 7, pp. 117–134). New York: Springer.

Curran, T., Hill, A. P., Hall, H. K., & Jowett, G. E. (2015). Relationships between the coach- created motivational climate and athlete engagement in youth sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 37, 193–198.

Currie, J. (2013). Pollution and infant health. Child Development Perspectives,7, 237–242.

Currie, J., & Walker, R. (2011). Traffic congestion and infant health: Evidence from EZPass. Amer ican Economic Jour nal s : Applied Economics, 3, 65–90.

Curtis, J. R., Treece, P. D., Nielsen, E. L., Gold, J., Ciechanowski, P. S., Shannon, S. E. et al. (2016). Randomized trial of communication facilitators to reduce family distress and intensity of end-of-life care. Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, 193, 154–162. doi:10.1164/rccm.201505-0900OC

Curtis, R., & Pearson, F. (2010). Contact with birth parents: Differential psychological adjustment for adults adopted as infants. Journal of Social Work , 10, 347–367. doi:10.1177/1468017310369273

Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82, 766–779.

Cyr, C., & Alink , L. R . A . (2017). Child maltreatment: The central roles of parenting capacities and attachment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 81–86.

D’Angelo, D., Mastroianni, C., Hammer, J. M., Piredda, M., Vellone, E., Alvaro, R. et al. (2015). Continuity of care during end of life: An evolutionary concept analysis. International Journal of Nursing Knowledge, 26, 80–89. doi:10.1111/2047-3095.12041

D’Onofrio, B. M., Class, Q. A., Lahey, B. B., & Larsson, H. (2014). Testing the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis for psychopathology using family-based quasi- experimental designs. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 151–157.

D’Onofrio, B. M., Goodnight, J. A., Van Hulle, C. A., Rodgers, J. L., Rathouz, P. J., Waldman, I. D., & Lahey, B. B. (2009). Maternal age at childbirth and offspring disruptive behaviors: Testing the causal hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 50, 1018–1028.

da Costa, J. F., & Oliveira, T. C. (2016). Managing careers: Anchored, plateaued or drifting? In C. Machado & J. P. Davim (Eds.), Organizational management (pp. 31–62). New York: Springer.

Dadgari, A., Hamid, T. A., Hakim, M. N., Mousavi, S. A., Dadvar, L., Mohammadi, M. et al. (2015). The role of self-efficacy on fear of falls and fall among elderly community dwellers in Shahroud, Iran. Nursing Practice Today, 2, 112–120. Retrieved from npt.tums. ac.ir/index.php/npt/article/view/54/58.

Dai, B., Zhang, B., & Li, J. (2013). Protective factors for subjective well-being in Chinese older adults: The roles of resources and activity. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 1225–1239. doi:10.1007/s10902-012-9378-7

Dalton, C. (2014). Including smart architecture in environments for people with dementia. In J. van Hoof, G. Demiris, & E. J. M. Wouters (Eds.), Handbook for smart homes, health care and well-being (pp. 1–10). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-0194-8_57-1

Damian, R. I., & Simonton, D. K. (2015). Four psychological perspectives on creativity. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 603

Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource. doi:10.1002/978111 8900772.etrds0134

Daniels, H. (2011). Vygotsky and psychology. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 673–696). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Dannar, P. R. (2013). Millennials: What they offer our organizations and how leaders can make sure they deliver. Journal of Values- Based Leadership, 6, Article 3. Retrieved from scholar.valpo.edu/cgi/viewcontent. cgi?article=1073&context=jvbl.

Dannefer, D., & Miklowski, C. (2006). Devel- opments in the life course. In J. A. Vincent, C. R. Phillipson, & M. Downs (Eds.), The futures of old age (pp. 30–39). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Darejeh, A., & Singh, D. (2014). Increasing Microsoft Office usability for middle- aged and elder users with less computer literacy. Journal of Industrial and Intelligent Information, 2. Retrieved from www.jiii.org/ uploadfile/2013/1230/20131230041823416.pdf.

Darling, N., Cumsille, P., & Martínez, M. L. (2008). Individual differences in adolescents’ beliefs about the legitimacy of parental authority and their own obligation to obey: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 79, 1103–1118.

Davidov, M., Vaish, A., Knafo-Noam, A., & Hastings, P. D. (2016). The motivational foundations of prosocial behavior from a developmental perspective—evolutionar y roots and key psychological mechanisms: Introduction to the special section. Child Development, 87, 1655–1667.

Davidson, F. H., & Davidson, M. M. (1994). Changing childhood prejudice: The caring work of the schools. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey/ Greenwood.

Davies, L. (2003). Singlehood: Transitions within a gendered world. Canadian Journal on Aging, 22, 343–352. doi:10.1017/S0714980800004219

Davies, P. T., Martin, M. J., Coe, J. L., & Cummings, E. M. (2016). Transactional cascades of destructive interparental conflict, children’s emotional insecurity, and psychological problems across childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopatholog y, 28, 653–671.

Davis, A., McMahon, C. M., Pichora-Fuller, K. M. Russ, S., Lin, F., Olusanya, B. O., et al. (2016). Aging and hearing health: The life-source approach. The Gerontologist, 56, S256–S267. doi:10.1093/geront/gnw033

Davis, B., Dionne, R., & Fortin, M. (2014). Parenting in two cultural worlds in the presence of one dominant worldview: The American Indian experience. In H. Selin (Ed.), Parenting across cultures (pp. 367–377). New  York: Springer.

Davis, E. M., Kim, K., & Fingerman, K. L. (in press). Is an empty nest best?: Coresidence with adult children and parental marital quality before and after the Great Recession. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw022

Davis, E. P., & Sandman, C. A. (2010). The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human cognitive development. Child Development, 81, 131–148.

Davis, K. M., Gagnier, K. R., Moore, T. E., & Todorow, M. (2013). Cognitive aspects of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, 81–92.

Davis, P. E., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2011). Self-knowledge in childhood: Relations with children’s imaginary companions and understanding of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 680–686.

De Beni, R., & Palladino, P. (2000). Intrusion errors in working memory tasks: Are they related to reading comprehension ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 131–143.

de Bloom, J., Geurts, S. A. E., & Kompier, M. A. J. (2012). Effects of short vacations, vacation activities and experiences on employee health and well-being. Stress and Health, 28, 305–318. doi:10.1002/smi.1434

de Bloom, J., Geurts, S. A. E., Taris, T. W., Sonnentag, S., de Weerth, C., & Kompier, M. A. J. (2010). Effects of vacation from work on health and well-being: Lots of fun, quickly gone. Work and Stress, 24, 196–216. doi:10. 1080/02678373.2010.493385

de Haan, M., Wyatt, J. S., Roth, S., Vargha- Khadem, F., Gadian, D., & Mishkin, M. (2006). Brain and cognitive-behavioral development after asphyxia at term birth. Developmental Science, 9, 441–442.

de Jong, P., Rouwendal, J., van Hattum, P., & Brouwer, A. (2012). Housing preferences of an ageing population: Investigation in the diversity among Dutch older adults. Retrieved from arno.uvt.nl/show.cgi?fid=123055.

De Lisi, R., & Wolford, J. L. (2002). Improving children’s mental rotation accuracy with computer game playing. Journal of Genetic Psychology 163, 272–283.

De Neys W., & Everaerts, D. (2008). Developmental trends in everyday conditional reasoning: The retrieval and inhibition interplay. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 252–263.

de Pommereau, I. (2012). How BMW reinvents the factory for older workers. Retrieved from www .csmonitor.com/World/Europe/2012/0902/How- BMW-reinvents-the-factory-for-older-workers.

de Rezende, L. F. M., Lopes, M. R., Rey-López, J. P., Matsudo ,V. K. R., & do Carmo Luiz, O. (2014). Sedentary behavior and health outcomes: An overview of systematic reviews. PLoS One, 9, e105620.

de Vries, B. (1996). The understanding of friendship: An adult life course perspective. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 249–268). San Diego: Academic Press.

De Wolff, M. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.

DeAndrea, D. C., Ellison, N. B., LaRose, R., Steinfield, C., & Fiore, A. (2012). Serious

social media: On the use of social media for improving students’ adjustment to college. The Internet and Higher Education, 15, 15–23. doi:10.1016/j.iheduc.2011.05.009

Deane, G., Spitze, G. Ward, R. A., & Zhuo, Y. (2016). Close to you? How parent-adult child contact is influenced by family patterns. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 71, 344–357. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbv036

Deary, I. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 453–482.

Deary, I. J., Pattie, A., & Starr, J. M. (2013). The stability of intelligence from age 11 to age 90 years: The Lothian birth cohort of 1921. Psychological Science, 24, 2361–2368. doi:10.1177/095679761348648

Deater-Deckard, K., Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Alampay, L. P., Sorbring, E., Bacchini, D., et al. (2011). The association between parental warmth and control in thirteen cultural groups. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 790–794.

Debast, I., van Alphen, S. P. J., Rossi, G., Tummers, J. H. A., Bolwerk, N., Derksen, J. J. L., et al. (2014). Personality traits and personality disorders in late middle and old age: Do they remain stable? A literature review. Clinical Gerontologist, 37, 253–271. doi:10/1080/0731 7115.2014.885917

Declercq, E. (2012). The politics of home birth in the United States. Birth, 39, 281–285.

Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center. (2016). DoD Worldwide numbers of rTBI. Retrieved from dvbic.dcoe.mil/dod-worldwide-numbers-tbi.

Degner, J., & Dalege, J. (2013). The apple does not fall far from the tree, or does it? A meta-analysis of parent–child similarity in intergroup attitudes. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1270–1304.

Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Spelke, E. S. (2015). The infancy of the human brain. Neuron, 88, 93–109.

DeKeyser, R., Alfi-Shabtay, I., & Ravid, D. (2010). Cross-linguistic evidence for the nature of age effects in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 413–438.

Dekovic, M., & Janssens, J. M. (1992). Parents’ child-rearing style and child’s sociometric status. Developmental Psychology, 28, 925–932.

del Valle, J. F., Bravo, A., & Lopez, M. (2010). Parents and peers as providers of support in adolescents’ social network: A developmental perspective. Journal of Community Psychology, 38, 16–27.

Delaney, C. (2000). Making babies in a Turkish village. In J. S. DeLoache & A. Gottlieb (Eds.), A world of babies: Imagined childcare guides for seven societies. New  York: Cambridge University Press.

DeLiema, M., Yon, Y., & Wilber, K. H. (2016). Tricks of the trade: Motivating sales agents to con older adults. The Gerontologist, 56, 335–344. doi:10.1093/geront/gnu039

DeLoache, J. S., Chiong, C., Sherman, K., Islam, N., Vanderborght, M., Troseth, G. L., Strouse, G. A., & O’Doherty, K. (2010). Do babies learn from baby media? Psychological Science, 21, 1570–1574.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

604 R e f e R e n c e s

DeLucia-Waack, J. L. (2010). Children of divorce groups. In G. L. Greif & P. H. Ephross (Eds.), Group work with populations at risk (3rd ed., pp. 93–114). New  York: Oxford University Press.

Demir, A., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow,  S. (2010). Narrative skill in children with unilateral brain injury: A possible limit to functional plasticity. Developmental Science, 13, 636–647.

Demir, M., Orthel-Clark, H., Özdemir, M., & Özdemir, S. B. (2015). Friendship and happi- ness among young adults. In M. Demir (Ed.), Friendship and happiness (pp. 117-135). New York: Springer.

Denford, S., Abraham, C., Campbell, R., & Busse, H. (2017). A comprehensive review of reviews of school-based interventions to improve sexual- health. Health Psychology Review, 11, 33–52.

Deng, C.-P., Armstrong, P. I., & Rounds, J. (2007). The fit of Holland’s RIASEC model to US occupations. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 71, 1–22. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2007.04.002

Denham, J., O’Brien, B. J., & Charchar, F. J. (2016). Telomere length maintenance and cardio- metabolic disease prevention through exercise training. Sports Medicine, 46, 1213–1237. doi:10.1007/s40279-016-0482-4

Denney, N. W. (1984). A model of cognitive development across the life span. Develop- mental Review, 4, 171–191. doi:10.1016/0273- 2287(84)90006-6

Denney, N. W., Pearce, K. A., & Palmer,  A.  M. (1982). A developmental study of adults’ per- formance on traditional and practical problem- solving tasks. Experimental Aging Research, 8, 115–118. doi:10.1080/03610738208258407

DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Singled out: How singles are stereotyped, stigmatized, and ignored, and still live happily ever after. New  York: St Martin’s Press.

Deprest, J., Ghidini, A., Van Mieghem, T., Bianchi, D. W., Faas, B., & Chitty, L. S. (2016). In case you missed it: The Prenatal Diagnosis editors bring you the most significant advances of 2015. Prenatal Diagnosis, 36, 3–9.

DeSilver, D. (2016). More older Americans are working, and working more, than they used to. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.org/fact- tank/2016/06/20/more-older-americans-are- working-and-working-more-than-they-used-to/.

Desmond, N., & López Turley, R. N. (2009). The role of familism in explaining the Hispanic- white college application gap. Social Problems, 56, 311–334. doi:10.1525/sp.2009.56.2.311

Dey, S., & Ghosh, J. (2016). Factors in the d i s t r i b u t i o n o f s u cce s s f u l m a r r i a g e . International Journal of Social Sciences and Management, 3, 60–64. doi:10.3126/ijssm. v3i1.14315

Di Battista, A. M., Heinsinger, N. M., & William Rebeck, G. (2016). Alzheimer’s disease genetic risk factor APOE-ε4 also affects normal brain function. Current Alzheimer Research, 13, 1200–1207.

Di Domenico, S., Rodrigo, A. H., Ayaz, H., Fournier, M. A., & Ruocco, A. C. (2015).

Decision-making conflict and the neural efficiency hypothesis of intelligence: A f unc tional ne ar-inf rare d sp e c tro s copy investigation. NeuroImage, 109, 307–317. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.039

Di Fabio, A. (2016). Life design and career counseling innov ative outcomes . The Career Development Quarterly, 64, 35–48. doi:10.1002/cdq.12039

Di Masso, A., Dixon, J., & Hernández, B. (2017). Place attachment, sense of belonging and the micro-politics of place satisfaction. In G. Fleury-Bahi, E. Pol, & O. Navarro (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology and quality of life research (pp. 85–104). New York: Springer.

Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959–964.

Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., & Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefontal cortex deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(4, Serial No. 252).

Diamond, L. M. (2007). A dynamical systems approach to the development and expression of female same-sex sexuality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 142–161.

DiBello, K. K. (2015). Grief & depression at the end of life. Nurse Practitioner, 40, 22–28. doi:10.1097/01.NPR.0000463781.50345.95

Dick-Read, G. (1959). Childbirth without fear. New York: Harper & Brothers.

Dickens, B. M., Boyle, J. M., Jr., & Ganzini,  L. (2008). Euthanasia and assisted suicide. In P.  A.  Singer & A. M. Viens (Eds.), The Cambridge textbook of bioethics (pp. 72–77). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Dickinson, G. E. (1992). First childhood death experiences. Omega: The Journal of Death and D ying, 25, 169–182. doi:10.2190/ M8F0-TN3F-EFCB-H8N3

Diehl, M., Marsiske, M., Horgas , A . L ., Rosenberg, A., Saczynski, J. S., & Willis, S. L. (2005). The Revised Observed Tasks of Daily Living: A performance-based assessment of everyday problem solving in older adults. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 24, 211–230. doi:10.1177/0733464804273772

Diemer, M. A. (2007). Parental and school influences upon the career development of poor youth of color. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 70, 502–524.

Diersch, N., Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Schütz- Bosbach, S., & Rieger, M. (2012). Representing others’ actions: The role of expertise in the aging mind. Psychological Research, 76, 525–541. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0404-x

DiGiacomo, M., Davidson, P. M., Byles, J., & Nolan, M. T. (2013). An integrative and socio- cultural perspective of health, wealth, and adjustment in widowhood. Health Care for Women International, 34, 1067–1083. doi:10. 1080/07399332.2012.712171

Dijkstra, J. K., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Borch, C. (2013). Popularity and adolescent friendship

networks: Selection and influence dynamics. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1242–1252.

Dillaway, H., Byrnes, M., Miller, S., & Rehnan, S. (2008). Talking “among us ”: How women from different racial-ethnic groups define and discuss menopause. Health C are for Women International, 29, 766–781. doi:10.1080/07399330802179247

Dilworth-Anderson, P., Boswell, G., & Cohen, M. D. (2007). Spiritual and religious coping values and beliefs among African American caregivers: A qualitative study. Journal of Applied Gerontolog y, 26, 355–369. doi:10.1177/0733464807302669

DiPietro, J. A., Caulfield, L., Costigan, K. A., Merialdi, M., Nguyen, R. H. N., Zavaleta, N., & Gurewitsch, E. D. (2004). Fetal neurobehavioral de velopment : A t ale of two c itie s . Developmental Psychology, 40, 445–456.

DiPietro, J. A., Costigan, K. A., & Voegtline, K. M. (2015). Studies in fetal behavior: Revisited, renewed, and reimagined. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 80(3), 1–151.

DiPietro, J. A., Novak, M. F., Costigan, K. A., Atella, L. D., & Reusing, S. P. (2006). Maternal psychological distress during pregnancy in relation to child development at age two. Child Development, 77, 573–587.

Dirks, M. A., Persram, R., Recchia, H. E., & Howe, N. (2015). Sibling relationships as sources of risk and resilience in the development and maintenance of internalizing and externalizing problems during childhood and adolescence. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 145–155.

Dishion, T. J., Poulin, F., & Burraston, B. (2001). Peer group dynamics associated with iatrogenic effects in group interventions with high-risk young adolescents. In D. W. Nangle & C. A. Erdley (Eds.), The role of friendship in psychological adjustment (pp. 79–92). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Divan, H. A., Kheifets, L., Obel, C., & Olsen,  J. (2012). Cell phone use and behavioural prob- lems in young children. Journal of Epidemiol- ogy and Community Health, 66, 524–529.

Dixon, P. (2009). Marriage among African Americans: What does the research reveal? Journal of African American Studies, 13, 29–46. doi:10.1007/s12111-008-9062-5

Dobbins, M., DeCorby, K., Manske, S., & Goldblatt, E. (2008). Effective practices for school-based tobacco use prevention. Preventive Medicine, 46, 289–297.

Dobkin, P. L., & Hassed, C. S. (2016). Scientific underpinnings and evidence pertaining to mindfulness. In P. L. Dobkin & C. S. Hassed (Eds.), Mindful medical practitioners: A guide for clinicians and educators (pp. 9–31). New York: Springer.

Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Malone, P. S. (2008). Testing an idealized dynamic cascade model of the development of serious violence in adolescence. Child Development, 79, 1907–1927.

Doerwald, F., Scheibe, S., Zacher, H., & Van Yperen, N. W. (2016). Emotional competencies

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 605

across adulthood: State of knowledge and implications for the work context. Work, Aging and Retirement, 2, 159–216. doi:10.1093/ worker/waw013

Doka, K. J., & Mertz, M. E. (1988). The meaning and significance of great-grandparenthood. The Gerontologist, 28, 192–197. doi:10.1093/ geront/28.2.192

Dommaraju, P. (2010). The changing demography of marriage in India. Podcast retrieved from ari.nus.edu.sg/Publication/Detail/1630.

Donaldson, C. D., Handren, L. M., & Crano, W. D. (2016). The enduring impact of parents’ monitoring, warmth, expectancies, and alcohol use on their children’s future binge drinking and arrests: A longitudinal analysis. Prevention Science, 17, 606–614.

Doohan, E.-A. M., Carrère, S., & Riggs, M. L. (2010). Using relational stories to predict the trajectory toward marital dissolution: The oral history interview and spousal feelings of flooding, loneliness, and depression. Journal of Family Communication, 10, 57–77. doi:10.1080/15267430903396401

Dorn, L. D., Dahl, R. E., Woodward, H. R., & Biro, F. (2006). Defining the boundaries of early adolescence: A user’s guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 10, 30–56.

Doss, B. D., & Rhoades, G. K. (2017). The transition to parenthood: Impact on couples’ romantic relationships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 25–28. doi:10.1016/j. copsyc.2016.04.003

Doubeni, C. A., Schootman, M., Major, J. M., Torres Stone, R. A., Laiyemo, A. O., Park, Y., et al. (2012). Health status, neighborhood socioeconomic context, and premature mortality in the United States: The National Institutes of Health—AARP Diet and Health Study. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 680–688. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2011.300158

Doumen, S., Smits, I., Luyckx, K., Duriez, B., Vanhalst, J., Verschueren, K., et al. (2012). Identity and perceived peer relationship quality in emerging adulthood: The mediating role of attachment-related emotions. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1417–1425.

Dowd, J. J. (2012). Aging and the course of desire. Journal of Aging Studies, 26, 285–295. doi:10.1016/j.aging.2012.01.004

Doyle, J. M., & Kao, G. (2007). Are racial identities of multiracials stable? Change self- identification among single and multiple race individuals. Social Psychology Quarterly, 70, 405–423.

Dozier, M., Zeanah, C. H., & Bernard, K. (2013). Infant and toddlers in foster care. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 166–171.

Drew, L. M., & Silverstein, M. (2004). Inter- generational role investments of great- grandparents: Consequences for psychological well-being. Ageing and Society, 24, 95–111. doi:10.1017/S0144686X03001533

Driscoll, A. K., Russell, S. T., & Crockett, L. J. (2008). Parenting styles and youth well-being

across immigrant generations. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 185–209.

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self- discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.

Duff, L. J., Ericsson, K. A., & Baluch, B. (2007). In search of the loci for sex differences in throwing: The effects of physical size and differential recruitment rates on high levels of dart performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 71–78.

Dulac, M. C., & Auertin-Leheudre, M. (2016). Exercise: An important key to prevent physical and cognitive frailty. Journal of Frailty and Aging, 5, 3–5. doi:10.14283/jfa.2015.72

Dunham, P. J., Dunham, F., & Curwin, A. (1993). Joint-attentional states and lexical acquisition at 18 months. Developmental Psychology, 29, 827–831.

Dunlop, W. L., Guo, J., & McAdams, D. P. (2016). The autobiographical author through time: Examining the degree of stability and change in redemptive and contaminated personal narratives. Social Psychological and Per sonality Science , 7, 428–436. doi:10.1177/1948550616644654

Dunn, J., & Brophy, M. (2005). Communica- tion, relationships, and individual differ- ences in children’s understanding of mind. In J. W.  Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why lan- guage matters for theory of mind (pp.  50–69). New York: Oxford.

Dunn, J., & Davies, L. (2001). Sibling relationships and interpersonal conflict. In J. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development (pp. 273–290). New  York: Cambridge University Press.

Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1981). Social behavior of young siblings in the family context: Differences between same-sex and different- sex dyads. Child Development, 52, 1265–1273.

Dunn, J., Slomkowski, C., & Beardsall, L. (1994). Sibling relationships from the preschool period through middle childhood and early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 315–324.

Dunphy-Lelii, S., LaBounty, J., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). The social context of infant intention understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15, 60–77.

Dunson, D. B., Colombo, B., & Baird, D. D. (2002). Changes in age in the level and duration of fertility in the menstrual cycle. Human Reproduction, 17, 1399–1403.

Dupuis, S. (2010). Examining the blended family: The application of systems theory toward an understanding of the blended family system. Journal of Couple and Relationship Therapy, 9, 239–251. doi:10.1080/15332691.2 010.491784

Durik, A. M., Hyde, J. S., & Clark, R. (2000). Sequelae of cesarean and vaginal deliveries: Psychosocial outcomes for mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 36, 251–260.

D u tt a , D. , C a l v a n i , R . , B e r n ab e i , R . , Leeuwenburgh, C., & Marzetti, E. (2012). Contribution of impaired mitochondrial

autophagy to cardiac aging: Mechanisms and therapeutic opportunities. Circulation Research, 110, 1125–1138. doi:10.1161/ CIRCRESAHA.111.246108

Dutton, W. H., Helsper, E. J., Whitty, M. T., Li, N., Buckwalter, J. G., & Lee, E. (2009). The role of the Internet in reconfiguring marriages: A cross- national study. Interpersona: An International Journal on Personal Relationships, 3(Suppl. 2). R e t r i e v e d f ro m ab p r i . f i l e s . w o rd p re s s . com/2010/12/interpersona-3-suppl-2_1.pdf.

Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: Theory and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 19–46.

Eagleton, S. G., Williams, A. L., & Merten, M. J. (2016). Perceived behavioral autonomy and trajectories of depressive symptoms from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 198–211.

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2013). The nature- nurture debates: 25 years of challenges in understanding the psychology of gender. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 340–357.

Eagly, A. H., Karau, S. J., & Makhijani, M. G. (1995). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 125–145.

Earle, S., Komaromy, C., & Layne, L. (Eds.). (2012). Understanding reproductive loss: Perspectives on life, death and fertility. Farnham, UK: Ashgate.

Easterbrook, M. A., Kisilevsky, B. S., Muir, D. W., & Laplante, D. P. (1999). Newborns discriminate schematic faces from scrambled faces. Cana- dian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 231–241.

Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S., Ross, J., Hawkins, J., et al. (2008). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2007. Morbidity & Mortality Weekly Report, 57, 1–131.

Ebberwein, C. A. (2001). Adaptability and the characteristics necessary for managing adult career transition: A qualitative investigation. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 62(1-B), 545.

Eddleston, K. A., Baldridge, D. C., & Veiga, J. F. (2004). Toward modeling the predictors of managerial career success: Does gender matter? Journal of Managerial Psychology, 19, 360–385. doi:10.1108/02683940410537936

Edelson, L. R., Mokdad, C., & Martin. N. (2016). Prompts to eat novel and familiar fruits and vegetables in families with 1–3 year-old children: Relationships with food acceptance and intake. Appetite, 99, 138–148.

Eden Alternative. (2017). Mission, vision, values, principles. Retrieved from www .e denalt .org/ab out- the-e den-alternative/ mission-vision-values/.

Edwards, C. A. (1994). Leadership in groups of school-age girls. Developmental Psychology, 30, 920–927.

Edwards, L. A., Wagner, J. B., Simon, C. E., & Hyde,  D.  C. (2016). Functional brain organi- zation for number processing in pre-verbal infants. Developmental Science, 19, 757–769.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

606 R e f e R e n c e s

Edwards, M. R. (2012). A temporal multifaceted adaptation approach to the experiences of employed mothers. Marriage and Family Review, 48, 732–768. doi:10.1080/01494929.20 12.700911

Edwards, R. C., Thullen, M. J., Isarowong, N., Shiu, C-S., Henson, L., & Hans, S. L. (2012). Supportive relationships and the trajectory of depressive symptoms among young , African American mothers. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 585–594.

Ehrlich, K. B., Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2012). Tipping points in adolescent adjustment: Predicting social functioning from adolescents’ conflict with parents and friends. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 776–783.

Eime, R . M., Young, J. A ., Harvey, J. T., Charity, M. J., & Payne, W. R. (2013). A systematic review of the psychological and social benefits of participation in sport for children and adolescents: Informing development of a conceptual model of health through sport. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 10, 98.

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665–697.

Eisenberg, N., & Shell, R. (1986). Prosocial moral judgment and behavior in children: The mediating role of cost. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 426–433.

Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Spinrad, T., Gershoff, E., Valiente, C., Losoya, S. L., et al. (2008). Understanding parent-adolescent conflict discussions: Concurrent and across-time prediction from youths’ dispositions and parenting. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 73, Serial No. 290.

Eisenberg, N., Michalik, N., Spinrad, T. L., Hofer, C., Kupfer, A., Valiente, C., et al. (2007). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’s symptoms: A longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 22, 544–567.

Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Prosocial development. In R M. Lerner (Ed)., Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol. 3., pp. 610–656). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Eisenberg, N., VanSchyndel, S. K., & Spinrad, T. L. (2016). Prosocial motivation: Inferences from an opaque body of work. Child Development, 87, 1668–1678.

Eisma, M. C., Schut, H. A. W., Stroebe, M. S., Voerman, K., van den Bout, J., Stroebe, W. et al. (2015). Psychopathology symptoms, rumination and autobiographical memory specificity : Do associations hold after bereavement? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 478–484. doi:10.1002/acp.3120 a

Eizenman, D. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (1998). Infants’ perception of object unity in translating and rotating displays. Developmental Psychology, 34, 426–434.

El Nokali, N. E., Bachman, H. J., & Votruba- Drzal, E. (2010). Parent involvement and children’s academic and social development in elementary school. Child Development, 81, 988–1005.

Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Weinbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Tau b , E . ( 1 9 9 5 ) . I n c re a s e d co r t i c a l representation of the fingers of the left hand in strings players. Science, 270, 305–307.

Elkind, D. (1978). The child’s reality: Three developmental themes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Elkind, D., & Bowen, R. (1979). Imaginary audience behavior in children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 15, 38–44.

Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920–958.

Ellis, J. W. (2010). Yours, mine, ours? Why the Texas legislature should simplify caretaker consent capabilities for minor children and the implications of the addition of Chapter 34 to the Texas Family Code. Texas Tech Law Review, 42, 987. Retrieved from papers.ssrn.com/sol3/ papers.cfm?abstract_id=1811045.

Else-Quest, N. M., Higgins, A., Allison, C., & Morton, L. C. (2012). Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 947–981.

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127.

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 33–72.

Emerson, R. W., & Cantlon, J. F. (2015). Continuity and change in children’s longitudinal neural response to numbers. Developmental Science, 18, 314–326.

Endenburg, N., van Lith, H. A., & Kirpensteijn, J. (2014). Longitudinal study of Dutch children’s attachment to companion animals. Society & Animals, 22, 390–414.

Engel, S. M., Berkowitz, G. S., Wolff, M. S., & Yehuda, R . (2005). Psychological trauma associated with the World Trade Center attacks and its effect on pregnancy outcome. Paediatric & Perinatal Epidemiology, 19, 334–341.

Engelberg, A. (2016). Religious Zionist singles: Caught between “family values” and “young adulthood.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 55, 349–364. doi:10.1111/jssr.12259

English, D., Lambert S. F., & Ialongo, N. S. (2014). Longitudinal associations between experience d racial discrimination and depressive symptoms in African American adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1190–1196.

English, T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2014). Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in daily life. International Jo ur n al o f B e h a v i o ral D e v e l o p m e n t . doi:10.1177/0165025413515404

English, T., & Carstensen L. L. (2016). Socioemotional selectivity theory. In N. A Pachana (Ed.), Encyclopedia of geropsychology. New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-287-080-3_110-1

Englund, M. M., White, B., Reynolds, A. J., Schweinhart, L. J., & Campbell, F. A. (2014).

Health outcomes of the Abecedarian, Child- Parent Center, and HighScope Perry preschool programs. In A. J. Reynolds, A. J. Rolnick, & J. A. Temple (Eds.), Health and education in early childhood: Predictors, interventions, and policies (pp. 257–285). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2016a). Sexual harassment. Retrieved from www.eeoc.gov/laws/types/sexual_harassment .cfm.

Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2016b). Charges alleging sexual harassment FY2010-FY2015. Retrieved from www.eeoc .gov/eeoc/statistics/enforcement/sexual_ harassment_new.cfm.

Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (2016c). Age discrimination. Retrieved from www.eeoc.gov/laws/types/age.cfm.

Equality and Human Rights Commission. (2016). What is the Equality Act? Retrieved from www.equalityhumanrights .com/en/ equality-act-2010/what-equality-act.

Erickson, K. I., Gildengers, A. G., & Butters, M. A. (2013). Physical activity and brain plasticity in late adulthood. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15, 99–108. Retrieved from www .ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3622473/.

Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Creative genius: A view from the expert-performance approach. In D. K. Simonton (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of genius (pp. 321–349). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Ericsson, K. A., & Towne, T. J. (2010). Expertise. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 404–416. doi:10.1002/wcs.47

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

Erikson, E. H. (1982). The life cycle completed: Review. New York: Norton.

Erikson, E. H., Erikson, J. M., & Kivnick, H. Q. (1986). Vital involvement in old age. New York: Norton.

Esch, T. (2014). The neurobiology of meditation and mindfulness. In S. Schmidt & H. Walach (Eds.), Meditation—Neuroscientific approaches and philosophical implication (pp. 153–173). New York: Springer.

Eskritt, M., & McLeod, K. (2008). Children’s note taking as a mnemonic tool. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 52–74.

Espy, K. A., Fang, H., Johnson, C., Stopp, C., Wiebe, S. A., & Respass, J. (2011). Prenatal tobacco exposure: Developmental outcomes in the neonatal period. Developmental Psychology, 47, 153–169.

Etezadi, S., & Pushkar, D. (2013). Why are wise people happier? An explanatory model of wisdom and emotional well-being in older adults. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 929– 950. doi:10.1007/s10902-012-9362-2

Ethics Committee of the American Society for Reproductive Medicine. (2013). Oocyte or embryo donation to women of advanced reproductive age: An Ethics Committee opinion. Fer tilit y and Ster ilit y, 100, 337–340.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 607

Ethics Committee of the American Society for Reproductive Medicine. (2015). Sex selection for nonmedical reasons. Fertility and Sterility, 103, 1418–1422.

European Association for Palliative Care. (2011). The EAPC ethics task force on palliative care and euthanasia. Retrieved from www.eapcnet .eu/Themes/Ethics/PCeuthanasiataskforce/ tabid/232/Default.aspx.

Evans, D. G. R., Barwell, J., Eccles, D. M., Collins, A., Izatt, L., Jacobs, C., et al. (2014). The Angelina Jolie effect: How high celebrity profile can have a major impact on provision of cancer-related services. Breast Cancer Research, 16, 442. doi:10.1186/s13058-014-0442-6. Retrieved from breast-cancer-research.biomedcentral .com/articles/10.1186/s13058-014-0442-6.

Evans, J. St. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223–241.

Even-Zohar, A., & Garby, A. (2016). Great- grandparents’ role perception and its contribution to their quality of life. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 14, 197–219. doi:10.1080/15350770.2016.1195246

Evertsson, M., Grunow, D., & Aisenbrey, S. (2016). Work interruptions and young women’s career prospects in Germany, Sweden, and the US. Work, Employment and Society, 30, 291–308. doi:10.1177/0950017015598283

Faber, A. J. (2004). Examining remarried couples through a Bowenian family systems lens. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 40, 121–133. doi:10.1300/J087v40n03_08

Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Jones, S., Smith, M., Guthrie, I., Poulin, R ., et al. (1999). Regulation, emotionality, and preschoolers’ socially competent peer interactions. Child Development, 70, 432–442.

Fabius, C. D. (2016). Toward an integration of narrative identity, generativity, and storytelling in African American elders. Journal of Black Studies, 47, 423–434. doi:10.1177/0021934716638801

Fagard, J. (2013). The nature and nurture of human infant hand preference. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288, 114–123.

Fajgenbaum, D., Chesson, B., & Lanzl, R. G. (2012). Building a network of grief support on college campuses: A national grassroots initiative. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 26, 99–120. doi:10.1080/87568225.2012.659159

Falbo, T. (2012). Only children: An updated review. Journal of Individual Psychology, 68, 38–49.

Falbo, T., & Polit, E. F. (1986). Quantitative review of the only child literature: Research evidence and theor y development. Psycholog ical Bulletin, 100, 176–186.

Farr, R. H., Diamond, L. M., & Boker, S. M. (2014). Female same-sex sexuality from a dynamical systems perspective: Sexual desire, motivation, and behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 1477–1490.

Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A. J., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Empathy, perspective taking

and prosocial behaviour: The importance of parenting practices. Infant and Child Development, 21, 175–188.

Farrant, B. M., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: Longitudinal analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. Child Development, 83, 223–235.

Farrington, D. P., & Koegl, C. J. (2015). Monetary benefits and costs of the Stop Now and Plan Program for boys aged 6–11, based on the prevention of later offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 31, 263–287.

Farris, D. N. (2016).  Boomerang kids: The demography of previously launched adults. New York: Springer.

Farver, J. M., & Shin, Y. L. (1997). Social pretend play in Korean- and Anglo-American preschoolers. Child Development, 68, 544–556.

Fasig, L. G. (2000). Toddlers’ understanding of ownership: Implications for self-concept development. Social Development, 9, 370–382.

Fazel, S., Bakiyeva, L., Cnattingius, S., Grann, M., Hultman, C. M., Litchtenstein, P., et al. (2012). Perinatal risk factors in offenders with severe personality disorder: A population-based investigation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 737–750.

Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2016). Crime in the United States, 2015. Washington DC: Author.

Fein, E., & Schneider, S. (2013). Not your mother’s rules: The new secrets for dating. New  York: Grand Central Publishing.

Feinberg, M. E., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Cumsille, P. (2003). Sibling differentiation: Sibling and parental relationships trajectories in adolescence. Child Development, 74, 1261–1274.

Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at one and two years. Child Development, 71, 310–322.

Feldman, K. (2010). Post parenthood redefined: Race, class, and family structure differences in the experience of launching children. Doctoral dissertation completed at Case Western Reserve University. Retrieved from e t d . o h i o l i n k . e d u / p g _ 1 0 ? 0 : : N O : 1 0 : P 1 0 _ ACCESSION_NUM:case1267730564.

Feldman, R. (2015). Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 1007–1023.

Felix, E., De Haan, H., Vaandrager, L., & Koelen, M. (2015). Beyond thresholds: The everyday lived experience of the house by older people. Journal of Housing for the elderly, 29, 329–347. doi:10.1080/027633893.2015.1055027

Felkey, A. J. (in press). Covenant marriages: Increasing commitment or costs? Eastern Economic Journal. doi:10.1057/eej.2016.3

Feltz, A . (2015). Ever yday attitudes about euthanasia and the slippery slope argument. In M. Cholbi  & J. Varelius (Eds.), New directions in the ethics of assisted suicide and euthanasia (pp. 217–237). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-22050-55_13

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., et al. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (Whole No. 173).

Fergusson, D. M.,  & Woodward, L. J. (2000). Teenage pregnancy and female educational underachievement : A prospective study of a New Zealand birth cohort. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 62, 147–161.

Fernández-Alcántara , M., Cruz-Quintana , Pérez-Marfil, M. N., Catena-Martinez, A., Pérez-Garcia, M., & Turnbull, O. H. (2016). Assessment of emotional and emotional recognition in complicated grief. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00126. Retrieved from www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/ articles/PMC4751347/

Fernández-Carro, C. (2016). Ageing at home, co-residence or institutionalisation? Preferred care and residential arrangements of older adults in Spain. Ageing and Society, 36, 586–6132. doi:10.1017/S0144686X1400138X

Fernyhough, C. (2010). Inner speech. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of the mind. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ferrarini, T., & Norström, T. (2010). Family policy, economic development, and infant mortality: A longitudinal comparative analysis. International Journal of Social Welfare, 19, S89– S102. doi:10.1111/j.1468–2397.2010.00736.x

Ferreol-Barbey, M., Piolat, A., & Roussey, J. (2000). Text recomposition by eleven-year-old children: Effects of text length, level of reading comprehension, and mastery of prototypical schema . Archive s de P sycholog ie , 68, 213–232.

Fidler, E. (2012). Sickle cell trait: A review and recommendations for training. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 34, 28–32.

Field, M. J., & Cassel, C. K. (2010). Approaching death: Improving care at the end of life. In D. Meier, S. L. Isaacs, & R. G. Hughes (Eds.), Palliative care: Transforming the care of serious illness (pp. 79–91). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Field, T. (2010). Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting, and safety practices: A review. Infant Behavior  & Development, 33, 1–6.

Field, T. M.,  & Widmayer, S. M. (1982). Motherhood. In B. J. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Field, T., Diego, M., & Hernandez-Reif, M. (2010). Preterm infant message therapy research: A review. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 115–124.

Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (2010). Marriage in the new millennium: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 630–649. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00722.x

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

608 R e f e R e n c e s

Fingerman, K. L., Pillemer, K. A., Silverstein, M., & Suitor, J. J. (2012). The baby boomers’ intergen- erational relationships. The Gerontologist, 52, 199–209. doi:10.1093/geront/gnr139

Finkbeiner, N. W. B., Max, J. E., Longman, S., & Debert, C. (2016). Knowing what we don’t know : Long-term psychiatric outcomes following adult concussion in sports. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61, 27–276. doi:10.1177/0706743716644953

Fiori, K. L., & Denckla, C. A. (2015). Friendship and happiness among middle-aged adults. In M. Demir (Ed.), Friendship and happiness (pp. 137–154). New York: Springer.

Fischer-Shofty, M., Levkovitz, Y.,  & Shamay- Tsoory, S. G. (2013). Oxytocin facilitates accurate perception of competition in men and kinship in women. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi:10.1093/scan/ nsr100

Fisher, H. (2016). Anatomy of love: A natural history of mating, marriage, and why we stray (revised and updated). New  York: W. W. Norton.

Fisher, L . L . (2010). Se x , romance , and relationships: AARP survey of midlife and older adults. Retrieved from assets.aarp.org/ rgcenter/general /srr_09.pdf.

Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social cognition: From brains to culture (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fitzgerald, J. (1987). Research on revision in writing. Review of Educational Research, 57, 481–506.

Fivush, R. (2014). Maternal reminiscing style: The sociocultural construction of autobiographical memory across childhood and adolescence. In P J. Bauer  & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 568–585). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Flavell, J. H. (1996). Piaget’s legacy. Psychological Science, 7, 200–203.

Fl avell, J. H. (2000). D e velopment of children’s knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 15–23.

Fleming, P. J.,  & Blair, P. S. (2015). Making informed choices on co-sleeping with your baby. BMJ, 350:h563.

Flynn, J. R. (1999). Searching for justice: The discovery of IQ gains over time. American Psychologist, 54, 5–20.

Flynn, J. R., & Weiss, L. G. (2007). American IQ gains from 1932 to 2002: The WISC subtests and educational progress. International Journal of Testing, 7, 209–224.

Fogarty, K.,  & Evans, G. D. (2010). Being an involved father: What does it mean? Retrieved from edis.ifas.ufl.edu/he141.

Formosa, M. (2014). Four decades of universities of the Third Age: Past, present, future. Ageing and Society, 34, 42–66. doi:10.1017/ S0144686X12000797

Foshee, V. A., & Reyes, H. L. (2012). Dating abuse: Prevalence, consequences, and predictors.

In R J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 1, pp. 602–615). New York: Springer.

Foshee, V. A ., Benefield, T., Dixon, K. S., Chang, L-Y., Senkomago, V., Ennett, S. T. et  al. (2015). The effects of Moms and Teens for Safe Dates: A dating abuse prevention program for adolescents exposed to domestic violence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 995–1010.

Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Ennett, S. T., Cance, J. D., Bauman, K. E., & Bowling, J. M. (2012). Assessing the effects of Families for Safe Dates, a family-based teen dating abuse prevention program. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 349–356.

Foster, B. P., Lonial, S.,  & Shastri, T. (2011). Mentoring, career plateau tendencies, turnover intentions and implications for narrowing pay and position gaps due to gender—Structural equations modeling. Journal of Applied Business Research, 27, 71–84. Retrieved from www.journals.cluteonline.com/index.php/ JABR/article/view/6467/6545.

Foster, E. M., & Watkins, S. (2010). The value of reanalysis: TV viewing and attention problems. Child Development, 81, 368–375.

Foster, S. E., Jones, D. J., Olson, A. L., Forehand, R., Gaffney, C. A., Zens, M. S., et  al. (2007). Family socialization of adolescents’ self- reported cigarette use: The role of parents’ history of regular smoking and parenting style. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 481–493.

Foster, S. H. (1986). Learning discourse topic management in the preschool years. Journal of Child Language, 13, 231–250.

Fouad, N. A. (2007). Work and vocational psychology: Theory, research, and applications. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 543–564. doi:10.1146/annurev.psyc.58.110405.085713

Fouad, N., Ghosh, A., Chang, W-h., Figueiredo, C., & Bachhuber, T. (2016). Career exploration among college students. Journal of College Stu- dent Development, 57, 460–464. doi:10.1353/ csd.2016.0047

Fowler, K. L. (2008). “The wholeness of things”: Infusing diversity and social justice into death education. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 57, 53–91. doi:10.2190/OM.57.1.d

Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences inf luence the de velopment of brain architecture. Child Development, 81, 28–40.

Fozard, J. L., & Gordon-Salant, S. (2001). Changes in vision and hearing with aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 241–266). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Fraley, R. C.,  & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity : A dy namic mo del for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review, 112, 60–74. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.60

Franceschi, K. A. (2005). The experience of the transition to motherhood in women who have suffered maternal loss in adolescence.

Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 65(8-B), 4282.

Frank, D. J., Jordano, M. L., Browne, K.,  & Touron,  D. R. (2016). Older adults’ use of retrieval strategies in everyday life. Gerontology, 62, 624–635. doi:10.1159/000446277

Frank, M. G., Weber, M. D., Watkins, L. R.,  & Maier, S. F. (2016). Stress-induced neuroinflam- matory priming: A liability factor in the etiology of psychiatric disorders. Neurobiology of Stress, 66, 82–90. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2015.12.004

Frank, N., (2016). Moving beyond anti-LGBT politics: Commentary on “Same-sex and different-sex parent households and child health outcomes: Findings from the National Sur vey of Children’s Health.” Journal of Developmental  & Behavioral Pediatrics, 37, 245–247. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000295

Franken, A., Prinstein, M. J., Dijkstra, J. K., Steglich, C. E. G., Harakeh, Z., & Vollebergh, W. A. M. (2016). Early adolescent friendship selected based on externalizing behavior: the moderating role of pubertal development. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 1647–1657.

Franses, P. H. (2016). When did classic composers make their best work? Creativity Research Journal, 28, 219–221. doi:10.1080/10400419. 2016.1162489

Frase, P., & Gornick, J. C. (2013). The time divide in cross-national perspective: The work week, education and institutions that matter. Social Forces, 91, 697–724. doi:10.1093/sf/sos189

Frazier, L. D., Hooker, K., Johnson, P. M.,  & Kaus, C. R. (2000). Continuity and change in possible selves in later life: A 5-year longitudinal study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 237–243. doi:10.1207/S15324834BASP2203_10

Frazier, L. D., Johnson, P. M., Gonzalez, G. K., & Kafka, C. L. (2002). Psychosocial influences on possible selves: A comparison of three cohorts of older adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 308–317. doi:10.1080/01650250143000184

Fredricks, J. A.,  & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Family socialization, gender, and sport motivation and involvement. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 27, 3–31.

Freitas, A. A.,  & de Magalhães, J. P. (2011). A review and appraisal of the DNA damage theory of aging. Mutation Research/Reviews in Mutation Research, 728, 12–22. doi:10.1016/j. mrrev.2011.06.01

French, S. E., Seidman, E., Allen, L.,  & Aber, J. (2006). The development of ethnic identity during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1–10.

Fried, A. (2005). Depression in adolescence. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 1, pp. 332–334). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Friedman, E. M., & Seltzer, J. A. (2010). Providing for older parents: Is it a family affair? California Center for Population Research paper #PWP- CCPR-2010-12. Retrieved from papers.ccpr. ucla.edu/papers/PWP-CCPR-2010-012/PWP- CCPR-2010-012.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 609

Friesen, M. D., Horwood, L. J., Fergusson, D. M., & Woodward, L. J. (2017). Exposure to parental separation in childhood and later parenting quality as an adult: Evidence from a 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychol- ogy and Psychiatry, 58, 30–37. doi:10.1111/ jcpp.12610

Frith, C. D.,  & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Social Cognition, 63, 287–313. doi:10.1146/ annurevpsych-120710-100449

Froehlich, D. E., Beausaert, S.,  & Segers, M. (2016). Aging and the motivation to stay employable. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 31, 756–770. doi:10.1108/JMP-08-2014-0224

Fr uhauf, C . A . (2007). Grandchildren’s perceptions of caring for grandparents. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(3-A), 1120.

Fujimoto, K., Unger, J. B., & Valente, T. W. (2012). A network method of measuring affiliation- based peer influence: Assessing the influences of teammates’ smoking on adolescent smoking. Child Development 83, 442–451.

Fuligni, A. J., Kiang, L., Witkow, M. R.,  & Baldelomar, O. (2008). Stability and change in ethnic labeling among adolescents from Asian and Latin American immigrant families. Child Development, 79, 944–956.

Fung, H. H., & Siu, T. M. Y. (2010). Time, culture, and life-cycle changes of social goals. In T. W. Miller (Ed.), Handbook of stressful transitions across the lifespan (pp. 441–464). New  York: Springer.

Furman, W.,  & Rose, A. J. (2015). Friendships, romantic relationships, and peer relationships. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol 3., pp. 932–974). Hoboken NJ: Wiley.

Furukawa, E., Tangney, J.,  & Higashibara, F. (2012). Cross-cultural continuities and discontinuities in shame, guilt, and pride: A study of children residing in Japan, Korea, and the USA. Self and Identity, 11, 90–133.

Gaddis, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1985). The male experience of pubertal change. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 61–69.

Gagliardi, A. (2005). Postpartum depression. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 2, pp. 867–870). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Gagne, J. R., Miller, M. M.,  & Goldsmith, H. H. (2013). Early—but modest—gender differences in focal aspects of childhood temperament. Per- sonality and Individual Differences, 55, 95–100.

Galán, C. A., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J.,  & Wilson, M. N. (2017). Neighborhood depri- vation during early childhood and conduct problems in middle childhood: Mediation by aggressive response generation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, in press.

Galle, M. E., & McMurray, B. (2014). The devel- opment of voicing categories: A quantita- tive review of over 40 years of infant speech perception research. Psychonomic Bulletin  & Review, 21, 884–906.

Galván, A. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to rewards. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 88–93.

Gamaldo, A. A.,  & Allaire, J. C. (2016). Daily fluctuations in everyday cognition: Is it meaningful? Journal of Aging and Health, 28, 834–849. doi:10.1177/0898264315611669

Gamba, F. (2015). The digital age of grieving rituals: Mobility and the hybridization of memory. A project. Retrieved from http:// wp.lancs.ac.uk/futures-of-the-end-of-life/ files/2015/11/Fiorenza-Gamba.pdf.

Gamble, W. C., Ramakumar, S.,  & Diaz, A. (2007). Maternal and paternal similarities and differences in parenting: An examination of Mexican-American parents of young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 72–88. doi:10.1016/j. ecresq.2006.11.004

Gandy, S., & DeKosky, S. T. (2013). Toward the treatment and prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: Rational strategies and recent progress. Annual Review of Medicine, 64, 367– 383. doi:10.1146/annurevmed-092611-084441

Ganong, L.,  & Coleman, M. (2017). Stepfamily relationships: Development, dynamics, and interventions (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

Gans, D. (2007). Normative obligations and parental care in social context. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(5-A), 2115.

Garber, J.,  & Rao, U. (2014). Depression in children and adolescents. In M. Lewis & K. D. Rudolph (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 489–520). New  York: Springer Science+Business Media.

Garcia, J. R., Reiber, C., Massey, S. G.,  & Merriwether, A. M. (2013). Sexual hook-up culture. Monitor on Psychology, 44, 60–67.

Garciaguirre, J. S., Adolph, K. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). Baby carriage: Infants walking with loads. Child Development, 78, 664–680.

Gardiner, P. A., Mishra, G. D.,  & Dobson, A. J. (2016). The effect of socioeconomic status across adulthood on trajectories of frailty in older women. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 17, 372.e1–372.e3. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2015.12.090

Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (1995). Reflections on multiple intelligences: Myths and messages. Phi Delta Kappan, 77, 200–203, 206–209.

Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (2002). MI millennium: Multiple intelligences for the new millennium [Video recording]. Los Angeles , CA: Into the Classroom Media.

Gardner, H. (2011). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, M., Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Sports participation and juvenile delinquency: The role of the peer context among adolescent boys and girls with varied histories of problem

behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45, 341–353.

Garrard, E.,  & Wilkinson, S. (2005). Passive euthanasia. Journal of Medical Ethics, 31, 64–68. doi:10.1136/jme.2003.005777

Garrido, M. M., Prigerson, H. G., Bao, Y.,  & Maciejewski, P. K. (2016). Chemotherapy use in the months before death and estimated costs of care in the last week of life. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 51, 875–881. doi:10.1016/j.painsymman.2015.12.323

Garthe, R. C., Sullivan, T. N., & McDaniel, M. A. (2017). A meta-analytic review of peer risk factors and dating violence. Psychology of Violence, 7, 45–57.

Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Rothbart, M. K., Robertson, C., Iddins, E., Ramsay, K.,  & Schlect, S. (2010). A latent growth examination of fear development in infancy: Contributions of maternal depression and the risk for toddler anxiety. Developmental Psychology, 46, 651–668.

Gartstein, M. A., Knyazev, G. G., & Slobodskaya, H. R. (2005). Cross-cultural differences in the structure of infant temperament: United States of America and Russia. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 54–61.

Garvey, C.,  & Berninger, G. (1981). Timing and turn taking in children’s conversations. Discourse Processes, 4, 27–59.

Gass, K., Jenkins, J., & Dunn, J. (2007). Are sibling relationships protective? A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 167–175.

Gaudron, J.-P.,  & Vautier, S. (2007). Analyzing individual differences in vocational, leisure, and family interests: A multitrait-multimethod approach. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 70, 561–573. doi:10.1016/j. jvb.2007.01.004

Gavin, L. A.,  & Furman, W. (1996). Adolescent girls’ relationships with mothers and best friends. Child Development, 67, 375–386.

Ge, X., Brody, G. H., Conger, R. D., Simons, R. L., & Murphy, V. M. (2002). Contextual amplification of pubertal transition effects on deviant peer affiliation and externalizing behavior among African American children. Developmental Psychology, 38, 45–54.

Geary, D. C. (2002). Sexual selection and human life history. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 30, pp. 41–102). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Geerdts, M. S., Van de Walle, G. A., & LoBue, V. (2015). Daily animal exposure and children’s biological concepts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 132–146.

Gelman, R.,  & Meck, E. (1986). The notion of principle: The case of counting. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gelman, S. A . (2003). The essential child. New York: Oxford.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

610 R e f e R e n c e s

Gelman, S. A., Taylor, M. G.,  & Nguyen, S. P. (2004). Mother-child conversations about gender. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69 (Serial No. 275).

Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national study. Psychological Science, 20, 594–602.

George, L. K., Palmore, E., & Cohen, H. J. (2014). The Duke Center for the Study of Aging: One of our earliest roots. The Gerontologist, 54, 59–66. doi:10.1093/geront/gnt049

Gerry, D. W., Faux, A. L., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Effects of Kindermusik training on infants’ rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science, 13, 545–551.

Gershoff, E. T. (2013). Spanking and child development: We know enough now to stop hitting our children. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 133–137.

Gervais, S. J., Wiener, R. L., Allen, J., Farnum, K. S., & Kimble, K. (2016). Do you see what I see? The consequences of objectification in work settings for experiencers and third-party predictors. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 16, 143–174. doi:10.1111/asap.12118

Ghanim, D. (2015). The virginity trap in the Middle East. New York: Springer.

Ghetti, S. (2008). Rejection of false events in childhood: A metamemory account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 16–20.

Ghetti, S.,  & Lee, J. (2011). Children’s episodic memor y. W IRE s Cog nitive Science , 2, 365–373.

Ghetti, S., Hembacher, E.,  & Coughlin, C. A. (2013). Feeling uncertain and acting on it during the preschool years: A metacognitive approach. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 160–165.

Ghosh, S., Capistrant, B.,  & Friedemann- Sánchez, G. (2017). Who will care for the elder caregiver? Outlining theoretical approaches and future research questions. In T. Samanta (Ed.),  Cross-cultural and cross-disciplinary perspectives in social gerontology (pp. 23–43). New York: Springer.

Gibbs, J. C., Clark, P. M., Joseph, J. A., Green, J. L., Goodrick, T. S.,  & Makowski, D. (1986). Relations between moral judgment, moral courage, and field independence. Child Development, 57, 185–193.

Gibran, K. (1923). The prophet. New York: Knopf. Gibson, E. J.,  & Walk, R. D. (1960). The “visual

cliff.” Scientific American, 202, 64–71. Giles , J. W.,  & He yman, G. D. (2005).

Reconceptualizing children’s suggestibility: Bidirectional and temporal properties. Child Development, 76, 40–53.

Gilleard, C .,  & Higgs , P. (2010). Aging without agency: Theorizing the fourth age. Aging and Mental Health, 14, 121–128. doi:10.1080/13607860903228762

Gilleard, C., & Higgs, P. (2016). Connecting life span development with the sociology of the life course: A new direction. Sociology, 50, 301–315. doi:10.1177/0038038515577906

Gilligan, M., Suitor, J. J., Kim, S., & Pillemer, K. (2013). Differential effects of perceptions of

mothers’ and fathers’ favoritism on sibling tension in adulthood. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 68B, S593–S598. doi:10.1093/ geronb/gbt039

Gilliver, M., Carter, L., Macoun, D., Rosen, J.,  & Williams, W. (2012). Music to whose ears? The effect of social norms on young people’s risk per- ceptions of hearing damage resulting from their music listening behavior. Noise and Health, 14, 47–51. Retrieved from www.noiseandhealth.org/ article.asp?issn=1463-1741;year=2012;volume= 14;issue=57;spage=47;epage=51;aulast=Gilliver.

Gilsanz, V.,  & Ratib, O. (2012). Hand bone age (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

Giménez-Dasi, M., Pons, F.,  & Bender, P. K. (2016). Imaginary companions, theory of mind and emotion understanding in young children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24, 186–197.

Giordano, G. (2005). How testing came to dominate American schools: The history of educational assessment. New York: Peter Lang.

Giordano, P. C., Copp, J. E., Longmore, M. A., & Manning, W. D. (2016). Anger, control, and intimate partner violence in young adulthood. Journal of Family Violence , 31, 1–13. doi:10.1007/s10896-015-9753-3

Giordano, P. C., Kaufman, A., Manning, W. D., & Longmore, M. A. (2015). Teen dating violence: the influence of friendships and school context. Sociological Focus, 48, 150–171.

Giordano, P. C., Manning, W. D., Longmore, M. A., & Flanigan, C. M. (2012). Developmental shifts in the character of romantic and sexual relationships from adolescence to young adulthood. In A. Booth, S. L. Brown, N. S. Landale, W. D. Manning,  & S. M. McHale (Eds.), Early adulthood in a family context (pp. 133–164). New York: Springer.

Gire, J. (2014). How death imitates life: Cultural influences on conceptions of death and dying. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 6(2). doi:10.9707/2307-0919.1120

Givertz, M., Segrin, C.,  & Hansal, A. (2009). The association between satisfaction and commitment differs across marital couple types. Communication Research, 36, 561–584. doi:10.1177/0093650209333035

Gizer, I. R.,  & Waldman, I. D. (2012). Double dissociation between lab measures of inattention and impulsivity and the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) and the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 1011–1023.

Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. New York: Little, Brown.

Glick, G. C.,  & Rose, A. J. (2011). Prospective associations between friendship adjustment and social strategies: Friendship as a context for building social skills. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1117–1132.

Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children. (2011). States with full prohibition. Retrieved from www.endcorporalpunishment .org.

Godfrey, D. (2016). End-of-life issues for LGBT elders. In D. A. Harley & P. B. Teaster (Eds.),

Handbook of LGBT elders (pp. 439–454). New York: Springer.

Golant, S. M. (2012). Out of their residential comfort and mastery zones: Toward a more relevant environmental gerontology. Journal of Housing for the Elderly, 26, 26–43. doi:10.108 0/027663893.2012.655654

Gold, J. M. (2016). Stepping in, stepping out: Creating stepfamily rhythm. Alexandria, VA: American Counseling Association.

Goldbach, J. T., Cardoso, J. D., Cervantes, R. C., & Duan, L. (2015). The relation between stress and alcohol use among Hispanic adolescents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 960–968.

Goldberg, A . E. (2009). Lesbian and gay parents and their children: Research on the family life cycle. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Goldfield, B. A.,  & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17, 171–184.

Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C., & Franklin, A. (2007). How children make language out of gesture: Morphological structure in gesture systems developed by American and Chinese deaf children. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 87–135.

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, K. A.,  & Lemery, K. S. (1997). Toddler and childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environ- ment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 891–905.

Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19, 515–523.

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.

Golinkoff, R. M. (1993). When is communication a “meeting of minds”? Journal of Child Language, 20, 199–207.

Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (Baby) talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 339–344.

Golkar, A., Johansson, E., Kasahara, M., Osika, W., Perski, A., & Savic, I. (2014). The influence of work-related chronic stress on the regulation of emotion and on functional connectivity in the brain. PLOS ONE 9: e104550. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0104550

Golombok , S. (2013). Families created by reproductive donation: Issues and research. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 61–65.

Gonyea, J. G. (2013). Midlife, multigenerational bonds, and caregiving. In R. C. Talley & R. J. V Montgomery (Eds.), Caregiving across the lifespan (pp. 105–130). New York: Springer.

Gonza, G.,  & Burger, A. (in press). Subjective well-being during the 2008 economic crisis: Identification of mediating and moderating factors . Journal of Happiness Studies. doi:10.1007/s10902-016-9797-y

Good, T. L.,  & Brophy, J. E. (2008). Looking in classrooms. Boston: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 611

Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R. Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathol- ogy: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 1–27.

Goodnow, J. J. (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Goodsell, T. L. (2013). Familification: Family, neighborhood change, and housing policy. Housing Studies, 28, 845–868. doi:10.1080/02 673037.2013.768334

Goodwyn, S. W.,  & Acredolo, L. P. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word: Is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Child Development, 64, 688–701.

Gordon, B. N., Baker-Ward, L.,  & Ornstein, P. A. (2001). Children’s testimony: A review of research on memory for past experiences. Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review, 4, 157–181.

Gordon, R. A., Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Brooks- Gunn, J. (2004). Extended households and the life course of young mothers: Understanding the associations using a sample of mothers with premature, low birth weight babies. Child Development, 75, 1013–1038.

Gordon, T. A. (2013). Good grief: Exploring the dimensionality of grief experiences and social work support. Journal of Social Work in End- of-Life and Palliative Care, 9, 27–42. doi: 10.1080/15524256.2012.758607

Gosso, Y., Morais, M. L. S.,  & Otta, E. (2007). Pretend play of Brazilian children: A window into different cultural worlds. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 38, 539–558.

Gostin, L. O., & Roberts, A. E. (2016). Physician- assisted dying: A turning point? JAMA, 315(3), 249–250. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.16586

Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 243–268.

Gottman, J. M.,  & Levenson, R. W. (2000). The timing of divorce: Predicting when a couple will divorce over a 14-year period. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 737–745. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00737.x

Gottman, J. M.,  & Silver, N. (2015). The seven principles for making marriage work (rev. ed.). New York: Harmony Books.

Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

Graber, J. A . (2013). Pubertal timing and the development of psychopathology in adolescence and beyond. Hormones and Behavior, 64, 262–269.

Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 491– 505. doi:10.1038/nrn3256

Graham-Bermann, S. A.,  & Brescoll, V. (2000). Gender, power, and violence: Assessing the family stereotypes of the children of batterers. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 600–612.

Graham, E. K.,  & Lachman, M. E. (2012). Personality and aging. In S. K. Whitbourne &

M. J. Sliwinski (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of adulthood and aging (pp. 254– 272). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). A meta-analysis of writing instruction for adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 445–476.

Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P.,  & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182.

Grammer, J. K., Purtell, K. M., Coffman, J. L., & Ornstein, P. A. (2011). Relations between children’s metamemor y and strateg ic performance: Time-varying covariates in early elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 139–155

Grammer, J., Coffman, J. L.,  & Ornstein, P. (2013). The effect of teachers’ memory- relevant language on children’s strategy use and knowledge. Child Development, 84, 1989–2002.

Granbom, M., Slaug, B., Löfqvist, C., Oswald, F., & Iwarsson, S. (2016). Community relocation in very old age: Changes in housing accessibility. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 70, 7002270020p1–7002270020p9. doi:10.5014/ ajot.2016.016147

Grant, L. M. (2007). Second generation Chinese American women and the midlife transition: A qualitative analysis. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 67(10-B), 6095.

Graves, J. M., Mackelprang, J. L., Barbosa- Leiker, C., Miller, M. E.,  & Li, A. Y. (2017). Quality of life among working and non- working adolescents. Quality of Life Research, 26, 107–120.

Graves, R.,  & Landis, T. (1990). Asymmetry in mouth opening during different speech tasks. International Journal of Psychology, 25, 179–189.

Grawitch, M. J., Barber, L. K.,  & Justice, L. (2010). Rethinking the work-life interface: It’s not about balance, it’s about resource allocation. Health and Well-Being, 2, 127–159. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01023.x

Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. R. (2000). Factors influencing racial comparisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 26–54.

Greenberg, M. T.,  & Crnic, K. A . (1988). Longitudinal predictors of developmental status and social interaction in premature and full-term infants at age two. Child Development, 59, 554–570.

Greenberg, M. T., Lengua, L. J., Coie, J. D., Pinderhughes, E. E., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999). Predicting developmental outcomes at school entry using a multiple-risk model: Four American communities. Developmental Psychology, 35, 403–417.

Greenberger, E., O’Neil, R.,  & Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking workplace and homeplace: Relations between the nature of adults’ work and their parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 30, 990–1002.

Greene, D., Tschanz, J. T., Smith, K. R., Østbye, T., Corcoran, C., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., et  al. (2014). Impact of offspring death on cognitive health in late life: The Cache County study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 1307–1315. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2013.05.002

Greenfield, E. A., & Marks, N. F. (2005). Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults’ psychological well-being. Journals of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 59, S258–S264. doi:10.1093/geronb/59.5.S258

Greenough, W. T., & Black, J. E. (1992). Induction of brain structure by experience: Substrates for cognitive development. In M. Gunnar  & C. Nelson (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology: Vol. 24: Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 155–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Greer, D. M., Want, H. H., Robinson, J. D., Varelas, P. N., Henderson, G. V., & Wijdicks, E. F. M. (2016). Variability of brain death policies in the United States. JAMA Neurology, 73, 213–218. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.3943

Gregory, A . M., Rijsdijk , F., Lau, J. Y. F., Napolitano, M., McGuffin, P.,  & Eley, T. C. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on interpersonal cognitions and associations with depressive symptoms in 8-year-old twins. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 762–775.

Gretebeck, R. J., Ferraro, K. F., Black, D. R., Holland, K.,  & Gretebeck, K. A. (2012). Longitudinal change in physical activity and disability in adults. American Journal of Health Behavior, 36, 385–394. doi:10.5993/ AJHB.36.3.9

Greving, K. A . (2007). Examining parents’ marital satisfaction trajectories: Relations with children’s temperament and family demo g raphic s . D i sser t ation Ab stract s International. Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(4-A), 1676.

Griffin, M. L., Hogan, N. L., Lambert, E. G., Tucker-Gail, K. A.,  & Baker, D. N. (2010). Job involvement, job stress, job satisfaction, and organizational commitment and the burnout of correctional staff. Criminal Ju s t i c e an d B e h a v i o r, 3 7 , 2 3 9 – 2 5 5 . doi:10.1177/0093854809351682

Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L.,  & Sternberg, R. J. (2002). School-based tests of the triarchic theor y of intelligence: Three settings , three samples, three syllabi. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 167–208.

Gripshover, S. J.,  & Markman, E. M. (2013). Teaching young children a theory of nutrition: Conceptual change and the p otential for increa se d vegetable consumption. Psychological Science, 24, 1541–1553.

Grizenko, N., Fortier, M., Gaudreau-Simard, M., Jolicoeur, C.,  & Joober, R. (2015). The effect of maternal stress during pregnancy on IQ and ADHD symptomatology. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, 92–99.

Groh, A. M., Pasco Fearon, R. M., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J.,  & Roisman, G. I. (2017). Attachment in the

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

612 R e f e R e n c e s

early life course: Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemotional development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 70–76.

Grosse, G., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Tomasello, M. ( 2 0 1 3 ) . T h r e e - y e a r- o l d s h i d e t h e i r communicative intentions in appropriate contexts. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2095–2101.

Grossman, I., Karasawa, M., Izumi, S., Na, J., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., et  al. (2012). Aging and wisdom: Culture matters. P sycholog ical Science , 23, 1059–1066. doi:10.1177/0956797612446025

Grotevant, H. D.,  & McDermott, J. M. (2014). Adoption: Biological and social processes linked to adaptation. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 235–265.

Grotevant, H. D., McRoy, R. G., Wrobel, G. M., & Ayes-Lopez, S. (2013). Contact between adoptive and birth families: Perspectives from the Minnesota/Texas Adoption Research Project. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 193–198.

Gruenewald, T. L., Liao, D. H., & Seeman, T. E. (2012). Contributing to others, contributing to oneself: Perceptions of generativity and health in later life. Journal of Gerontology: P sy ch o l o g i c al S c i e n c e s , 6 7 , 6 6 0 – 6 6 5 . doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs034

Grühn, D., Lumley, M. A., Diehl, M., Labouvie- Vief, G. (2013). Time-based indicators of emotional complexity: Interrelations and correlates. Emotion, 13, 226–237. doi:10.1037/ a0030363

Grusec, J. E. (2011). Socialization processes in the family: Social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 243–269.

Grysman, A.,  & Hudson, J. A. (2013). Gender differences in autobiographical memory: D e v e l o p m e n t a l a n d m e t h o d o l o g i c a l considerations. Developmental Review, 33, 239–272.

Gu, X., Liu, X., Van Dam, N. T., Hof, P. R., & Fan, J. (2013). Cognition-emotion integration in the anterior insular cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 20–27. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr367

Gubernskaya, Z. (2010). Changing attitudes toward marriage and children in six countries. Sociolog ical Perspectives, 53, 179–200. doi:10.1525/sop.2010.53.2.179

Gudmundson, J. A.,  & Leerkes, E. M. (2012). Links between mothers’ coping styles, toddler reactivity, and sensitivity to toddlers’ negative emotions. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 158–166.

Gunderson, E. A., & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14, 1021–1032.

Gunes, S., Hekim, G. N. T., Arslan, M. A.,  & Asci, R. (2016). Effects of aging on the male reproductive system. Journal of Assisted Reproduction and Genetics, 33, 441–454. doi:10.1007/s10815-016-0663-y

Güngör, D.,  & Bornstein, M. H. (2010). Culture-general and -specific associations

of attachment avoidance and anxiety with perceived parental warmth and psychological control among Turk and Belgian adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 593–602.

Gunnell, D.,  & Chang, S.-S. (2016). Economic recession, unemployment, and suicide. In R. C. O’Connor & J. Pirkis (Eds.), The international handbook of suicide prevention (2nd ed., pp. 284–300). Malden, MA: Wiley.

Gupta, S., Tracey, T. J. G., & Gore, P. A. (2008). Structural examination of RIASEC scales in high school students: Variation across ethnicity and method. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72, 1–13.

Gurucharri, C.,  & Selman, R. L. (1982). The development of interpersonal understanding during childhood, preadolescence, and adolescence: A longitudinal follow-up study. Child Development, 53, 924–927.

Gutiérrez, I. T., Miller, P. J., Rosengren, K. S., & Schein, S. S. (2014). Affective dimensions of death: Children’s books, questions, and understandings. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 43–61. doi:10.1111/mono.12078

Guttmacher Institute. (2013). Facts on American teens’ se xual and reproductive health. New York: Author.

Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J.,  & Wrosch, C. (2013). Developmental regulation across the life span: Toward a new synthesis. Developmental Psychology, 49, 964–972. doi:10.1037/a0029231

Hagestad, G. O., & Dannefer, D. (2001). Concepts and theories of aging: Beyond microfication in social science approaches. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (5th ed., pp. 3–21). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Hagestad, G. O., & Neugarten, B. L. (1985). Age and the life course. In R. H. Binstock  & E. Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (2nd ed., pp. 35–61). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Haier, R. J., Schroeder, D. H., Tang, C., Head, K., & Colom, R. (2010). Gray matter correlates of cognitive ability tests used for vocational guid- ance. BMC Research Notes, 3. Retrieved from www.biomedcentral.com/1756-0500/3/206.

Haight, B. K.,  & Haight, B. S. (2007). The handbook of structured life review. Baltimore, MD: Health Professions Press.

Haight , B. K .,  & Haight , B. S. (2013). Confidentiality in the structured life review. International Journal of Reminiscence and Life Review, 1, 36–38. Retrieved from www.ijrlr.org/ ojs/index.php/IJRLR/article /view/21/6.

Hains, C.-A. M.,  & Hulbert-Williams, N. J. (2013). Attitudes toward euthanasia and physician-assisted suicide: A study of the multivariate effects of healthcare training, patient characteristics, religion and locus of control. Journal of Medical Ethics, 39, 713–716. doi:10.1136/medethics-2012-100729

Haist, F., Adamo, M., Han, J., Lee, K., & Stiles, J. (2013). The functional architecture for face- processing expertise: FMRI evidence of the developmental trajectory of the core and

extended face systems. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2893–2908.

Håkansson, P., & Witmer, H. (2015). Social media and trust: A systematic literature review. Journal of Business and Economics, 6, 517–524. doi:10.15341/jbe(2155-7950)/03.06.2015/010

Hakeem, G. F., Oddy, L., Holcroft, C. A.,  & Abenhaim, H. A . (2015). Incidence and determinants of sudden infant death syndrome: a population-based study on 37 million births. World Journal of Pediatrics, 11, 41–47.

Halberda J., & Feigenson L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the “Number Sense”: The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1457–1465.

Hales, S. (2016). Attachment and the end of life experience. In J. Hunter & R. Maunder (Eds.), Improving patient treatment with attachment theory (pp. 93–103). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-233000-0_7

Haley, W. E. (2013). Family caregiving at the end- of-life: Current status and future directions. In R. C. Talley & R. J. V Montgomery (Eds.), Caregiving across the lifespan (pp. 157–175). New York: Springer.

Halgunseth, L. C., Ispa, J. M.,  & Rudy, D. (2006). Parental control in Latino families: An integrated review of the literature. Child Development, 77, 1282–1297.

Halim, M. D. (2016). Princesses and superheroes: Social-cognitive influences on early gender rigidity. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 155–160.

Halim, M. L., Ruble, D., Tamis-Lemonda, C., & Shrout, P. E. (2013). Rigidity in gender- typed behaviors in early childhood. Child Development, 84, 1269–1284.

Hall, J. A. (2016). Same-sex friendships. In C. R. Berger, M. E. Roloff, S. R. Wilson, J. P. Dillard, J. Caughlin, & D. Solomon (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of interper- sonal communication. New  York: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic138.

Hall, S. S. (2006). Marital meaning: Exploring young adult’s belief systems about marriage. Journal of Family Issues, 27, 1437–1458. doi:10.1177/0192513X06290036

Hall, S. S.,  & Willoughby, B. J. (2016). Relative work and family role centralities: Beliefs and behaviors related to the transition to adulthood. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 37, 75–88. doi:10.1007/s10834-014-9436-x

Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., Groeneveld, M. G., van Berkel, S. R., Endendijk, J. J., van der Pol, L. D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2016). Wait until your mother gets home! Mothers’ and fathers’ discipline strategies. Social Development, 25, 82–98.

Hallford, D., & Mellor, D. (2013). Reminiscence- based therapies for depression: Should they be used only with older adults? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 20, 452–468. doi:10.1111/cpsp.12043

Halliwell, D.,  & Franken, N. (2016). “He was supposed to be with me for the rest of my life”: Meaning-making in bereaved siblings’ online

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 613

stories. Journal of Family Communication, 16, 337–354. doi:10.1080/15267431.2016.1194841

Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

Halpern, L. F., MacLean, W. E., & Baumeister, A. A. (1995). Infant sleep-wake characteristics: Rela- tion to neurological status and the prediction of developmental outcome. Developmental Review, 15, 255–291.

Hambrick, D. Z., Macnamara, B. N., Campitelli, G., Ullén, F.,  & Mosing, M. A. (2016). Chapter One—Beyond born versus made: A new look at expertise. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 64, 1–55. doi:10.1016/ bs.plm.2015.09.001

Hamilton, B. E., Martin, J. A.,  & Ventura, S. J. (2010). Births: Preliminary data for 2008. National Vital Statistics Reports, Vol. 58. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Hamilton, R . K . B., Hiatt Racer, K .,  & Newman, J. P. (2015). Impaired integration in psychopathology: A unified theory of psychopathic dysfunction. Psychological Review, 122, 770–791. doi:10.1037/a0039703

Hamm, J. V. (2000). Do birds of a feather flock together? The variable bases for African American, Asian American, and European American adolescents’ selection of similar friends. Developmental Psychology, 36, 209–219.

Hammar, M.,  & Östgren, C. J. (2013). Healthy aging and age-adjusted nutrition and physical fitness. Best Practices and Research Clinical Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 27, 741–752. doi:10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2013.01.004

Hammer, L. B., Neal, M. B., Newsom, J. T., Brockwood, K.-J.,  & Colton, C. L. (2005). A longitudinal study of the effects of dual- earner couples’ utilization of family-friendly workplace supports on work and family outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 799–810.

Hance, V. M. (2000). An existential perspective describing undergraduate students’ ideas about meaning in work: A q-method study. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 61(3-A), 878.

Hannon, E. E., Soley, G., & Levine, R. S. (2011). Constraints on infants’ musical rhythm perception: effects of interval ratio complexity and enculturation. Developmental Science, 14, 865–872.

Hansen, S. R. (2006). Courtship duration as a correlate of marital satisfaction and stability. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67(4-B), 2279.

Hansson, R . O.,  & Stroebe, M. S. (2007). Bereavement in late life: Coping, adaptation, and developmental influences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Harden, K. P. (2014). Genetic influences on adolescent sexual behavior: Why genes matter for environmentally oriented researchers. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 434–465.

Hareven, T. K. (1995). Introduction: Aging and generational relations over the life course. In T. K. Hareven (Ed.), Aging and generational

relations over the life course: A historical and cross-cultural perspective (pp. 1–12). Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter.

Hareven, T. K. (2001). Historical perspectives on aging and family relations. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (5th ed., pp. 141–159). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Harley, D. A., Gassaway, L.,  & Dunkley, L. (2016). Isolation, socialization, recreation, and inclusion of LGBT elders. In D. A. Harley & P. B. Teaster (Eds.), Handbook of LGBT elders (pp. 563–581). New York: Springer.

Harris, D. L. (2016). Social expectations of the bereaved. In D. L. Harris  & T. C. Bordere (Eds.), Handbook of social justice in loss and grief: Exploring diversity, equity, and inclusion (pp. 165–174). New York: Routledge.

Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (2017). Self-regulated strategy development: Theoretical bases, criti- cal instructional elements, and future research. In R. Fidalgo, K. R. Harris,  & M. Braaksma (Eds.), Design principles for teaching effective writing: Theoretical and empirical grounded principles (in press). Hershey, PA: Brill.

Harris, M. A ., Wetzel, E., Robins, R . W., Donnellan, M. B., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2017). The development of global and domain self- esteem from ages 10 to 16 for Mexican-origin youth. International Journal of Behavioral Development.

Harrison, K., Bost, K. K., McBride, B. A., Donovan, S. M. Grigsby-Toussaint, D. S., Kim, J., et al., (2011). Toward a developmental concep- tualization of contributors to overweight and obesity in childhood: The Six-Cs model. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 50–58.

Harrist, A. W., Zaia, A. F., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Subtypes of social withdrawal in early childhood: Sociometric status and social-cognitive differences across four years. Child Development, 68, 278–294.

Harrist, A., Topham, G., Hubbs-Tait, L., Shriver, L., & Swindle, T. (2017). Psychosocial factors in chil- dren’s obesity: Examples from an innovative line of inquiry. Child Development Perspectives, 11.

Hart, W., Adams, J.,  & Tullett, A. (2016). “It’s complicated”—Sex differences in perceptions of cross-sex friendships. Journal of Social Psychology, 156, 190–201. doi:10.1080/002245 45.2015.1076762

Harter, S. (1994). Developmental changes in self-understanding across the 5 to 7 shift. In A. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), Reason and responsibility: The passage through childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3). New York: Wiley.

Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Developmental and sociocultural foundations (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

Harter, S., Waters, P., & Whitesell, N. R. (1998). Relational self-worth: Differences in perceived worth as a person across interpersonal contexts among adolescents. Child Development, 69, 756–766.

Harter, S., Whitesell, N. R.,  & Kowalski, P. S. (1992). Individual differences in the effects of educational transitions on young adolescents’ perceptions of competence and motivational orientation. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 777–807.

Hartung, P. J., & Santilli, S. (2017). The theory and practice of career construction. In M. McMahon (Ed.), Career counselling : Constructivist appro ache s (2nd e d., pp. 174–184). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.

Hartup, W. W. (1992). Friendships and their devel- opmental significance. In H. McGurk (Ed.), Contemporary issues in childhood social devel- opment. (pp. 175–205). London: Routledge.

Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1999). Friendships and adaptation across the life span. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 76–79.

Harvard School of Public Health. (2010). The nutrition source: Alcohol: Balancing risks and benefits. Retrieved from www.hsph.harvard. edu/nutritionsource/alcohol-full-story.

Harvey, I. S., Johnson, L.,  & Heath, C. (2013). Womanism, spirituality, and self-health management behaviors of African American older women. Women, Gender, and Families of Color, 1, 59–84. doi:10.1353/wgf.2013.0002

Haselton, M. G.,  & Galperin, A. (2013). Error management in relationships . In J. A . Simpson  & L. Campbell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of close relationships (pp. 234–254). New York: Oxford University Press.

Hashimoto, K. (2013). Understanding depression: Linking brain-derived neurotrophic factor, transglutaminase 2 and serotonin. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 13, 5–7.

Hatania, R.,  & Smith, L. B. (2010). Selective attention and attention switching: Toward a u n i f i e d d e v e l o p m e n t a l a p p r o a c h . Developmental Science, 13, 622–635.

Hawley, P. H. (2016). Eight myths of child social development: An evolutionary approach to power, aggression, and social competence. In D. C. Geary & D. B. Berch (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on child development and education (pp. 145–166). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Haworth, J.,  & Lewis , S. (2005). Work , leisure and well-being . British Journal of Guidance  & Counselling, 33, 67–78. doi:10.1080/03069880412 331335902

Hay, D. F. (2017). The early development of human aggression. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 102–106.

Hayden, E. C. (2016). Should you edit your children’s genes? Nature, 530, 402–405.

Hayflick, L. (1996). How and why we age (2nd ed.). New York: Ballantine.

Hayslip Jr, B.,  & Blumenthal, H. (2016). G r a n d p a r e n t h o o d : A d e v e l o p m e n t a l perspective. In M. H. Meyer  & E. A. Daniele (Eds.), Gerontology: Changes, challenges, and solutions (Vol.1, pp. 271–298). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

614 R e f e R e n c e s

Hayslip, B., Jr., Caballero, D., Ward-Pinson, M., & Riddle, R. R. (2013). Sensitizing young adults to their biases about middle-aged and older persons: A pedagogical approach. Educational Gerontology, 39, 37–44. doi:10.1080/03601277. 2012.660865

Hayward, R. D., Krause, N., Ironson, G., Hill, P. C., & Emmons, R. (2016). Health and well-being among the non-religious: Atheists, agnostics, and no preference compared with the religious group members. Journal of Religion and Health, 55, 1024–1037. doi:10.1007/ s10943-015-0179-2

Hazell, P. L. (2009). Eight-year follow-up of the MTA sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 461–462.

Health.gov. (2015). Dietar y guidelines for Americans 2015–2020 (8th ed.). Retrieved from health.gov/dietar yguidelines/2015/ resources/2015-2020_Dietary_Guidelines.pdf.

Heathcote, D. (2016). Working memory and performance limitations. In D. Groome  & M. W. Eysenck (Eds.), An introduction to applied cognitive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 99–123). New York: Routledge.

Hechtman, L., Swanson, J. M., Sibley, M. H., Stehli, A., Owens, E. B., Mitchell, J. T. et  al. (2016). Functional adult outcome 16 years after childhood diagnosis of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: MTA results. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 945–952.

Heck, A., Hock, A., White, H., Jubran, R., & Bhatt, R. S. (2016). The development of attention to dynamic facial emotions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 100–110.

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60. doi:10.1037/ a0017668

Hege wisch, A .,  & Hartman, H. (2014). Occupational segregation and the gender wage gap: A job half done. Retrieved from www.dol. gov/wb/resources/occupational_segregation_ and_wage_gap.pdf.

Heilman, K . M. (2016). Possible brain mechanisms of creativity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31, 285–296. doi:10.1093/ arclin/acw009

Heine, S. J., & Buchtel, E. E. (2009). Personality: The universal and the culturally specific. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 369–394. doi:10.1146 /annurev.psych.60.110707.163655.

Heinzel, S., Lorenz, R. C., Brockhaus, W.-R., Wüstenberg, T., Kathmann, N., Heinz, A., et al. (2014). Working memory load-dependent brain response predicts behavioral training gains in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1224– 1233. doi:10.1523/JENUROSCI.2463-13.2014

Heller, S. B. (2014). Summer jobs reduce violence among disadvantaged youth. Science, 346, 1219–1223.

Helpguide.org. (2016). Adult day care services. Retrieved from www.helpguide.org/articles/ caregiving/adult-day-care-services.htm.

Helson, R., Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P.,  & Jones, C. (2002). The growing evidence for

personality change in adulthood: Findings from research with personality inventories. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 287–306. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00010-7

Hemer, S. R. (2010). Grief as social experience: Death and bereavement in Lahir, Papua, Ne w Guine a . TA JA : T he Au strali an Journal of Anthropolog y, 21, 281–297. doi:10.1111/j.1757-6547.2010.00097.x

Henderson, H. A.,  & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theor y and the study of cog nition-emotion interactions across development. Developmental Review, 27, 396–427.

Henderson, N. D. (2010). Predicting long-term firefighter performance from cognitive and physical ability measures. Personnel Psychology, 63, 999–1039.

Henderson, S., & Gilding, M. (2004). “I’ve never clicked this much with anyone in my life”: Trust and hyperpersonal communication in online friendships. New Media  & Society, 6, 487–506. doi:10.1177/146144804044331

Hendriks, G.-J., Keijsers, G. P. J., Kampman, M., Hoogduin, C. A. L., & Voshaar, R. C. O. (2012). Predictors of outcome of pharmacological and psychological treatment of late-life panic disorder with agoraphobia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 146–150. doi:10.1002/gps.2700

Hendry, M., Pasterfield, D., Lewis, R., Carter, B., Hodgson, D.,  & Wilkinson, C. (2013). Why do we want the right to die? A systematic review of the international literature on the views of patients, careers and the public on assisted dying. Palliative Medicine, 27, 13–26. doi:10.1177/0269216312463623

Henrie, J. A. (2010). Religiousness, future time perspective, and death anxiety among adults. Dissertation submitted to West Virginia University.

Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. (2016). 10 FAQs: Medicare’s role in end-of-life care. Retrieved from kff.org/medicare/fact-sheet/10- faqs-medicares-role-in-end-of-life-care/.

Heo, J., Lee, Y., McCormick, B. P., & Pedersen, P. M. (2010). Daily experience of serious leisure, flow and subjective well-being of older adults. Leisure Studies, 29, 207–225. doi:10.1080/ 02614360903434092

Hepper, P. (2015). Behavior during the prenatal period: Adaptive for development and survival. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 38–43.

Heppner, R. S. (2013). The lost leaders: How corporate America loses women leaders. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Herlitz, A.,  & Lovén, J. (2013). Sex differences and the own-gender bias in face recognition: A meta-analytic review. Visual Cognition, 21, 1306–1336.

Herman, M. (2004). Forced to choose: Some determinants of racial identification in multiracial adolescents. Child Development, 75, 730–748.

Heron, M. (2016). Deaths: Leading causes for 2014. National Vital Statistics Reports, 65, no 5. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health

Statistics. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/ data/nvsr/nvsr65/nvsr65_05.pdf.

Herrenbrueck, L., Xia, X., Eastwick, P., Finkel, E., & Hui, C. M. (2016). Smart-dating in speed- dating: How a simple search model can explain matching decisions. Retrieved from www.sfu.ca/ econ-research/RePEc/sfu/sfudps/dp16-02.pdf.

Herrnstein, R. J.,  & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York: Free Press.

Hershey, D. A.,  & Henkens, K. (2014). Impact of different types of retirement transitions on perceived satisfaction with life. The Gerontolo- gist, 54, 232–244. doi:10/1093/geront/gnt006

Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S.,  & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Fit body, fit mind? Scientific American Mind , 20, 24–31. doi:10.1038/scientific-americanmind0709-24

Hespos, S. J.,  & van Marle, K. (2012). Physics for infants: Characterizing the origins of knowledge about objects, substances, and number. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3, 19–27.

Hess, T. M.,  & Pullen, S. M. (1994). Adult age differences in impression change processes. Psychology and Aging, 9, 237–250. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.9.2.237

Heyman, G. D. (2009). Children’s reasoning about traits. In P. Bauer (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 37, pp. 105–143). London: Elsevier.

Heyman, G. D., & Legare, C. H. (2004). Children’s beliefs about gender differences in the academic and social domains. Sex Roles, 50, 227–239.

Hiatt, K., Stelle, C., Mulsow, M., & Scott, J. P. (2007). The importance of perspective: Evaluation of hospice care from multiple stakeholders. Amer- ican Journal of Hospice & Palliative Medicine, 24, 376–382. doi:10.1177/1049909107300760

Hill, J. L., Brooks-Gunn, J.,  & Waldfogel, J. (2003). Sustained effects of high participation in an early intervention for low-birth-weight premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 39, 730–744.

Hill, P. L., Duggan, P. M., & Lapsley, D. K. (2012). Subjective invulnerability, risk behavior, and adjustment in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 32, 489–501.

Hill, R. T., Thomas, C., English, L., & Callaway, K. (2016). The importance and impact of child care on a woman’s transition to motherhood. In C. Spitzmueller  & R. A. Matthews (Eds.), Research perspectives on work and the transition to motherhood (pp. 241–265). New York: Springer.

Hills, L. (2013). Why legacy matters more in midlife. In Lasting female educational leadership: Leadership legacies of women leaders (pp. 15–26). New York: Springer.

Hills, W. E. (2010). Grandparenting roles in the evolving American family. In D. Wiseman (Ed.), The American family: Understanding its changing dynamics and place in society (pp. 65–78.) Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

Hillsdon, M., Brunner, E., Guralnik, J., & Marmot, M. (2005). Prospective study of physical activity and physical function in early old age.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 615

American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28, 245–250. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2004.12.008

Hines, M. (2015). Gendered development. In R . M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol. 3, 20:1–486). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Hinton, A.,  & Chirgwin, S. (2010). Nursing education: Reducing reality shock for graduate indigenous nurses—It’s all about time. Australian Journal of Advanced Nursing, 28, 60–66. Retrieved from search.informit.com. au/documentSummary;dn=053211061337305; res=IELHEA.

Hipp, L., Morrissey, T. W.,  & Warner, M. E. (in press). Who participates and who benefits from employer-provided child-care assistance? Journal of Marriage and Family. doi:10.1111/ momf.12359

Hipwell, A. E., Keenan, K., Loeber, R., & Battista, D. (2010). Early predictors of sexually intimate behaviors in an urban sample of young girls. Developmental Psychology, 46, 366–378.

Hirsh-Pasek, K.,  & Golinkoff, R. M. (2008). King Solomon’s take on word learning: An integrative account from the radical middle. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 36, pp. 1–29). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.

Hodnett, E. D., Gates, S., Hofmeyr, G.J., & Sakala, C. (2012). Continuous support for women during childbirth. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 10.

Hoeppner, B. B., Paskausky, A. L., Jackson, K. M., & Barnett, N. P. (2013). Sex differences in college student adherence to NI AAA drinking guidelines. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 37, 1779–1786. doi:10.1111/acer.12159

Hofer, J., Busch, H., Chasiotis, A., Kärtner, J., & Campos, D. (2008). Concern for generativ- ity and its relation to implicit pro-social power motivation, generative goals, and sat- isfaction with life: A cross-cultural inves- tigation. Journal of Personality, 76, 1–30. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00478.x

Hofer, M. A. (2006). Psychobiological roots of early attachment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 84–88.

Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1997). Language development. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Hoff, E. L. (2014). Language development (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.

Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73, 418–433.

Hoff, E., Core, C., Rumiche, R., Señor, M.,  & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 1–27.

Hoffman, E., Kaneshiro, S.,  & Compton, W. C. (2012). Peak-experiences among Americans in midlife. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 52, 479–503. doi:10.1177/0022167811433851

Hoffman, M. L. (1988). Moral development. In M. H. Bornstein and M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced

textbook (2nd ed., pp. 497–538). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hoffman, M. L . (1994). Discipline and internalization. Developmental Psychology, 30, 26–28.

Hofhuis, J., van der Rijt, P. G. A.,  & Vlug, M. (2016). Diversity climate enhances work outcomes through trust and openness in workgroup communication. SpringerPlus, 5, 714. doi:s40064-016-2499-4

Hogan, A. M., de Haan, M., Datta, A., & Kirkham, F. J. (2006). Hypoxia: An acute, intermittent and chronic challenge to cognitive development. Developmental Science, 9, 335–337.

Hogge, W. A. (1990). Teratology. In I. R. Merkatz & J. E. Thompson (Eds.), New perspectives on prenatal care. New York: Elsevier.

Holden, G. W., & Miller, P. C. (1999). Enduring and different: A meta-analysis of the similarity in parents’ child rearing. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 223–254.

Holland, J. L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Holland, J. L. (1987). Current status of Holland’s theory of careers: Another perspective. Career Development Quarterly, 36, 24–30.

Holland, J. L. (1996). Exploring careers with a typology: What we have learned and some new directions. American Psychologist, 51, 397–406.

Holland, J. M. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Holland, J. M.,  & Neimeyer, R. A. (2010). An examination of stage theory of grief among individuals bereaved by natural and violent causes: A meaning-oriented contribution. OMEGA: Journal of Death and Dying, 61, 103–120. doi:10.2190/OM.61.2.b

Holland, J. M., Thompson, K. L., Rozalski, V., & Lichtenthal, W. G. (2014). Bereavement-related regret trajectories among widowed older adults. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69B, 40–47. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt050

Holland, K. J.,  & Cortina, L. M. (2016). Sexual harassment: Undermining the wellbeing of working women. In M. L. Connerley & J. Wu (Eds.), Handbook on well-being of working women (pp. 83–101). New York: Springer.

Hollich, G. J., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2007). Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1051–1061.

Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emer- gentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (Serial No. 262).

Holowka, S.,  & Petitto, L. A . (2002). Left hemisphere cerebral specialization for babies while babbling. Science, 297, 1515.

Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Allali, G., Izzetoglu, M., Wang, C., & Mahoney, J. R. (2016). Neurological

gait abnormalities moderate the functional brain signature of the posture first hypothesis. Brain Topography, 29, 334–343. doi:10.1007/ s10548-015-0465-z

Hom, P. W.,  & Kinicki, A. J. (2001). Toward a greater understanding of how dissatisfaction d r i v e s e mp l o y e e tu r n o v e r. Ac a d e my of Management Journal, 44, 975–987. doi:10.2307/3069441

Honda-Howard, M.,  & Homma, M. (2001). Job satisfaction of Japanese career women and its influence on turnover intention. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 23–38. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00073

Hong, J. S., Lee, J., Espelage, D. L., Hunter, S. C., Patton, D. U.,  & Rivers, T. (2016). Understanding the correlates of face-to-face and cyberbullying victimization among U.S. adolescents: A social-ecological analysis. Violence and Victims, 31, 638–663.

Hooker, K. (1999). Possible selves in adulthood. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (pp. 97–122). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Hooker, K., Fiese, B. H., Jenkins, L., Morfei, M. Z., & Schwagler, J. (1996). Possible selves among parent s of inf ant s and prescho olers . Developmental Psychology, 32, 542–550. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.542

Hopkins, B.,  & Westra, T. (1988). Maternal handling and motor de velopment : An intercultural study. Genetic, Social, & General Psychology Monographs, 14, 377–420.

Horn, J. L. (1982). The aging of human abilities. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 847–870). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Horn, J. L., & Hofer, S. M. (1992). Major abilities and development in the adult period. In R. J. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual development (pp. 44–99). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Horwitz, B. N., Reynolds, C. A., Walum, H., Ganiban, J., Spotts, E. L., Reiss, D., et al. (2016). Understanding the role of mate selection processes in couples’ pair-bonding behavior. Behavior Genetics, 46, 143–149. doi:10.1007/ s10519-015-9766-y

Horwood, S., & Beanland, V. (2016). Inattentional blindness in older adults: Effects of attentional set and to-be-ignored distractors. Attention, Perception,  & Psychophysics, 78, 818-828. doi:10.3758/s13414-015-1057-4

Hospice Foundation of America. (2016a). What is hospice? Retrieved from http:// hospicefoundation.org/End-of-Life-Support- and-Resources/Coping-with-Terminal-Illness/ Hospice-Services.

Hospice Foundation of America. (2016b). A caregiver’s guide to the dying process. Retrieved from http://hospicefoundation.org/hfa/media/ Files/Hospice_TheDyingProcess_Docutech- READERSPREADS.pdf.

Hostetler, A. J., & Paterson, S. E. (2017). Toward a community psychology of aging: A lifespan perspective. In M. A. Bond, I. Serrano-Garcia, C. B. Keys, & M. Shinn (Eds.), APA handbook of

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

616 R e f e R e n c e s

community psychology: Methods for community research and action for diverse groups and issues (Vol. S, pp. 605–622). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hotz, V. J.,  & Pantano, J. (2013). Strategic parenting, birth order and school performance. IZA Working Paper 7680.

Houston, D. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2003). Infants’ long-term memory for the sound patterns of words and voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1143–1154.

Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H., Jr. (2012). Dis- sociable forms of implicit learning in aging. In M. Naveh-Benjamin & N. Ohta (Eds.). Memory and aging: Current issues and future directions (pp. 125–151). New York: Psychology Press.

Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H., Jr. (2016). Implicit learning and memory. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (pp. 631–635). Malden, MA: Wiley.

Howard, J. H., Jr., & Howard, D. V. (2013). Aging mind and brain: Is implicit learning spared in healthy aging? Frontiers in Psychology, November 7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00817. Retrie ved from journal.frontiersin.org/ article/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00817/full.

Howard, L. W.,  & Cordes, C. L. (2010). Flight from unfairness: Effects of perceived injustice on emotional exhaustion and employee withdrawal. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25, 409–428. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9158-5

Howe, M. L. (2014). The co-emergence of the self and autobiographical memory: An adaptive view of early memory. In P J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 545–567). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Howe, N.,  & Ross, H. S. (1990). Socialization, perspective taking and the sibling relationship. Developmental Psychology, 26, 160–165.

Howes, C., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). Sequences in the development of competent play with peers: Social and social pretend play. Developmental Psychology, 28, 961–974.

Howes, C., Unger, O.,  & Seidner, L. B. (1990). Social pretend play in toddlers: Parallels with social play and with solitary pretend. Child Development, 60, 77–84.

Hoyt, A. T., Canfield, M. A., Romitti, P. A., Botto, L. D., Anderka, M. T., Krikov, S., et  al. (2016). Association between maternal periconceptional exposure to secondhand tobacco smoke and major birth defects. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 215, 613.e1–613.e11.

Hsu, H.-C. (2005). Gender disparity of successful aging in Taiwan. Women and Health, 42, 1–21. doi:10.1300/J013v42n01_01

Huang, G. C., Unger, J. B., Soto, D., Fujimoto, K., Pentz, M. A., Jordan-Marsh, M., et al. (2014). Peer influences: the impact of online and offline friendship networks on adolescent smoking and alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54, 508–514.

Hubbard, R. R. (2010). Afro-German biracial iden- tity development. Retrieved from digarchive

.librar y.vcu.edu/bitstream/10156/2804/1/ Afro-German%20HEMBAGI%20%28F3%29 .pdf.

Hughes, C., White, N., & Ensor, R. (2014). How does talk about thoughts, desires, and feelings foster children’s socio-cognitive development? Mediators, moderators and implications for intervention. In K. H. Lagattuta (Ed.), Children and emotion: New insights into developmental affective sciences (pp. 95–105). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

Hughes, J. M., Bigler, R. S., & Levy, S. R. (2007). Consequences of learning about historical racism among European American and African American children. Child Development, 78, 1689–1705.

Huisman, M., Read, S., Towriss , C . A ., Deeg, D. J. H., & Grundy, E. (2013). Socioeco- nomic inequalities in mortality rates in old age in the World Health Organization Europe region. Epidemiologic Reviews, 35, 84–97. doi:10.1093/epirev/mxs010

Hull, T.H. (2009). Caught in transit: Questions about the future of Indonesian fertility. Population Bulletin of the United Nations, Special Issue Nos. 48/49, 375–387.

Hulme, C.,  & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2015). Educa- tional interventions for children’s learning dif- ficulties. In A. Thapar, D. S. Pine, J. F. Leckman, S. Scott, M. J. Snowling, & E. Taylor (Eds.), Rut- ter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (6th ed., pp. 533–544). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2011). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties: Nature, causes, and treatments. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 139–142.

Hulme, C.,  & Snowling, M. (2016). Reading disorders and dyslexia. Current Opinion and Pediatrics, 28, 731–735.

Human Genome Project. (2003). Genomics and its impact on science and society: A 2003 primer. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Energy.

Hunt, E.,  & Carlson, J. (2007). Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 194–213.

Hunt, J. B.,  & Fitzgerald, M. (2013). The relationship between emotional intelligence and transfor mational le adership: An investigation and review of competing claims in the literature. American International Journal of Social Science, 2, 30–38. Retrieved f r o m w w w. r e s e a r c h g a t e . n e t / p r o f i l e / Ja m e s _ Hu nt 1 1 / p u b l i c at i o n / 2 6 4 3 1 1 6 6 0 _ Th e _ R el at i o n sh i p _ b e t w e e n _ E m o t i o n a l _ I n t e l l i g e n c e _ a n d _ Tr a n s f o r m a t i o n a l _ Leadership_An_Investigation_and_Review_ of_Competing_Claims_in_the_Literature/ links/56b15d5408ae795dd5c505a0.pdf.

Hunt, J. B., & Fitzgerald, M. (2014). An evidence- based assessment of the relationship between emotional intelligence and transformational leadership. International Journal of Arts and Commerce, 3, 85–100. Retrieved from www .researchgate.net/profile/James_Hunt11/ publication/292802393_An_Evidence-based_

Assessment_of_the_Relationship_Between_ Emotional_Intelligence_and_Transformational_ Leadership/links/56b16e2208ae56d7b06a0f5c. pdf.

Hunt, J. M., & Weintraub, J. R. (2016). The coaching manager: Developing top talent (3rd  ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1998). Mass media and children’s development. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.

Huston, A. C., Bobbitt, K. C., & Bentley, A. (2015). Time spent in child care: How and why does it affect social development? Developmental Psychology, 51, 621–634.

Huth-Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G. A., & von Eye, A. (2004). The impact of maternal characteristics and contextual variables on infant–mother attachment. Child Develop- ment, 75, 480–496.

Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J.,  & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.

Hwang, M. J. (2007). Asian social workers’ perceptions of glass ceiling, organizational fairness and career prospects. Journal of Social Service Research, 33, 13–24. doi:10.1300/ J079v33n04_02

Hwang, N.,  & Domina, T. (2017). The links between youth employment and educational attainment across racial groups. Journal of Research on Adolescence, in press.

Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 373–398.

Hyer, L., Mullen, C. M.,  & Jackson, K. (2017). The unfolding of unique problems in later life families. In G. L. Welch  & A. W. Harrist (Eds.),  Family resilience and chronic illness: interdisciplinary and translational perspectives (pp. 197–224). New York: Springer.

Iarocci, G., Yager, J., Rombough, A., McLaughlin, J. (2008). The development of social competence among persons with Down syndrome: From survival to social inclusion. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 35, 87–119.

Ibrahim, R., & Hassan, Z. (2009). Understanding singlehood from the experiences of never-mar- ried Malay Muslim women in Malaysia: Some preliminary findings. European Journal of Social Sciences, 8, 395–405. Retrieved from server1.doc- foc.us/uploads/Z2016/01/12/w29wSDYY47/ 40f707fa034e0fea7894a419dea90541.pdf.

Iervolino, A. C., Hines, M., Golombok, S. E., Rust, J.,  & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic and environmental inf luences on sex-typed behavior during the preschool years. Child Development, 76, 826–840.

Igarashi, H., Hooker, K. Coehlo, D. P.,  & Manoogian, M. M. (2013). “My nest is full:” Intergenerational relationships at midlife. Journal of Aging Studies, 27, 102–112. doi:10.1016/j. jaging.2012.12.004

Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). The or y of mind and proso cial b ehavior in childho o d:

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 617

A  meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1192–1205.

Inhelder, B.,  & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

International Labour Organization. (2013). When work becomes a sexual battleground. Retrieved from www.ilo.org/global/about-the- ilo/newsroom/features/WCMS_205996/lang– en/index.htm.

Intuit. (2016). Intuit 2020 report: Twenty trends that will shape the next decade. Retrieved from http-download.intuit.com/http.intuit/CMO/ intuit/futureofsmallbusiness/intuit_2020_ report.pdf.

Irish, L. S. (2009). Modern tribal tattoo designs. East Petersburg, PA: Fox Chapel Publishing.

Irwin, N. (2016). With “gigs” instead of jobs, workers bear new burdens. New  York Times (March 31). Retrieved from www.nytimes .com/2016/03/31/upshot/contractors-and- temps-accounted-for-all-of-the-growth-in- employment-in-the-last-decade.html.

Isaacowitz, D. M. (2014). Change in perceptions of personality disorder in late life: The view from socioemotional aging. Journal of Personality, 28, 166–171. doi:10.1521/pedi.2014.28.1.166

Ismail, Z., Smith, E. E., Geda, Y., Sultzer, D., Brodaty, H., Smith, G., et al. (2016). Neuropsy- chiatric symptoms as early manifestations of emergent dementia: Provisional diagnostic cri- teria for mild behavioral impairment. Alzheim- er’s  & Dementia, 12, 195–202. doi:10.1016/j. jalz.2015.05.107

Iverson, J. M.,  & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language develop- ment. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371.

Ivory, B. T. (2004). A phenomenological inquiry into the spiritual qualities and transformational themes associated with a self-styled rite of passage into adulthood. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section A. Humanities & Social Sciences, 65(2-A), 429.

Iwarsson, S., Slaug, B.,  & Fänge, A. M. (2012). The Housing Enabler Screening Tool: Feasibility and interrater agreement in a real estate company practice context. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 31, 641–660. doi:10.1177/0733464810397354

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 260–280.

Izumi, S., & Son, C. V. (2016). “I didn’t know he was dying”: Missed opportunities for making end-of-life decisions for older family members. Journal of Hospice  & Palliative Nursing, 18, 74–81. doi:10.1097/0000000000000215

Jackson, M. A . (2013). Counseling older workers confronting ageist stereotypes and discrimination. In P. Brownell  & J. J. Kelly (Eds.), Ageism and mistreatment of older workers (pp. 135–144). New York: Springer.

Jacobson, J. L.,  & Jacobson, S. W. (1996). Intellectual impairment in children exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls in utero. New England Journal of Medicine, 335, 783–789.

Jacobson, J. L., Jacobson, S. W., & Humphrey, H. E. B. (1990). Effects of in utero exposure to polychlo- rinated biphenyls and related contaminants on cognitive functioning in young children. Jour- nal of Pediatrics, 116, 38–45.

Jacobson, S. W.,  & Jacobson, J. L. (2000). Teratogenic insult and neurobehavioral function in infancy and childhood. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology: Vol. 31. The effects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development (pp. 61–112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Jainer, A. K., Kamatchi, R., Marzanski, M.,  & Somashekar, B. (2013). Current advances in the treatment of major depression: Shift towards receptor specific drugs. In R.  Woolfolk  & L. Allen (Eds.), Mental disor- ders: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp.  269–288). doi:10.5772/46217. Retrieved from cdn.intechopen.com/pdfs/41703/InTech- Current_advances_in_the_treatment_of_ major_depression_shift_towards_receptor_ specific_drugs.pdf.

James, J. B., Matz-Costa, C.,  & Smyer, M. A. (2016). Retirement security: It’s not just about the money. American Psychologist, 71, 334– 344. doi:10.1037/a0040220

James, J., Ellis, B. J., Schlomer, G. L., & Garber, J. (2012). Sex-specific pathways to early puberty, sexual debut, and sexual risk taking: Tests of an integrated evolutionary-developmental model. Developmental Psychology, 48, 687–702.

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

Janson, H.,  & Mathiesen, K . S. (2008). Temperament profiles from infancy to middle childhood: Development and associations with behavioral problems. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1314–1328.

Janssen I, R. R. (2005). Linking age-related changes in skeletal muscle mass and composition with metabolism and disease. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 9, 408–419. Retrieved from www.researchgate.net/profile/Ian_Janssen/ publication/7375203_Linking_age-related_ change s_in_skele t al_mu s cle_ma ss_and_ composition_with_metabolism_and_disease/ links/00b7d535a3066571fa000000.pdf.

Jaswal, V. K.,  & Kondrad, R. L. (2016). Why children are not always epistemically vigilant: Cognitive limits and social considerations. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 240–244.

Jenkins, C. L. (Ed.). (2003). Widows and divorcees in later life: On their own again. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.

Jenkins, J. A. (2016). Disrupt aging: A bold new path to living your best life at every age. New York: PublicAffairs Publishing.

Jernigan, T. L., & Stiles, J. (2017). Construction of the human forebrain. WIRES Cognitive Science, 8, e1409.

Jeste, D. V.,  & Palmer, B. W. (2013). A call for a new positive psychiatry of ageing. British Journal of Psychiatry, 202, 81–83. doi:10.1192/ bjp. bp.112.110643

Jiang, W., Zhao, F., Guderley, N., & Manchaiah, V. (2016). Daily music exposure dose and hearing

problems using personal listening devices in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review. International Journal of Audiology, 55, 197–205. doi:10.3109/14992027.2015.1122237

Jiang, Z. (2016). The relationship between career adaptability and job content plateau: The mediating roles of fit perceptions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 95, 1–10. doi:10.1016/j. jvb.2016.06.001

Jiao, Z. (1999, April). Which students keep old friends and which become new friends across a school transition? Paper presented at the 1999 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, New Mexico.

Jipson, J. L., & Gelman, S. A. (2007). Robots and rodents: Children’s inferences about living and nonliving kinds. Child Development, 78, 1675–1688.

John, O. P.,  & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 351–372). New York: Guilford.

Johnson, H. A., Zabriskie, R. B., & Hill, B. (2006). The contribution of couple leisure involvement, leisure time, and leisure satisfaction to marital satisfaction. Marriage  & Family Review, 40, 69–91. doi:10.1300/ J002v40n01_05

Johnson, K. J.,  & Mutchler, J. E. (2014). The emergence of a positive gerontology: From disengagement to social involvement. The Gerontologist, 54, S93–S100. doi:10.1093/ geront/gnt099

Johnson, M. D. (2016). Online dating. In M. D. Johnson (Ed.), Great myths of intimate relationships: Dating, sex, and marriage (pp. 52–70). New York: Wiley.

Johnson, M. H., Grossman, T.,  & Cohen Kadosh, K. (2009). Mapping functional brain development: Building a social brain through interactive specialization. Developmental Psychology, 45, 151–159.

Johnson, R. M., LaValley, M., Schneider, K. E., Musci, R. J., Pettoruto, K.,  & Rothman, E. F. (2017). Marijuana use and physical dating violence among adolescents and emerging adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 174, 47–57.

Johnson, R . M., Miller, M., Vriniotis, M., Azrael,  D., & Hemenway, D. (2006). Are household firearms stored less safely in home with adolescents? Analysis of a national random sample of parents. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 788–792.

Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S-J.,  & Barth, M. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive development: Internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cognitive Science, 34, 807–825.

Johnson, S. P. (2001). Visual development in human infants: Binding features, surfaces, and objects. Visual Cognition, 8, 565–578.

Johnson, S. P. (2013). Development of the visual system. In J. L. R. Rubenstein & P Rakic (Eds.), Neural circuit development and function in the brain (Vol 3., pp. 249–269). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

618 R e f e R e n c e s

Johnston, K. E., Swim, J. K., Saltsman, B. M., Deater-Deckard, K.,  & Petrill, S. A. (2007). Mothers’ racial, ethnic , and cultural socialization of transracially adopted Asian children. Family Relations, 56, 390–402. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2007.00468.x

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Miech, R. A., Bachman, J. G.,  & Schulenberg, J. E. (2017). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2016: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan.

Joiner, T. (2010). Myths about suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Jolie, A. (2013). My medical choice. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2013/05/14/opinion/ my-medical-choice.html.

Jolie, A. (2015). Diary of a surgery. Retrieved f rom mobile.ny time s .com/2015/03/24/ opinion/angelina-jolie-pitt-diary-of-a-surgery. html?referrer=&_r=0.

Jones, B. F. (2010). Age and great invention. Review of Economics and Statistics, 92, 1–14. Retrieved from www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/ pdfplus/10.1162/rest.2009.11724.

Jones, L., Bellis, M. A., Wood, S., Hughes, K., McCoy, E., Eckley, L. et al. (2012). Prevalence and risk of violence against children with disabilities: A systematic review and meta- analysis of observational studies. The Lancet, 380, 889–907.

Jonsson, T., Stefansson, H., Steinberg, S., Jonsdottir, I., Jonsson, P. V., Snaedal, J., et  al. (2013). Variant of TREM2 associated with the risk of Alzheimer’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 368, 107–116. doi:10.1056/ NEJMoa1211103

Jopp, D. S.,  & Hertzog, C. (2010). Assessing adult leisure activities: An extension of a self- report activity questionnaire. Psychological Assessment , 22, 108–120. doi:10.1037/ a0017662

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C.,  & Locuniak, M. N. (2008). Development of number combination skill in the early school years: When do fingers help? Developmental Science, 11, 662–668.

Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 54–78.

Joyner, M. J.,  & Barnes, J. N. (2013). I am 80 going on 18: Exercise and the fountain of youth. Journal of Applied Physiology, 114, 1–2. doi:10.1152/ japplphysiol.01313.2012

Juffer, F. (2006). Children’s awareness of adoption and their problem behavior in families with 7-year-old internationally adopted children. Adoption Quarterly, 9, 1–22. doi:10.1300/ J145v09n02_01

Jung, C. G. (1960/1933). The stages of life. In G. Adler, M. Fordham,  & H. Read (Eds.), The collected works of C. J. Jung: Vol. 8. The structure and dynamics of the psyche. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Jung, R. E.,  & Haier, R. J. (2007). The parieto- frontal integration the or y (P-FIT ) of

intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135–154. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001185

Jung, R. E., Flores, R. A.,  & Hunter, D. (2016). A new measure of imagination ability : Anatomical brain imag ing correlates . Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 496. doi:10.3389/ fpsyq.2016.00496. Available at www.ncbi.nlm .nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4834344/.

Jusczyk, P. W. (1995). Language acquisition: Speech sounds and phonological development. In J. L. Miller & P. D. Eimas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition: Vol. 11. Speech, language, and communication (pp. 263–301). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Jusczyk, P. W. (2002). How infants adapt speech- processing capacities to native-language structure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 15–18.

Justice, L. M., Pullen, P. C.,  & Pence, K. (2008). Influence of verbal and nonverbal references to print on preschoolers’ visual attention to print during storybook reading. Developmental Psychology, 44, 855–866.

Juvonen, J.,  & Graham, S. (2014). Bullying in schools: The power of bullies and the plight of victims. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 159–185.

Kahana, E.,  & Kahana, B. (2003). Patient proactivity enhancing doctor-patient-family communication in cancer prevention and care among the aged. Patient Education and Counseling, 50, 67–73. doi:10.1016/ S0738-3991(03)00083-1

Kahana, E., Kahana, B.,  & Zhang, J. (2005). Motivational antecedents of preventive proactivity in late life: Linking future orientation and exercise. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 438–459 (Figure 1). doi:10.1007/ s11031-006-9012-2.

Kahana, E., Kelley-Moore, J.,  & Kahana, B. (2012). Proactive aging: A longitudinal study of stress, resources, and well-being in late life. Aging and Mental Health, 16, 438–451. doi:10.1080/13607863.20 11.644519

Kahn, P. H., Gary, H. E.,  & Shen, S. (2013). Children’s social relationships with current and near-future robots. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 32–37.

Kaijura, H., Cowart, B. J.,  & Beauchamp, G. K. (1992). Early developmental change in bitter taste responses in human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 25, 375–386.

Kail, R. (2004). Cognitive development includes global and domain-specific processes. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 50, 445–455.

Kail, R. V. (2013). Influences of credibility of testimony and strength of statistical evidence on children’s and adolescents’ reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 747–754.

Kail, R.,  & Bisanz, J. (1992). The information- p ro ce s s i n g p e r s p e c t i v e o n co g n i t i v e development in childhood and adolescence. In R. J. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual development. (pp. 229–260). New  York: Cambridge University Press.

Kaiser Family Foundation. (2016). Total number of Medicare beneficiaries. Retrieved from http://kff.org/medicare/state-indicator/total- medicare-beneficiaries/?currentTimeframe=0 &sortModel=%7B%22colId%22:%22Location% 22,%22sort%22:%22asc%22%7D.

Kalanithi, L. (2016). My marriage didn’t end when I became a widow. Retrieved from opinionator. blogs.nytimes.com/2016/01/06/my-marriage- didnt-end-when-i-became-a-widow/.

Kalish, E. (2016). Millennials are the least wealthy, but most optimistic, generation. Retrieved from www.urban.org/research/publication/ millennials-are-least-wealthy-most-optimistic- generation.

Kalish, R. A. (1984). Death, grief, and caring relationships (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Kalish, R. A. (1987). Death and dying. In P. Silverman (Ed.), The elderly as modern pioneers (pp. 320–334). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Kalish, R. A., & Reynolds, D. (1976). Death and ethnicity: A psychocultural study. Los Angeles: University of Southern California Press.

Kalmijn, M. (2013). Adult children’s relationships with married parents, divorced parents, and stepparents: Biology, marriage, or residence? Journal of Marriage and Family, 75, 1181– 1193. doi:10.1111 /jomf.12057

Kalmijn, M.,  & Flap, H. (2001). Assortative meeting and mating: Unintended consequences of organized settings for partner choices. Social Forces, 79, 1289–1312. doi:10.1353/ sof.2001.0044

Kan, D.,  & Yu, X. (2016). Occupational stress, work-family conflict and depressive symptoms among Chinese bank employees: The role of psychological capital. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13, 134. doi:10.3390/ijerp13010134

Kaplan, H., & Dove, H. (1987). Infant development among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Developmental Psychology, 23, 190–198.

Kaplan, S. (2015). When are you dead? It may depend on which hospital makes the call. Washing ton Post. Retrieved from www .washingtonpost.com/news/morning-mix/ wp/2015/12/29/when-are-you-dead-it-may- depend-on-which-hospital-makes-the-call/.

Kapoor, U., Pfost, K. S., House, A. E., & Pierson, E. (2010). Relation of success and nontraditional career choice to selection for dating and friendship. Psychological Reports, 107, 177– 184. doi:10.2466/07.17.PR0.107.4.177-184

Karam, E., Kypri, K.,  & Salamoun, M. (2007). Alcohol use among college students: An international perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 213–221. doi:10.1097/ YCO.0b013e3280fa836c

Karasik , L B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.,  & Adolph, K. E. (2016). Decisions at the brink: Locomotor experience affects infants’ use of social information on an adjustable drop-off. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 797.

Karasik , L . B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.,  & Adolph, K . E . (2011). Transition from

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 619

crawling to walking and infants’ actions with objects and people. Child Development, 82, 1199–1209.

Kärnä, A., Voeten, M., Little, T. D., Alanen, E., Poskiparta, E.,  & Salmivalli, C. (2013). Effectiveness of the KiVa antibullying program: Grades 1–3 and 7–9. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 535–551.

Karney, B. R. (2010). Keeping marriages healthy, and why it’s so difficult. Retrieved from www .apa.org/science/about/psa/2010/02/sci-brief .aspx.

Karney, B. R.,  & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3–34. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.1881.1.3

Karney, B. R.,  & Crown, J. S. (2007). Families under stress: An assessment of data, theory, and research on marriage and divorce in the military (MG-599-OSD). Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation.

Karniol, R . (1989). The role of manual manipulative states in the infant’s acquisition of perceived control over objects. Developmental Review, 9, 205–233.

Karniol, R. (2016). A language-based, three-stage, social-interactional model of social pretend play: Acquiring pretend as an epistemic operator, pretending that, and pretending with (the P-PT-PW model). Developmental Review, 41, 1–37.

Karp, N., & Wood, E. (2012). Choosing home for someone else: Guardian residential decision- making. Utah Law Review, 2012, 1445–1490. Retrieved from http://epubs.utah.edu/index .php/ulr/article/view/837/646.

Karraker, K. H., Vogel, D. A., & Lake, M. A. (1995). Parents’ gender-stereotyped perceptions of newborns: The eye of the beholder revisited. Sex Roles, 33, 687–701.

Kassim, P. N. J.,  & Alias, F. (2016). Religious, ethical and legal considerations in end-of- life issues: Fundamental requisites for medi- cal decision making. Journal of Religion and Health, 55, 119–134. doi:10.1007/210943-014- 9995-z

Kast, N. R., Eisenberg, M. E.,  & Sieving, R. E. (2016), The role of parent communication and connectedness in dating violence victimization among L atino adolescents . Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31, 1932–1955.

Kastenbaum, R. (1999). Dying and bereavement. In J. C. Cavanaugh & S. K. Whitbourne (Eds.), Gerontology: An interdisciplinary perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.

Kastenbaum, R.,  & Thuell, S. (1995). Cookies baking, coffee brewing: Toward a contextual theory of dying. Omega: The Journal of Death and Dying, 31, 175–187. doi:10.2190/ LQPX-71DE-V5AA-EPFT

Katz-Wise, S. L., Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender-role attitudes and behavior across the transition to parenthood. Developmental Psychology, 46, 18–28.

Katz, L. F.,  & Woodin, E. M. (2002). Hostility, hostile detachment, and conflict engagement

in marriages: Effects on child and family functioning. Child Development, 73, 636–652.

Kaufman, J. C. (2016). Creativity 101 (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

Kaufman, J. C., Kaufman, S. B.,  & Plucker, J. A. (2013). Contemporary theories of intelligence. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology. New  York: Oxford University Press.

Kavsek, M.,  & Bornstein, M. H. (2010). Visual habituation and dishabituation in preterm infants: A review and meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 951–975.

Kavšek, M.,  & Braun, S. K. (2016). Binocular vision in infancy: Responsiveness to uncrossed horizontal disparity. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 219–226.

Kavšek, M., Yonas, A., & Granrud, C. E. (2012). Infants’ sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: A review and meta-analysis of looking studies. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 109–128.

Kawabata, Y.,  & Crick , N. R . (2011). The significance of cross-racial/ethnic friendships: Associations with peer victimization, peer support, sociometric status, and classroom diversity. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1763–1775.

Kawabata, Y.,  & Crick, N. R. (2016). Differential associations between maternal and paternal parenting and physical and relational aggression. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 254–263.

Kayser, K. (2010). Couples therapy. In J. R. Brandell (Ed.), Theory and practice in clinical social work (2nd ed., pp. 259–288). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Kearney, C. A., & Spear, M. (2013). Assessment of selective mutism and school refusal behavior. In D. McKay & E. A. Storch (Eds.), Handbook of assessing variants and complications in anxiety disorders (pp. 29–42). New  York: Spring Science+Business Media.

Keel, P. K. Bulimia nervosa. In K. D. Brownell & B. T. Walsh (Eds.), Eating disorders and obesity (3rd ed., pp. 187–191). New York: Guilford.

Keitel, A.,  & Daum, M. M. (2015). The use of intonation for turn anticipation in observed conversations without visual signals as a source of information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 108.

Keith, J. (1990). Age in social and cultural context: Anthropological perspectives. In R. H. Binstock & L. K. George (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (3rd ed., pp. 91–111). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Kelemen, D. (2003). British and American children’s preferences for teleo-functional explanations of the natural world. Cognition, 88, 201–221.

Kelemen, D.,  & DiYanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children’s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition & Development, 6, 3–31.

Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (2006). Infant visual perception. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 109–160). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Kelly, D., Xie, H., Nord, C. W., Jenkins, F., Chan, J. Y., & Kastberg, D. (2013). Performance of U.S. 15-year-old students in mathematics, science, and reading literacy in an international context: First look at PISA 2012. Washington DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

Kelly, P. (2011). Corporal punishment and child maltreatment in New Zealand. Acta Paediatrica, 100, 14–20.

Kendall, J.,  & Hatton, D. (2002). Racism as a source of health disparity in families with chil- dren with attention deficit hyperactivity disor- der. Advances in Nursing Science, 25, 22–39.

Kersting, A.,  & Nagl, M. (2016). Grief after perinatal loss. In A . Milunsky  & J. M. Milunsky (Eds.), Genetic disorders and the fetus: Diagnosis, prevention, and treatment (Vol. 7, pp. 1048–1062). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118981559.ch31

Kersting, A., Brähler, E., Glaesmer, H., & Wagner, B. (2011). Prevalence of complicated grief in a representative population-based sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 131, 339–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.032

Khalil, A., Syngelaki, A., Maiz, N., Zinevich, Y., & Nicolaides, K. H. (2013). Maternal age and adverse pregnancy outcomes: A cohort study. Ultrasound in Obstetrics and Gynecology, 42, 634–643.

Khashan, A. S., Baker, P. N., & Kenny, L. C. (2010). Preterm birth and reduced birthweight in first and second teenage pregnancies: A register- based cohort study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 10, 36.

Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M.,  & Hurt, H. (2012). Early adolescent sexual debut : The mediating role of working memory ability, sensation seeking, and impulsivity. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1416–1428.

Kiang , L ., Yip, T., Gonzales-Backen, M., Witkow, M.,  & Fuligni, A. J. (2006). Ethnic identity and the daily psychological well- being of adolescents from Mexican and Chinese backgrounds. Child Development, 77, 1338–1350.

Kiaye, R. E.,  & Singh, A. M. (2013). The glass ceiling: A perspective of women working in D urb an. G ender in Management : An Inter national Jour nal , 28, 28–42. doi:10.1108/17542411311301556

Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48, 171–184.

Kievit, R. A., Scholte, H. S., Waldorp, L. J.,  & Borsboom, D. (2016). Inter- and intra-individual differences in fluid reasoning show distinct cor- tical responses. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/039412. Retrieved from http://biorxiv.org/content/ biorxiv/early/2016/02/10/039412.full.pdf.

Kilson, M., & Ladd, F. (2009). Is that your child? Mothers talking about rearing biracial children. Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.

Kim, H. K., Capaldi, D. M., & Crosby, L. (2007). Generalizability of Gottman and Colleagues’ affective process models of couples’ relationship

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

620 R e f e R e n c e s

outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, 55–72. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00343.x

Kim, H.-J.,  & Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I. (2016). Living arrangement and loneliness among lesbian, gay, and bisexual older adults. The Gerontologist, 56, 548–558. doi:10.1093/ geront/gnu083

Kim, J.-Y., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C.,  & Osgood, D. (2007). Longitudinal linkages between sibling relationships and adjustment from middle childhood through adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 960–973.

Kim, J.-Y., McHale, S. M., Osgood, D.,  & Crouter, A. C. (2006). Longitudinal course and family correlates of sibling relationships from childhood through adolescence. Child Development, 77, 1746–1761.

Kim, P., Strathearn, L., & Swain, J. E. (2016). The maternal brain and its plasticity in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 77, 113–123. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.001

Kim, S.,  & Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: A meta-analysis of parental involvement and students’ academic achieve- ment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 919–934.

Kimmel, D. (2014). Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender aging concerns. Clinical Gerontologist, 37, 49–63. doi:10.1080/073171 15.2014.847310

King , A . (2016). Older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults: Identities, intersections, and institutions. New York: Routledge.

King, P. E.,  & Furrow, J. L. (2008). Religion as a resource for positive youth development: Religion, social capital, and moral outcomes. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, S, 34–49.

King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (2002). The reflec- tive judgment model: Twenty years of research on epistemic cognition. In B. K. Hofer  & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 37–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (2004). Reflective judgment : Theor y and research on the development of epistemic assumptions through adulthood. Educational Psychologist, 39, 5–18. doi:10.1207/ s15326985ep3901_2

King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (2015). Cognitive development in the emerging adult: The emergence of complex cognitive skills. In J. J. Arnett (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of emerging adulthood (pp. 105–125). New  York: Oxford University Press.

King, S. V., Burgess, E. O., Akinyela, M., Counts- Spriggs, M., & Parker, N. (2006). The religious dimensions of the grandparent role in three- generation African American households. Journal of Religion, Spirituality  & Aging, 19, 75–96. doi:10.1300/ J496v19n01_06

King, S., Dancause, K., Turcotte-Tremblay, A-M., Veru, F., & Laplante, D. P. (2012). Using natural disasters to study the effects of prenatal maternal stress on child health and human development. Birth Defects Research (Part C), 96, 273–288.

Kinney, J. M., & Cavanaugh, J. C. (1993 Novem- ber). Until death do us part : Striving to find meaning while caring for a spouse with dementia. Paper presented at the meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, New Orleans.

Kinney, J. M., Ishler, K. J., Pargament, K. I.,  & Cavanaugh, J. C. (2003). Coping with the uncontrollable: The use of general and religious coping by caregivers to spouses with dementia. Journal of Religious Gerontology, 14, 171–188. doi:10.1300/ J078v14n02_06

Kippen, R., Chapman, B.,  & Yu, P. (2009). What’s love got to do with it? Homogamy and dyadic approaches to understanding marital instability. Retrieved from www.cbe.anu.edu .au/researchpapers/cepr/DP631.pdf.

Kisilevsky, B. S. (2016). Fetal auditory processing: Implications for language development? In N. Reissland  & B. S. Kisilevsky (Eds.), Fetal development: Research on brain and behavior, environmental inf luences, and emerging technologies (pp. 133–152). New  York: Springer.

Kisker, E. E., Lipka, J., Adams, B. L., Rickard, A., Andrew-Ihrke, D., Yanez, E. E., et  al. (2012). The potential of culturally based supplemen- tal mathematics curriculum to improve the mathematics performance of Alaska native and other students. Journal for Research in Math- ematics Education, 43, 75–113. doi:10.5951/ jresematheduc.43.1.0075

Kivett, V. R. (1991). Centrality of the grandfather role among older rural black and white men. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 46, S250–S258. doi:10.1093/ geronb/46.5.S250

Klackl, J., Jonas, E.,  & Kronbichler, M. (2013). Existential neuroscience: Neurophysiological correlates of proximal defenses against death- related thoughts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 333–340. doi:10.1093/scan/ nss003

Klaczynski, P. (2013). Culture and develop- ments in heuristics and biases from pre- school through adolescence (pp. 150–192). In P.  Barrouillet & C. Gauffroy (Eds.), The devel- opment of thinking and reasoning. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Klaczynski, P. A.,  & Lavallee, K. L. (2005). Domain-specific identity, epistemic regulation, and intellectual ability as predictors of belief- biased reasoning: A dual-process perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 1–24.

Klahr, A. M., & Burt, S. A. (2014). Elucidating the etiology of individual differences in parenting: A meta-analysis of behavioral genetic research. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 544–586.

Kleiber, D. A. (2013). Redeeming leisure in later life. In T. Freire (Ed.), Positive leisure science (pp. 21–38). New York: Springer.

Kleiber, D. A., Hutchinson, S. L.,  & Williams, R. (2002). Leisure as a resource in transcending negative life events: Self- protection, self- restoration, and personal transformation. Leisure Sciences, 24, 219–235. doi:10.1080/ 01490400252900167

Klimczuk, A. (2017). Economic foundations for creative ageing policy: Putting theory into practice (Vol. II). New York: Springer.

Klimstra, T. A., Luyckx, K., Branje, S., Teppers, E., Goossens, L.,  & Meeus, W. H. J. (2013). Personality traits, interpersonal identity, and relationship stability: Longitudinal linkages in late adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 1661–1673.

Knafo-Noam, A., Uzefovsky, Israel, S., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2015). The prosocial person- ality and its facets: Genetic and environmental architecture of mother-reported behavior of 7-year-old twins. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 112.

Knee, D. O. (2010). Hospice care for the aging population in the United States. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 203–221). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Knight, G. P.,  & Carlo, G. (2012). Prosocial development among Mexican American youth. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 258–263.

Knipscheer, K.,  & van Tilburg, T. (2013). Generational contact and support among late adult siblings within a verticalized family. In M. Silverstein & R. Giarusso (Eds.), Kinship and cohort in an aging society: From generation to generation (pp. 59–76). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Knowles, M. S., Holton, E. F., III, & Swanson, R. A. (2015). The adult learner (8th ed.). New York: Routledge.

Ko, H.-J., Mejia, S., & Hooker, K. (2014). Social possible selves, self-regulation, and social goal progress in older adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 219– 227. doi:10.1177/0165025413512063

Kochanska, G., Aksan, N.,  & Joy, M. E. (2007). Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of par- enting in early socialization: Two longitudi- nal studies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 222–237.

Kochanska, G., Gross, J. N., Lin, M-H.,  & Nichols, K. E. (2002). Guilt in young children: Development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards. Child Development, 73, 461–482.

Koenig, B. L., Kirkpatrick, L. A.,  & Ketelaar, T. (2007). Misperception of sexual and romantic interests in opposite-sex friendships: Four hypotheses. Personal Relationships, 14, 411– 429. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00163.x

Koepke, S.,  & Denissen, J. A. (2012). Dynamics of identity development and separation- individuation in parent-child relationships during adolescence and emerging adulthood. Developmental Review, 32, 67–88.

Koestenbaum, P. (1976). Is there an answer to death? Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 621

socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago: Rand McNally.

Kohlberg, L., & Ullian, D. Z. (1974). Stages in the development of psychosexual concepts and attitudes. In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. L. Van Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior. New York: Wiley.

Kohn, J. (2015). Top tips for eating right during preg- nancy. Available at www.eatright.org/resource/ health/pregnancy/what-to-eat-when-expecting/ eating-right-during-pregnancy.

Kojima, G., Iliffe, S., Jivraj, S., & Walters, K. (2017). Association between frailty and quality of life among community-dwelling older people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Epidemiology  & Community Health, 70, 716–721. doi:10.1136/jech-2015-206717

Kojola, E., & Moen, P. (2016). No more lock-step retirement: Boomers’ shifting meanings of work and retirement. Journal of Aging Studies, 36, 59–70. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2015.12.003

Kolb, B., & Teskey, G. C. (2012). Age, experience, injury, and the changing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 54, 311–325.

Kolb, D. M., Williams, J., & Frohlinger, C. (2010). Her place at the table: A woman’s guide to negotiating five key challenges to leadership success. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Kolberg, K. J. S. (1999). Environmental influences on prenatal development and health. In T. L. Whitman  & T. V. Merluzzi (Eds.), Life-span perspectives on health and illness (pp. 87–103). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kolko, J. (2016). Trump was stronger where the economy was weaker. Retrieved from fivethirtyeight .com/features/trump-was - stronger-where-the-economy-is-weaker/.

Kornadt, A . E., Hess, T. M., Voss, P.,  & Rothermund, K. (in press). Subjective age across the life span: A differentiated, longi- tudinal approach. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/ gbw072

Kornhaber, M., Fierros, E., & Veenema, S. (2004). Multiple intelligences: Best ideas from research and practice. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Koropeckyj-Cox T.,  & Call, V. R. A. (2007). Characteristics of older childless persons and parents: Cross-national comparisons. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 1362–1414. doi:10.1177/0192513X07303837

Koss, C.,  & Ekerdt, D. J. (in press). Residential reasoning and the tug of the Fourth Age. The Gerontologist. doi:10.1093/geront/gnw010

Kost, K.,  & Maddow-Zimet, I. (2016). U.S. Teenage pregnancies, births and abortions, 2011: National trends by age, race and ethnicity. New York: Guttmacher Institute.

Kotre, J. N. (2005). Generativity: Reshaping the past into the future. Science and Theology News, September, 42–43. Retrieved from www.johnkotre.com/images/generativity_s_t_ news_2005.pdf.

Kotter-Grühn, D. (2016). Aging self. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (pp. 55–59). Malden, MA: Wiley.

Kouros, C. D., Papp, L. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, E. M. (2014). Spillover between marital quality and parent-child relationship quality: Parental depressive symptoms as moderators. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 315–325.

Koutamanis, M., Vossen, H. G. M., Peter, J.,  & Valkenburg, P. M. (2013). Practice makes perfect: The longitudinal effect of adolescents’ instant messaging on their ability to initiate offline friendships. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2265–2272.

Kowal, A.,  & Kramer, L. (1997). Children’s understanding of parental differential treatment. Child Development, 68, 113–126.

Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., & Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1073–1137.

Kowalski, S. D.,  & Bondmass, M. D. (2008). Physiological and psychological symptoms of grief in widows. Research in Nursing & Health, 31, 23–30. doi:10.1002/nur.20228

Kozbelt, A., & Durmysheva, Y. (2007). Lifespan creativity in a non-Western artistic tradition: A study of Japanese Ukiyo-e printmakers. Interna- tional Journal of Aging & Human Development, 65, 23–51. doi:10.2190/166N-6470-1325- T341

Kramer, L. (2010). The essential ingredients of successful sibling relationships: An emerging framework for advancing theory and practice. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 80–86.

Krassner, A. M., Gartstein, M. A., Park, C., Dragan, W. L., Lecannelier, F., & Putnam, S. P. (2017). East-west, collectivist-individualist: A cross-cultural examination of temperament in toddlers from Chile, Poland, South Korea, and the U.S. European Journal of Development Psychology, 14, 449–464.

Kratzer, J. M. W., & Stevens Aubrey, J. (2016). Is the actual ideal?: A content analysis of college students’ descriptions of ideal and actual hookups. Sexuality and Culture, 20, 236–254. doi:10.1007/s12119-015-9318-x

Krause, N. (2006). Religion and health in late life. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 499– 518). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Krause, N. (2012). Feelings of gratitude toward God among older whites, older African Americans, and older Mexican Americans. Research on Aging, 34, 156–173. doi:10.1177/0164027511417884

Krause, N. (2016). Assessing the relationships among wisdom, humility, and life satisfaction. Journal of Adult Development, 23, 140–149. doi:10.1007/s10804-016-9230-0

Krause, N.,  & Bastida, E. (2011). Prayer to the saints or the Virgin and health among o l d e r Mex i c a n A m e r i c a n s . Hi sp ani c Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 33, 71–87. doi:10.1177/0739986310393628

Krebs, D., & Gillmore, J. (1982). The relationships among the f irst st age s of co g nitive development, role-taking abilities, and moral development. Child Development, 53, 877–886.

Kreppner, J. M., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., et  al. (2007). Nor mality and imp air ment follow ing profound early institutional deprivation: A longitudinal follow-up into early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 931–946.

Kretch, K. S.,  & Adolph, K. E. (2013a). Cliff or step? Posture-specific learning at the edge of a drop-off. Child Development, 84, 226–240.

Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2013b). No bridge too high: Infants decide whether to cross based on the probability of falling not the severity of the potential fall. Developmental Science, 16, 336–351.

Krispin, O., Sternberg, K. J., & Lamb, M. E. (1992). The dimensions of peer evaluation in Israel: A cross-cultural perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 299–314.

Krull, A. C. (2013). Health care professionals, friends , family, and God: Interactions contributing to caregivers’ long-term care decisions. Sociological Spectrum, 33, 329–340. doi:10.1080/02732173.2013.732886

Kübler-Ross, E. (1969). On death and dying. New York: Macmillan.

Kübler-Ross, E. (1974). Questions and answers on death and dying. New York: Macmillan.

Kuhl, J.,  & Keller, H. (2008). Affect-regulation, self-development and parenting: A function- design approach to cross-cultural differences. In R. Sorrentino  & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures (pp. 19–47). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagons, L., Blair, C. B., & the Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development, 85, 1898–1914.

Kukutai, T. H. (2007). White mothers, brown children: Ethnic identification of Maori- European children in New Zealand. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, 1150–1161. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00438.x

Kulik, L. (2011). Developments in spousal power relations: Are we moving toward equality? Marriage and Family Review, 47, 419–435. doi:10.1 080/01494929.2011.619297

Kulik, L. (2016). Long-term marriages. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (pp. 820–823). New York: Wiley.

Kumar, V., Abbas, A. K., Aster, J. C.,  & Fausto, N. (2010). Robbins and Cotran pathologic basis of disease, professional edition (8th ed.). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.

Kunz, J. A. (2007). Older adult development. In J. A. Kunz & F. G. Soltys (Eds.), Transforma- tional reminiscence: Life story work (pp. 19–39). New York: Springer.

Kurdek, L. A. (2004). Are gay and lesbian cohabiting couples really different from h e te ro s exu a l m a r r i e d co u p l e s ? Jo ur - nal of Marriage and Family, 66, 880–900. doi:10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00060.x

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

622 R e f e R e n c e s

Kurtz-Costes, B., Copping, K. E., Rowley, S. J., & Kinlaw, C. R . (2014). Gender and age differences in awareness and endorsement of gender stereotypes about academic abilities. European Journal of Psychology and Education, 29, 603–618.

Kurup, R. K., & Kurup, P. A. (2003). Hypothalamic digoxin, hemispheric dominance, and ne urobiolo g y of love and af fe c tion. International Journal of Neuroscience, 113, 721–729. doi:10.1080/00207450390200026

Kurzban, R., Burton-Chellew, M. N., & West, S. A. (2015). The evolution of altruism in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 575–599.

Kypri, K., Paschall, M. J., Langley, J., Baxter, J., Cashell-Smith, M.,  & Bourdeau, B. (2009). Drinking and alcohol-related harm among New  Zealand university students: Findings from a national web-based survey. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 307–314. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00834.x

LaBotz, M.,  & Griesemer, B. A. (2016). Use of p erformance-enhancing substances . Pediatrics, 138, e20161300.

Labouvie-Vief, G. (2015). Integrating emotions and cog nition throughout the lifespan. New York: Springer.

Labouvie-Vief, G., Grühn, D., & Mouras, H. (2009). Dynamic emotion–cognition interactions in development: Arousal, stress, and the process- ing of affect. In H. B. Bosworth & C. Hertzog (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Research method- ologies and empirical advances (pp. 181–196). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Lachman, M. E. (2004). Development in midlife. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 305–331. doi:10.1146/ annurev.psych.55.090902.141521

L acourse, E ., B oivin, M., Brendgen, A ., Petitclerc, A., Girard, A., Vitaro, F., et  al. (2014). A longitudinal twin study of physical aggression during early childhood: Evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome. Psychological Medicine, 44, 2617–2627.

Ladd, G. W. (2003). Probing the adaptive significance of children’s behavior and relationships in the school context: A child by environment perspective. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 44–104). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Ladd, G. W. (2006). Peer rejection, aggressive or withdrawn behavior, and psychological maladjustment from ages 5 to 12: An examination of four predictive models. Child Development, 77, 822–846.

Ladd, G. W.,  & Ladd, B. K. (1998). Parenting behaviors and parent–child relationships: C o r rel ate s o f p e e r v i c t i m i z at i o n i n kindergarten? Developmental Psychology, 34, 1450–1458.

Ladd, G. W., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Parents and children’s peer relationships. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4 (2nd ed., pp. 377–409). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ladd, G. W., Herald-Brown, S. L.,  & Reiser, M. (2008). Does chronic classroom peer rejection

predict the development of children’s classroom participation during the grade school years? Child Development, 79, 1001–1015.

LaFreniere, P., & MacDonald, K. (2013). A post- genomic view of behavioral development and adaptation to the environment. Developmental Review, 33, 89–109.

Lagattuta, K. H.,  & Wellman, H. M. (2002). Differences in early parent–child conversations about negative versus positive emotions: Implic ations for the de velopment of psychological understanding. Developmental Psychology, 38, 564–580.

Lahey, J. N. (2010). International comparison of age discrimination laws. Research on Aging, 32, 679–697. doi:10.1177/0164027510379348

Lahti, J., Sabia, S., Singh-Manoux, A., Kivimäki, M., Tatsuse, T., Yamada, M. et al. (2016). Leisure time physical activity and subsequent physi- cal and mental health functioning among midlife Finnish, British, and Japanese employ- ees: A follow-up study in three occupational cohorts. BMJ Open, 6, e009788. doi:10.1136/ bmjopen-2015-009788

Lai, D. W. L. (2010). Filial piety, caregiving appraisal, and caregiving burden. Research on Aging, 32, 200–223. doi:10.1177/0164027509351475

Laible, D. J., & Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of maternal and paternal support and control to adolescent social competence, self- worth, and sympathy. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 759–782.

Lam, J. (2015). Picky eating in children. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 3, 41.

Lamanna, M. A., Riedmann, A., & Stewart, S. D. (2015). Marriages, families, and relationships: Making choices in a diverse society (12th ed.). Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning.

Lamaze, F. (1958). Painless childbirth. London: Burke.

Lamb, M. E. (2012). Mothers, fathers, families, and circumstances: Factors affecting children’s adjustment. Applied Developmental Science, 16, 98–111.

Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2010). The development and significance of father-child relationships in two-parent families. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 94–153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

L amb, S. (2014). Permanent personhood or meaningful decline? Toward a critical anthropology of successful aging. Journal of Aging Studies, 29, 41–52. doi:10.1016/j. jaging.2013.12.006

L ambert, B. L .,  & Bauer, C . R . (2012). Developmental and behavioral consequences of prenatal cocaine exposure: A review. Journal of Perinatology, 32, 819–828.

Lamela, D., Figueiredo, B.,  & Bastos, A. (2014). The Portuguese version of the Psychological Adjustment to Separation Test—Part A (PAST—A): A study with recently and non-recently divorced adults. Journal of Happiness Studies. 15, 387–406. doi:10.1007/ s10902-013-9427-x

Lamela, D., Figueiredo, B., Bastos, A., & Feinberg, M. (2016). Typologies of post-divorce coparenting

and parental well-being, parenting quality and children’s psychological adjustment. Child Psychiatry  & Human development, 47, 716–728. doi:10.1007/s10578-015-0604-S

Lampkin-Hunter, T. (2010). Single parenting. Bloomington, IN: Xlibris.

Landis, M., Peter-Wright, M., Martin, M.,  & Bodenmann, G. (2013). Dyadic coping and marital satisfaction of older spouses in long- term marriages. GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 26, 39–47. doi:10.1024/1662-9647/a000077

Lang, F. R.,  & Heckhausen, J. (2001). Perceived control over development and subjective well- being: Differential benefits across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 509–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514 .81.3.509

Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2006). Motivation and interpersonal regulation across adulthood: Managing the challenges and constraints of social contexts. In C. Hoare (Ed.), Handbook of adult development and learning (pp. 149–166). New York: Oxford University Press.

Lang, J. C.,  & Lee, C. H. (2005). Identity accu- mulation, others’ acceptance, job-search self- efficacy, and stress. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 293–312. doi:10.1002/job.309

Langer, N., Pedroni, A., Gianotti, L. R. R., Hänggi, J., Knoch, D., & Jäncke, L. (2012). Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1393–1406. doi:10.1002/hbm.21297

Langeslag, S. J. E., van der Veen, F. M., & Fekkes, D. (2012). Blood levels of serotonin are differentially affected by romantic love in men and women. Journal of Psychophysiology, 26, 92–98. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000071

Langlais, M. R., Anderson, E. R., & Greene, S. M. (2016). Consequences of dating for post- divorce maternal well-being. Journal of Mar- riage and Family, 78, 1032–1046. doi:10.1111/ jomf.12319

Lansford, J. E. (2009). Parental divorce and children’s adjustment . Per spective s on Psychological Science, 4, 140–152.

Lansford, J. E., Bornstein, M. H., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D. et al. (2016). How international research on parenting advances understanding of child development. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 202–207.

Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M.,  & Graef, R. (2014). Mood variability and the psycho- social adjustment of adolescents. In M. Csikszentmihalyi (Ed.)., Applications of flow in human development and education (pp. 285– 304). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer Science+Business Media.

Laurent, H. K., Lucas, T., Pierce, J., Goetz, S., & Granger, D. A. (2016). Coordination of corti- sol response to social evaluative threat with autonomic and inflammatory responses is moderated by stress appraisals and affect. Bio- logical Psychology, 118, 17–24. doi:10.1016/j. biopsycho.2016.04.066

Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (1994). Interpersonal conflict during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 197–209.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 623

Lavelli, M.,  & Fogel, A . (2013). Interdyad differences in early mother-infant face-to- face communication: Real-time dynamics and developmental pathways. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2257–2271.

Lavner, J. A., & Bradbury, T. N. (2017). Protecting relationships from stress. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 11–14. doi:10.1016/j. copsyc.2016.03.003

Lavner, J. A., Karney, B. R.,  & Bradbury, T. N. (2016). Does couples’ communication predict marital satisfaction, or does marital satisfaction predict communication? Journal of Marriage and Family, 78, 680–694. doi:10.1111/ jomf.12301

Lavner, J. A., Lamkin, J., Miller, J. D., Campbell, W. K., & Karney, B. R. (2016). Narcissism and newlywed marriage: Partner characteristics and marital trajectories. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7, 169–179. doi:10.1037/per0000137

Lawton, L. E.,  & Tulkin, D. O. (2010). Work- family balance, family structure and family- friendly employer programs. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Population Association of America, Dallas. Retrieved f rom p aa2010. pr ince ton.e du/dow nlo ad .aspx?submissionId=100573.

Lawton, M. P. (1982). Competence, environmental press, and the adaptation of old people. In M. P. Lawton, P. G. Windley,  & T. O. Byerts (Eds.), Aging and the environment: Theoretical approaches (pp. 33–59). New York: Springer.

Lawton, M. P. (1989). Environmental proactivity in older people. In V. L. Bengtson  & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), The course of later life: Research and ref lections (pp. 15–23). New  York: Springer.

Lawton, M. P.,  & Nahemow, L. (1973). Ecol- ogy of the aging process. In C. Eisdorfer  & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and ag ing (pp. 619–674). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Lazarus, R. S. (1984). Puzzles in the study of daily hassles. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 7, 375–389. doi:10.1007/BF00845271

Lazarus, R. S.,  & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

Lazarus, R. S., DeLongis, A., Folkman, S.,  & Gruen, R. (1985). Stress and adaptational outcomes: The problem of confounded measures. American Psychologist, 40, 770–779. 10.1037/0003-066X.40.7.770

LeanIn.org. (2016). Women in the workplace 2016. Retrieved from womenintheworkplace.com.

Leaper, C. (2015). Gender and social-cognitive development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol. 2, 19:1–48). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Leaper, C., & Smith, T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children’s language use: Talkativeness , affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40, 993–1027.

Lebo, P. B., Quehenberger, F., Kamolz, L.-P.,  & Lumenta, D. B. (2015). The Angelina Jolie effect

revisited: Exploring a media-related impact on public awareness. Cancer, 121, 3959–3964. doi:10.1002/cncr.29461

Ledebt, A. (2000). Changes in arm posture during the early acquisition of walking. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 79–89.

Ledebt, A., van Wieringen, P. C. W., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2004). Functional significance of foot rotation in early walking. Infant Behavior  & Development, 27, 163–172.

LeDoux, J. E., & Gorman, J. M. (2001). A call to action: Overcoming anxiety through active coping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1953–1955. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.1953.

Lee, G. R. (2014). Current research on widowhood: Devastation and human resilience. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69B, 2–3. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt111

Lee, H. J., Macbeth, A. H., Pagani, J. H., & Young, W. S. (2009). Oxytocin: The great facilitator of life. Progress in Neurobiology, 88, 127–151. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.001

Lee, K. S. (2010). Gender, care work, and the complexity of family membership in Japan. Gender  & Society, 24, 647–671. doi:10.1177/0891243210382903

Lee, M.-D. (2007). Correlates of consequences of intergenerational caregiving in Taiwan. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 59, 47–56. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04274.x

Lee, P. C. B. (2003). Going beyond career plateau: Using professional plateau to account for work outcomes. Journal of Management Development, 22, 538–551. doi:10.1108/02621710310478503

Lee, S. J., Altschul, I.,  & Gershoff, E. T. (2013). D o e s w a r mth m o d e r ate l o n g i tu d i n a l associations between spanking and child aggression in early childhood? Developmental Psychology, 49, 2017–2028.

Lee, T.-Y. (2010). The loss and grief in immigration: Pastoral care for immigrants. Pastoral Psychology, 59, 159–169. doi:10.1007/ s11089-009-0261-3

Leekam, S. (2016). Social cognitive impairment and autism: What are we trying to explain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150082.

Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. M.,  & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning . Child Development, 80, 762–775.

Lefkowitz, E. S., Vukman, S. N., & Loken, E. (2012). Young adults in a wireless world. In A. Booth, S. L. Brown, N. S. Landale, W. D. Manning, & S. M. McHale (Eds.), Early adulthood in a fam- ily context (pp. 45–56). New York: Springer.

Legare, C. H., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 177–194.

Leichtentritt, R. D., Leichtentritt, J., Barzllal, Y., & Pedatsur-Sukenik, N. (2013). Unanticipated death of a partner: The loss experience of bereaved girlfriends of fallen Israeli soldiers. Death Studies, 37, 803–829. doi:10.1080/0748 1187.2012.699907

Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189–204.

Lemieux, A. (2012). Post-formal thought in gerontagogy or beyond Piaget. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 2, 399–406. doi:10.4236/ jbbs.2012.23046

Lenhart, A. (2015). Teen, social media and technology overview 2015. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.

Lent, R. W. (2005). A social cognitive view of career development and counseling. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Career development and counseling: Putting theory and research to work (pp. 101–127). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Lent, R. W. (2013). Career-life preparedness: Revisiting career planning and adjustment in the new workplace. Career Development Quar- terly, 61, 2–14. doi:10.1002/j.2161-0045.2013. 00031.x

Leon, G. R., Gillum, B., Gillum, R., & Gouze, M. (1979). Personality stability and change over a 30-year period: Middle to old age. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 517–524. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.47.3.517

LeRoux, H., & Fisher, J. E. (2006). Strategies for enhancing medication adherence in the elderly. In W. T. O’Donohue & E. R. Levensky (Eds.). Promoting treatment adherence: A practical handbook for health care providers (pp. 353– 362). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Leshikar, E. D., Cassidy, B. S., & Gutchess, A. H. (2016). Cognitive, Affective,  & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 302–314. doi:10.3758/ s13415-015-1-0390-3

Lesnoff-Caravaglia, G. (2010). Technology and aging: The herald of a new age. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Volume 3: Societal issues (pp. 247–277). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Leung, R. K., Toumbourou, J. W.,  & Hemphill, S. A. (2014). The effects of peer influence and selection processes on adolescent alcohol use: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Health Psychology Review, 8, 426–457.

Leuty, M. E., Hansen, J.-I. C.,  & Speaks, S. Z. (2016). Vocational and leisure interests: A profile-level approach to examining interests. Journal of Career Assessment, 24, 215–239. doi:10.1177/1069072715580321

Levete, S. (2010). Coming of age. New  York: Wayland/The Rosen Publishing Group.

Levine, I. S. (2009). Best friends forever: Surviv- ing a breakup with your best friend. New York: Penguin.

Levine, L. E. (1983). Mine: Self-definition in 2-year-old boys. Developmental Psychology, 19, 544–549.

Levine, L. J., Kaplan, R. L., & Davis, E. L. (2013). How kids keep their cool: Young children’s use of cognitive strategies to regulate emotion. In D. Hermans, B. Rimé,  & B. Mesquita (Eds.), Changing emotions (pp. 3–9). Hove, UK: Psy- chology Press.

Levine, S. C., Foley, A., Lourenco, S., Ehrlich, S., & Ratliff, K. (2016). Sex differences in spatial

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

624 R e f e R e n c e s

cognition: Advancing the conversation. WIREs Cognitive Science, 7, 127–155.

Levinger, G. (1980). Toward the analysis of close relationships. Journal of Experimental S o c i a l P s y c h o l o g y , 1 6 , 5 1 0 – 5 4 4 . doi:10.1016/0022-1031(80)90056-6

Levinger, G. (1983). Development and change. In H. H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, J. H. Harvey, T. L. Hutson, G. Levinger, et al. (Eds.), Close relationships (pp. 315–359). New York: Freeman.

Levitt, A. G.,  & Utman, J. A. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-case study. Journal of Child Language, 19, 19–49.

Levy, B. A., Gong, Z., Hessels, S., Evans, M. A., & Jared, D. (2006). Understanding print: Early reading development and the contributions of home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 63–93.

Levy, B. R.,  & Bavishi, A. (in press). Survival advantage mechanism: Inflammation as a mediator of self-perceptions of aging on longevity. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw035

Levy, G. D., Taylor, M. G., & Gelman, S. A. (1995). Traditional and evaluative aspects of flexibility in gender roles, social conventions, moral rules, and physical laws. Child Development, 66, 515–531.

LeWine, H. (2013). Alcohol: A heart disease- cancer balancing act. Retrieved from www .health.har vard.edu/blog/alcohol-a-heart- disease-cancer-balancing-act-201302155909.

Lewinsohn, P. M. (1975). The behavioral study and treatment of depression. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler,  & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (Vol. 1, pp. 19–64). New York: Academic Press.

Lewis, D. C., Seponski, D. M.,  & Camp, T. G. (2011). Religious and spiritual values transactions: A constant-comparison analysis of grandmothers and adult-granddaughters. Journal of Religion, Spirituality  & Aging, 23, 184–205. doi:10.1080/15528030.2011.533407

Lewis, J. L.,  & Groh, A. (2016). It’s about the people…: Seniors’ perspectives on age-friendly communities. In T. Moulaert & S. Garon (Eds.), Age-friendly communities in international comparison (pp. 81–98). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-24031-2_6

Lewis, J. R. (2013). Hair-pulling, culture, and unmourned death. International Journal of Psychoanalytic Self Psychology, 8, 202–217. doi:10.1080/1 5551024.2013.768749

Lewis, M. (1997). The self in self-conscious emo- tions. In J. G. Snodgrass  & R. L. Thompson (Eds.), The self across psychology: Self-aware- ness, self-recognition, and the self-concept (pp. 119–142). New  York: New  York Academy of Science.

Lewis, M. (2011). The origins and uses of self- awareness or the mental representations of me. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 120–129.

Lewis, M. (2016). The emergence of human emotions. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis,  &

J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 272–292). New  York: Guilford.

Lewis, M.,  & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum.

Lewis, M., Lamson, A., & White, M. (2016). The state of dyadic methodology: An Analysis of the literature on interventions for military couples. Journal of Couple  & Relationship therapy, 15, 135–157. doi:10.1080/15332691.2015 .1106998

Lewis, M., Takai-Kawakami, K., Kawakami, K., & Sullivan, M. W. (2010). Cultural differences in emotional responses to success and failure. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 53–61.

Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). Infants’ perception of the audible, visible, and bimodal attributes of multimodal syllables. Child Development, 71, 1241–1257.

Lewontin, R. (1976). Race and intelligence. In N. J. Block & G. Dworkin (Eds.), The IQ controversy (pp. 78–92). New York: Pantheon.

Leys, D., Murao, K.,  & Pasquier, F. (2014). Vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. In B. Norrving (Ed.), Oxford textbook of stroke and cerebrovascular disorders (pp. 215–224). New York: Oxford University Press.

Li-Korotky, H.-S. (2012). Age-related hearing loss: Quality of care for quality of life. The Gerontologist, 52, 265–271. doi:10.1093/ geront/gnr159

Li, C. M., Zhang, X., Hoffman, H. J., Cotch, M. F., Themann, C. L.,  & Wilson, M. R. (2014). Hearing impairment associated with depres- sion in US adults, National Health and Nutri- tion Examination Survey, 2005-2010. JAMA Otolaryngology—Head  & Neck Surgery, 140, 293–302. doi:10.1001/jamaoto.2014.42

Li, J.,  & Prigerson, H. G (2016). Assessment and associated features of prolonged grief disorder among Chinese bereaved individuals. C omprehen sive P sy chi atr y, 66, 9–16. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.12.001

Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing: Age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12, 230–237. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00341

Li, Y., Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., Dong, T., Archodidou, A., Kim, I.-H., et  al. (2007). Emergent leadership in children’s discussion groups. Cognition & Instruction, 25, 75–111.

Liang, H., & Eley, T. C. (2005). A monozygotic twin differences study of nonshared environmental influence on adolescent depressive symptoms. Child Development, 76, 1247–1260.

Liben, L. S. (2016). We’ve come a long way, baby (But we’re not there yet): Gender past, present, and future. Child Development, 87, 5–28.

Liben, L. S.,  & Bigler, R . S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67 (Serial No. 269).

Libertus, K.,  & Needham, A. (2010). Teach to reach: The effects of active versus passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Research, 50, 2750–2757.

Libertus, K., Joh, A. S., & Needham, A. W. (2016). Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. Developmental Science, 19, 1058–1066.

Lichtenberg, P. (2016). Financial exploitation, financial capacity, and Alzheimer’s disease. Amer ic an P sy cholog i st , 71, 312–320. doi:10.1037/a0040192

Lichtenthal, W. G., & Sweeney, C. (2014). Families “at risk” of complicated bereavement. In D. W. Kissane & F. Parnes (Eds.), Bereavement care for families (pp. 249–265). New  York: Routledge.

Lichter, D. T.,  & Carmalt, J. H. (2009). Religion and marital quality among low-income couples. Social Science Research, 38, 168–187. doi:10.1016 /j.ssresearch.2008.07.003

Liddle, H. A. (2016). Multidimensional family therapy: Evidence base for transdiagnostic treatment outcomes, change mechanisms, and implementation in community settings. Family Process, 55, 558–576.

Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science, 12, 264–271.

Lieber, J. (2001, October 10). Widows of tower disaster cope, but with quiet fury. USA Today, pp. A1–A2.

Light, L. L. (2012). Dual-process theories of memory in old age: An update. In M. Naveh- Benjamin  & N. Ohta (Eds.). Memory and aging: Current issues and future directions (pp. 97–124). New York: Psychology Press.

Lighthouse International. (2014). Age-related macular degeneration (AMD). Retrieved from lighthouse.org/about-low-vision- blindness/ v i s i o n - d i s o r d e r s / a g e - r e l a t e d - m a c u l a r- degeneration-amd/.

Lilgendahl, J. P.,  & McAdams, D. P. (2011). C o n s t r u c t i n g s to r i e s o f s el f - g ro w th : How individual differences in patterns of autobiographical reasoning relate to well-being in midlife. Journal of Personality, 79, 391–428. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00688.x

Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D.,  & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1–34.

Limb, G. E., & Shafer, K. (2014, January). The impact of kin support on American Indian families. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Social Work and Research, San Antonio, TX.

Limb, G. E., Shafer, K.,  & Sandoval, K. (2014). The impact of kin support on urban American Indian families. Child  & Family Social Work, 19, 423–442. doi:10.1111/cfs.12041

Lin, C. C.,  & Fu, V. R. (1990). A comparison of childrearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian-American parents. Child Development, 61, 429–433.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 625

Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L.,  & Linn, M. C. (2010). New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1123–1135.

Lindbergh, C. A., Dishman, R. K., & Miller, L. S. (2016). Functional disability I mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta- analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 26, 129–159. doi:10.1007/s11065-016-9321-5

Linden, D. E. J. (2016). Neuroimaging and neurophysiology in psychiatry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Linden, J. A.,  & Olshaker, J. S. (2014). Elder mistreatment. In J. H. Kahn, B. G. Magauran, Jr., & J. S. Olshaker (Eds.), Geriatric emergency medicine: Principles and practice (pp. 355–360). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lindgren, L ., Bergdahl, J.,  & Nyberg , L . (2016). Longitudinal evidence for smaller hippocampus volume as a vulnerability factor for perceived stress. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 3527– 3533. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw154

Lindsey, E. W., Cremeens, P. R., & Caldera, Y. M. (2010). Mother-child and father-child mutuality in two contexts: Consequences for young children’s peer relationships. Infant and Child Development, 19, 142–160.

Linebarger, D. L.,  & Vaala, S. E. (2010). Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: An ecological perspective. Developmental Review, 30, 176–202.

Linver, M. R., Roth, J. L.,  & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Patterns of adolescents’ participation in organized activities: Are sports best when combined with other activities? Developmental Psychology, 45, 354–367.

Lipp, I., Benedek, M., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Bergner, S. et al. (2012). Inves- tigating neural efficiency in the visuo- spatial domain: An fMRI study. PLoS ONE, 7, e51316. doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0051316. Retrieved from www.plosone.org/ article/ info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fj our nal.p one .0051316.

Lips-Wiersma, M., Wright, S., & Dik. B. (2016). Meaningful work: Differences among blue-, pink-, and white-collar occupations. Career Development International, 21, 534–551. doi:10.1108/DCI-04-2016-0052

Lips, H. M. (2016). The gender pay gap and the wellbeing of working women. In M. L. Connerley  & J. Wu (eds.), Handbook on well-being of working women (pp. 141–157). New York: Springer.

Lipsitt, L. P. (1990). Learning and memory in infants. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 53–66.

Lipsitt, L. P. (2003). Crib death: A biobehavioral phenomenon. Psychological Science, 12, 164–170.

Lipton, A., Costa, L., Sieber, P., Dougall, W. C., & Braun, A. (2014). New targeted therapies for bone metastases. In V. Vassiliou, E. Chow,  & D. Kardamakis (Eds.), Bone metastases (pp. 235–246). New York: Springer.

Litovsky, R. (2015). Development of the auditory system. In G. G. Celesia  & G. Hickok (Eds.),

Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 129, The human auditory system, pp. 55–72). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Littlecott, H. J., Moore, G. F., Moore, L., Lyons, R. A., & Murphy, S. (2016). Associations between breakfast consumption and educa- tional outcomes in 9- to11-year-old children. Public Health Nutrition, 19, 1575–1582.

Liu, C.-C., Kanekiyo, T., Xu, H., & Bu, G. (2013). Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer’s disease: Risk , mechanisms and therapy. Nature Reviews Neurology, 9, 106–118. doi:10.1038/ nrneurol.2012.263

Liu, D., Gelman, S. A.,  & Wellman, H. M. (2007). Components of young children’s trait understanding: Behavior-to-trait and trait-to- behavior predictions. Child Development, 78, 1543–1558.

Liu, G., Dupre, M. E., Gu, D., Mair, C. A.,  & Chen, F. (2012). Psychological well-being of the institutionalized and community-residing oldest old in China: The role of children. Social Science and Medicine, 75, 1874–1882. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.019

Liu, R X., Lin, W., & Chen, Z. Y. (2010). School performance, peer association, psychological and behavioral adjustments: A comparison between Chinese adolescents with and without siblings. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 411–417.

Livesley, W. J.,  & Bromley, D. B. (1973). Person perception in childhood and adolescence. New York: Wiley.

Livingston, G. (2014). Four-in-ten couples are saying “I do,” again. Retrieved from www .pewsocialtrends.org/2014/11/14/four-in-ten- couples-are-saying-i-do-again/.

Lo, M., & Aziz, T. (2009). Muslim marriage goes online: The use of Internet matchmaking by American Muslims. Journal of Religion and Popular Culture, 2(3). doi:10.3138/ jrpc.21.3.005

Lobo, M. A., & Galloway, J. C. (2012). Enhanced handling and positioning in early infancy advances development throughout the first year. Child Development, 83, 1290–1302.

LoBue, V.,  & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13, 221–228.

Lochman, J. E. Baden, R. E., Boxmeyer, C. L., Powell, N. P., Qu, L., Salekin, K. L. et  al. (2014). Does a booster intervention augment the preventive effects of an abbreviated version of the coping power program for aggressive children? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 367–381.

Loheswaran, G., Barr, M. S., Rajji, T. K., Blumberger, D. M., Le Foll, B., & Daskalakis, Z. J. (2016). Alcohol intoxication by binge drinking impairs neuroplasticity. Brain Stimulation, 9, 27–32. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.011

Lomanowska, A. M.,  & Guitton, M. J. (2016). Online intimacy and well-being in the digital age. Internet Interventions, 4(Part2), 138–144. doi:10.1016/j.invent.2016.06.005. Retrieved from www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/ pii/S2214782916300021.

LongTermCare.gov. (2016). How much care will you need? Retrieved from longtermcare.gov/ the-basics/how-much-care-will-you-need/.

Lopata, H. Z. (1996). Widowhood and husband sanctification. In D. Klass, P. R. Silverman, & S. L. Nickman (Eds.), Continuing bonds: New understandings of grief (pp. 149–162). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

Lopez, A. B., Huynh, V. W., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). A longitudinal study of religious identity and participation during adolescence. Child Development, 82, 1297–1309.

Lord, K., Livingston, G., Robertson, S., & Cooper, C. (2016). How people with dementia and their families decide about moving to a care home and support their needs: Development of a decision aid, a qualitative study. BMC Geriatrics, 16, 68. doi:10.1186/s12877-016-0242-1. Retrieved from bmcgeriatr.biomedcentral.com/ articles/10.1186/s12877-016-0242-1.

Loretto, W., & Vickerstaff, S. (2013). The domes- tic and gendered context for retirement. Human Relations, 66, 65–86. doi:10.1177/ 0018726712455832

Lovinger, D. M., & Roberto, M. (2013). Synaptic effects induced by alcohol. Behavioral Neurobiology of Alcohol Addiction, 13, 31–86. doi:10.1007/7854_2011_143

Lozoff, B., Wolf, A. W.,  & Davis, N. S. (1985). Sleep problems seen in pediatric practice. Pediatrics, 75, 477–483.

Lu, F. J. H., Lee, W. P., Chang, Y-K., Chou, C-C., Hsu, Y-W., Lin, J-H. et al. (2016). Interaction of athletes’ resilience and coaches’ social support on the stress-burnout relationship: A conjunctive moderation perspective. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 22, 202–209.

Lubans, D., Richards, J., Hillman, C., Faulkner, G., Beauchamp, M., Nilsson, M. et  al. (2016). Physical activity for cognitive and mental health in youth: A systematic review of mechanisms. Pediatrics, 38(3), e20161642.

Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P, Webb, R. M.,  & Bleske-Rechek, A. (2006). Tracking exceptional human capital over two decades. Psychological Science, 17, 194–199.

Lucero-Liu, A. A. (2007). Exploring intersections in the intimate lives of Mexican origin women. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section  A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(3-A), 1175.

Lucette, A., Ironson, G., Pargament, K. I.,  & Krause, N. (2016). Spirituality and religiousness are associated with fewer depressive symptoms in individuals with medical conditions. Psychosomatics, 57, 505–513. doi:10.1016/j. psym.2016.03.005

Luders, E. (2014). Exploring age-related brain degeneration in meditation practitioners. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307, 82–88. doi:10.1111/nyas.12217

Luders, E.,  & Cherbuin, N. (2016). Searching for the philosopher’s stone: Promising links between meditation and brain preservation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373, 38–44. doi:10.1111/nyas.13082

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

626 R e f e R e n c e s

Ludke, R. L.,  & Smucker, D. R. (2007). Racial differences in the willingness to use hospice services. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 10, 1329–1337. doi:10.1089/jpm.2007.0077

Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012). Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 592– 615. doi:10.1037/a0025948

Luke, B., Gopal, D., Cabral, H., Diop, H., & Stern, J. (2016). Perinatal outcomes of singleton siblings: The effects of changing maternal fertility status. Journal of Assisted Reproduction and Genetics, 9, 1203–1213.

Lukowski, A. F.,  & Bauer, P. J. (2014). Long- term memory in infancy and early childhood. In P.  J.  Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 230–254). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Lundström, J. N.,  & Jones-G otman, M. (2009). Romantic love modulates women’s identification of men’s body odors. Hormones and Behavior, 55, 280–284. doi:10.1016/j. yhbeh.2008.11.009

Lung, F-W., & Shu, B-C. (2011). Sleeping position and health status of children at six-, eighteen-, and thirty-six-month development. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 713–718.

Luo, R., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2016). Mothers’ verbal and nonverbal strategies in relation to infants’ object-directed actions in real time and across the first three years in ethnically diverse families. Infancy, 21, 65–89.

Luo, S.,  & Zhang, G. (2009). What leads to romantic attraction: Similarity, reciprocity, security, or beauty? Evidence from a speed- dating study. Journal of Personality, 77, 933–964. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00570.x

Luong, G., Charles, S. T.,  & Fingerman, K. L. (2011). Better with age: Social relationships a c ro s s a d u l th o o d . Jo ur n al o f S o c i al and Personal Relationships, 28, 9–23. doi:10.1177/0265407510391362

Lushington, K., Pamula, Y., Martin, J., & Kennedy, J. D. (2013). Developmental changes in sleep: Infancy and preschool years. In A. R. Wolfson and H. W. Montgomer y-Downs (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of infant, child, and adolescent sleep and behavior (pp. 34–47). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Luthans, F., Avolio, B. J., Avey, J. B., & Norman, S. M. (2007). Positive psychological capital: Mea- surement and relationship with performance and satisfaction. Personnel Psychology, 60, 541–572. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007. 00083.x

Luxenberg, S. (2014). Welcoming love at an older age, but not necessarily marriage. New  York Times (April 25). Retrieved from www.nytimes .com/2014/04/26/your-money/welcoming- love-at-an-older-age-but-not-necessarily- marriage.html?_r=0.

Lyall, K., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Fallin, M. D., Ladd-Ascota, C., Lee, B. K., et al. (2017). The changing epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual Review of Public Health, 38, 9.1–9.22.

Lyng, S. (2012). Existential transcendence in late modernity: Edgework and hermeneutic reflexivity. Human Studies, 35, 401–414. doi:10.1007 /s10746-012-9242-0

Lyster, S-A Halaas, Lervåg, A. O.,  & Hulme, C. (2016). Preschool morphological training produces long-term improvements in reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 29, 1269–1288.

Lytton, H. (2000). Toward a model of family- environmental and child-biological influences on development. Developmental Review, 20, 150–179.

Lytton, H.,  & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.

Maatz, L., & Hedgepath, A. (2013). Women and work: 50 years of change since the American Women Report. Retrieved from www.dol.gov/ wb/resources/women_and_work.pdf.

MacArthur, C. A . (2015). Instruction in evaluation and revision. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham,  & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (2nd ed., pp. 272–287). New York: Guilford.

Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A de velopment al account . Amer ic an Psychologist, 45, 513–520.

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

Maccoby, E. E.,  & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Mace, J. H., & Clevinger, A. M. (2013). Priming voluntar y autobiog raphic al memor ies : Implic ations for the organi z ation of autobiographical memor y and voluntar y recall processes. Memory, 21, 524–536. doi:10. 1080/09658211.2012.744422

Maciejewski, P. K., Zhang, B, Block, S. D.,  & Prigerson, H. G. (2007). An empirical examination of the stage theory of grief. JAMA, 297, 716–723. doi:10.1001/jama.297.7.716

Mackenzie, P. (2012). Normal changes of ageing. InnovAiT. doi:10.1093/ innovait/ins099

Macnamara , B. N., Hambrick , D. Z .,  & Oswald, F. L. (2014). Deliberate practice and performance in music, games, sports, education, and professions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 25, 1608–1618. doi: 10.1177/0956797614535810

MacWhinney, B. (1998). Models of the emergence of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 199–227.

Magai, C. (2008). Long-lived emotions: A life course perspective on emotional development. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones,  & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 376–392). New York: Guilford.

Magee, L.,  & Hale, L. (2012). Longitudinal associations between sleep duration and subsequent weight gain: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16, 231–241.

Magnuson, K., & Duncan, G. (2006). The role of family socioeconomic resources in black and

white test score gaps among young children. Developmental Review, 26, 365–399.

Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K.,  & Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 1091–1101.

Mahady, G. B., Locklear, T. D., Doyle, B. J., Huang, Y., Perez, A. L., & Caceres, A. (2008). Menopause, a universal female experience: Lessons from Mexico and Central America. Current Women’s Health Reviews, 4, 3–8. doi:10.2174/157340408783572033

Mahon, N. E., Yarcheski, A., Yarcheski, T., Cannella, B. L., & Hanks, M. M. (2006). A meta- analytic study of predictors for loneliness during adolescence. Nursing Research, 55, 308–315.

Maitre, N. L., Slaughter, J. C., Aschner, J. L.,  & Key, A. P. (2014). Hemispheric differences in speech-sound event-related potentials in intensive care neonates: Associations and predictive value for development in infancy. Journal of Child Neurology, 29, 903–911.

Makel, M. C., Kell, H. J., Lubinski, D., Putallaz, M., & Benbow, C. P. (2016). When lightning strikes twice: Profoundly gifted, profoundly accomplished. Psychological Science, 27, 1004–1018.

Malberg, N. T. (2014). Revisiting, repairing, and restoring: The developmental journey of a bereaved adolescent. In P. Cohen, K. M. Sossin, & R. Ruth (Eds.), Healing after parent loss in childhood and adolescence (pp. 135–158). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Malkinson, R., & Bar-Tur, L. (2004–2005). Long term bereavement processes of older parents: The three phases of grief. Omega: The Journal of Death and Dying, 50, 103–129. doi:10.2190/ W346-UP8T-RER6-BBD1

Malone, M. L., & Camp, C. J. (2007). Montessori- Based Dementia Programming®: Providing tools for engagement. Dementia: The International Journal of Social Research and Practice, 6, 150–157. doi:10.1177/1471301207079099

M a n d a r a , J . , G ay l o r d - H a r d e n , N . K . , Richard, M. H.,  & Ragsdale, B. L. (2009). The effects of changes in racial identity and self-esteem on changes in African American adolescents’ mental health. Child Development, 80, 1660–1675.

Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W.,  & Pisoni, D. B. (1995). Infants’ recognition of the sound patterns of their own names. Psychological Science, 6, 314–317.

Mandemakers, J. J., & Monden, C. W. S. (2013). Does the effect of job loss on psychological distress differ by educational level? Work, Employ ment and Societ y, 27, 73–93. doi:10.1177/0950017012460312

Mange, A. P.,  & Mange, E. J. (1990). Genetics: Human aspects (2nd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinhauer Associates.

Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1992). Developmental changes in infant–stranger interaction. Infant Behavior & Development, 15, 191–208.

Mankus, A. M., Boden, M. T., & Thompson, R. J. (2016). Sources of variation in emotional awareness: Age, gender, and socioeconomic

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 627

status. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 28–33. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.043

Mantler, J., Matejicek, A., Matheson, K.,  & Anisman, H. (2005). Coping with employment uncertainty: A comparison of employed and unemploye d workers . Jour nal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10, 200–209. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.10.3.200

Maple, M., Edwards, H. E., Minichiello, V.,  & Plummer, D. (2013). Still part of the family: The importance of physical, emotional and spiri- tual memorial places and spaces for parents bereaved through the suicide death of their son or daughter. Mortality, 18, 54–71. doi:10.1080/ 13576275.2012.755158

Maple, M., Edwards, H., Plummer, D.,  & Minichiello, V. (2010). Silenced voices: Hearing the stories of parents bereaved through the suicide death of a young adult child. Health and Social Care in the Community, 18, 241– 248. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2524.2009.00886.x

Maratsos, M. (1998). The acquisition of grammar. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. New York: Wiley.

Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley.

Marcia, J.,  & Josselson, R. (2013). Eriksonian personality research and its implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Personality, 81, 617–629. doi:10.1111/ jopy.12014

Marcus , G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J.,  & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (Serial No. 228).

Marcus, R. (2013). Denouncing binge drink- ing is not victim-blaming. Opinion article appearing in the Washington Post. Retrieved from www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/ ruth-marcus-missing-the-point-on-binge- drinking/2013/10/24/56c8a70a-3ce0-11e3- a94f-b58017bfee6c_story.html.

Mares, M-L., & Pan, Z. (2013). Effects of Sesame Street: A meta-analysis of children’s learning in 15 countries. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34, 140–151.

Margett, T. E.,  & Witherington, D. C. (2011). The nature of preschoolers’ concept of living and artificial objects. Child Development, 82, 2067–2082.

Margrett, J. A., Allaire, J. C., Johnson, T. L., Daugherty, K. E., & Weatherbee, S. R. (2010). Everyday problem solving. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 1: Psychological aspects (pp. 79–101). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Markovits, H., Benenson, J.,  & Dolenszky, E. (2001). Evidence that children and adolescents have internal models of peer interactions that are gender differentiated. Child Development, 72, 879–886.

Marks, A. K., Patton, F., & García Coll, C. (2011). Being bicultural: A mixed-methods study of adolescents’ implicitly and explicitly measured multie thnic identitie s . D e velopment al Psychology, 47, 270–288.

Markus, H.,  & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954

Marschik, P. B., Einspieler, C., Strohmeier, A., Plienegger, J., Garzarolli, B., & Prechtl, H. F. R. (2008). From the reaching behavior at 5 months of age to hand preference at preschool age. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 511–518.

Marsh, H. W., & Martin, A. J. (2011). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 59–77.

Marsh, K.,  & Musson, G. (2008). Men at work and at home: Managing emotion in telework. Gender, Work  & Organization, 15, 31–48. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0432.2007

Marsiglio, W.,  & Roy, K. (2013). Fathers’ nurturance of children over the life course. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 353– 376). New York: Springer.

Martens, A., Goldenberg, J. L.,  & Greenberg, J. (2005). A terror management perspective on ageism. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 223–239. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00403.x

Martin-Storey, A. (2016). Gender, sexuality, and gender nonconformity: Understanding variation in functioning. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 257–262.

Martin-Uzzi, M., & Duval-Tsioles, D. (2013). The experience of remarried couples in blended families. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 54, 43–57. doi:10.1080/10502556.2012.743828

Martin, C. L.,  & Halverson, C. F. (1987). The roles of cognition in sex role acquisition. In D. B. Carter (Ed.), Current conceptions of sex roles and sex typing: Theory and research (pp. 123–137). New York: Praeger.

Martin, C. L.,  & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.

Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Evans, S. M.,  & Wyman, H. (1999). Social cognition on the playground: Children’s beliefs about playing with girls versus boys and their relationships to sex-segregated play. Journal of Social  & Personal Relationships, 16, 751–772.

Martin, C. L., Kornienko, O., Schaefer, D. R., Hanish, L. D., Fabes, R. A., & Goble, P. (2013). The role of sex of peers and gender-typed activities in young children’s peer affiliative networks: A longitudinal analysis of selection and inf luence. Child Development, 84, 921–937.

Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Ventura, S. J., Osterman, M. J. K.,  & Mathews, T. J. (2013). Births: Final data for 2011. National Vital Statistics Reports, 62(1). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Martin, J., & Ross, H. (2005). Sibling aggression: Sex differences and parents’ reactions. In t e r n a t i o n a l Jo ur n a l o f B e h a v i o ra l Development, 29, 129–138.

Martin, M., Long, M. V.,  & Poon, L. W. (2002). Age changes and differences in personality traits and states of the old and very old. Journals

of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P144–P152. doi:10.1093/ geronb/57.2.P144

Martin, S. P., Astone, N. M., & Peters, H. E. (2014). Fewer marriages, more divergence: Marriage projections for millennials to age 40. Retrieved from www.urban.org/research/ publication/ fewer-marriages-more-divergence- marriage- p r o j e c t i o n s - m i l l e n n i a l s - a g e - 4 0 / v i e w / full_report.

Mash, H. B. H., Fullerton, C. S., & Ursano, R. J. (2013). Complicated grief and bereavement in young adults following close friend and sibling loss. Depression and Anxiety, 30(12), 1202– 1210. doi:10.1002/da.22068

Masunaga, H.,  & Horn, J. (2001). Expertise and age-related changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293– 311. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.293

Masur, E. F. (1995). Infants’ early verbal imitation and their later lexical development. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 41, 286–306.

Materne, C. J., Luszcz, M. A., & Goodwin-Smith, I. (2014). Increasing constructive engagement and positive affect for residents with severe and very severe dementia through group-based activities. Australasian Journal on Ageing. doi:10.1111/ajag.12127

Mather, M. (2016). The affective neurosci- ence of aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 213–238. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych- 122414-033540

Mathur, M. B., Epel, E., Kind, S., Manisha, D., Parks, C. G., Sandler, D. P. et al., (2016). Per- ceived stress and telomere length: A system- atic review, meta-analysis, and methodologic considerations for advancing the field. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 54, 158–169. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.02.002

Mattis, J.,  & Sehgal, A . (2016). Circadian rhythms, sleep, and disorders of aging. Trends in Endocrinology & Metabolism, 27, 192–203. doi:10.1016/j.tem.2016.02.003

Mattys, S. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition, 78, 91–121.

Mauno, S., Cheng, T.,  & Lim, V. (2017). The far- reaching consequences of job insecurity: A review on family-related outcomes. Marriage and Family Review, doi: 10.1080/01494929.2017.1283382

Maxfield, M., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T.,  & Greenberg, J. (2010). Mortality salience effects on the life expectancy estimates of older adults as a function of neuroticism. Journal of Aging Research. doi: 10.4061/2010/260123. Retrieved from www.hindawi.com/journals/ jar/2010/260123/.

May, P. A ., Blankenship, J., Marais, A-S., Gossage, J. P., Kalberg, W. O., Joubert, B., et al. (2013). Maternal alcohol consumption produc- ing fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD): Quantity, frequency, and timing of drinking. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 133, 502–512.

Mayer, R. E. (2014). What problem solvers know: Cognitive readiness for adaptive problem solving. In H. F. O’Neil, R. S. Perez,  & E. L. Baker (Eds.), Teaching and measuring cognitive readiness (pp. 149–160). New York: Springer.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

628 R e f e R e n c e s

Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development, 73, 969–982.

Mayo Clinic. (2015). Aerobic exercise. Retrieved from www.mayoclinic.org/healthy-lifestyle/ fitness/basics/aerobic-exercise/hlv-20049447.

Mayo Clinic. (2016). Managing your cholesterol. R e t r i e v e d f ro m w w w. m ay o c l i n i c . o rg / cholesterol-site/scs-20089333.

Mazur, E.,  & Li, Y. (2016). Identity and self- presentation on social networking web sites: A comparison of online profiles of Chinese and American emerging adults. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5, 101–118. doi:10.1037/ppm0000054

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100– 122. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.100

McAdams, D. P. (2015). The art and science of personality development. New York: Guilford.

McAdams, D. P., & Guo, J. (2015). Narrating the generative life. Psychological Science, 26, 475– 483. doi:10.1177/0956797614568318

McAdams, T. A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Narusyte, J., Ganiban, J. M., Reiss, D., Spotts, E., et  al. (2017). Associations between the parent– child relationship and adolescent self-worth: A genetically informed study of twin parents and their adolescent children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 46–54.

McAlaney, J., Bewick, B., & Hughes, C. (2011). The international development of the “social norms” approach to drug education and prevention. Drugs: Education, Prevention, and Policy, 18, 81–89. doi:10.3109/09687631003610977

McCall, R. B. (1979). Infants. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

McCarty, W. P.,  & Skogan, W. G. (2013). Job- related burnout among civilian and sworn police personnel. Police Quarterly, 16, 66–84. doi:10.1177/1098611112457357

McClinton, B. E. (2010). Preparing for the third age: A retirement planning course outline for lifelong learning programs. Master’s thesis from California State University, Long Beach. Retrieved from gradworks.umi.com/1486345 .pdf.

McClung, M. R ., Grauer, A ., Boonen, S., Bolognese, M. A., Brown, J. P., Diez-Perez, A., et al. (2014). Romosozumab in postmenopausal women with low bone mineral density. New England Journal of Medicine, 370, 412–420. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1305224.

McCollum, L.,  & Pincus, T. (2009). A biopsy- chosocial model to complement a biomedical model: Patient questionnaire data and socio- economic status usually are more significant than laboratory tests and imaging studies in prognosis of rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatoid Disease Clinics of North America, 35, 699–712. doi:10.1016/j.rdc.2009.10.003

McCormick, C. B. (2003). Metacognition and learning. In I. B. Weiner (Editor-in-Chief ) and W. M. Reynolds & G. E. Miller (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 7. Educational Psychology (pp. 79–102). New York: Wiley.

McCoyd, J. L. M.,  & Walter, C. A. (2016). Grief and loss across the lifespan: A biopsychosocial perspective (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

McCrae, R. R. (2016). Integrating trait and process approaches to personality: A sketch of an agenda. In U. Kumar (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of personality assessment (pp. 3–18). Malden, MA: Wiley.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor theory perspective (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

McCrae, R . R .,  & Terracciano, A . (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality  & Social Psychology, 88, 547–561. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88. 3.547

McCutchen, D., Covill, A., Hoyne, S. H, & Mildes, K. (1994). Individual differences in writing: Implications of translating fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 256–266.

McDonald, K. L., Bowker, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Laursen, B.,  & Duchene, M. S. (2010). Interactions between rejection sensitivity and supportive relationships in the prediction of adolescents’ internalizing difficulties. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 39, 563–574.

McElwain, N. L., Booth-LaForce, C.,  & Wu, X. (2011). Infant-mother attachment and children’s friendship quality: Maternal mental- state talk as an intervening mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1295–1311.

McGarry, K., & Schoeni, R. F. (2005). Widow(er) p o v e r t y a n d o u t- o f - p o c k e t m e d i c a l expenditures near the end of life. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 60, S160–S168. doi:10.1093/ geronb/60.3.S160

McGinley, M., Lipperman-Kreda, S., Byrnes, H. F., & Carlo, G. (2010). Parental, social and dispositional pathways to Israeli adolescents’ volunteering. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 386–394.

McGraw, M. B. (1935). Growth: A study of Johnny and Jimmy. East Nor walk , CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

McGuire, S.,  & Shanahan, L. (2010). Sibling experiences in diverse family contexts. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 72–79.

McHale, J. P., Laurette, A ., Talbot, J.,  & Pourquette, C. (2002). Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of coparenting and family group process. In J. P. McHale  & W. Grolnick (Eds.), Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of families (pp. 127–165). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

McHale, S. M., Kim, J. Y., Whiteman, S. D.,  & Crouter, A. C. (2004). Links between sex- typed activities in middle childhood and gender development in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 40, 868–881.

McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Whiteman, S. D. (2012). Sibling relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74, 913–930.

McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Whiteman, S. D. (2013). Sibling relationships. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and

the family (pp. 329–351). New York: Springer Science+Business Media.

McHale, S. M., Whiteman, S. D., Kim, J.-Y.,  & Crouter, A. C. (2007). Characteristics and correlates of sibling relationships in two-parent African American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 227–235.

McKay, B. (2016). A cholesterol conundrum. Wall Street Journal, February 9, D1–D2.

McKee-Ryan, F., Song, Z., Wanberg, C. R.,  & Kinicki, A. J. (2005). Psychological and physical well-being during unemployment: A meta- analytic study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 53–76. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.53

McKee, A. C., Cairns, N. J., Dickson, D. W., Folkerth, R. D., Keene, C. D., Litvan, I., et  al. (2016). The first NINDS/NIBIB consensus meeting to define neuropathological criteria for the diagnosis of chronic traumatic encephalopathy. Acta Neuropathologica, 131, 75–86. doi:10.1007/s00401-015-1515-z

McKee, K. J.,  & Schüz, B. (2015). Psychosocial factors in healthy aging. Psychology & Health, 30, 607–626. doi:10.1080/08870446.2015. 1026905

McLanahan, S.,  & Jencks, C. (2015). Was Moynihan right? What happens to children of unmarried mothers. Education Next, 15, 14–21.

McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural devel- opment: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578–591.

McLean, K. C. (2016). The co-authored self : Family stories and the construction of personal identity. New York: Oxford University Press.

McLean, K. C., & Pasupathi, M. (2012). Processes of identity development: Where I am and how I got there. Identity, 12, 8–28. doi:10.1080/15283 488.2011.632363

McLymont, R. (2016). State of women-owned businesses. The Network Journal, (Spring), 18–19.

Mc Nu tt , B . ,   & Ya k u sh ko , O. ( 2 0 1 3 ) . Disenfranchised grief among lesbian and gay bereaved individuals. Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, 7, 87–116. doi:10.1080/15538605. 2013.758345

McQuade, J. D. (2017). Peer victimization and changes in physical and relational aggression: The moderating role of executive functioning abilities. Aggressive Behavior, in press.

Meaney, M. J. (2010) Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene X environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 41–79.

Me dic are.gov. (2016). W hat’s Me dic are supplemental insurance (Medigap)? Retrieved from www.medicare.gov/supplement-other- insurance/medigap/whats-medigap.html.

MedlinePlus. (2014). Aging changes in the female reproductive system. Retrieved from www.nlm .nih.gov/medlineplus/ency/article/004016.htm.

Medwell, J.,  & Wray, D. (2014). Handwriting automaticity: The search for performance thresholds. Language and Education, 28, 34–51.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 629

Meeker, J. D., & Benedict, M. D. (2013). Infertility, pregnancy loss and adverse birth outcomes in relation to maternal secondhand tobacco smoke exposure. Current Women’s Health Reviews, 9, 41–49.

Meeus, W., van de Schoot, R., Keijsers, L., Schwartz, S. J.,  & Branje, S. (2010). On the progression and stability of adolescent iden- tity formation: A five-wave longitudinal study in early-to-middle and middle-to-late adoles- cence. Child Development, 81, 1565–1581.

Mehta, C. M.,  & Strough, J. (2009). Sex segregation in friendships and normative contexts across the life span. Developmental Review, 29, 201–220.

Meijer A. M.,  & van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2007). Contribution of infants’ sleep and crying to marital relationship of first-time parent couples in the 1st year after childbirth. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 49–57. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.49

Meilaender, G. (2013). Bioethics: A primer for Christians (3rd ed.). Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans Publishing.

Melby-Lervåg, M.,  & Lervåg, A. (2014). Effects of educational interventions targeting reading comprehension and underlying components. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 96–100.

Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. H.,  & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352.

Melby, J. N., Conger, R. D., Fang, S., Wickrama, K.  A. S.,  & Conger, K. J. (2008). Adolescent family experiences and educational attainment during early adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1519–1536.

Melson, G. F. (2003). Child development and the human-companion animal bond. American Behavioral Scientist, 47, 31–39.

Melson, G. F. (2010). Play between children and domestic animals. In E. Enwokah, (Ed.), Play as engagement and communication (pp. 23–39). Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.

Ménard, C., Hodes, G. E., & Russo, S. J. (2016). Pathogenesis of depression: Insights from human and rodent studies. Neuroscience, 321, 138–162. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience. 2015.05.053

Mendez-Luck, C. A., Geldhof, G. J., Anthony, K. P., Steers, W. N., Mangione, C. M., & Hays, R. D. (2016). Orientation to the caregiver role among Latinas of Mexican origin. The Gerontologist, 56, e99–e108. doi:10.1093/geront/gnw087

Mendle, J.,  & Ferrero, J. (2012). Detrimental psychological outcomes associated with pub er t al timing in adole s cent b oy s . Developmental Review, 32, 49–66.

Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2007). Detrimental psychological outcomes associ- ated with early pubertal timing in adolescent girls. Developmental Review, 27, 151–171.

Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1996). The human infant’s response to vanilla flavors in

mother’s milk and formula. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 13–19.

Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics, 107, e88.

Mennella, J.,  & Beauchamp, G. K. (1997). The ontogeny of human flavor perception. In G. K. Beauchamp  & L. Bartoshuk (Eds.), Tasting and smelling: Handbook of perception and cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Mensch, B. S., Singh, S., & Casterline, J. B. (2006). Trends in the timing of first marriage among men and women in the developing world. In C. B. Lloyd, J. R. Behrman, N. P. Stromquist, & B. Cohen (Eds.), The changing transitions to adulthood in developing countries: Selected studies (pp. 180–171). Washington, DC: National Research Council.

Mercer, T. (2016). Technology-assisted memory. In A. Attrill  & C. Fullwood (Eds.), Applied cyberpsychology (pp. 74–88). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1057/9781137517036_5

Merianos, A. L., King, K. A., Vidourek, R. A., & Nabors, L. A. (2015). Recent alcohol use and binge drinking based on authoritative parenting among Hispanic youth nationwide. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 1966–1976.

Merton, T. (1949). Seeds of contemplation. Norfolk, CT: New Directions.

Merton, T. (1955). No man is an island. San Diego: Harvest Books.

Merton, T. (1962). New seeds of contemplation. New York: New Directions.

Mervis, C. B., & Johnson, K. E. (1991). Acquisition of the plural morpheme: A case study. Developmental Psychology, 27, 222–235.

Merz, E.-M.,  & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2016). Childhood memories, family ties, sibling support and loneliness in ever-widowed older adults: Quantitative and qualitative results. Ageing and Society, 36, 534–561. doi:10.1017/ S0144686X14001329

Meunier, D., Stamatakis, E. A.,  & Tyler, L. K. (2014). Age-related functional reorganization, structural changes, and preserved cognition. Neurobiology of Aging, 35, 42–54. doi:10.1016/j. neurobiolaging.2013.07.003

Meyer, M., Gelman, S. A., Roberts, S. O., & Leslie, S.-J. (2016). My heart made me do it: Children’s essentialist belief about heart transplants. Cognitive Science, 1–19.

Michel, A. (2016). Burnout and the brain. Obser ver, 29 (Februar y). Retrieved from www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/ publications/obser ver/2016/februar y-16/ burnout-and-the-brain.html.

Midgette, E., Haria, P., & MacArthur, C. (2008). The effects of content and audience awareness goals for revision on the persuasive essays of fifth- and eighth-grade students. Reading and Writing, 21, 131–151.

Midlarsky, E., Kahana, E.,  & Belser, A. (2015). Prosocial behavior in late life. In D. A Schroeder & W. G. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 415–432). New York: Oxford University Press.

Miething, A., Almquist, Y. B., Rostila, M., Edling,  C.,  & Rydgren, J. (2016). Friendship networks and psychological well-being from late adolescence to young adulthood: A gender-sp e cific str uctural e quation modeling approach. BMC Psychology, 4, 34. doi:10.1186/s40359-016-0143-2. Retrieved from bmcpsychology.biomedcentral.com/ articles/10.1186/s40359-016-0143-2.

Mietkiewicz, M.-C.,  & Venditti, L. (2004). Les arrière-grands-pères le point de vue de leurs arrière-petits-enfants [Great-grandfathers from their great-grandchildren’s point of view]. Psychologie  & NeuroPsychiatrie du Vieillissement, 2, 275–283.

Miga, E. M., Gdula, J. A.,  & Allen, J. P. (2012). Fighting fair: Adaptive marital conflict strategies as predictors of future adolescent peer and romantic relationship quality. Social Development, 21, 443–460.

Miles, J. (2009). Autobiographical reflection and perspective transformation in adult learners returning to study: Research in progress. Retrieved from www.avetra.org.au/ papers-2009/papers/37.00.pdf.

Mileva-Seitz, V. R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Battaini, C., & Luijk, M. P. C. M. (2017). Parent- child bed-sharing: The good, the bad, and the burden of evidence. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 32, 4–27.

Miller, A. B., Wall, C., Baines, C. J., Sun, P., To,  T.,  & Narod, S. A. (2014). Twenty-five year follow-up for breast cancer incidence and mortality of the Canadian National Breast Screening study: Randomised screening trial. BMJ, 348, g366. doi:10.1136/bmj.g366

Miller, A. C. (2016). Opinions on the legitimacy of brain death among Sunni and Shi’a scholars. Journal of Religion and Health, 55, 394–402. doi:10.1007/s10943-015-0157-8

Miller, D. I.,  & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 37–45.

Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. A. (2016). Changing norms to change behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 339–361. doi:10.1146/ annurev-psych-010814-015013

Miller, G. E.,  & Chen, E. (2010). Harsh family climate in early life presages the emergence of proinflammatory phenotype in adolescence. Psychological Science, 21, 848–856.

Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality  & Social Psychology, 62, 541–554.

Miller, K. I., Shoemaker, M. M., Willyard, J.,  & Addison, P. (2008). Providing care for elderly par- ents: A structurational approach to family care- giver identity. Journal of Family Communication, 8, 19–43. doi:10.1080/15267430701389947

Miller, L. C., Johnson, A., Duggan, L., & Behm, M. (2011). Consequences of the “Back to Sleep” program in infants. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 26, 364–368.

Miller, P. H. (2011). Piaget’s theory: Past, present, and future. In U. Goswami (Ed.),

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

630 R e f e R e n c e s

The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 649–672). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Miller, P. J.,  & Rosengren, K. S. (2014). Final thoughts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 113–124. doi:10.1111/mono.12082

Miller, P. J., Rosengren, K. S.,  & Gutiérrez, I. T. (2014). Introduction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 1–18. doi:10.1111/mono.12076

Miller, P. M., Danaher, D. L.,  & Forbes, D. (1986). Sex-related strategies of coping with interpersonal conflict in children aged five to seven. Developmental Psychology, 22, 543–548.

Miller, S. A. (2009). Children’s understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 749–773.

Miller, T. W., Nigg, J. T.,  & Miller, R. L. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in African American children: What can be learned from the past ten years? Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 77–86.

Miller-Martinez, D.,  & Wallace, S. P. (2007). Structural contexts and life-course processes in the social networks of older Mexican immigrants in the United States. In S. Carmel, C. Morse,  & F. Torres-Gil (Eds.), Lessons on aging from three nations: Volume I. The art of aging well (pp. 141–154). Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Milligan, C., Turner, M., Blake, S., Brearley, S., Seamark , D., Thomas, C. et  al. (2016). Unpacking the impact of older adults’ home death on family care-givers’ experiences of home. Health  & Place, 38, 103–111. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.01.005

Minnotte, K . L . (2010). Methodologies of assessing marital success. Retrieved from workfamily.sas.upenn.edu/wfrn-repo/object/ w2yp4p7if8lm7q4k.

Mischel, W. (1970). Sex-typing and socialization. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

Mitchell, B. A. (2006). The boomerang age: Transitions to adulthood in families. New Brunswick, NJ: AldineTransaction.

Mitchell, B. A. (2010). Happiness in midlife parental roles: A contextual mixed methods analysis. Family Relations, 59, 326–339. doi:10.1111/ j.1741-3729.2010.00605.x

Mitchell, T. B., Amaro, C. M.,  & Steele, R. G. (2016). Pediatric weight management interventions in primary care settings: A meta- analysis. Health Psychology, 35, 704–713.

Mitteldorf, J. (2016). An epigenetic clock controls aging. Biogerontology, 17, 257–265. doi:10.1007/s10522-015-9617-5

Mize, J.,  & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers’ social coaching, mother-child relationship style, and children’s peer competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312–332.

Mize, J., Pettit, G. S.,  & Brown, E. G. (1995). Mothers’ supervision of their children’s peer play: Relations with beliefs, perceptions, and knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 31, 311–321.

Modecki, K. L., Hagan, M., Sandler, I., & Wolchik, S. (2015). Latent profiles of non-residential father engagement six years after divorce predict long term offspring outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 123–136.

Moeller, J. R., Lewis, M. M.,  & Werth, J. L., Jr. (2010). End of life issues. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 1: Psychological aspects (pp. 202–231). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Moen, P. (2016a). Encore adulthood: Boomers on the edge of risk, renewal, & purpose. New York: Oxford University Press.

Moen, P. (2016b). Work over the gendered life course. In M. J. Shanahan, J. T. Mortimer,  & M.  K. Johnson (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 249–275). New York: Springer.

Moen, P.,  & Roehling, P. (2005). The career mystique: Cracks in the American dream. Oxford, UK: Rowman & Littlefield.

Moerk, E. L. (2000). The guided acquisition of first language skills. Westport, CT: Ablex.

Molin, P.,  & Rockwood, K. (2016). The new criteria for Alzheimer’s disease—Implications for geriatricians. Canadian Geriatrics Journal, 19, 66–73. doi:10.5770/cgj.19.207

Molina, B. S. G., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Arnold, L. E., Vitiello, B., Jensen, P. S., et  al. (2009). The MTA at 8 years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for combined- type ADHD in a multisite study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 484–500.

Molina, L. C. (2012). A qualitative exploration of the effect of age at migration on the acculturative process of Filipino immigrants: Implications for public health studies. Dissertation in partial fulfillment of the doctor of philosophy degree in public health, University of California Los Angeles. Retrieved from escholarship.org/uc/ item/09t9b4g1.

Mollborn, S. (2017). Teenage mothers today: What we know and how it matters. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 63–69.

Monahan, A. (2017). When a promise is not a promise: Chicago-style pensions. UCLA Law Review, 64. Retrieved from ssrn.com/ abstract=2777736.

Monahan, K. C., Steinberg, L.,  & Cauffman, E. (2013). Age differences in the impact of employment on antisocial behavior. Child Development, 84, 791–801.

Monahan, K., Steinberg, L.,  & Piquero, A. R. (2015). Juvenile justice policy and practice: A developmental perspective. Crime and Justice, 44, 577–619.

Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L. Stephens, B. R., et al. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419–422.

Monk, C., Fifer, W. P., Myers, M. M., Sloan, R. P., Trien, L., & Hurtando, A. (2000). Maternal stress responses and anxiety during pregnancy: Effects on fetal heart rate. Developmental Psychology, 36, 67–77.

Monk, C., Spicer, J., & Champagne, F. A. (2012). Linking prenatal adversity to developmental

outcomes in infants: The role of epigenetic pathways. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 1361–1376.

Montague, D. P., & Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: A new look at infants’ perception of emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 826–838.

Moon, P. J., (2017). Hospice admission assessment: A narrative view. American Journal of Hospice & Palliative Care, 34, 201– 204. doi:10.1177/1049909115624375

Moore, K. L., & Persaud, T. V. N. (1993). Before we are born (4th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders.

Moorman, S. M.,  & Greenfield, E. A. (2010). Personal relationships in later life. In J. C. Cavanaugh  & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 20–52). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Moorman, S. M., & Inoue, M. (2013). Persistent problems in end-of-life planning among young- and middle-aged American couples. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 68, 97–106. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs103

Morfei, M. Z., Hooker, K., Fiese, B. H.,  & Cordeiro, A. M. (2001). Continuity and change in parenting possible selves: A longitudinal follow-up. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23, 217–223. doi:10.1207/153248301750433777

Morgan, B., & Gibson, K. R. (1991). Nutritional and environmental interactions in brain development. In K. R. Gibson & A. C. Peterson (Eds.), Brain maturation and cognitive development: Comparative and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 91–106). New  York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Morgan, J. P.,  & Roberts, J. E. (2010). Helping bereaved children and adolescents: Strategies and implications for counselors. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 32, 206–217.

Morgan, P. L., Staff, J., Hillemeier, M. M., Farkas, G.,  & Maczuga, S. (2013). Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis from kindergarten to eighth grade. Pediatrics, 132, 85–93.

Morris, W. L., Sinclair, S.,  & DePaulo, B. M. (2007). No shelter for singles: The perceived legitimacy of marital status discrimination. Group Processes  & Intergroup Relation, 10, 457–470. doi:10.1177/1368430207081535

Mortimer, J. T., & Staff, J. (2004). Early work as a source of developmental discontinuity during the transition to adulthood. Development  & Psychopathology, 16, 1047–1070.

Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.

Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G.,  & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months . Child Development, 72, 718–735.

Mosten, F. S. (2009). Collaborative divorce handbook: Helping families without going to court. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Mõttus, R., Johnson, W.,  & Deary, I. J. (2012). Personality traits in old age: Measurement

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 631

and rank-order stability and some mean-level change. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 243–249. doi:10.1037/a0023690

Mountain, G., Cahill, J.,  & Thorpe, H. (2017). Sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood: A systematic review and meta- analysis. Infant Behavior and Development, 46, 14–32.

Mounts, N. S. (2011). Parental management of peer relationships and early adolescents’ social skills. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 416–427.

Moye, J., Sabatino, C. P.,  & Brendel, R. W. (2013). Evaluation of the capacity to appoint a health-care proxy. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 326–336. doi:10.1016/ j.jagp.2012.09.001

Moyer, A. M., Miller, V. M.,  & Faubion, S. S. (2016). Could personalized management of menopause based on genomics become a reality? Future Medicine, 17, 659–662. doi:10.2217/pgs.16.17

Mozaffarian, D., Benjamin, E. J., Go, A. S., Arnett, D. K., Blaha, M. J., Cushman, M. et al. (2016). Heart disease and stroke statistics—2016 Update. Circulation, 133, e38–e360. doi:10.1161/ CIR.0000000000000350. Retrieved from circ .ahajournals.org/content/early/2015/12/16/ CIR.0000000000000350.

Mühlig-Versen, A., Bowen, C. E., & Staudinger, U. M. (2012). Personality plasticity in later adulthood: Contextual and personal resources are needed to increase openness to new experiences. Psychology and Aging, 27, 855–866. doi:10 .1037/a0029357

Muise, A., Impett, E. A., Kogan, A., & Desmarais, S. (2013). Keeping the spark alive: Being motivated to meet a partner’s sexual needs sustains sexual desire in long-term romantic relationships. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 267–273. doi:10.1177/1948550612457185

Muller, E. D.,  & Thompson, C. L. (2003). The experience of grief after bereavement: A phenomenological study with implications for mental health counseling. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 25, 183–203. doi .org/10.17744/mehc.25.3.wu4n7dljyekuh4ef

Mulvey, K. L. (2016). Children’s reasoning about social exclusion: Balancing many factors. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 22–27.

Munoz, R. T., & Fox, M. D. (2013). Legal aspects of brain death and organ donorship. In D. Novitzky & D. K. C. Cooper (Eds.), The brain- dead organ donor (pp. 21–35). New  York: Springer.

Murphy, N.,  & Messer, D. (2000). Differential benefits from scaffolding and children working alone. Educational Psychology, 20, 17–31.

Mussolin, C., Nys, J., Leybaert, J., & Content, A. (2016). How approximate and exact number skills are related to each other across development: A review. Developmental Review, 39, 1–15.

Mustanski, B. S., Viken, R . J., Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L.,  & Rose, R. J. (2004). Genetic and environmental influences on pubertal development: Longitudinal data from Finnish

twins at ages 11 and 14. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1188–1198.

Mutchler, J. E., Baker, L. A.,  & Lee, S. A. (2007). Grandparent s re sp onsible for grandchildren in Native-American families. Social Science Quarterly, 88, 990–1009. doi:10.1111/j.1540-6237.2007.00514.x

Muter V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development : Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 663–681.

Mwanyangala, M. A., Mayombana, C., Urassa, H., Charles, J., Mahutanga, C., Abdullah, S., et  al. (2010). Health status and quality of life among older adults in rural Tanzania. Global Health Action, 3. Retrieved from journals. s f u . c a / c o a c t i o n / i n d e x . p h p / g h a / a r t i c l e / viewArticle/2142/6055.

Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s online reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329–336.

Naef, R., Ward, R., Mahrer-Imhof, R., & Grande, G. (2013). Characteristics of the bereavement experience of older persons after spousal loss: An integrative review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 50, 1108–1121. doi:10.1016/j. ijnurstu.2012.11.026

Nagaratnam, N., Nagaratnam, K.,  & Cheuk, G. (2016). Ear-related problems in the elderly. In N. Nagaratnam, K . Nagaratnam,  & G. Cheuk (Eds.), Diseases in the elderly (pp. 357–371). Ne w  York : Spr inger. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-25787-7_17

Nahemow, L. (2000). The ecological theory of aging: Powell Lawton’s legacy. In R. L. Rubinstein  & M. Moss (Eds.), The many dimensions of aging (pp. 22–40). New  York: Springer.

Naigles, L . G.,  & Gelman, S. A . (1995). O verextensions in comprehension and production revisited: Preferential-looking in a study of dog, cat, and cow. Journal of Child Language, 22, 19–46.

Nánez, J., Sr.,  & Yonas, A. (1994). Effects of luminance and texture motion on infants’ defensive reactions to optical collision. Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 165–174.

Narayan, A. J., Englund, M. M.,  & Egeland, B. (2013). Developmental timing and continuity of exposure to interparental violence and externali z ing b ehav ior a s prosp e ctive predictors of dating violence. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 973–990.

Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders ref lect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139–158.

Nation, K., Clark, P., Marshall, C., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and sp e cific lang uage impairment? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 199–211.

National Academy of Social Insurance. (2016). Social Security benefits, finances, and policy options: A primer. Retrieved from www.nasi .org/socialsecurityprimer.

National Alliance for Caregiving and AARP. (2015). C areg iv ing in the U.S.: 2015 report. Retrieved from www.caregiving .org/w p-content/uploads/2015/05/2015_ CaregivingintheUS_Final-Report- June-4_ WEB.pdf.

National Association for the Education of Young Children. (2016). What to look for in a program. Retrieved from families.naeyc.org/ what-to-look-for-in-a-program.

National Cancer Institute. (2006). DES: Questions and answers. Washington DC: Author.

National Center for Assisted Living. (2016). Choosing an assisted living residence: A consumer’s guide. Retrieved from: www .ahcancal.org/ncal/resources/Documents/ Choosing%20An%20Assisted%20Living%20 Residence%202013.pdf.

National Center for Educational Statistics. (2016). Immediate college enrollment rate. Retrieved from nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator _cpa.asp.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2016a). Health, United States, 2015. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/hus/hus15.pdf.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2016b). Changes in life expectancy by race and Hispanic origin in the United States, 2013–2014. Retrieved from www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/ databriefs/db244.htm.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2016c). Mean age of mothers is on the rise: United States 2000–2014. Retrieved from www.cdc .gov/nchs/products/databriefs/db232.htm.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2016d). Unmarried childbearing. Retrieved from www .cdc.gov/nchs/fastats/unmarried-childbearing. htm.

National Center for State Courts. (2016). Elder abuse resource guide. Retrieved from www .ncsc .org/Topics/Children-Families-and- Elders/Elder-Abuse/Resource-Guide.aspx.

National Eye Institute. (2015). Facts about diabetic eye disease. Retrieved from nei.nih. gov/health/diabetic/retinopathy.

National Federation of State High School Associations. (2017). 2015–2016 High school athletics participation survey results. Retrieved from www.nfhs.org/ParticipationStatistics/ ParticipationStatistics.

National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (2010). Women’s health initiative. Retrieved from www.nhlbi.nih.gov/whi/.

N a t i o n a l I n s t i t u t e o f A r t h r i t i s a n d Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases. (2014a). What is osteoarthritis? Retrieved from www .niams.nih.gov/Health_Info/Osteoarthritis/ osteoarthritis_ff.asp.

National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases. (2014b). What is rheumatoid arthritis? Retrieved from www.niams.nih . g o v / H e a l t h _ I n f o / R h e u m a t i c _ D i s e a s e / rheumatoid_arthritis_ff.pdf.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

632 R e f e R e n c e s

National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases. (2015a). Osteoporosis: Peak bone mass in women. Retrieved from w w w. n i a m s . n i h . g o v / H e a l t h _ I n f o / B o n e / Osteoporosis/bone_mass.asp.

N a t i o n a l I n s t i t u t e o f A r t h r i t i s a n d Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases. (2015b). Bone mass measurement: What the numbers mean. Retrieved from www.niams.nih.gov/ health_info/bone/bone_health/bone_mass_ measure.asp.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016a). Major depression among adults. Retrieved from www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/ prevalence/major-depression-among-adults .shtml.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016b). Mental health medications. Retrieved from w w w.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/mental- health-medications/index.shtml#part_149856.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016c). Anxiety disorders. Retrieved from www.nimh .nih.gov/health/topics/anxiety-disorders/ index.shtml.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016d). Obsessive-compulsive disorder. Retrieved from www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/obsessive- compulsive-disorder-ocd/index.shtml.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016e). Any anxiety disorder among adults. Retrieved from www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/ pre valence/any-anxiety-disorder-among- adults.shtml.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2016f ). Post- traumatic stress disorder. Retrieved from www .nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/post-traumatic- stress-disorder-ptsd/index.shtml.

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. (2013). Spina bifida fact sheet. NIH  Publication No. 13-309. Author : Bethesda, MD.

National Institute on Aging. (2012) Longer lives and disability. Retrieved from www.nia.nih.gov/ research/publication/global-health-and-aging/ longer-lives-and-disability.

National Institute on Aging. (2015a). Aging hearts and arteries: A scientific quest. Retrieved from d2cauhfh6h4x0p.cloudfront.net/s3fs-public/ hearts_and_arteries.pdf.

Nat i o n a l I n s t i tu te o n A g i n g . ( 2 0 1 5 b ) . Alzheimer’s disease: Unraveling the mystery. The hallmarks of AD. Retrieved from w w w.nia .nih.gov/alzheimers/publication/ part-2-what-happens-brain-ad/hallmarks-ad.

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). (2002). A call to action: Changing the culture of drinking at U.S. colleges. Final report of the Task Force on College Drinking. NIH Pub. No. 02–5010. Rockville, MD: Author.

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). (2007). What colleges need to know now: An update on college drinking research. Washington, DC: National Institutes of Health.

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). (2014). Treatment of

alcohol problems: Finding and getting help. Retrieved from http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/ publications/Treatment/treatment.htm.

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NI A A A). (2015). C ollege drinking. Retrieved from http://pubs.niaaa .nih.gov/public ations/Colle geFac tShe e t/ CollegeFactSheet.pdf.

National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities. (2016). Overview. Retrieved from www.nimhd.nih.gov/about/overview/.

National Institutes of Health. (2015). Autism spectrum disorder. Bethesda, MD: Author.

National Osteoporosis Foundation. (2016a). Bone health basics: Get the facts. Retrieved from www.nof.org/prevention/general-facts/.

National Osteoporosis Foundation. (2016b). Food and your bones: Osteoporosis nutrition guidelines. Retrieved from www.nof.org/ patients/treatment/nutrition/.

National Osteoporosis Foundation. (2016c). Calcium/Vitamin D. Retrieved from www.nof .org/patients/treatment/calciumvitamin-d/.

National Women’s Law Center. (2016). The wage gap: The who, how, why, and what to do. Retrieved from nwlc.org/resources/the-wage- gap-the-who-how-why-and-what-to-do/.

Nawijn, J., De Bloom, J.,  & Geurts, S. (2013). Pre-vacation time: Blessing or burden? Leisure Sciences, 35, 33–44. doi:10.1080/01490400.20 13.739875

Nayernouri, T. (2011). Euthanasia, terminal illness and quality of life. Archives of Iranian Medicine, 14, 54–55. Retrieved from http://sid .ir/En/VEWSSID/J_pdf/86920110109.pdf.

Neal, M. B.,  & Hammer, L. B. (2007). Working couples caring for children and aging parents: Effects on work and well-being. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Neblett, E. W., Rivas-Drake, D., & Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2012). The promise of racial and ethnic protective factors in promoting ethnic minority youth development. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 295–303.

Neft, N., & Levine, A. D. (1997). Where women stand: An international report on the status of women in over 140 countries, 1997–1998. New York: Random House.

Nehamas, A. (2016). On friendship. New  York: Basic Books.

Neime yer, R . A .,  & Wogrin, C . (2008). Psychotherapy for complicated bereavement: A meaning-oriented approach. Illness, Crisis, and Loss, 16, 1–20. doi:10.2190/IL.16.1.a

Neiss, M., B., Sedikides, C., & Stevenson, J. (2006). Genetic influences on level and stability of self- esteem. Self and Identity, 5, 247–266.

Nell, V. (2002). Why young men drive dangerously: Implications for injury prevention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 75–79.

Nelson, E. L., Campbell, J. M.,  & Michel, G. F. (2013). Unimanual to bimanual: Tracking the development of handedness from 6 to 24 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 181–188.

Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society

for Research in Child Development, 38 (Serial No. 149).

Nelson, K. (2001). Language and the self: From the “Experiencing I” to the “Continuing Me.” In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 15–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nelson, L. J., Badger, S.,  & Wu, B. (2004). The influence of culture in emerging adulthood: Perspectives of Chinese college students. Inter- national Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 26–36. doi:10.1080/01650250344000244

Nelson, M. A. (1996). Protective equipment. In O.  Bar-Or (Ed.), The child and adolescent athlete (pp. 214–223). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Nerenberg, L. (2010). Elder abuse prevention: A  review of the field. In J. C. Cavanaugh  & C.  K.  Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol.3: Societal issues (pp. 53–80). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Nesdale, D. (2001). The development of preju- dice in children. In M. A. Augoustinos  & K. J.  Reynolds (Eds.), Understanding prejudice, racism, and social conflict (pp. 57–73). London: Sage.

Neugarten, B. L.,  & Weinstein, K. K. (1964). The chang ing American g randparent . Journal of Marriage and Family, 26, 299–304. doi:10.2307/349727

Neville, H. J., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Bell, T. A., Fanning, J., Klein, S., et  al. (2013). Family- based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. PNAS, 110, 12138–12143.

New  York Times. (2016). No kegs, no liquor: College crackdown targets drinking and sexual assault. Retrieved from www.nytimes .com/2016/10/30/us/college-crackdown- drinking-sexual-assault.html.

Newberg, A. B., Serruya, M., Wintering, N., Moss, A. S., Reibel, D. & Monti, D. A. (2014). Meditation and neurodegenerative diseases. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307, 112–123. doi:10.1111/nyas .12187. Retrieved from onlinelibrar y.wiley.com/ doi/10.1111/nyas.12187/full

Newbury, J., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., & Moran, C. (2016). Interrelationships between working memory, processing speed, and language development in the age range 2–4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 1146–1158.

Newcombe, N. (2013). Cognitive development: Changing views of cognitive change. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, 479–491.

Newman, A., Thompson, J.,  & Chandler, E. M. (2013). Continuous care: A home hospice benefit. Clinical Journal of Oncology Nursing, 17, 19–20. doi:10.1188/13.CJON.19-20

Newman, B. M., & Newman, P. R. (2015). Theories of human development (2nd ed.). New  York: Psychology Press.

Newman, R. S., Rowe, M. L.,  & Ratner, N. B. (2016). Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: The role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 633

development. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1158–1173.

Newsom, J. T. (1999). Another side to caregiving: Negative reactions to being helped. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 183–187. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00043

Newton, E. K., Thompson, R. A.,  & Goodman, M. (2016). Individual differences in toddlers’ prosociality: Experiences in early relationships explain variability in prosocial behavior. Child Development, 87, 1715–1726.

Newton, N. J.,  & Jones, B. K. (2016). Passing on: Personal attributes associated with midlife expressions of intended legacies. Developmental Psychology, 52, 341–353. doi:10.1037/a0039905

Newton, N. J.,  & Stewart, A . J. (2012). Personality development in adulthood. In S. K. Whitbourne & M. J. Sliwinski (Eds.), The Wiley- Blackwell handbook of adulthood and aging (pp. 209–235). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Ng, R., Allore, H. G., Monin, J. K., & Levy, B. R. (2016). Retirement as meaningful: Positive retirement stereotypes associated with longevity. Journal of Social Issues, 72, 69–85. doi:10.1111/josi.12156

Ni, Y., Chiu, M. M., & Cheng, Z-J. (2010). Chinese children learning mathematics: From home to school. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), Oxford handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 143–154). New York: Oxford University Press.

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Child-care and family predictors of preschool attachment and stability from infanc y. Developmental Psycholog y, 37, 847–862.

NICHD. (2004). The NICHD community connection. Washington DC: Author.

Nickerson, C . J.,  & Thurkettle, M. A . (2013). Cognitive maturity and readiness for e v idence-b a s e d nursing prac tice. Journal of Nursing Education, 52, 17–23. doi:10.3928/01484834-20121121-04

Nicolas, G., Wallon, D., Charbonnier, C., Quenez, O., Rousseau, S., Richard, A.-C., et  al. (2016). Screening of dementia genes by whole- exome sequencing in early-onset Alzheimer disease: Input and lessons. European Journal of Human Genetics, 24, 710–716. doi:10.1038/ ejhg.2015.173

Nielsen, M. K., Neergaard, M. A., Jensen, A. B., Bro, F.,  & Guldin, M.-B. (2016). Do we need to change our understanding of anticipatory grief in caregivers? A systematic review of caregiver studies during end-of-life caregiving and bereavement. Clinical Psychology Review, 44, 75–93. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.01.002

Nigro, N., & Christ-Crain, M. (2012). Testosterone treatment in the aging male: Myth or reality? Swiss Medical Weekly. doi:10.4414/ smw.2012.13539. Retrieved from www.smw .ch/content/smw -2012-13539/.

NIHSeniorHealth.gov. (2016). Depression. R e t r i e v e d f r o m n i h s e n i o r h e a l t h . g o v / depression/aboutdepression/01.html.

Nikolaidis, A., Baniqued, P. L., Kranz, M. B., Scavuzzo, C. J., Barbey, A. K., Kramer, A. F.

et al. (2017). Multivariate associations of fluid intelligence and NAA. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4): 2607-2616. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw070

Nilsen, E. S.,  & Graham, S. A. (2012). The development of preschoolers’ appreciation of communicative ambiguity. Child Development, 83, 1400–1415.

Nimrod, G. (2007a). Retirees’ leisure: Activities, benefits, and their contribution to life satisfaction. Leisure Studies, 26, 65–80. doi:10.1080/02614360500333937

Nimrod, G. (2007b). Expanding, reducing, con- centrating and diffusing: Post retirement leisure behavior and life satisfaction. Leisure Sciences, 29, 91–111. doi:10.1080/01490400600983446

Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Clair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., et  al. (2012). Intelli- gence: New findings and theoretical develop- ments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159.

Norris, D. R. (2016). Job loss, identity, and mental health. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

North American Menopause Society. (2016). The e xperts do ag ree about hormone therapy. Retrieved from www. menopause . o r g / f o r - w o m e n / m e n o p a u s e f l a s h e s / menopause-symptoms-and-treatments/the- experts-do-agree-about-hormone-therapy.

Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2014). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 266–272. doi:10.1037/a0031772

Nosek, M., Kennedy, H. P.,  & Gudmundsdottir, M. (2012). Distress during the menopause transition: A rich contextual analysis of midlife women’s narratives. Sage Open, 2. 10.1177/2158244012455178

NotAlone.gov. (2016). Not alone: Together against sexual assault. Retrieved from www.notalone .gov/.

Nunes, T., Bryant, P.,  & Barros, R. (2012). The development of word recognition and its significance for comprehension and fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 959–973.

Nurmsoo, E.,  & Bloom, P. (2008). Preschoolers’ perspective taking in word learning: Do they blindly follow eye gaze? Psychological Science, 19, 211–215.

Nuzum, D., Meaney, S., O’Donoghue, K.,  & Morris, H. (2015). The spiritual and theological issues raised in stillbirth for healthcare chaplains. Journal of Pastoral Care & Counseling, 69, 163–170. doi:10.1177/1542305015602714

Nyaradi, A., Li, J., Hickling, S., Foster, J., & Oddy, W. H. (2013). The role of nutrition in children’s neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 97.

Nye, C. D., Su, R., Rounds, J.,  & Drasgow, F. (2012). Vocational interests and performance: A quantitative summary of over 60 years of research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 384–403.

O’Donoghue, M. (2005). White mothers negotiating race and ethnicity in the mothering of biracial, Black-White adolescents. Journal

of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work, 14(3), 125–156. doi:10.1300 /J051v14n03_07

O’Hara, M. W. (2009). Postpartum depression: What we know. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 1258–1269.

O’Hara, M. W.,  & McCabe, J. E. (2013). Postpartum depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 379–407.

O’Leary, K. D. (1993). Through a psychological lens: Personality traits, personality disorders, and levels of violence. In R. J. Gelles  & D. R. Loseke (Eds.), Current controversies on family violence (pp. 7–30). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

O’Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–677.

O’Rourke, L., Humphris, G.,  & Baldacchino, A. (2016). Electronic communication based interventions for hazardous young drinkers: A systematic re v ie w. Neuroscience  & Biob ehav ioral Re v ie w s , 68, 880–890. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiorev.2016.07.021

O’Rourke, N.,  & Cappeliez, P. (2005). Marital satisfaction and self-deception: Reconstruction of relationship histories among older adults. Social Behavior and Personality, 33, 273–282. doi:10.2224/sbp.2005.33.3.273

Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 91–121.

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321.

Oburu, P. O.,  & Palmérus, K. (2005). Stress related factors among primary and part- time caregiving grandmothers of Kenyan grandchildren. International Journal of Aging  & Human Development, 60, 273–282. doi:10.2190/XLQ2-UJEMTAQR-4944

Ochs, E.,  & Kremer-Sadlik, T. (2013). Fast- forward family: Home, work, and relationships in middle-class America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Ocklenburg, S., Beste, C.,  & Güntürkün, O. (2013). Handedness: A neurogenetic shift of perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 2788–2793.

OECD. (2006). Starting strong II: Early childhood education and care. Paris: OECD Publishing.

O E C D. ( 2 0 1 6 ) . B al an c in g p ai d w o r k , unp aid work and lei sure . Re tr ie ve d f r o m w w w . o e c d . o r g / g e n d e r / d a t a / balancingpaidworkunpaidworkandleisure.htm.

Oedekoven, C. S. H., Jansen, A., Kircher, T. T., & Leube, D. T. (2013). Age-related changes in parietal lobe activation during an episodic memory retrieval task. Journal of Neural Transmission, 120, 799–806. doi:10.1007/ s00702-012-0904-x

Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Fryar, C. D.,  & Flegal, K. M. (2015). Prevalence of obesity among adults and youth: United States, 2011– 2014. NCHS data brief, no. 219. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

634 R e f e R e n c e s

Ojanen, T.,  & Perry, D. G. (2007). Relational schemas and the developing self: Perceptions of mother and of self as joint predictors of early adolescents’ self-esteem. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1474–1483.

Okonski, B. (1996, May 6). Just say something. Newsweek, 14.

Olinghouse, N. G. (2008). Student- and instruction-level predictors of narrative writing in third-grade students. Reading and Writing, 21, 3–26.

Olinghouse, N. G., Graham, S.,  & Gillespie, A. (2015). The relationship of discourse and topic knowledge to fifth graders’ writing p er for mance. Jour nal of Educ ational Psychology, 107, 391–406.

Oliveira, B. S., Zunzunegui, M. V., Quinlan, J., Fahmi, H., Tu, M. T., & Guerra, R. O. (2016). Systematic review of the association between chronic social stress and telomere length: A life course perspective. Aging Research Reviews, 26, 37–52. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2015.12.006

Olson, D. H., & McCubbin, H. (1983). Families: What makes them work. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Olson, I. R., & Newcombe, N. S. (2014). Binding together the elements of episodes: Relational memory and the developmental trajectory of the hippocampus. In P J. Bauer  & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 285–308). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Olson, K. R. (2016). Prepubescent transgender children: what we do and do not know. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 155–156.

Olson, K. R., Durwood, L., DeMeules, M.,  & McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Mental health of transgender children who are supported in their identities. Pediatrics, 137(3):e20153223.

Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating contributions of self- regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and Psychopatholog y, 23, 253–266.

Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L.,  & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control. Development  & Psychopathology, 17, 25–45.

Olszewski-Kubilius, P., Subotnik , R . F.,  & Worrell, F. C. (2017). The role of domains in the conceptualization of talent. Roeper Review, 39, 59–69.

Oltjenbruns, K. A., & Balk, D. E. (2007). Life span issues and loss, grief, and mourning: Part 1. The importance of a developmental context: Childhood and adolescence as an example; Part 2. Adulthood. In D. Balk, C. Wogrin, G. Thornton, & D. Meagher (Eds.), Handbook of thanatology: The essential body of knowledge for the study of death, dying, and bereavement (pp. 143–163). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.

Omori, M., & Smith, D. T. (2009). The impact of occupational status on household chore hours among dual earner couples. Sociation Today, 7. Retrieved from www.ncsociology.org/ sociationtoday/v71/chore.htm.

Oostenbroek, J., Slaughter, V., Nielson, M.,  & Suddendorf, T. (2013). Why the confusion around neonatal imitation? A review. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 31, 328–341.

Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Constantini, S., Davis, J., et al. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26, 1334–1338.

Operskalski, J. T., Paul, E. J., Colom, R., Barbey, A. K., & Grafman, J. (2015). Lesion mapping the four- factor structure of emotional intelligence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 649. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00649

Opfer, J. E., & Gelman, S. A. (2011). Development of the animate-inanimate distinction. In U.  Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 213–238). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Opfer, J. E.,  & Siegler, R. S. (2004). Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A micro- genetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 301–332.

Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2012). Development of quantitative thinking. In K. Holyoak and R. Morrison (Eds.), Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 585–605). New  York: Oxford University Press.

Orel, N. A., & Coon, D. W. (2016). The challenges of change: How can we meet the care needs of the ever-evolving LGBT family? Generations, 40, 41–45.

Orel, N. A., & Fruhauf, C. A. (2013). Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender grandparents. In A. E. Goldberg & K. R. Allen (Eds.), LGBT-parent families (pp. 177–192). New York: Springer.

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2016). 2015 PISA results in focus. Paris: OECD Publishing.

Oriña, M. M., Collins, W. A., Simpson, J. A., Salvatore, J. E., Haydon, K. C.,  & Kim, J. S. (2011). Developmental and dyadic perspectives on commitment in adult romantic relationships. Psychological Science, 22, 908–915.

Ornstein, P. A., Coffman, J., Grammer, J., San Souci, P.,  & McCall, L. (2010). Linking the classroom context and the development of children’s memory skills. In J. L. Meece  & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research on schools, schooling, and human development (pp. 42–59). New York: Routledge.

Ortiz-Walters, R .,  & Gilson, L. L. (2013). Mentoring programs for under-represented groups. In J. Passmore, D. B. Peterson,  & T. Freire (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of coaching and mentoring (pp. 266–282). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Ostrov, J. M., & Godleski, S. A. (2010). Toward an integrated gender-linked model of aggression subtypes in early and middle childhood. Psychological Review, 117, 233–242.

Oswald, A. J.,  & Wu, S. (2010). Objective confirmation of subjective measures of human well-being: Evidence from the USA. Science, 327, 576–579. doi:10.1126/science.1180606

Oswald, F. L., & Hough, L. (2012). I-O 2.0 from Intelligence 1.5: Staying (just) behind the cutting edge of intelligence theories. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 5, 172–175.

Ottaway, A. J. (2010). The impact of parental divorce on the intimate relationships of adult offspring: A review of the literature. Graduate Journal of Counseling Psychology, 2(1), Article 5. Retrieved from epublications. marquette.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article= 1037&context=gjcp.

Oudekerk, B. A., Allen, J. P., Hessel, E. T., & Molloy, L. E. (2015). The cascading development of autonomy and relatedness from adolescence to adulthood. Child Development, 86, 472–485. doi:10.1111/cdev.12313

Over, H.,  & Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen- month-old infants show increased helping following priming with affiliation. Psychological Science, 20, 1189–1193.

Owen, C. J. (2005). The empty nest transition: The relationship between attachment style and women’s use of this period as a time for growth and change. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 65(7-B), 3747.

Ownby, R. L., Hertzog, C., & Czaja, S. J. (2012). Relations between cognitive status and medication adherence in patients treated for memor y disorders. Ageing Research. doi:10.4081/ar.2012.e2. Retrieved from www .pagepress.org/journals/index.php/ar/article/ viewArticle/2729.

Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identif ying pathways b etwe en s o c i o e c o n o m i c s t at u s a n d l a n g u a g e development. Annual Reviews of Linguistics, 3, 285–308.

Paech, J., Luszczynska, A.,  & Lippke, S. (2016). A rolling stone gathers no moss—The long way from good intentions to physical activity mediated by planning, social support, and self- regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1024. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01024

Paggi, M. E., Jopp, D.,  & Hertzog, C. (2016). The importance of leisure activities in the relationship between physical health and well- being in a life span sample. Gerontology, 62, 450–458. doi:10.1159/000444415

Palkovitz, R.,  & Palm, G. (2009). Transitions w ithin fathering . Father ing, 7, 3–22. doi:10.3149/ fth.0701.3

Panay, N. (2016). Body identical hormone replace- ment: The way forward? In A. R. Genazzani & B. C. Tarlatzis (Eds.), Frontiers in gynecologi- cal endocrinology (pp. 203–208). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-23865-4_24

Papa , A .,  & Litz , B. (2011). Grief. In W. T. O’Donohue & C. Draper (Eds.), Stepped care and e-health: Practical applications to behavioral disorders (pp. 223–245). New York: Springer.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 635

Parault, S. J.,  & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2000). The development of conceptual categories of attention during the elementary school years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 245–262.

Pargament, K. I. (2013). Searching for the sacred: Toward a nonreductionistic theory of spirituality. In K. I., Pargament, J. J. Exline, & J. W. Jones (Eds.), APA handbook of psychol- ogy, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1): Context, theory, and research (pp. 257–273). Washing- ton, DC: American Psychological Association.

Park, C. L. (2007). Religiousness/spirituality and health: A meaning systems perspective. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30, 319–328. doi:10.1007 /s10865-007-9111-x

Park, C. L. (2013). The Meaning Making Model: A framework for understanding meaning, spiri- tuality, and stress-related growth in health psy- chology. The European Health Psychologist, 15, 40–47. Retrieved from openhealthpsychology. net/ehp/issues/2013/v15iss2_June2013/EHP_ June_2013.pdf#page=13.

Park, C. L., Edmondson, D.,  & Hale-Smith, A. (2013). Why religion? Meaning as motivation. In K. I., Pargament, J. J. Exline, & J. W. Jones (Eds.), APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1): Context, theory, and research (pp. 151–171). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Park, D. C.,  & Festini, S. B. (2017). Theories of memory and aging: A look at the past and a glimpse of the future. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 72, 82–90. doi:10.1093/ geronb/gbw066

Park, G., Lubinski, D.,  & Benbow, C. P. (2008). Ability differences among people who have commensurate degrees matter for scientific creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 957–961.

Park, J., Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., Curhan, K., Markus, H. R., Kawakami, N., et  al. (2013). Clarifying the links between social support and health: Culture, stress, and neuroticism matter. Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 226–235. doi:10.1177/1359105312439731

Park, Y. C.,  & Pyszczynski, T. (2016). Cultural universals and differences in dealing with death. In L. A. Harvell & G. S. Nisbett (Eds.), Denying death: An interdisciplinary approach to terror management theory (pp. 193–214). New York: Routledge.

Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (1998). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 463–552). New  York: Wiley.

Parke, R. D., & O’Neil, R. (2000). The influence of significant others on learning about relationships: From family to friends. In R . S. L . Mills  & S. Duck (Eds .), The de velopment al psycholog y of per sonal relationships (pp. 15–47). New York: Wiley.

Parker, J. G.,  & Seal, J. (1996). Forming, losing, renewing, and replacing friendships: Applying temporal parameters to the assessment of children’s friendship experiences. Child Development, 67, 2248–2268.

Parker, L. D. (2008). A study about older African American spousal caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 68(10-B), 6589.

Parkes, C. M. (2013). Elisabeth Kübler-Ross, On death and dying: A reappraisal. Mortality, 18, 94–97. doi:10.1 080/13576275.2012.758629

Parkinson’s Disease Foundation. (2016a). What is Parkinson’s disease? Retrieved from www.pdf .org/about_pd.

Parkinson’s Disease Foundation. (2016b). Prescription medications. Retrieved from www .pdf.org/parkinson_prescription_meds.

Parkman, A. M. (2007). Smart marriage: Using your (business) head as well as your heart to find wedded bliss. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Parrella, N.,  & Vormittag, K. (2017). Health promotion and wellness. In A. A., Paulman & L. S. Nasir (Eds.), Family medicine (pp. 99–111). New York: Springer.

Parritz, R. H. (1996). A descriptive analysis of toddler coping in challenging circumstances. Infant Behavior & Development, 19, 171–180.

Parten, M. (1932). Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 243–269.

Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 1321–1323.

Pascalis, O., Loevenbruck, H., Quinn, P., Kandel, S., Tanaka, J.,  & Lee, K. (2014). On the linkage between face processing, language processing, and narrowing during development. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 65–70.

Pascual, B., Aguardo, G., Sotillo, M., & Masdeu, J. C. (2008). Acquisition of mental state language in Spanish children: A longitudinal study of the relationship between the production of mental verbs and linguistic development. Developmental Science, 11, 454–466.

Passarino, G., De Rango, F.,  & Montesanto, A. (2016). Human longevity: Genetics or lifestyle? It takes two to tango. Immunity & Ageing, 13, 12. doi:10.1186/s12979-016-0066-z. Retrieved from immunityageing.biomedcentral.com/ articles/10.1186/s12979-016-0066-z.

Pasupathi, M. (2013). Making meaning for the good life: A commentary on the special issue. Memory, 21, 143–149. doi:10.1080/09658211.20 12.744843

Patrick, S., Sells, J. N., Giordano, F. G., & Tollerud, T. R. (2007). Intimac y, dif ferentiation, and personality variables as predictors of marital satisfaction. The Family Journal, 15, 359–367. doi:10.1177/1066480707303754

Patry, D. A., Blanchard, C. M., & Mask, L. (2007). Measuring university students’ regulatory leisure coping styles: Planned breathers or avoidance? Leisure Sciences, 29, 247–265. doi:10.1080/01490400701257963

Patterson, C. J. (2013). Family lives of lesbian and gay adults. In G. W. Peterson  & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 659–681). New York: Springer.

Patterson, G. R . (1980). Mothers : The unacknowledged victims. Monographs of the

Society for Research in Child Development, 45(5, Serial No. 186).

Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77, 847–860.

Patton, L. D., Renn, K. A., Guido, F. M.,  & Quaye, S. J. (2016). Student development in col- lege (3rd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Pauker, K., Ambady, N., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2010). Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81, 1799–1813.

Paulson, D., Bassett, R., Kitsmiller, E., Luther, K., & Conner, N. (2016). When employment and caregiving collide: Predictors of labor force participation in prospective and current caregivers. Clinical Gerontologist. doi:10.1080/ 07317115.2016.1198856

Paus, T. (2010). Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: Myelin or axon? Brain  & Cognition, 72, 26–35.

Pearlin, L. I., Mullan, J. T., Semple, S. J., & Skaff, M. M. (1990). Caregiving and the stress process: An overview of concepts and their measures. The Gerontologist, 30, 583–594. doi:10.1093/ geront/30.5.583

Pears, K. C., Kim, H. K., Fisher, P. A., & Yoerger, K. (2013). Early school engagement and late elementary outcomes for maltreated children in foster care. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2201–2211.

Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., Waxmonsky, J. G., Greiner, A. R., Gnagy, E. M., Pelham, W. E. et  al. (2016). Treatment sequencing for childhood ADHD: A multiple-randomization study of adaptive medication and behavioral interventions. Journal of Clinical Child  & Adolescent Psychology, 45, 396–415.

Pellegrino, J. W.,  & Kail, R. (1982). Process analyses of spatial aptitude. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Pellicano, E. (2013). Testing the predictive power of cognitive atypicalities in autistic children: Evidence from a 3-year follow-up study. Autism Research, 6, 258–267.

Penson, R . T. (2004). Bereavement across cultures. In R. J. Moore  & D. Spiegel (Eds.), Cancer, culture, and communication (pp. 241– 279). New York: Kluwer/Plenum.

Perrone, K. M., Tschopp, K. M., Snyder, E. R., Boo, J. N., & Hyatt, C. (2010). A longitudinal examination of career expectations and outcomes of academically talented students 10 and 20 years post-high school graduation. Journal of Career Development, 36, 291–309. doi:10.1177/0894845309359347

Perry, W. I. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Peters, A. M. (1995). Strategies in the acquisition of syntax. In P. Fletcher  & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 462–483). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

636 R e f e R e n c e s

Peterson, L. (1983). Role of donor competence, donor age, and peer presence on helping in an emergency. Developmental Psychology, 19, 873–880.

PetersonLund, R . R . (2013). Living on the edge: A re vie w of the literature. Nursing Science Quarterly, 26, 303–310. doi:10.1177/0894318413500311

Petrill, S. A . (2016). Behavioural genetic studies of reading and mathematics skills. In Y. Kovas  & S. Malykh (Eds.), Behavioural genetics for education (pp. 60–76). Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children’s adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908–923.

Pew Research Center. (2013a). Living to 120 and beyond: Americans’ views on aging, medical advances and radical life extension. Retrieved from www.pewforum.org/2013/08/06/living- to-120-and-beyond-americans-views-on-aging- medical-advances-and-radical-life-extension/.

Pew Research Center. (2013b). Religious leaders’ views on radical life extension. Retrieved from www.pewforum.org/2013/08/06/religious- leaders-views-on-radical-life-extension/.

Pew Research Center. (2015). Median age in years, by sex, race and ethnicity: 2013. Retrieved from www.pewhispanic.org/2016/04/19/statistical- portrait-of-hispanics-in-the-united-states/ ph_2015-03_statistical-portrait-of-hispanics- in-the-united-states-2013_current-09/.

Pfaff, D. W.,  & Fisher, H. (2012). Generalized brain arousal mechanisms and other biological, environmental and psychological mechanisms that contribute to libido. In A. Fotopoulou, D. W. Pfaff, & M. A. Conway (Eds.), From the couch to the lab: Trends in neuropsychoanalysis (pp. 65–84). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Pfoff, M. K., Zarotney, J. R.,  & Monk, T. H. (2014). Can a function-based therapy for spousally bereaved seniors accrue benefits in both functional and emotional domains? Death Studies, 38, 381–386. doi:10.1080/ 07481187.2013.766658

Phinney, J. S. (2005). Ethnic identity development in minority adolescents. In C. B. Fisher  & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 1, pp. 420–423). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Phipps, M. G., Rosengard, C., Weitzen, S., Meers, A., & Billinkoff, Z. (2008). Age group differences among pregnant adolescents: Sexual behavior, health habits, and contraceptive use. Journal of Pediatric & Adolescent Gynecology, 21, 9–15.

Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. New York: Harcourt Brace.

Piaget, J. (1951). Plays, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

Piaget, J.,  & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. Boston: Routledge  & Kegan Paul.

Pickering, C. E. Z.,  & Phillips, L. R. (2014). Development of a causal model for elder mistreatment. Public Health Nursing, 31, 363–372. doi:10.1111 /phn.12108

Piehler, T. F., & Dishion, T. J. (2007). Interpersonal dynamics within adolescent friendships: Dyadic mutuality, deviant talk, and patterns of antisocial behavior. Child Development, 78, 1611–1624.

Pienta, A. M., Hayward, M. D., & Jenkins, K. R. (2000). Health consequences of marriage for the retirement years. Journal of Family Issues, 21, 559–586. doi:10.1177/019251300021005003

Pillay, H., Kelly, K.,  & Tones, M. (2006). Career aspirations of older workers: An Australian study. International Journal of Training and Development, 10, 298–305. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2419.2006.00263.x

Pineda-Pardo, J. A., Martinez, K., Román, F. J., & Colom, R . (2016). Structural efficiencies within a parieto-frontal network and cognitive differences . Intelligence , 54, 105–116. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.002

Pinto, K. M.,  & Coltrane, S. (2009). Division of labor in Mexican origin and Anglo families: Structure and culture. Sex Roles, 60, 482–495. doi:10.1007/s11199-008-9549-5

Pioneer Network. (2017). Mission, vision, values. Retrieved from pioneernetwork.net/AboutUs/ Values/.

Piquero, A. R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2014). Moffitt’s de velopmental ta xonomy of antisocial behavior. In G. Bruinsma & D. Weisburd (Eds.), Encyclopedia of criminology and criminal justice (pp. 3121–3127). New  York: Spring Science+Business Media.

Piquet, B. J. (2007). That’s what friends are for. Dissertation Abstracts International. Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 67(7-B), 4114.

Place, S.,  & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 1834–1849.

Plomin, R. (1990). Nature and nurture. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Plomin, R. (2013). Child development and molecular genetics: 14 years later. Child Development, 84, 104–120.

Plomin, R.,  & Deary, I. J. (2015). Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 98–108.

Plomin, R.,  & Spinath, F. (2004). Intelligence: Genes, genetics, and genomics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 112–129.

Plunkett , K . (1996). Connectionism and development: Neural networks and the study of change. New York: Oxford University Press.

Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J., Bolt, D. M., Hane, A., Burnson, C.,  & Winters, J. (2011). Infant physiological regulation and maternal risks as predictors of dyadic interaction

trajectories in families with a preterm infant. Developmental Psychology, 47, 91–105.

Poldrack, R. A. (2012). The future of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 62, 1216–1220. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011 .08.007

Polivka, L. (2010). Neoliberalism and the new politics of aging and retirement security. In J. C. Cavanaugh  & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 161–202). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Polivka, L.,  & Luo, B. (2015). The neoliberal political economy and erosion of retirement security. The Gerontologist, 55, 183–190. doi:10.1083/geront/gnv006

Pomerantz, E. M., Ng, F, F., Chung, C. S., & Qu, Y. (2014). Raising happy children who succeed in school: Lessons from China and the United States. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 71–76.

Poole, D. A.,  & Bruck, M. (2012). Divining testimony? The impact of inter viewing props on children’s reports of touching. Developmental Review, 32, 165–180.

Poole, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (1995). Interviewing preschoolers : Effects of nonsuggestive techniques, parental coaching, and leading questions on reports of nonexperienced events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 129–154.

Pope, T. M. (2017). Certified patient decision aids: Solving persistent problems with informed consent law. Journal of Law, Medicine, & Ethics, 45, 12–40. doi:10.1177/1073110517703097

Popenoe, D. (2009). Cohabitation, marriage, and child well-being: A cross-national perspective. Society: Social Science and Public Policy, 46, 429–436. doi:10.1007/s12115-009-9242-5

Porter, K. E., Brennan-Ing, M., Chang, S. C., dickey, l. m., Singh, A. A., Bower, K. L. et  al. (2016). Providing competent and affirming ser- vices for transgender and gender nonconform- ing older adults. Clinical Gerontologist, 39, 366–388. doi:10.1080/07317115.2016.1203383

Porter, R. H.,  & Winburg, J. (1999). Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 439–449.

Portnoy, A ., Rana, P., Zimmerman, C.,  & Rodin, G. (2015). The use of palliative sedation to treat existential suffering: A reconsideration. In P. Taboada (Ed.), Sedation at the end-of -life: An interdisciplinar y approach (pp. 41–54). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9106-9_4

Potočnik, K., Tordera, N., & Peiró, J. M. (2013). Truly satisfied with your retirement or just resigned? Pathways toward different patterns of retirement satisfaction. Journal of Applied Gerontolog y, 32, 164–187. doi:10.1177/0733464811405988

Poulson, C. L., Kymissis, E., Reeve, K. F., Andreatos, M., & Reeve, L. (1991). Generalized vocal imitation in infants . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 267–279.

Power, T. L., & Smith, S. M. (2008). Predictors of fear of death and self-mortality: An Atlantic

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 637

Canadian perspective. Death Studies, 32, 252–272. doi:10.1080/07481180701880935

Pratt, H. D. (2010). Perspectives from a non- traditional mentor. In C. A. Rayburn, F. L. Denmark, M. E. Reuder, & A.M. Austria, (Eds.), A handbook for women mentors: Transcending barriers of stereotype, race, and ethnicity (pp. 223–232). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

President’s Council on Fitness, Sports & Nutrition. (2012). Physical activity guidelines for Americans midcourse report: Strategies to increase physical activity among youth. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports. (2004). Physical activity for children: Current patterns and guidelines. Research Digest (Series 5, No. 2).

Pressley, M. (2002). Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced teaching (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Pressley, M.,  & Hilden, K. (2006). Cognitive strategies. In D. Kuhn  & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 511–556). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Preston, C., Goldring, E., Guthrie, J. E., Ramsey, R., & Huff, J. (2017). Conceptualizing essential components of effective high schools . Leadership and Policy in Schools, 16, in press.

Priaulx, N. (2013). The troubled identity of the bioethicist. Health Care Analysis, 21, 6–19. doi:10.1007/ s10728-012-0229-9

Price, T. S., Grosser, T., Plomin, R., & Jaffee, S. R. (2010). Fetal genotype for the xenobiotic metabolizing enzyme NQO1 inf luences intrauterine growth among infants whose mothers smoked during pregnancy. Child Development, 81, 101–114.

Price, V. (2016). Women in non-traditional work fields. In N. A. Naples, r. c. hoogland, M. Wickramasing,  & W. C. A. Wong (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell encyclopedia of gender and sexuality studies. Malden, MA: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118663219.wbegss546

Prince-Paul, M.,  & Daly, B. J. (2016). Ethical considerations in palliative care. In N. Coyle (ed.), Legal and ethical aspects of care (pp. 1–28). New York: Oxford University Press.

Prot, S., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., Suzuki, K., Swing, E., Lim, K. M., et al. (2014). Long- term relations among prosocial-media use, empathy, and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 25, 358–368.

Proulx, C. M. (2016). Marital trajectories. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (pp. 842–845). New York: Wiley.

Provins, K. A. (1997). Handedness and speech: A critical reappraisal of the role of genetic and environmental factors in the cerebral lateralization of function. Psychological Review, 104, 554–571.

Pruett, M. K., Insabella, G. M., & Gustafson, K. (2005). The collaborative divorce project: A court-based intervention for separating parents with young children. Family Court Review, 43, 38–51. doi:10.1111/j.1744-1617.2005.00006.x

Pryor, L., Brendgen, M., Boivin, M., Dubois, L., Japel, C. Fallissard, B. et al. (2016). Overweight during childhood and internalizing symptoms in early adolescence: The mediating role of peer victimization and the desire to be thinner. Journal of Affective Disorders, 202, 203–209.

Puhl, R. M.,  & Brownell, K. D. (2005). Bulimia nervosa. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 1, pp. 192–195). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Pynoos, J. Caraviello, R., & Cicero, C. (2010). Hous- ing in an aging America. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 129–159). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Queen, T. L., Berg, C. A., & Lowrance, W. (2015). A framework for decision making in couples across adulthood. In T. M. Hess, J. Strough, & C. E. Löckenhoff (Eds.), Aging and decision making: Empirical and applied perspectives (pp. 371–392). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Quinn, J. F.,  & Cahill, K. E. (2016). The new world of retirement income security in America. American Psychologist, 71, 321–333. doi:10.1037/a0040276

Quinn, P. C., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Tanaka, J. W. (2016). Narrowing in categorical responding to other-race face classes by infants . Developmental Science, 19, 362–371.

Quirin, M., Loktyushin, A., Arndt, J., Küstermann, E., Lo, Y.-Y., Kuhl, J. et  al. (2012). Existential neuroscience: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of neural responses to reminders of one’s mortality. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 193–198. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq106

Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 1715–1737.

Racz, S. J.,  & McMahon, R. J. (2011). The relationship between parental knowledge and monitoring and child and adolescent conduct problems: A 10-year update. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 377–398.

Raikes, H., Luze, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., et al. (2006). Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77, 924–953.

Rakic, P. (1995). Corticogenesis in humans and nonhuman primates. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Ralph, L. J.,  & Brindis, C. D. (2010). Access to reproductive healthcare for adolescents: Establishing healthy behaviors at a critical juncture in the lifecourse. Current Opinion in Obstetrics & Gynecology, 22, 369–374.

Ramalingam, M., Durgadevi, P., & Mahmood, T. (2016). In vitro fertilization. Obstetrics, Gynae- cology, and Reproductive Medicine, 26, 200–209.

Randall, A . K.,  & Bodenmann, G. (2017). Stress and its associations with relationship

satisfaction. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 96–106. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.010

Rangul, V., Bauman, A., Holmen, T. L., & Midthjell, K. (2012). Is physical activity maintenance from adolescence to young adulthood associated with reduced CVD risk factors, improved men- tal health and satisfaction with life: The HUNT Study, Norway. International Journal of Behav- ioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 103.

Rape, Abuse, and Incest National Network. (2016). Statistics. Retrieved from www.rainn .org/statistics.

Raschick, M.,  & Ingersoll-Dayton, B. (2004). The costs and re wards of caregiving among aging spouses and adult children. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 53, 317–325. doi:10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.0008.x

Rawlins, W. K. (2004). Friendships in later life. In J. F. Nussbaum & J. Coupland (Eds.), Handbook of communication and aging research (2nd ed., pp. 273–299). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Reardon, S. (2016). First CRISPR clinical trial gets green light from US panel. Nature News, doi:10.1038/nature.2016.20137.

Redzanowski, U., & Glück, J. (2013). Who knows who is wise? Self and peer ratings of wisdom. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 68, 391–394. doi:10.1093/geronb/ gbs079

Reed, A. E.,  & Carstensen, L. L. (2012). The theor y behind the age-related positivity effect. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/ fpsyg.2012.00339

Reese, E., & Cox, A. (1999). Quality of adult book reading affects children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 35, 20–28.

Rehan, W., Antfolk, J., Johansson, A., & Santtila, P. (2017). Do single experiences of childhood abuse increase psychopathology symptoms in adult- hood? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, in press.

Reich, P. A. (1986). Language development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Reimer, M. S. (1996). “Sinking into the ground”: The development and consequences of shame in adolescence. Developmental Review, 16, 321–363.

Remedios, J. D., Chasteen, A. L., & Packer, D. J. (2010). Sunny side up: The reliance on positive age stereotypes in descriptions of future older selves. Self and Identity, 9, 257–275. doi:10.1080/15298860903054175

Ren, Q., & Treiman, D. J. (2014). Living arrangements of the elderly in China and consequences for their emotional well-being. Population Studies Center Research Report 14-814, University of Michigan. Retrieved from www.psc.isr .umich.edu/pubs/pdf/rr14-814.pdf.

Renshaw, K. D., Rodrigues, C.,  & Jones, D. H. (2008). Psychological symptoms and marital satisfaction in spouses of operation Iraqi freedom veterans: Relationships with spouses’ perceptions of veterans’ experiences and symptoms. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 586–594. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.586

Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants’ use of attentional cues to identif y the referent of another

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

638 R e f e R e n c e s

person’s emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.

Repacholi, B. M.,  & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Emotional eavesdropping: Infants selectively respond to indirect emotional signals. Child Development, 78, 503–521.

Repacholi, B. M., Meltzoff, A. N.,  & Olsen, B. (2008). Infants’ understanding of the link between visual perception and emotion: “If she can’t see me doing it, she won’t get angry.” Developmental Psychology, 44, 561–574.

Repetti, R.,  & Wang, S-w. (2017). Effects of job stress on family relationships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 15–18. doi:10.1017/j. copsyc.2016.03.010

Revenson, T. A., Griva, K., Luszczynska, A., Morrison, V., Panagopoulou, E., Vilchinsky, N. et al. (2016). Caregiving in the illness context. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Rhea, S-A., Bricker, J. B., Wadsworth, S. J.,  & Corley, R. P. (2013). The Colorado Adoption Project. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 358–365.

Rhoades, K. A. (2008). Children’s responses to interparental conflict: A meta-analysis of their associations with child adjustment. Child Development, 79, 1942–1956.

Rhodes, M. (2013). How two intuitive theories shape the development of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 12–16.

Rideout, V. (2016). Measuring time spent with media: The Common Sense census of media use by U.S. 8- to 18-year-olds. Journal of Children and Media, 10, 138–144.

Ridings, C., & Gefen, D. (2004). Virtual community attraction: Why people hang out online. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2004.tb00229.x.

Rijken, A. J. (2009). Happy families, high fertility? Childbearing choices in the context of family and partner relationships. Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the doctor of philosophy degree, University of Utrecht.

Rijken, A. J., & Knijn, T. (2009). Couples’ decisions to have a first child: Comparing pathways to early and late parenthood. Demographic Re s e ar ch , 2 1 , 7 6 5 – 8 0 2 . d o i : 1 0 . 4 0 5 4 / DemRes.2009.21.26. Retrieved from core. ac.uk/download/pdf/6405965.pdf.

Riley, M. W. (1979). Introduction. In M. W. Riley (Ed.), Aging from birth to death: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 3–14). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Rindermann, H., & Thompson, J. (2013). Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps? Intelligence, 41, 821–831.

Ripp, J., Jones, E., & Zhang, M. (2016). Common functional problems. In J. L. Hayashi  & B. Leff (Eds.), Geriatric home-based medical care (pp. 151–172). New  York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-23365-9_8

Ritchie, K. L. (1999). Maternal behaviors and cognitions during discipline episodes: A com- parison of power bouts and single acts of non- compliance. Developmental Psychology, 35, 580–589.

R i t t l e - J o h n s o n , B . ( 2 0 1 7 ) . D e v e l o p i n g mathematics knowledge. Child Development Perspectives, 11, in press.

Rittle-Johnson, B.,  & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and procedural knowledge of mathematics. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 1118–1134). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rivas-Drake, D., Seaton, E. K., Markstrom, C., Quintana, S., Syed, M., Lee, R. M., et al. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity in adolescence: Implications for psychosocial, academic, and health outcomes. Child Development, 85, 40–57.

Rizzuto, T. E., Cherry, K. E.,  & LeDoux, J. A. (2012). The aging process and cognitive capabilities. In J. W. Hedge  & W. C. Borman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of work and aging (pp. 236–255). New  York: Oxford University Press.

Robbins, A.,  & Wilner, A. (2001). Quarterlife crisis: The unique challenges of life in your twenties. New York: Putnam.

Roberto, K. A. (2016). The complexities of elder abuse. American Psychologist, 71, 302–311. doi:10.1037/a0040259

Roberto, K. A.,  & Skoglund, R. R. (1996). Interactions with grandparents and great- grandparents: A comparison of activities, influences, and relationships. International Journal of Aging  & Human Development, 43, 107–117. doi:10.2190/8F1D-9A4D-h) QY-W9DD

Roberto, K. A., McPherson, M. C., & Brossoie, N. (2013). Intimate partner violence in late life: A review of the empirical literature. Violence Against Women, 19, journals .sagepub. com/doi/full/10.1177/1077801213517564. doi:10.1177/1077801213517564

Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., Bogg, T., & Caspi, A. (2006). De-investment in work and non- normative personality trait change in young adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 20, 461–474. doi:10.1002/per.607

Roberts, K. P., Evans, A. D.,  & Duncanson, S. (2016). Binding an event to its source at encoding improves children’s source monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 52, 2191–2201.

Robertson, C. E., Ratai, E.,  & Kanwisher, N. (2016). Reduced GABAergic action in the autistic brain. Current Biology, 26, 1–6.

Robinaugh, D. J.,  & McNally, R. J. (2013). Remembering the past and envisioning the future in bereaved adults with and w i t h o u t c o mp l i c at e d g r i e f . C l i n i c a l P s y c h o l o g i c a l S c i e n c e , 1 , 2 9 0 – 3 0 0 . doi:10.1177/2167702613476027

Robins, S., Treiman, R.,  & Rosales, N. (2014). Letter knowledge in parent-child conversations. Reading and Writing, 27, 407–429.

Robinson-Wood, T.,  & Weber, A . (2016). Deconstructing multiple oppressions among LGBT older adults. In D. A. Harley  & P. B. Teaster (Eds.), Handbook of LGBT elders (pp. 65–81). New York: Springer.

Robinson, O. C.,  & Smith, J. A . (2010). Investigating the form and dynamics of crisis episodes in early adulthood: The application of a composite qualitative method. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 7, 170–191. doi:10.1080/14780880802699084

Robinson, O. C., & Wright, G. R. T. (2013). The prevalence, types and perceived outcomes of crisis episodes in early adulthood and midlife: A structured retrospective autobiographical study. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 37, 407–416. doi:10.1177/0165025413492464

Robinson, O. C., Wright, G. R. T., & Smith, J. A. (2013). The holistic phase model of early adult crisis. Journal of Adult Development, 20, 27–37. doi:10.1007/s10804-013-9153-y

Robinson, O.C. (2015). Emerging adulthood, early adulthood and quarter-life crisis: Updat- ing Erikson for the twenty-first century. In. R. Žukauskiene (Ed.) Emerging adulthood in a European context (pp. 17–30). New  York: Routledge.

Robson, S. M., Hansson, R. O., Abalos, A.,  & Booth, M. (2006). Successful aging: Criteria for aging well in the workplace. Journal of C areer D e velopment , 33, 156–177. doi:10.1177/0894845306292533

Roby, A. C.,  & Kidd, E. (2008). The referential communication skills of children with imaginar y companions . D e velopmental Science, 11, 531–540.

Rockwood, K. (2016). Conceptual models of frailty: Accumulation of deficits. Canadian Journal of C ardiolog y, 32, 1046–1050. doi:10.1016/j.cjca.2016.03.020

Rocque, G. B.,  & Cleary, J. F. (2013). Palliative care reduces morbidity and mortality in cancer. Nature Reviews Clinical Oncology, 10, 80–89. doi:10.1038/nrclinonc.2012.211

Rodeck, C. H.,  & Whittle, M. J. (Eds.). (2009). Fetal medicine: Basic science and clinical practice. London: Churchill Livingstone.

Rodriguez-Galán, M. B. (2014). The ethnography of ethnic minority families and aging: Familism and beyond. In K. E. Whitfield & T. A. Baker (Eds.), Handbook of minority aging (pp. 435– 453). New York: Springer.

Roffwarg, H. P., Muzio, J. N.,  & Dement, W. C. (1966). Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science, 152, 604–619.

Rogers, L. A.,  & Graham, S. (2008). A meta- analysis of single subject design writing intervention research. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 879–906.

Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.

Rohmann, E., Führer, A.,  & Bierhoff, H.-W. (2016). Relationship satisfaction across European cultures: The role of love styles. Cross-Cultural Research, 50, 178–211. doi:10.1177/1069397116630950

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 639

Rohr, R. (2011). Falling upward: A spirituality for the two halves of life. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Rohr, R. (2013). Immortal diamond: The search for our true self. San Francisco, CA: Jossey- Bass.

Rollins, N. C., Bhandari, N., Hajeebhoy, N., Horton, S., Lutter, C. K., Martines, J., et  al. (2016). Breastfeeding 2: Why invest, and what will it take to improve breastfeeding practices? Lancet, 387, 491–504.

Roosevelt, F. D. (1935). Statement on signing the Social Security Act. Retrieved from www.ssa .gov/history/fdrstmts.html#signing.

Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (1999). Children’s goals and strategies in response to conflicts within a friendship. Developmental Psychology, 35, 69–79.

Rose, A. J.,  & Asher, S. R. (2017). The social tasks of friendships: Do boys and girls excel in different tasks? Child Development Perspectives, 11, 3–8.

Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.

Rose, D. M., & Gordon, R. (2010). Retention prac- tices for engineering and technical profession- als in an Australian public agency. Australian Journal of Public Administration, 69, 314–325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8500.2010.00693.x

Rosenbaum, J. L., Smith, J. R., & Zollfrank, B. C. C. (2011). Neonatal end-of-life support care. Perinatal and Neonatal Nursing, 25, 61–69. doi:10.1097 /JPN.0b013e318209e1d2

Rosenblatt, P. C. (1996). Grief that does not end. In D. Klass, P. R. Silverman, & S. L. Nickman (Eds.), Continuing bonds: New understandings of grief (pp. 45–58). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

Rosengren, K. S., Gutiérrez, I. T., & Schein, S. S. (2014a). Cognitive dimensions of death in context. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 62–82. doi:10.1111/ mono.12079

Rosengren, K. S., Miller, P. J., Gutiérrez, I. T., Chow, P. I., Schein, S. S.,  & Anderson, K. N. (2014b). Children’s understanding of death: Toward a contextualized and integrated account. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 1–162.

Ross, J., Yilmaz, M., Dale, R., Cassidy, R., Yildirim, I., & Zeedyk, M. S. (2017). Cultural differences in self-recognition: the early development of autonomous and related selves? Developmental Science, 20(3). doi:10.1111/desc.12387

Ross, N. A., Garner, R., Bernier, J., Feeny, D. H., Kaplan, M. S., McFarland, B., et  al. (2012). Trajectories of health-related quality of life by socioeconomic status in a nationally representative Canadian cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 66, 593–598. doi:10.1136/jech.2010.115378

Rosso, B. D., Dekas, K. H.,  & Wrzesniewski, A. (2010). On the meaning of work : A theoretical integration and review. Research

in Organizational Behavior, 30, 91–127. doi:10.1016/j.riob.2010.09.001

Rostila, M., Saarela, J., Kawachi, I.,  & Hjern, A. (2015). Testing the anniversary reaction: Causal effects of bereavement in a nationwide follow-up study from Sweden. Psychiatric Epidemiology, 30, 239–247. doi:10.1007/ s10654-015-9989-5

Roszko, E. (2010). Commemoration and the state: Memory and legitimacy in Vietnam. Sojourn: Journal of Social Issues in Southeast Asia, 25, 1–28. doi:10.1353/soj.0.0041

Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: Temperament and personality in development. New York: Guilford.

Rothbart, M. K.,  & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh,  & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (pp. 167–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Rovee-Collier, C. (1987). Learning and memory in infancy. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 98–148). New York: Wiley.

Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Review, 104, 467–498.

Rovee-Collier, C. (1999). The development of infant memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 80–85.

Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child- directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83, 1762–1774.

Rowe, M. L., Radebe’s, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Development, 83, 508–525.

Rowe, M. M., & Sherlock, H. (2005). Stress and ver- bal abuse in nursing: Do burned out nurses eat their young? Journal of Nursing Management, 13, 242–248. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2834.2004.00533.x

Rowles, G. D. (2006). Commentary: A house is not a home: But can it become one? In H. W. Wahl et  al. (Eds.), The many faces of health, competence and well-being in old age (pp. 25–32). New York: Springer.

Rowles, G. D.,  & Watkins, J. F. (2003). History, habit, heart, and hearth: On making spaces into places. In K. W. Schaie, H.-W. Wahl, H. Mollenkopf,  & F. Oswald (Eds.), Aging independently: Living arrangements and mobility (pp. 77–96). New York: Springer.

Roxburgh, S. (2002). Racing through life. The distribution of time pressures by roles and roles resources among full-time workers. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 23, 121–145. doi:10.1023/A:1015734516575

Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Bowker, J. C. (2015). Children in peer groups. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol 4., pp. 175–222). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Chen, X., Bowker, J. C., McDonald, K. L.,  & Heverly-Fitt, S. (2015).

Peer relationships. In M. H. Bornstein  & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp. 587–644). New York: Psychology Press.

Rubin, S. S.,  & Malkinson, R. (2001). Parental response to child loss across the life cycle: Clin- ical and research perspectives. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Con- sequences, coping, and care (pp. 169–197). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 858–932). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Rudd, M. D., Berman, A. L., Joiner, T. E., Nock, M. K., Silverman, M. M., Mandrusiak, M., et al. (2006). Warning signs for suicide: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 36, 255–262.

Rueger, S. Y., Chen, P., Jenkins, L. N., & Choe, H. J. (2014). Effects of perceived support from moth- ers, father, and teachers on depressive symp- toms during the transition to middle school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43, 655–670.

Ruiz, M. E., Phillips, L. R., Kim, H.,  & Woods, D. L. (2016). Older Latinos: Applying the ethnocultural gerontological nursing model. Journal of Transcultural Nursing, 27, 8–17. doi:10.1177/1043695615569539

Ruppanner, L.,  & Maume, D. J. (2016). Shorter work hours and work-to-family interference: Surprising findings from 32 countries. Social Forces, 95, 693–720. doi:10.1093/sf/sow057

Rushton, J. P., & Bons, T. A. (2005). Mate choice and friendship in twins. Psychological Science, 16, 555–559.

Russell, A., & Finnie, V. (1990). Preschool children’s social status and maternal instructions to assist group entry. Developmental Psychology, 26, 603–611.

Rutland, A.,  & Killen, M. (2017). Fair resource allocation among children and adolescents: The role of group and developmental processes. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 56–62.

Ryan, E. G. (2013). How to write about rape prevention without sounding like an asshole. Jezebel blog. Retrieved from http://jezebel .com/how-to-write-about-rape-prevention- without-sounding-lik-1446529386.

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., & Kulich, C. (2010). Politics and the glass cliff: Evidence that women are preferentially selected to contest hard-to- win seats. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34, 56–64. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01541.x

Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Morgenroth, T., Rink, F., Stoker, J., & Peters, K. (2016). Getting on top of the glass cliff: Reviewing a decade of evidence, explanations , and impact. The Leadership Quarterly, 27, 446–455. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.10.008

Rye, M. S., Folck, C. D., Heim, T. A., Olszewski, B. T., & Traina, E. (2004). Forgiveness of an ex-spouse:

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

640 R e f e R e n c e s

How does it relate to mental health following a divorce? Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 41, 31–51. doi:10.1300/ J087v41n03_02

Saarento, S.,  & Salmivalli, C. (2015). The role of classroom peer ecology and bystanders’ responses in bullying. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 201–205.

Saewyc, E. M. (2011). Research on adolescent sexual orientation: Development, health disparities, stigma, and resilience. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 256–272.

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N.,  & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y., & Joels, T. (2002). Shedding further light on the effects of various types and quality of early child care on infant–mother attachment relationship: The Haifa study of early child care. Child Development, 73, 1166–1186.

Sahni, R., Fifer, W. P.,  & Myers, M. M. (2007). Identifying infants at risk for sudden infant death syndrome. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 19, 145–149.

Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., et al. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross- road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS ONE, 8(10): e78103.

Sakaki, M., Nga, L.,  & Mather, M. (2013). Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal context at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults’ memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1206–1224. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00392

Sakraida, T. J. (2005). Divorce transition differences of midlife women. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 26, 225–249. doi:10.1080/01612840590901699

Sakyi, K. S., Surkan, P. J., Fombonne, E., Chollet, A., & Melchior, M. (2015). Childhood friendships and psychological difficulties in young adulthood: An 18-year follow-up study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, 815–826.

Sala Frigerio, C.,  & De Strooper, B. (2016). Alzheimer’s disease mechanisms and emerging roads to novel therapeutics. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 39, 57–79. doi:10.1146/ annurev-neuro-070815-014015

Salomaa, R. (2014). Coaching of key talents in multinational companies. In A. Al Ariss (Ed.), Global talent management (pp. 43–63). New York: Springer.

Salovey, P.,  & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, cognition, and p er sonalit y, 9, 185–211. doi:10.2190/ DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG

Salthouse, T. A. (2014a). Relations between running memor y and fluid intelligence. In t e l l ig e n c e , 4 3 , 1 – 7 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / j . intell.2013.12.002

Salthouse, T. A. (2014b). Correlates of cognitive change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.1037/a0034847

Samanta, T. (2017). Bridging the gap: Theory and research in social gerontology. In T. Samanta

(Ed.), Cross-cultural and cross-disciplinary perspectives in social gerontology (pp. 3–22). New York: Springer.

Samuelson, L. K., & McMurray, B. (2017). What does it take to learn a word? WIREs Cognitive Science, 8, e1421.

Sánchez-Álv arez , N., E xtremera , N.,  & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). The relation between emotional intelligence and subjective well-being: A meta-analytic investigation. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11, 276–285.

Sanders, M. (2014). A public health approach to improving parenting and promoting children’s well-being. Unpublished manuscript.

Sandler, I. N., Wolchik, S. A. Ayers, T. S., Tein, J.-Y., & Luecken, L. (2013). Family bereavement program (FBP) approach to promoting resilience following the death of a parent. Family Science, 4, 87–94. doi:10.1080/194246 20.2013.821763

Sandler, I. N., Wolchik, S. A.,  & Ayers, T. S. (2007). Resilience rather than recovery: A contextual framework on adaptation following bereavement. Death Studies, 32, 59–73. doi:10.1080/07481180701741343

Sandler, I., Ingram, A., Wolchik, S., Tein, J.-Y., & Winslow, E. (2015). Long-term effects of parenting-focused preventive interventions to promote resilience of children and adolescents. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 164–171.

Sanford, L. (2014). Why lighting is important for the aging eye. Retrieved from www.lighthouse .org/eye-health/the-basics-of-the-eye/the- aging-eye /lighting/.

Sangrigoli, S., Pallier, C., Argenti, A .-M., Ventureyra, V. A. G., & de Schonen, S. (2005). Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood. Psychological Science, 16, 440–444.

Sann, C.,  & Streri, A. (2007). Perception of object shape and texture in human newborns: Evidence from cross-modal transfer tasks. Developmental Science, 10, 399–410.

Sanson, A., Prior, M., Smart, D., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Gender differences in aggression in childhood: Implications for a peaceful world. Australian Psychologist, 28, 86–92.

Sargent, C. S., & Borthick, A. F. (2013). Evidence for insisting on cognitive conflict tasks: Impact on accounting majors in upper level courses. Issues in Accounting Education, 28 759–777. doi:10.2308/iace-50518

Sargent, L. D., Lee, M. D., Martin, B.,  & Zikic, J. (2013). Reinventing retirement: New pathways, new arrangements, new meanings. Human Relations, 66, 3–21. doi:10.1177/0018726712465658

Saroglou, V., Pichon, I., Trompette, L ., Verschueren, M.,  & Dernelle, R . (2005). Prosocial behavior and religion: New evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44, 323–348.

Saudino, K. J. (2009). Do different measures tap the same genetic influences? A multi- method study of activity level in young twins. Developmental Science, 12, 626–633.

Saudino, K. J., & Micalizzi, L. (2015). Emerging trends in behavioral genetic studies of child temperament. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 144–148.

Savela, S., Saijonmaa, O., Strandberg, T. E., Koistinen, P., Strandberg, A. Y., Tilvis, R. S., et  al. (2013). Physical activity in midlife and telomere length measured in old age. E xper iment al Gerontolog y, 48, 81–84. doi:10.1016 /j/exger.2012.02.003

Savickas, M. L. (2013). Career construction theor y and practice. In S. D. Brown  & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Career development and counseling: Putting theory and research to work (pp. 147–183). New York: Wiley.

Saxe, G. B. (1988). The mathematics of child street vendors. Child Development, 59, 1415–1425.

Sbarra, D. A . (2015). Divorce and health: Current trends and future directions . Psychosomatic Medicine , 77, 227–236. doi:10.1097/0000000000000168

Scantlebury, N., Cunningham, T., Dockstader, C., & Laughlin, S. (2014). Relations between white matter maturation and reaction time in childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 99–112.

Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.

Scarr, S.,  & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.

Schaal, B., Marlier, L .,  & Soussignan, R . (1998). Olfactory function in the human fetus: Evidence from selective neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic fluid. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 1438–1449.

Schaie, K. W.,  & Zanjani, F. (2006). Intellectual development across adulthood. In C. Hoare (Ed.), Oxford handbook of adult development and learning (pp. 99–122). New York: Oxford University Press.

Scharlach, A. E. & Lehning, A. J. (2016). Creating aging-friendly communities. New York: Oxford University Press.

Scheele, D., Wille, A., Kendrick, K. M., Stoffel- Wagner, B., Becker, B., Güntürkün, O., et  al. (2013). Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men viewing the face of their female partner. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. doi:10.1073/ pnas.1314190110. Article available at www .pnas.org/content/110/50/20308.full.

Scheff, S. W., Neltner, J. H.,  & Nelson, P. T. (2014). Is synaptic loss a unique hallmark o f A l z h e i m e r ’s d i s e a s e ? Bi o ch e mi c al Pharmacology, 88, 517–528. doi:10.1016/ j.bcp.2013.12.028

Scheibe, S., Kunzmann, U.,  & Baltes, P. B. (2007). Wisdom, life longings, and optimal development. In J. A. Blackburn  & C. N. Dulmus (Eds.), Handbook of gerontology: Evidence-based approaches to theory, practice, and policy (pp. 117–142). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

S cheidt , R . J.,  & S chw ar z , B. (2010). Environmental gerontology: A sampler of

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 641

issues and application. In J. C. Cavanaugh  & C.  K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol.  1: Psychological aspects (pp. 156–176). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Perspectives.

Schermerhorn, A. C.,  & Cummings, E. M. (2008). Transactional family dynamics: A new framework for conceptualizing family influence processes. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 36, pp. 187–250). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic Press.

Schermerhorn, A. C., Chow, S., & Cummings, E. M. (2010). Developmental family processes and interparental conflict: Patterns of microlevel influences. Developmental Psychology, 46, 869–885.

Schlabach, S. (2013). The importance of family, race, and gender for multiracial adolescent well-being. Family Relations, 62, 154–174. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2012.—758.x

Schlomer, G. L.,  & Belsky, J. (2012). Maternal age, investment, and parent-child conflict: A mediational test of the terminal investment hypothesis. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 443–452. doi:10.1037/a0027859

Schmidt, F. L.,  & Hunter, J. E. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work : Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162–173.

Schmidt, F. L.,  & Hunter, J. E. (2014). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Allensworth, M., Allik, J., Ault, L., Austers, I., et  al. (2004). Patterns and universals of adult romantic attachment across 62 cultural regions: Are models of self and of other pancultural constructs? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 367–402. doi:10.1177/0022022104266105

Schneider, J. (2013). The death of an adult child: Contemporary psychoanalytic models of mourning. In S. Arbiser & G. Saragnano (Eds.), On Freud’s inhibitions, symptoms, and anxiety (pp. 219–230). London, UK: Karnac Books.

Schneider, W. (2015). Memory development from early childhood through emerging adulthood. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International.

Schneiders, J., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J., Feron, F. J., van Os., J., & deVries, M. W. (2006). Mood reactivity to daily negative events in early adolescence: Relationship to risk for psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 42, 543–554.

S choklit sch, A .,  & B auman, U. (2012). Generativity and aging: A promising future research topic? Journal of Aging Studies, 26, 262–272. doi:10.1016/jaging.2012.01.002

Schoon, I. (2001). Teenage job aspirations and career attainment in adulthood: A 17-year follow-up study of teenagers who aspired to become scientists, health professionals, or engineers . International Journal of Behavioral Development , 25, 124–132. doi:10.1080/01650250042000186

Schoulte, J. (2011). Bereavement among African Americans and Latino/a Americans. Journal

of Mental Health Counseling, 33, 11–20. doi:10.17744/mehc.33.1.r4971657p7176307

Schuetze, P., Molnar, D. S., & Eiden, R. D. (2012). Profiles of reactivity in cocaine-exposed children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33, 282–293.

Schultz, D. P.,  & Schultz, S. E. (2017). Theories of personality (11th ed.). Boston: Cengage Learning.

Schulz-Helk, R. J., Poole, J. H., Dahdah, M. N., Sullivan, C., Date, E. S., Salerno, R. M. et  al. (2016). Long-term outcomes after moderate- to-severe traumatic brain injur y among military veterans: Successes and challenges. Brain Injury, 30, 271–279. doi:10.3109/02699 052.2015.1113567

Schulz, D. J.,  & Enslin, C. (2014). The female executive’s perspective on career planning and advancement in organizations. Sage Open. doi:10.1172/2158244014558040

Schulz, R.,  & Morycz, R. (2013). Psychosocial actors, health, and quality of life. In M. D. Miller & L. K. Salai (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry (pp. 343–371). New  York: Oxford University Press.

Schwab, A . K.,  & Greitemeyer, T. (2015). The world’s biggest salad bowl: Facebook connecting cultures. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45, 243–252. doi:10.1111/ jasp.12291

Schwartz-Mette, R. A.,  & Rose, A. J. (2012). Co-rumination me diates contagion of internalizing symptoms within youths’ friendships. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1355–1365.

Schwartz, A. E., Leos-Urbel, J.,  & Wiswall, M. (2015). Making summer matter: The impact of youth employment on academic performance. National Bureau of Economic Research, Working Paper No. 21470.

Schwartz, B. (2015) Why we work. New  York: TED Books.

Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grow up”: Adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 1952–1953.

Schwartz, C., Issanchou, S., & Nicklaus, S. (2009). Developmental changes in the acceptance of the five basic tastes in the first year of life. British Journal of Nutrition, 102, 1375–1385.

Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & The Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2000). Friendship as a moderating fac- tor in the pathway between early harsh home environment and later victimization in the peer group. Developmental Psychology, 36, 646–662.

Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: How to predict and modif y the adoption and maintenance of he alth behaviors. Applied Psychology, 57, 1–29. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x

Schwerdtfeger, A.,  & Friedrich-Mai, P. (2009). Social interaction moderates the relationship between depressive mood and heart rate vari- ability: Evidence from an ambulatory moni- toring study. Health Psychology, 28, 501–509. doi:10.1037/a0014664

Scott, W. A., Scott, R.,  & McCabe, M. (1991). Family relationships and children’s personality: A cross-cultural, cross-source comparison. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 1–20.

Scrimgeour, M. B., Blandon, A. Y., Stifter, C. A., & Buss, K. A. (2013). Cooperative coparenting moderates the association between parenting practices and children’s prosocial behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 27, 506–511.

Seaman, P. M. (2012). Time for my life now: Early boomer women’s anticipation of volunteering in retirement. The Gerontologist, 52, 245–254. doi:10.1093/geront/gns001

Segal, D. L., Qualls, S. H., & Smyer, M. A. (2011). Aging and mental health (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

Seidl, A., & Johnson, E. L. (2006). Infants’ word segmentation revisited: Edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science, 9, 565–573.

Selkoe, D. J. (2016). The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease at 25 years. EMBO Molecular Medicine, 8, 595–608. doi:10.15252/ emmm.201606210

Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding : Development and clinical analyses. New York: Academic Press.

Selman, R. L. (1981). The child as a friendship philosopher: A case study in the growth of interpersonal understanding. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman (Eds.), The development of children’s friendships. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Sénéchal, M. (2012). Child language and literacy development at home. In B. H. Wasik & B. Van Horn (Eds.), Handbook on family literacy (pp. 38–50). New York: Routledge.

Seward, R. R.,  & Stanley-Stevens, L. (2014). Fathers, fathering, and fatherhood across cultures. In H. Selin (Ed.), Parenting across cultures (pp. 459–474). New York: Springer.

Seychell, A. (2016). Keeping your happy marriage: Tips for dealing with marriage issues. Retrieved f rom p o sitive p s ycholo g y pro g ram.com/ marriage-fulfillment-lifelong-relationship/.

Seyda, B. A., & Fitzsimons, A. M. (2010). Infant deaths. In C. A. Corr  & D. A. Balk (Eds.), Children’s encounters with death, bereavement, and coping (pp. 83–107). New York: Springer.

Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V.,  & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind devel- opment. Developmental Psycholog y, 47, 1239–1247.

Shanafelt, T. D., Mungo, M., Schmitgen, J., Storz, K. A., Reeves, D., Hayes, S. N., et  al. (2016). Longitudinal study evaluating the association between physician burnout and changes in professional work effort. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 91, 422–431. doi:10.1016/j. mayocp.2016.02.001

Shanahan, L., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C.,  & Osgood, D. (2007). Warmth with mothers and fathers from middle childhood to late adolescence: Within- and between-families comparisons. Developmental Psychology, 43, 551–563.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

642 R e f e R e n c e s

Shanahan, M. J., Elder, G. H., Burchinal, M.,  & Conger, R. D. (1996). Adolescent earnings and relationships with parents: The work- family nexus in urban and rural ecologies. In J. T. Mortimer  & M. D. Finch (Eds.), Adoles- cents, work, and family: An intergenerational developmental analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Shao, J., Li, D., Zhang, D., Zhang, L., Zhang, Q., & Qi, X. (2013). Birth cohort changes in the depressive symptoms of Chinese older adults: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 1101–1108. doi:10.1002/gps.3942

Share, D. L. (2008). Orthographic learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 36, pp. 31–84). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.

Shariff, A. F.,  & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 395–399.

Sharma, A.,  & Sharma, V. (2012). The psycho- cultural analysis of sex discrimination. Advances in Asian Social Science, 2, 411–414. Retrieved f r o m w w w. w o r l d s c i e n ce p u b l i s h e r. o rg / journals/index.php/AASS/article/viewFile/ 380/362.

Sharma, M. (2014). Yoga as an alternative and complementar y approach for stress management: A systematic review. Journal o f E v i d e n c e - B a s e d C o m p l e m e n t a r y and Alternative Medicine , 19, 59–67. doi:10.1177/2156587213503344

Shaw, D. S.,  & Shelleby, E. C. (2014). Early- starting conduct problems: Intersection of conduct problems and poverty. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 503–528.

Shaw, S. S. (2007). Losing a parent twice. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and O ther D ementi a s , 21 , 389–390. doi:10.1177/1533317506292860

Shayshon, B.,  & Popper-Giveon, A. (in press). “These are not the realities I imagined”: An inquiry into the lost hopes and aspirations of beginning teachers. Cambridge Journal of Edu- cation. doi:10.1080/0305764X.2016.1214238

Shen, H. (2016). Why women earn less: Just two factors explain post-PhD pay gap. Nature, May  20. doi:10.1038/nature.2016.19950 Retrieved from www.nature.com/news/why- women-earn-less-just-two-factors-explain- post-phd-pay-gap-1.19950.

Sherman, D. K., Hartson, K. A., Binning, K. R., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Taborsky-Barba, S., et  al. (2013). Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: How self-affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 591–618.

Sherwin, S. (2011). Looking backwards, looking forward: Hope for Bioethics’ next twenty-five years. Bioethics, 25, 75–82. doi:10.1111/j.1467 -8519.2010.01866.x

Sheu, H., Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., Miller, M. J., Hennessy, K. D., & Duffy, R. D. (2010). Testing the choice model of social cognitive career

theory across Holland themes: A meta-analytic path analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 252–264. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2009.10.015

Shi, R. (2014). Functional morphemes and early language acquisition. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 6–11.

Shi, R.,  & Werker, J. F. (2001). Six-month old infants’ preference for lexical words. Psychological Science, 12, 70–75.

Shim, H. S. (2014). Vascular cognitive impairment and post-stroke cognitive deficits. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 14. doi:10.1007 /s11910-013-0418-4

Shin, H., & Ryan, A. M. (2014). Early adolescent friendships and academic adjustment : Examining selection and influence processes with longitudinal social network analysis. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2462–2472.

Shipp, A. J., Furst-Holloway, S., Harris, T. B., & Rosen, B. (2014). Gone today but here tomorrow: Extending the unfolding model of turnover to consider boomerang employ- ees. Personnel Psychology, 67, 421–462. doi:10.1111/peps.12039

Shirley, M. M. (1931). The first two years: A study of twenty-five babies: Vol 1. Postural and locomotor development. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

Shoji, K., Cieslak, R., Smoktunowicz, E., Rogala, A., Benight, C. C.,  & Luszczynska, A. (2016). Associations between job burnout and self- efficacy: A meta-analysis. Anxiety, Stress,  & Coping, 29, 367–386. doi:10.1080/10615806. 2015.1058369

Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., the Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care, and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, et al. (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129, e232–e246. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2668

Shore, J. C., Gelber, M. W., Koch, L. M., & Sower, E. (2016). Anticipatory grief : An evidence- based approach. Journal of Hospice  & Pal- liative Nursing, 18, 15–19. doi:10.1097/ NJH.0000000000000208

Shulman, S.,  & Kipnis, O. (2001). Adolescent romantic relationships: A look from the future. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 337–351.

Shutts, K. (2015). Young children’s preferences: Gender, race, and social status . Child Development Perspectives, 9, 262–266.

Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Social categories guide young children’s prefer- ences for novel objects. Developmental Science, 13, 599–610.

Sidebotham, P., Heron, J., & the ALSPAC Study Team. (2003). Child maltreatment in the “children of the nineties”: The role of the child. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27, 337–352.

Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46 (Serial No. 189).

Siegler, R. S. (1986). Unities in strategy choices across domains. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),

Minnesota symposia on child development (Vol. 19, pp. 1–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2005). Children’s thinking (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Siegler, R. S.,  & Robinson, M. (1982). The development of numerical understanding. In H. W. Reese  & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 16). New York: Academic Press.

Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: How do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Silk, J. S., Morris, A. S., Kanaya, T., & Steinberg, L. D. (2003). Psychological control and autonomy granting: Opposite ends of a continuum or distinct constructs? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 113–128.

Silva, L. C. R., de Araújo, A. L., Fernandes, J. R., Matias, M. de S. T., Silva, P. R., Duarte, A. J. S. et  al. (2016). Moderate and intense exercise lifestyles attenuate the effects of aging on telomere length and the survival and composition of T cell subpopulations. AGE, 38, 24. doi:10.1007/s11357-016-9879-0

Silver, M. P. (2016). An inquiry into self- identification with retirement. Journal of Women  & Aging, 28, 477-488. doi:10.1080/ 08952841.2015.1018068

Silverman, W. K., La Greca, A. M., & Wasserstein, S. (1995). What do children worry about? Worries and their relations to anxiety. Child Development, 66, 671–686.

Silvers, J. A., Buhle, J. T., & Ochsner, K. N. (2014). The neuroscience of emotion regulation: Basic mechanisms and their role in development, aging and psychopathology. In K. N. Ochsner & S. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive neuroscience (pp. 53–78). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Simons, B. S., Foltz, P. A ., Chalupa,  R .  L., Hylden, C. M., Dowd, T. C., & Johnson, A. E. (2016). Burnout in U.S. military orthopaedic residents and staff physicians . Militar y Me dicine , 181, 835–838. doi:10.7205/ MILMED-D-15-00325

Simons, L. G., Wickrama, K. A. S., Lee, T. K., Landers-Potts, M., Cutrona, C., & Conger, R. D. (2016). Testing family stress and family investment explanations for conduct problems among African American adolescents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 78, 498–515.

Simonton, D. K. (in press). Defining creativity: Don’t we also need to define what is not creative? Journal of Creative Behavior. doi:10.1002/jocb.137

Simonton, D. K .,  & Song , A . V. (2009). Eminence, IQ, physical and mental health, and achievement domain: Cox’s 282 geniuses revisited. Psychological Science, 20, 429–434.

Simpson, J. M. (2001). Infant stress and sleep deprivation as an aetiological basis for the sudden infant death syndrome. Early Human Development, 61, 1–43.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 643

Sinclair, R. R., Sears, L. E., Zajack, M., & Probst, T. (2010). A multilevel model of economic stress and employee well-being. In J. Houdmont & S. Leka (Eds.), Contemporary occupational health psychology: Global perspectives on research and practice (Vol. 1, pp. 1–20). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

Singh, J. A., Saag, K G., Bridges, S. L., Jr., Akl, E. A., Bannuru, R. R., Sullivan, M. C., et  al. (2016). 2015 American College of Rheumatology Guideline for the treatment of rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis & Rheumatology, 68, 1–26. doi:10.1002/art.39480

Sinnott, J. D. (2014). Adult development : Cognitive aspects of thriving close relationships. New York: Oxford University Press.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Skinner, E. A. (1985). Determinants of mother- s e n s i t i v e a n d c o n t i n g e n t- r e s p o n s i v e behavior: The role of childbearing beliefs and socioeconomic status. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children (pp. 51–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Skoog, T., & Stattin, H. (2014). Why and under what contextual conditions do early-maturing girls develop problem behaviors? Child Development Perspectives, 8, 158–162.

Skouteris, H., McNaught, S., & Dissanayake, C. (2007). Mothers’ transition back to work and infants’ transition to child care: Does work-based child care make a differ- ence? Child Care in Practice, 13, 33–47. doi:10.1080/13575270601103432

Slater, A., Bremner, G., Johnson, S. P., Sherwood, P., Hayes, R.,  & Brown, E. (2000). Newborn infants’ preference for attractive faces: The role of internal and external facial features. Infancy, 1, 265–274.

Slaughter, V. (2015). Theory of mind in infants and young children: A review. Australian Psychologist, 50, 169–172.

Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C., & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86, 1159–1174.

Sleddens, E. F. C., Gerards, M. P. L., Thijs, C., de Vries, N. K., & Kremers, S. P. J. (2011). General parenting, childhood overweight and obesity- inducing behaviors: A review. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 6, e12–e27.

Sleeter, C. E. (2016). Ethnicity and curriculum. In D. Wyse, L. Hayward, & J. Pandya (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of curriculum, pedagogy, and assessment (pp. 231–246). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Slobin, D. I. (1985). Cross-linguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 2. Theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Smith-Bynum, M. A. (2013). African American families: Research progress and potential in the age of Obama. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 683–704). New York: Springer.

Smith-Howell, E. R., Hickman, S. E., Meghani, S. H., Perkins, S. M.,  & Rawl, S. M. (2016). End- of-life decision making and communication of bereaved family members of African Americans with serious illness. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 19, 174–182. doi:10.1089/ jpm.2015.0314

Smith, E. R., Mackie, D. M.,  & Claypool, H. M. (2015). Social psychology (4th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

Smith, L. B. (2000). How to learn words: An associative crane. In R. Golinkoff & K. Hirsch- Pasek (Eds.), Breaking the word learning barrier (pp. 51–80). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Smith, L. B. (2009). From fragments to geometric shape: Changes in visual object recognition between 18 and 24 months. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 290–294.

Smith, W. J., Howard, J. T., & Harrington, K. V. (2005). Essential formal mentor characteristics and functions in governmental and non- governmental organizations from the program administrator’s and the mentor’s perspective. Public Personnel Management, 34, 31–58.

Smits, S. J., & Bowden, D. E. (2013). Leveraging psychological assets for the development and maintenance of leadership capabilities. International Leadership Journal, 5, 3–26. Available at www.tesu.edu/documents/ILJ_ Winter_2013.pdf.

Smoll, F. L., & Schutz, R. W. (1990). Quantifying gender differences in physical performance: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 26, 360–369.

Smyke, A . T., Zeanah, C. H. Fox, N. A ., Nelson, C . A .,  & Guthrie, D. (2010). Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment among young institutionalized children. Child Development, 81, 212–223. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01390.x

Snow, C. W. (1998). Infant development (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Snow, D. (2006). Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development, 77, 281–296.

Snow, M. E., Jacklin, C. N.,  & Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Sex-of-child differences in father– child interaction at one year of age. Child Development, 54, 227–232.

Snowling, M. J.,  & Hulme, C. (2015). Disorders of reading , mathematical, and motor development. In A. Thapar, D. S. Pine, J. F. Leckman, S. Scott, M. J. Snowling, & E. Taylor (Eds.). Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (6th ed., pp. 702–718). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Social Security and Medicare Boards of Trustees. (2016). A summary of the 2016 annual reports. Retrieved from www.ssa.gov/oact/trsum/.

Solomon, G. E. A.,  & Zaitchik, D. (2012). Folkbiology. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3, 105–115.

Sommer, S., Plumm, K. M., Terrance, C. A.,  & Tubré, T. (2013). Perceptions of younger single adults as a function of their gender and number of children. Journal of General Psychology, 140, 87–109. doi:10.1080 /00221309.2013.769931

Song, S., Georgiou, G. K., Su, M., & Hua, S. (2016). How well do phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming correlate with Chinese reading accuracy and fluency? A meta-analysis. Reading and Writing, 20, 99–123.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & Taylor, E. (2015). ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder. In A. Thapar, D. S. Pine, J. F. Leckman, S. Scott, M. J. Snowling, & E. Taylor (Eds.), Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry (6th ed., pp. 738–756). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Sorg, S. F., Delano-Wood, L., Luc, N., Schiehser, D., Hanson, K. L., Nation, D. A., et  al. (2014). White matter integrity in veterans with mild traumatic brain injury: Associations with executive function and loss of consciousness. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 29, 21–32. doi:10.1097 /HTR.0b013e31828a1aa4

Spector, F.,  & Maurer, D. (2009). Synesthesia: A new approach to understanding the development of perception. Developmental Psychology, 45, 175–189.

Spector, P. E., Allen, T. D., Poelmans, S., Cooper, C. L., Bernin, P., Hart, P., et al. (2005). An international comparative study of work- family stress and occupational strain. In S. A. Y. Poelmans (Ed.), Work and family: An international research perspective (pp. 71–84). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Spelke, E. S.,  & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89–96.

Spivak, I. M., Mikhelson, V. M., & Spivak, D. L. (2016). Telomere length, telomerase activity, stress, and aging. Advances in Gerontology, 6, 29–35. doi:10.1134/S2079057016010136

Sritharan, R., Heilpern, K., Wilbur, C. J.,  & Gawronski, B. (2010). I think I like you: Spontaneous and deliberate evaluations of potential romantic partners in an online dating context. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1062–1077. doi:10.1002/ejsp.703

Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D.,  & Potter, J. (2003). Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social Psycholog y, 84, 1041–1053. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041

St. George, I. M., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1994). Body size and menarche: The Dunedin study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 15, 573–576.

St. James-Roberts, I. (2007). Helping parents to manage infant crying and sleeping: A review of the evidence and its implications for services. Child Abuse Review, 16, 47–69.

St. James-Roberts, I.,  & Plewis, I. (1996). Individual differences, daily fluctuations, and developmental changes in amounts of infant waking, fussing, crying, feeding, and sleeping. Child Development, 67, 2527–2540.

Staff, J., Mont’Alvao, A .,  & Mortimer, J. (2015). Children at work. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol 4., pp. 345–375). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Stafford, L., Kline, S. L.,  & Rankin, C. T. (2004). Married individuals, cohabiters, and cohabiters who marry: A longitudinal study of

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

644 R e f e R e n c e s

relational and individual well-being. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 21, 231–248. doi:10.1177/0265407504041385

Stalp, M. C., & Conti, R. (2011). Serious leisure in the home: Professional quilters negotiate family space. Gender, Work and Organization, 18, 399–414. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0432.2009.0044.x

Staneva, A., Bogossian, F., Pritchard, M.,  & Wittkowski, A. (2015). The effects of maternal depression, anxiety, and perceived stress during pregnancy on preterm birth: A systematic review. Women and Birth, 28, 179–193.

Stanik, C. E., Riina, E. M.,  & McHale, S. M. (2013). Parent-adolescent relationship quali- ties and adolescent adjustment in two-parent African American families. Family Relations, 62, 597–608.

Staudinger, U. (2015). Images of aging: Outside and inside perspectives. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 35, 187–209. doi:10.1891/0198-8794.35.187

Staudinger, U. M., & Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research: Commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 215–241. doi:10.1146/ annurev. psych.121208.131659

Staudinger, U. M., Dörner, J., & Mickler, C. (2005). Wisdom and personality. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Jordan (Eds.), A handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives (pp. 191–219). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Steck, A.,  & Steck, B. (2016). Brain and mind: Subjective experience and scientific objectivity. New York: Springer.

Steeger, C. M., & Gondoli, D. M. (2013). Mother- adolescent conflict as a mediator between adolescent problem behaviors and maternal p s ycholo g ic al control. D e velopment al Psychology, 49, 804–814.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How ste- reotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor- mance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.

Steele, C. M.,  & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

Stein, D. J., & Vythilingum, B. (2009). Love and attachment: The psychobiology of social bonding . CNS Spectrums, 14, 239–242. Retrieved from 66.199.228.237/boundary/ Sexual_Addiction/love_and_attachment_ neurobiology.pdf.

Stein, J. H.,  & Reiser, L. W. (1994). A study of White middle-class adolescent boys’ responses to “semenarche” (the first ejaculation). Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 23, 373–384.

Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Parent-adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect . Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11, 1–19.

Steinberg, L. D. (1999). Adolescence (5th ed.). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Steinberg, L.,  & Monahan, K. C. (2007). Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1531–1543.

Steinberg, L.,  & Silk , J. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook

of parenting: Vol. 1 (2nd ed., pp. 103–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Stenberg, G. (2013). Do 12-month-old infants trust a competent adult? Infancy, 18, 873–904.

Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Sternberg, R. J. (1999). The theory of successful intelligence. Review of General Psychology, 3, 292–316.

Sternberg, R. J. (2006). A duplex theory of love. In R. J. Sternberg  & K. Weis (Eds.), The new psychology of love (pp. 184–199). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Sternberg, R. J. (2008). The triarchic theory of successful intelligence. In N. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of educational psychology (pp. 988–994). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Sternberg, R. J. (2015). Multiple intelligences in the new age of thinking. In S. Goldstein, D. Princiotta, & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of intelligence: Evolutionary theory (pp. 229–241). New York: Springer Verlag.

Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (1998). Human abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 479–502.

Sternberg, R. J., & Sternberg, K. (2017). Cognitive psychology (7th ed.). Boston: Cengage Learning.

Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L, & Kidd, K. K. (2005). Intelligence, race, and genetics. American Psychologist, 60, 46–59.

Sternberg, R. J., Jarvin, L.,  & Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Teaching for wisdom, intelligence, creativ- ity, and success. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.

Sterns, H. L.,  & Chang, B. (2010). Workforce issues and retirement. In J. C. Cavanaugh  & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Vol. 3: Societal issues (pp. 81–105). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Sterns, H. L.,  & Spokus, D. M. (2013). Lifelong learning and the world of work. In P. Taylor (Ed.), Older workers in an ageing society: Critical topics in research and policy (pp. 89–108). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Steuerle, C. E., & Quakenbush, C. (2015). Social Security and Medicare lifetime benefits and taxes. Retrieved from www.urban.org/ research/publication/social-security-and- medicare-lifetime-benefits-and-taxes/view/ full_report.

Stevens, H. (2016, August 12). Part-time work during the school year? Why it can be a good thing. Chicago Tribune. Retrieved from www. chicagotribune.com/lifestyles/ s t e v e n s / c t- b a c k- t o - s c h o o l - j o b - s t e v e n s - 0814-20160812-column.html.

Stevenson-Hinde, J.,  & Shouldice, A. (2009). Wariness to strangers: A behavior systems perspective revisited. In K. H. Rubin  & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 101–116). New York: Psychology Press.

Stevenson, H. W., & Lee, S. (1990). Contexts of achievement. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55 (Serial No. 221).

Stevenson, H. W.,  & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap. New York: Summit Books.

Stevenson, R. J., Oaten, M. J., Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M.,  & Wagland, P. (2010). Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: Development and acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 165–177.

Stewart, J. S., Goad Oliver, E., Cravens, K. S., & Oishi, S. (2017). Managing millennials: E m b r a c i n g g e n e r a t i o n a l d i f f e r e n c e s . Business Horizons, 60, 45–54. doi:10.1016/j. bushor.2016.08.011

Stice, E., Rohde, P., Gau, J.,  & Shaw, H. (2009). An effectiveness trial of a dissonance-based eating disorder prevention program for high- risk adolescent girls. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 825–834.

Stice, E., South, K.,  & Shaw, H. (2012). Future directions in etiologic, prevention, and treatment research for eating disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 845–855.

Stiles , J. (2008). Fundamentals of brain development. Cambridge M A: Har vard University Press.

Stokes, J. (2016). The influence of intergenerational relationships on marital quality following the death of a parent in adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 33, 3–22. doi:10.1177/0265407514558962

Stoltz, K. B. (2016). Midlife adults: At 40, the eyes had it, now at 50, the career does! When career vision begins to blur. In W. K. Killam, S. Degges-White,  & R. E. Michel (Eds.), Career counseling interventions: Practice with diverse clients (pp. 59–66). New York: Springer.

Stoner, S., O’Riley, A.,  & Edelstein, B. (2010). Assessment of mental health. In J. C. Cavanaugh & C. K. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Aging in America: Volume 2: Physical and mental health (pp. 141–170). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.

Strawbridge, W. J., Shema, S. J., Balfour, J. L., Higby, H. R .,  & Kaplan, G. A . (1998). Antecedents of frailty over three decades in an older cohort. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 53B, S9–S16. doi:10.1093/ geronb/53B.1.S9

Strenze, T. (2015). Intelligence and success. In S. Goldstein, D. Princiotta, & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of intelligence: Evolutionary theory (pp. 405–413). New York: Springer Verlag.

Strinic, V. (2015). Arguments in support and against euthanasia. British Journal of Medicine  & Medical Research, 9, 1–12. doi:10.9734/BJMMR/2015/19151

Stroebe, M. S., & Archer, J. (2013). Origins of mod- ern ideas on love and loss: Contrasting forerun- ners of attachment theory. Review of General Psychology, 17, 28–39. doi:10.1037/ a0030030

Stroebe, M., Finkenauer, C., Wijngaards-de Meij, L., Schut, H., van den Bout, J.,  & Stroebe, W. (2013). Partner-oriented self-regulation among bereaved parents: The costs of holding in grief for the partner’s sake. Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/0956797612457383

Stroebe, M., Schut, H.,  & Boerner, K. (2010). C o nt i nu i n g b o n d s i n a d ap t at i o n to

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 645

bereavement: Toward theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 259–268. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.007

Stroebe, M., Schut , H.,  & Stroebe, W. ( 2 0 0 5 ) . Att a ch m e nt i n co p i n g w i th bereavement : A theoretical integration. Review of General Psychology, 9, 48–66. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.1.48

Stroebe, M., Schut , H.,  & Stroebe, W. (2007). Health outcomes of bereavement. Lancet , 370, 1960–1973. doi:10.1016/ S0140-6736(07)61816-9

Stroebe, M., Schut, H.,  & van den Bout, J. (Eds.). (2012). Complicated grief : Scientific foundations for health care professionals. New York: Routledge.

Stroebe, W., Abakoumkin, G.,  & Stroebe, M. (2010). Beyond depression: Yearning for the loss of a loved one. OMEGA: Journal of Death and Dying, 61, 85–101. doi:10.2190/ OM.61.2.a

Strohmeier, D., Kärnä, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Intrapersonal and interpersonal risk factors for peer victimization in immigrant youth in Finland. Developmental Psychology, 47, 248–258.

Strohschein, L. (2012). Parental divorce and child mental health: Understanding predisruption effects. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 53, 489–502.

Strong, C., Juon, H-S., & Ensminger, M. E. (2016). Effect of adolescent cigarette smoking on adult substance use and abuse: The mediating role of educational attainment. Substance Use and Misuse, 51, 141–154.

Strough, J.,  & Berg, C. A. (2000). Goals as a mediator of gender differences in high- affiliation dyadic conversations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 117–125.

Sturaro, C, van Lier, P. A. C., Cuijpers, P., & Koot, H. M. (2011). The role of peer relationships in the development of early school-age externalizing problems. Child Development, 82, 758–765.

Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2010). Typologies of family functioning and children’s adjustment during the early school years. Child Development, 81, 1320–1335.

Sturman, D. A.,  & Moghaddam, B. (2012). Striatum processes reward differently in adolescents versus adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1719–1724.

Su, J.,  & Supple, A. J. (2016). School substance use norms and racial composition moderate parental and peer influences on adolescent substance use. American Journal of Community Psychology, 57, 280–290.

Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell, F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: A proposed direction forward based on psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12, 3–54.

Sugimoto-Matsuda, J. J.,  & Guerrero, A. P. S. (2016). Violence and abuse. In D. Alicata, N. Jacobs, A. Guerrero,  & M. Piasecki (Eds.), P roblem-ba sed behavioral science and psychiatry (pp. 113–133). New York: Springer.

Sugiura, M. (2016). Functional neuroimaging of normal aging: Declining brain, adapting

brain. Ageing Research Reviews, 30, 61–72. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2016.006

Suja, C., Shuhaib, B., Khathoom, H., & Simi, K. (2016). A review on dietary antioxidants. Research Jour- nal of Pharmacy and Technology, 9(2), 196–202. doi:10.5958/0974-360X.2016.00035.4

Sullivan, L., & Meschede, T. (2016). Race, gender, and senior economic well-being: How financial vulnerability over the life course shapes retirement for older women of color. Public Policy & Aging Report, 26, 58–62. doi:10.1093/ ppar/prw001

Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2003). Contextual determinants of anger and other negative expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 39, 693–705.

Sulloway, F. J., & Zweigenhaft, R. L. (2010). Birth order and risk taking in athletics: A meta- analysis and study of major league baseball. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 402–416.

Sumter, S. R., Vandenbosch, L., & Ligtenberg, L. (2017). Love me Tinder: Untangling emerging adults’ motivations for using the dating application Tinder. Telematics and Informatics, 34, 67–78.

Sung, S., Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I., S chlomer, G. L.,  & Belsky, J. (2016). Secure infant-mother attachment buffers the effect of early-life stress on age of menarche. Psychological Science, 27, 667–674.

Suo, C., Singh, M. F., Gates, N., Wen, W., Sachev, P., Brodaty, H. et  al. (2016). Therapeutically relevant structural and functional mechanisms triggered by physical and cognitive exercise. Molecul ar P sychi atr y, 21, 1633–1642. doi:10.1038/mp.2016.19

Super, C. M. (1981). Cross-cultural research on infancy. In H. C. Triandis & A. Heron (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Vol. 4. Developmental psychology. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Super, C. M., Herrera, M. G., & Mora, J. O. (1990). Long-term effects of food supplementation and psychosocial intervention on the physical growth of Colombian infants at risk of malnutrition. Child Development, 61, 29–49.

Super, D. E. (1957). The psychology of careers. New York: Harper & Row.

Super, D. E. (1976). Career education and the meanings of work. Washington, DC: U.S. Offices of Education.

Super, D. E. (1980). A life-span, life-space approach to career development. Journal of Vocational Behav ior, 16, 282–298. doi:10.1016/0001 -8791(80)90056-1

Super, D., Savickas, M., & Super, C. (1996). The life-span, life-space approach to careers. In D. Brown, L. Brooks, & Associates (Eds.), Career choice & development (3rd ed., pp. 121–178). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Suri, R., Lin, A. S., Cohen, L. S., & Atlshuler, L. L. (2014). Acute and long-term behavioral out- come of infants and children exposed in utero to either maternal depression or antidepres- sants: A review of the literature. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 75, e1142–e1152.

Suzuki, L.,  & Aronson, J. (2005). The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 320–327.

Sweeney, M. M. (2010). Remarriage and stepfamilies : Strategic sites for family scholarship in the 21st century. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 667–684.

Sweeper, S. (2012). Children’s adjustment after parental separation. In Patricia Noller  & Ger y C . Karantzas (Eds .), The Wiley- Blackwell handbook of couples and family relationships (pp. 345–360). Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Swindell, R . (2012). Successful ageing and international approaches to later-life learning. In G. Boulton-Lewis & M. Tam (Eds.), Active ageing, active learning (pp. 35–63). New York: Springer.

Swingle, A. L.-V. (2013). College student parents: Stress, role conflict, and coping. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University of Pennsylvania. Retrieved from dspace.iup.edu/ bitstream/handle/2069/2047/Amy%20Louise- Vannurded%20Swingle.pdf?sequence=1.

Symonds, W. C., Schwartz, R. B., & Ferguson, R. (2011). Pathways to prosperity: Meeting the challenge of preparing young Americans for the 21st century. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Graduate School of Education.

Tabor, J. (2016). Adjustment to divorce (children). In C. L. Shehan (Ed.), The Wiley Blackwell encyclopedia of family studies (Vol 1., pp. 25–30). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Tach, L., & Halpern-Meekin, S. (2009). How does premarital cohabitation affect trajectories of marital quality? Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 298–317. doi:10.1111/ j.1741-3737.2009.00600.x

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). Putting words together: Morphology and syntax in the preschool years. In J. Berko Gleason (Ed.), The development of language (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan.

Tahiroglu, D., Mannering, A. M.,  & Taylor, M. (2011). Visual and auditory imagery associated with children’s imaginary companions. Imagi- nation, Cognition, and Personality, 31, 99–112.

Talbot, L. A., Morrell, C. H., Fleg, J. L., & Metter, E. J. (2007). Changes in leisure time physical activity and risk of all-cause mortality in men and women: The Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Preventive Medicine, 45, 169–176. doi:10.1016/j. ypmed.2007.05.014

Tam, K.-P. (2013). Existential motive underlying cosmetic surgery: A terror management analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 947–955. doi:10.1111/jasp.12059

Tamayo, G. J., Broxson, A., Munsell, M., & Cohen, M Z. (2010). Caring for the caregiver. Oncology Nursing Forum, 37, E50–E57. doi:10.1188/10. ONF.E50-E57

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1996). Variation in children’s exploratory, nonsym- bolic, and symbolic play: An explanatory multi- dimensional framework. In C. Rovee-Collier & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 10, pp. 37–78). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

646 R e f e R e n c e s

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Cur- rent Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 121–126.

Tan, K. L. (2009). Bed sharing among mother- infant pairs in Kiang District, Peninsular Malaysia and its relationship to breast-feeding. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 420–425.

Tang, F., Morrow-Howell, N.,  & Choi, E. (2010). Why do older adult volunteers stop volunteering? Ageing and Society, 30, 859–878. doi:10.1017/ S0144686X10000140

Tang, P.-L., Chiou, C.-P., Lin, H.-S., Wang, C., & Liand, S.-L. (2011). Correlates of death anxiety among Taiwanese cancer patients. Cancer Nursing, 34, 286–292. doi:10.1097/ NCC.0b013e31820254c6

Tang, Y.-Y., & Posner, M. I. (2012). Tools of the trade: Theory and method in mindfulness neu- roscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neu- roscience, 8, 118–120. doi:10.1093/scan/nss112

Tanner, J. M. (1970). Physical growth. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 77–155). New  York: Wiley.

Tanner, J. M. (1990). Fetus into man: Physical growth from conception to maturity (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tardif, T., Fletcher, P., Liang, W., Zhang, Z., Kaciroti, N., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Baby’s first 10 words. Developmental Psychology, 44, 929–938.

Taylor, J. L., O’Hara, R., Mumenthaler, M. S., Rosen, A . C.,  & Yesavage, J. A . (2005). Cognitive ability, expertise, and age differences in following air-traffic control instructions. P sy cholog y and Ag ing , 20, 117–133. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.117

Taylor, M. G., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Boys will be boys; cows will be cows: Children’s essentialist reasoning about gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 80, 461–481.

Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., Maring, B. L., Gerow, L., & Charley, C. M. (2004). The characteristics of fan- tasy in school-age children: Imaginary compan- ions, impersonation, and social understanding. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1173–1187.

Taylor, M., Hulette, A. C., & Dishion, T. J. (2010). Longitudinal outcomes of young high-risk adolescents with imaginary companions. Developmental Psychology, 46, 632–1636.

Taylor, M., Sachet, A. B., Maring, B. L.,  & Mannering, A. M. (2013). The assessment of elaborated role-play in young children: Invisible friends, personified objects, and pretend identities. Social Development, 22, 75–93.

Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehav- ioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 273– 277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x

Teachman, J. (2008). Complex life course patterns and the risk of divorce in second marriages. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 294–305.

Teachman, J., Tedrow, L., & Kim, G. (2013). The demography of families. In G. W. Peterson  & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 39–63). New York: Springer.

Teaster, P. B., Harley, D. A., & Kettaneh, A. (2014). Aging and mistreatment: Victimization of older adults in the United States. In H. F. Ofahengaue Vakalahi, G. M. Simpson, & N. Giunta (Eds.), The collective spirit of aging across cultures (pp. 41–64). New York: Springer.

Tegethoff, M., Greene, N., Olsen, J., Meyer, A. H., & Meinlschmidt, G. (2010). Maternal psychoso- cial adversity is associated with length of gesta- tion and offspring size at birth: Evidence from a population-based cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72, 419–426.

Tenenbaum, H. R.,  & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38, 615–630.

Terkel, S. (1974). Working. New York: Pantheon. Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems

theories. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley.

Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56 (Serial No. 223).

Therborn, G. (2010). Families in global perspective. In A. Giddens  & P. W. Sutton (Eds.), Sociology: Introductory readings (3rd ed., pp. 119–124). Malden, MA: Polity Press.

Thiessen, E. D.,  & Saffran, J. R. (2003). When cues collide: Use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 39, 706–716.

Thomas, A.,  & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Thomas, A., Chess, S.,  & Birch, H. G. (1968). Temperament and behavior disorders in children. New York: NYU Press.

Thomas, F. (2008). Remarriage after spousal death: Options facing widows and implications for livelihood security. Gender and Development, 16, 73–83. doi:10.1080/13552070701876235

Thomas, J. R ., Alderson, J. A ., Thomas, K. T., Campbell, A. C., & Elliot, B. C. (2010). Developmental gender differences for over- hand throwing in Aboriginal Australian chil- dren. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 81, 432–441.

Thomas, M. L., Bangen, K. J., Ardelt, M.,  & Jeste, D. V. (2017). Development of a 12-item Abbreviated Three-Dimensional Wisdom Scale (3D-WS-12). Assessment, 24, 71-82. doi:10.1177/107319115595714

Thomas, R., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2011). Accumulating evidence for parent-child interaction therapy in the prevention of child maltreatment. Child Development, 82, 177–192.

Thompson, A. E.,  & Voyer, D. (2014). Sex differences in the ability to recognise non- verbal displays of emotion: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1164–1195.

Thompson, G. E., Cameron, R. E.,  & Fuller- Thompson, E. (2013). Walking the red road:

The role of First Nations grandparents in promoting cultural well-being. International Journal of Aging and Human development, 76, 55–78. doi:10.2190/AG.76.1.c

Thompson, R. A. (2000). The legacy of early attachments. Child Development, 71, 145–152.

Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Thompson, R. A.,  & Limber, S. (1991). “Social anxiety” in infancy: Stranger wariness and separation distress. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of social and evaluation anxiety. New York: Plenum.

Thompson, R. A., Laible, D. J.,  & Ontai, L. L. (2003). Early understandings of emotion, morality, and self : Developing a working model. Advances in Child Development  & Behavior, 31, 137–172.

Thornton, W. L., Paterson, T. S. E., & Yeung, S. E. (2013). Age differences in everyday problem solving: The role of problem context. Interna- tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 13–20. doi:10.1177/0165025412454028

Tiffany, D. W.,  & Tiffany, P. G. (1996). Control across the life span: A model for understanding self-direction. Journal of Adult Development, 3, 93–108. doi:10.1007/BF02278775.

Tincoff, R.,  & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10, 172–175.

Tobin, D. J. (2017). Introduction to skin aging. Journal of Tissue Viability, 26, 37–46. doi:10.1016/j.jtv.2016.03.022

Tolkko, T. (2016). Becoming an expert by experience: An analysis of service users’ learning process. Social Work in Mental Health, 14, 292–312. doi:10.1080/15332985 .2015.1038411

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722.

Tong, C.,  & Kram, K. E. (2013). The efficacy of mentoring—the benefits for mentees, mentors, and organizations. In J. Passmore, D. B. Peterson, & T. Freire (Eds.), The Wiley- Blackwell handbook of coaching and mentoring (pp. 217–242). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones,  & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 114–137). New York: Guilford.

Torges, C. M., Stewart, A. J.,  & Duncan, L. E. (2008). Achieving ego integrity: Personality development in late midlife. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1004–1019. doi:10.1016/ j.jrp.2008.02.006

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., Mayas, J.,  & Ballesteros, S. (2016). Video game training enhances visuospatial working memory and episodic memor y in older adults . Frontiers in

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 647

Human Neuroscience, 10, 206. doi:10.3389/ fnhum.2016.00206. Retrieved from www.ncbi .nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4859063/.

Torres, L., Lindstrom, K., Hannah, L., & Webb, F. (2016). Exploring barriers among primary care providers in referring patients to hospice. Journal of Hospice  & Palliative Nursing, 18, 167–172. doi:10.1097/NJH.0000000000000233

Tracy, B., Reid, R., & Graham, S. (2009). Teaching young students strategies for planning and drafting stories: The impact of self-regulated strategy development. Journal of Educational Research, 102, 323–331.

Trainor, L. J.,  & Heinmiller, B. M. (1998). The development of evaluative responses to music: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance. Infant Behavior  & Development, 21, 77–88.

Treiman, R.,  & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in acquisition of literacy. Advances in Child Development & Behavior, 31, 105–135.

Tremblay, R . E., Schall, B., Boulerice, B., Arsonault, L., Soussignan, R. G., & Paquette, D. (1998). Testosterone, physical aggression, and dominance and physical development in adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22, 753–777.

Trim, R. S., Meehan, B. T., King, K. M., & Chassin, L. (2007). The relation between adolescent substance use and young adult internalizing symptoms : Findings from a high-risk longitudinal sample. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21, 97–107.

Troutman, M., Nies, M. A.,  & Mavellia, H. (2011). Perceptions of successful aging in Black older adults. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 49, 28–34. doi:10.3928/02793695-20101201-01

Trzaskowski, M., Harlaar, N., Arden, R., Krapohl, E., Rimfield, K., McMillan, A., et  al. (2014). Genetic influence on family socioeconomic status and children’s intelligence. Intelligence, 42, 83–88.

Trzesniewski, K., H.,  & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking “Generation Me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976–2006. Perspective on Psychological Science, 5, 58–75. doi:10.1177/1745691609356789

Tsuno, N., & Homma, A. (2009). Aging in Asia— The Japan experience. Ageing International, 34, 1–14. doi:10.1007/s12126-009-9032-9

Tu, M. C.-H. (2006). Culture and job satisfaction: A comparative study between Taiwanese and Filipino caregivers working in Taiwan’s long- term care industry. Unpublished dissertation, Nova Southeastern University.

Tucker-Drob, E. M., Briley, D. A., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 349–355.

Tucker, M. S. (2011). Surpassing Shanghai: An agenda for American education built on the world’s leading systems. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press.

Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of

child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 789–857). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Turner, R. N., & Cameron, L. (2016). Confidence in contact: A new perspective on promoting cross-group friendship among children and adolescents. Social Issues and Policy Review, 10, 212–246.

Twenge, J. M.,  & Crocker, J. (2002). Race and self-esteem: Meta-analysis comparing Whites, Blacks, Hispanics, and American Indians and comment on Gray-Little and Hafdahl (2000). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 371–408.

Tynes, B. M. (2007). Role taking in online “classrooms”: What adolescents are learning about race and ethnicity. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1312–1320.

Tynes, B. M., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Rose, C. A., Lin, J., & Anderson, C. J. (2012). Online racial discrimination and the protective function of ethnic identity and self-esteem for African American adolescents. Developmental Psychol- ogy, 48, 343–355.

U.S. Department of Agriculture. (2017). Expenditures on children by families, 2015. Retrieved from www.cnpp.usda.gov/sites/ default/files/crc2015_March2017.pdf.

U.S. Department of Health  & Human Services. (2016). Child maltreatment 2014. Retrieved f r o m w w w. a c f . h h s . g o v / p r o g r a m s / c b / research-data-technology/statistics-research/ child-maltreatment.

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2010). The Surgeon General’s vision for a healthy and fit nation. Rockville, MD: Author.

U.S. Department of Labor Women’s Bureau. (2016a). Data and statistics. Retrieved from www.dol.gov/wb/stats/stats_data.htm.

U.S. Department of Labor Women’s Bureau. (2016b). Women’s earnings by race and ethnic- ity as a percentage of white, non-Hispanic men’s earnings, March 1987–2014. Retrieved from www.dol.gov/wb/stats/earnings_race_ethnic_ percent_white_nonhisp_mens_earn_87_14_ txt.htm.

U.S. Department of Labor. (2016) Nontradi- tional occupations. Retrieved from www.dol .gov/wb/stats/Nontraditional%20 Occupations .pdf.

Uchino, B. N., Cawthon, R. M., Smith, T. W., Light, K. C., McKenzie, J., Carlisle, M., et  al. (2012). Social relationships and health: Is feel- ing positive, negative, or both (ambivalent) about your social ties related to telomeres? Health Psychology, 31, 789–796. doi:10.1037/ a0026836

Ukraintseva, S. V., Yashin, A., Arbeev, K. G., Kulminski, A., Akushevich, I., Wu, D., et  al. (2016). Puzzling role of genetic risk factors in human longevity: “Risk alleles” as prolongevity variants . Biogerontolog y, 17, 109–127. doi:10.1007/s10522-015-9600-1

Umaña-Taylor, A., Diversi, M., & Fine, M. (2002). Ethnic identity and self-esteem among Latino adolescents: Distinctions among Latino populations. Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, 303–327.

UNICEF-WHO-The World Bank. (2016). Levels and trends in child malnutrition. New  York: Author.

UNICEF. (2007). The state of the world’s children, 2008. New York: Author.

UNICEF. (2016). Breastfeeding. Retrieved from www.unicef.org/nutrition/index_24824.html.

University of Massachusetts Medical School Center for Mindfulness. (2017). Stress reduc- tion program. Retrieved from www.umassmed .edu/cfm/mindfulness-based-programs/ .

Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., Whiteman, S. D., Thayer, S. M.,  & Delgado, M. Y. (2005). Adolescent sibling relationships in Mexican American families: Exploring the role of familism. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 512–522.

Updegraff, K. A., Thayer, S. M., Whiteman, S. D., Denning, D. J., & McHale, S. M. (2005). Aggres- sion in adolescents’ sibling relationships: Links to sibling and parent–adolescents relationship quality. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 54, 373–385.

Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2009). Sources of self- efficacy in mathematics: A validation study. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 89–101.

Utz, R.L.,  & Pascoe, A. (2016). Bereavement/ widowhood. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adulthood and aging (Vol. 1, pp. 118–121). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

Vacha-Haase, T., Donaldson, W. V., & Foster, A. (2014). Race-ethnicity and gender in older adults. In M. L. Minville  & A. D. Ferguson (Eds.), Handbook of race-ethnicity and gender in psychology (pp. 65–83). New York: Springer.

Vaillancourt, T., Brittain, H. L., McDougall, P., & Duku, E. (2013). Longitudinal links between childhood peer victimization, internalizing and externalizing problems, and academic functioning: Developmental cascades. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 1203–1215.

Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45, 534–543.

Vaish, A., Woodward, A.,  & Grossmann, T. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383–403.

Valenza, E., & Bulf, H. (2011). Early development of object unity: Evidence for perceptual completion in newborns. Developmental Science, 14, 799–808.

Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). Adolescents’ online communication: An integrated model of its attraction, opportunities, and risks. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 121–127.

Vallerand, R. J. (2008). On the psychology of passion: In search of what makes people’s lives most worth living. Canadian Psychology, 49, 1–13. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.1

Vallerand, R . J. (2015). The psychology of passion: A dualistic model. New York: Oxford University Press.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

648 R e f e R e n c e s

Vallerand, R. J., Paquet, Y., Philippe, F. L.,  & Charest, J. (2010). On the role of passion for work in burnout: A process model. Jour nal of Per sonalit y, 78, 289–312. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00616.x

van den Wijngaart, M. A . G., Vernooij- Dassen, M. J. F. J.,  & Felling, A. J. A. (2007). The influence of stressors, appraisal and personal conditions on the burden of spousal caregivers of persons with dementia. Ag ing  & Mental Health, 11, 626–636. doi:10.1080/13607860701368463

van der Geest, S. (2004). Dying peacefully: Considering good death and bad death in Kwahu-Tafo, Ghana. Social Science and Medicine , 58, 899–911. doi:10.1016/j. socscimed.2003.10.041

van der Pas, S.,  & Koopman-Boyden, P. (2010). Leisure and recreation activities and wellbeing among midlife New Zealanders . In C. Waldegrave  & P. Koopman-Boyden (Eds.), Midlife New Zealanders aged 40–64 in 2008: Enhancing well-being in an aging society (pp. 111–128). Hamilton, New Zealand: Family Centre Social Policy Research Unit, Lower Hutt, Wellington and the Population Studies Centre, University of Waikato. Retrieved from www.familycentre.org.nz/Publications/PDF’s/ EWAS_M2.pdf.

Van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. , Huizenga, H. M., & Jansen, B. R. J. (2014). What is and what could have been: Experiencing regret and relief across childhood. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 926–935.

van Goozen, S. H. M. (2015). The role of early emotion impairments in the development of persistent antisocial behavior. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 206–210.

van Hof, P., van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2002). The relation of unimanual and bimanual reaching to crossing the midline. Child Development, 73, 1352–1362.

Va n I Jz e n d o o r n , M . H . , B a k e r m a n s - Kranenburg, M. J.,  & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Methylation matters in child development: Toward developmental behavioral epigenetics. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 305–310.

van Klaveren, C., van den Brink, H. M.,  & van Praag, B. (2013). Intrahousehold work timing: the effect on joint activities and the demand for child care. European Sociological Review, 29, 1–18. doi:10.1093/esr/jcr035

van Muijden, J., Band, G. P. H.,  & Hommel, B. (2012). Online games training aging brains: Limite d transfer to cognitive control functions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00221. Retrieved from www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/ PMC3421963/.

Van Rijsewijk, L., Dijkstra, J. K., Pattiselanno, K., Steglich, C.,  & Veenstra, R. (2016). Who helps whom? Investigating the development of adolescent proso cial relationships . Developmental Psychology, 52, 894–908.

van Wanrooy, B. (2013). Couple strategies: Negotiating working time over the life course. In A. Evans, & J. Baxter (Eds.), Negotiating the

life course: Stability and change in life pathways (pp. 175–190). New York: Springer.

Vance, J. D. (2016). Hillbilly elegy: A memoir of a family and culture in crisis. New  York: HarperCollins.

Vander Wal, J. S., & Thelen, M. H. (2000). Eating and body image concerns among obese and average-weight children. Addictive Behaviors, 25, 775–778.

Vansteenkiste, S., Deschacht, N., & Sels, L. (2015). Why are unemployed aged fifty and over less likely to find a job? A decomposition analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 90, 55–65. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2015.07.004

Varkal, M. D., Yalvac, D., Tufan, F., Turan, S., Cengiz, M., & Emul, M. (2013). Metacognitive differences between elderly and adult outpatients with generalized anxiety disorder. European Geriatric Medicine, 4, 150–153. doi:10.1016/j. eurger.2012.12.001

Vazire, S.,  & Doris, J. M. (2009). Personality and personal control. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 274–275. doi:10.1016/j. jrp.2008.12.033

Vélez, C. E., Wolchik, S. A.,  & Sandler, I. N. (2014). Divorce effects on adolescents (pp. 1076–1085). In T. P. Gullotta  & M. Bloom (Eds.), Encyclopedia of primary prevention and health promotion. New  York: Springer Science+Business Media.

Verbrugge, L. M.,  & Jette, A . M. (1994). T h e d i s a b l e m e n t p r o c e s s . S o c i a l Science and Medicine , 38, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90294-1

Verbrugge, L. M., Brown, D. C., & Zajacova, A. (2017). Disability rises gradually for a cohort of older Americans, Journals of Gerontology: P sy ch o l o g i c al S c i e n c e s , 7 2 , 1 5 1 – 1 6 1 . doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw002

Victora, C. G., Bahl, R., Barros, A. J. D., França, G. V. A., Horton, S., Krasevec, J., et al. (2016). Breastfeeding 1: Breastfeeding in the 21st century: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet, 387, 475–490.

Vieno, A., Nation, M., Pastore, M., & Santinello, M. (2009). Parenting and antisocial behavior: A model of the relationship between adolescent self-disclosure, parental closeness, parental control, and adolescent antisocial behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1509–1519.

Villar, F., & Serrat, R. (2017). Changing the culture of long-term care through narrative care: Individual, interpersonal, and institutional dimensions. Journal of Aging Studies, 40, 44–48. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2016.12.007

Volling, B. L. (2012). Family transitions following the birth of a sibling: An empirical review of changes in the firstborn’s adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 497–528.

Volpe, U., Fiorillo, A., Jovanovic, N., & Bhugra, D. (2016). Mentoring and career coaching. In A. Fiorillo, U. Volpe,  & D. Bhugra (Eds.), Psychiatry in practice: Education, experience, and expertise (pp. 83–95). New York: Oxford University Press.

von Ranson, K. M.,  & Wallace, L. M. (2014). Eating disorders. In R. A. Barkley  & E. J.

Mash (Eds.), Child psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 801–847) New York: Guilford.

Vorhees, C. V.,  & Mollnow, E. (1987). Behavior teratogenesis: Long-term inf luences on behavior. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

Voss, M. W., Vivar, C., Kramer, A. F., & van Praag, H. (2013). Bridging animal and human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17, 525–544. doi:10.1016/j. tics.2013.08.001

Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F., & Martin, A. (2010). The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development, 81, 517–527.

Voyer, D., Postma, A., Brake, B.,  & Imperato- McGinley, J. (2007). Gender differences in object location memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 23–38.

Vraneković, J., Božović, I. B., Grubić, Z., Wagner, J., Pavlinić, D., Dahoun, S., Bena, F., Čulić, V., & Brajenović-Milić, B. (2012). Down syndrome: Parental origin, recombination, and maternal age. Genetic Testing and Molecular Biomarkers, 16, 70–73.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner,  & E. Soubermen (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wachs, T. D., & Bates, J. E. (2001). Temperament. In G. Bremner  & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 465–501). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Wadlington, W. (2005). Family law in America. Family Court Review, 43, 178–179.

Wadsworth, S. M., Hughes-Kirchubel, L.,  & Riggs, D. S. (2014). Research and training about military families: Where are we? In S. M. Wadsworth & D. S. Riggs (Eds.), Military deployment and its consequences for families (pp. 1–17). New York: Springer.

Wahl, H.-W. (2015). Theories of environmental inf luences on aging and behavior. In N. A . Pachana (Ed.), Encyclopedia of geropsychology (pp. 1–8). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-981-287-080-3_132-1

Waites, C. (2009). Building on strengths: Intergenerational practice with African- American families. Social Work, 54, 278–287. doi:10.1093/ sw/54.3.278

Walberg, H. J. (1995). General practices. In G. Cawelti (Ed.), Handbook of research on improving student achievement. Arlington, VA: Educational Research Service.

Walker, A . C .,  & B alk , D. E . (2007). B ereavement rituals in the Muscogee Creek tribe. Death Studies, 31, 633–652. doi:10.1080/07481180701405188

Walker, L. E. A. (1984). The battered woman syndrome. New York: Springer.

Walker, L. J. (1980). Cognitive and perspective- taking prerequisites for moral development. Child Development, 51, 131–139.

Walker, L. J.,  & Taylor, J. H. (1991). Family interactions and the development of moral reasoning. Child Development, 62, 264–283.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 649

Walker, L. J., Hennig, K. H.,  & Krettenauer, T. (2000). Parent and peer contexts for children’s moral re a s oning de velopment . C hild Development, 71, 1033–1048.

Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., et al. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross- modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 21–25.

Wallerstein, J. S.,  & Lewis, J. M. (2004). The unexpected legacy of divorce: Report of a 25-year study. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 21, 353–370. doi:10.1037/0736-9735.21.3.353

Walther, A . N. (1991). Divorce hangover. New York: Pocket Books.

Wang , Q. (2014). The cultured self and remembering. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (pp. 605–625). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Wang, S-w.,  & Repetti, R. L. (2016). Who gives to whom? Testing the support gap hypothesis with naturalistic obser vations of couple interactions. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 492–502. doi:10.1037/fam0000196

Wang, S.-H., Zhang, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2016). Young infants view physically possible support events as unexpected: New evidence for rule learning. Cognition, 157, 100–105.

Wang, W. (2015). The link between a college education and a lasting marriage. Retrieved from www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/12/04/ education-and-marriage/.

Wang, W.-C., & Giovanello, K. S. (2016). The role of medial temporal lobe regions in incidental and intentional retrieval of item and relational information in aging. Hippocampus, 26, 693– 699. doi:10.1002/hipo.22578

Wansink, B., & Sobal, J. (2007). Mindless eating: The 200 daily food decisions we overlook. Environment & Behavior, 39, 106–123.

Wanzer, S. H.,  & Glenmullen, J. (2007). To die well: Your right to comfort, calm, and choice in the last days of life. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press.

Waren, W.,  & Pals, H. (2013). Comparing characteristics of voluntarily childless men and women. Journal of Population Research, 30, 151–170. doi:10.1007/s12546-012- 9103-8

Warner, B., Altimier, L.,  & Crombleholme, T. M. (2007). Fetal surgery. Newborn  & Infant Nursing Reviews, 7, 181–188.

Warner, S. C.,  & Valdes, A. M. (2016). The genetics of osteoarthritis: A review. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology, 1, 140–153. doi:10.3390/jfmk1010140

Warnock, F. F., Craig, K., Bakeman, R., & Castral, T. (2014). Self-regulation (recovery) from pain: Association between time-based measures of infant pain behavior and prenatal exposure to maternal depression and anxiety. Clinical Journal of Pain, 30, 663–671.

Warr, P., Butcher, V.,  & Robertson, I. (2004). Activity and psychological well-being in older people. Aging  & Mental Health, 8, 172–183. doi:10.10 80/13607860410001649662

Warren-Findlow, J.,  & Issel, I. M. (2010). Stress and coping in African American women with chronic heart disease: A cultural cognitive coping model. Journal of Transcultural Nursing, 21, 45–54. doi:10.1177/1043659609348622

Wasik, B. A., Hindman, A. H., & Snell, E. K. (2016). Book reading and vocabulary development: A systematic review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 37, 39–57.

Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and emotional intelligence: A critical review. Educational Psychologist, 41, 207–225.

Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships. Child Development, 71, 164–172.

Waters, H. S. (1980). “Class news”: A single- subject longitudinal study of prose production and schema formation during childhood. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 152–167.

Watson, R. W. (2013). Frisky 60s flower children are still making love not war. Retrieved from www.psychologytoday.com/blog/love-and- gratitude/201302/frisky-60s-flower-children- are-still-making-love-not-war.

Wax, J. R., Pinette, M. G.,  & Cartin, A. (2010). Home versus hospital birth: Process and outcome. Obstetrical & Gynecological Survey, 65, 132–140.

Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Leddon, E., Geraghty, K., & Song, H.-J. (2013). Are nouns learned before verbs? Infants provide insights into a long-standing debate. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 155–159.

Wayland, S., Maple, M., McKay, K., & Glassock, G. (2016). Holding onto hope: A review of the literature exploring missing persons, hope and ambiguous loss. Death Studies, 40, 54–60. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2015.1068245

Weaver, C. C., Leffingwell, T. R., Lombardi, N. J., Claborn, K. R. Miller, M. E., & Martens, M. P. (2014). A computer-based feedback only interaction with and without a moderation skills component. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 46, 22–28. doi:10.1016/ j.sat.2013.08.011

Weaver, D. A. (2010). Widows and Social Security. Social Security Bulletin, 70, 89–109. Retrieved from heinonline.org/HOL/L andingPage? collection=journals&handle=hein.journals/ ssbul70&div=20&id=&page.

Weaver, J., & Schofield, T. (2015). Mediation and moderation of divorce effects on children’s behavior problems . Journal of Family Psychology, 29, 39–48.

Webb, N. B. (2010a). The child and death. In N. B. Webb (Ed.), Helping bereaved children: A handbook for practitioners (3rd ed., pp. 3–21). New York: Guilford Press.

Webb, N. B. (2010b). Assessment of the bereaved child. In N. B. Webb (Ed.), Helping bereaved children: A handbook for practitioners (3rd ed., pp. 22–47). New York: Guilford Press.

Webb, S. J., Monk, C. S., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological de velopment : Implications for human

development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 147–171.

Weber, D., Dekhtyar, S.,  & Herlitz, A. (2017). The Flynn effect in Europe—Effects of sex and region. Intelligence, 60, 39–45.

Webster-Marketon, J., & Glaser, R. (2008). Stress hormones and immune function. Cellular Immunolog y, 252, 16–26. doi:10.1016/j. cellimm.2007.09.006

Wechsler, H., Davenport, A ., Dowdall, G., Moeykens, B.,  & Castillo, S. (1994). Health and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1672–1677. doi:10.1001/ jama.1994.03520210056032

Wegman, L. A., Hoffman, B. J., Carter, N. T., Twenge, J. M., & Guenole, N. (in press). Placing job characteristics in context: Cross-temporal meta-analysis of changes in job characteristics since 1975. Jour nal of Management. doi:10.1177/149206316654545

Wegman, M. E. (1994). Annual summary of vital statistics—1993. Pediatrics, 95, 792–803.

Wei, W., Lu, H., Zhao, H., Chen, C., Dong, Q., & Zhou, X. (2012). Gender differences in children’s arithmetic p erformance are accounted for by gender differences in language abilities. Psychological Science, 23, 320–330.

Weichold, K.,  & Silbereisen, R . K. (2005). Puberty. In C. B. Fisher & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Encyclopedia of applied developmental science (Vol. 2, pp. 893–898). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Weinstock, M. P., Neuman, Y.,  & Glassner, A. (2006). Identification of informal reasoning fallacies as a function of epistemological level, grade level, and cognitive ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 327–341.

Weis, R.,  & Cerankosky, B. C. (2010). Effects of video-game ownership on young boys’ academic and behavioral functioning: A randomized, controlled study. Psychological Science, 21, 463–470.

Weisgram, E. S., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2010). Gender, values, and occupational interests among children, adolescents, and adults. Child Development, 81, 778–796.

Weisner, T. S.,  & Wilson-Mitchell, J. E. (1990). Nonconventional family lifestyles and sex typing in six-year-olds. Child Development, 61, 1915–1933.

Weisner, T. S., Garnier, H.,  & Loucky, J. (1994). Domestic tasks, gender egalitarian values and children’s gender typing in conventional and nonconventional families. Sex Roles, 30, 23–54.

Weiss, D. (2016). On the inevitability of aging: Essentialist beliefs moderate the impact of negative age stereotypes on older adults’ memor y performance and physiological reactivity. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw087

Wellman, H. M. (2012). Theory of mind: Better methods, clearer findings, more development. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 313–330.

Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York: Oxford.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

650 R e f e R e n c e s

Wellman, H. M.,  & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 523–573). New York: Wiley.

Wellman, H. M., Fang, F.,  & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of- mind scale. Child Development, 82, 780–792.

Weltzin, T. E. (2017). Eating disorders in males. In K. D. Brownell & B. T. Walsh (Eds.), Eating disorders and obesity (3rd ed., pp. 209–213). New York: Guilford.

Wentkowski, G. (1985). Older women’s perceptions of greatgrandparenthood: A research note. The Gerontologist, 25, 593–596. doi:10.1093 /geront/25.6.593

Wentzel, K. R., Filisetti, L., & Looney, L. (2007). Adolescent prosocial behavior: The role of self-processes and contextual cues. Child Development, 78, 895–910.

Werker, J. F.,  & Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 173–196.

Werner, E. (1994). Overcoming the odds. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 131–136.

Werner, E. E. (1989). Children of Garden Island. Scientific American, 260, 106–111.

Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 81–85.

Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adult- hood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. Chicago: Follet.

Werner, N. E., Eaton, A. D., Lyle, K., Tseng, H., & Holst, B. (2014). Maternal social coaching quality interrupts the development of relational aggression during early childhood. Social Development, 23, 470–486.

Werner, T. L.,  & Shannon C. S. (2013). Doing more with less: Women’s leisure during their partners’ military deployment. Leisure Sciences, 35, 63–80. doi:10.1080/01490400. 2013.739897

Wertsch, J. V., & Tulviste, P. (1992). L. S. Vygotsky and contemporary developmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 28, 548–557.

West, M. D. (2010). Embryonic stem cells: Prospects of regenerative medicine for the treatment of human aging. In G. M. Fahy, M. D. West, L. S. Coles, & S. B. Harris (Eds.), The future of aging (pp. 451–487). New York: Springer.

Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E., & Webster, J. D. (2010). Reminiscence and mental health: A review of recent progress in theory, research and interventions. Ageing and Society, 30, 697– 721. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x09990328

Westrate, N. M., Ferrari, M.,  & Ardelt, M. (2016). The many faces of wisdom: An investigation of cultural-historical wisdom exemplars reveals practical, philosophical, and benevolent prototypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 662–676. doi:10.1177/0146167216638075

Wettstein, R. M. (2013). Legal issues geriatric psy- chiatrists should understand. In M. D. Miller & L. K. Salai (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry (pp. 55–77). New York: Oxford University Press.

Whalley, L. J. (2015). Understanding brain aging and dementia: A lifecourse approach. New York: Columbia University Press.

White, H.,  & Venkatesh, B. (2016). Traumatic brain injury. In M. Smith, G. Citerio, & W. A. Kofke (Eds.), Oxford textbook of neurocritical care (pp. 210–224). New  York: Oxford University Press.

White, K. D. (2010). Covenant marriage: An unnecessary second attempt at fault-based divorce. Alabama Law Review, 61, 869. Retrie ve d f rom w w w.law.ua .e du/pubs/ lrarticles/Volume%2061/Issue%204/white.pdf.

White, L., & Gilbreth, J. G. (2001). When children have two fathers: Effects of relationships with stepfathers and noncustodial fathers on adolescent outcomes. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 155–167.

Whitehurst, G. J., & Vasta, R. (1975). Is language acquired through imitation? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 37–59.

Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, P. E. (1988). Children of different worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Whitty, M. T.,  & Buchanan, T. (2009). Looking for love in so many places: Characteristics of online daters and speed daters. Interpersona: An International Journal on Personal Relationships, 3(Suppl. 2), 63–86. Retrieved from crawl.prod.proquest.com.s3.amazonaws. com/fpcache/4228bb2686d8dc3cecf9ca4d31bc b15f.pdf?AWSAccessKeyId=AKIAJF7V7KNV 2KKY2NUQ&Expires=1476657636&Signatur e=LXYaIQm21fOgAwRDo40NnvcuCm8%3D.

Wickrama, K. A. S., O’Neal, C. W., & Lorenz, F. O. (2013). Marital functioning from middle to later years: A life course-stress process framework. Journal of Family Theory and Review,5, 15–34. doi:10.1111/jftr.12000

Wicks-Nelson, R .,  & Israel, A . C. (2015). Abnormal child and adolescent psychology (8th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

Widen, S. C.,  & Russell, J. A. (2013). Children’s recognition of disgust in others. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 271–299.

Widman, L., Choukas-Bradley, S., Noar, S. M., Nesi, J., & Garrett, K. (2016). Parent-adolescent sexual communication and adolescent safer sex behavior: A meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 170, 52–61.

Widom, C. S., Czaja, S. J.,  & DuMont, K. A. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of child abuse and neglect: Real or detection bias? Science, 347, 1480–1485.

Wijdicks, E. F. M., Varelas, P. N., Gronseth, G. S., & Greer, D. M. (2010). Evidence-based guideline update: Determining brain death in adults. Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurolog y, 74, 1911–1918. doi:10.1212/ WNL.0b013e3181e242a8

Wiles, J. L., & Jayasinha, R. (2013). Care for place: The contributions older people make to their

communities. Journal of Aging Studies, 27, 93–101. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2012.12.001

Wilhelm-Leen, E. R., Hall, Y. N., Horwitz, R. I., & Chertow, G. M. (2014). Phase angle, frailty and mortality in older adults. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 29, 147–154. doi:10.1007/ s11606-013-2585-z

Williams, J. C., Phillips, K. W.,  & Hall, E. V. (2014). Double jeopardy? Gender bias against women of color in science. Retrieved from www .uchastings.edu/news/articles/2015/01/double- jeopardy-report.pdf.

Williams, S. T., Conger, K. J., & Blozis, S. A. (2007). The development of interpersonal aggression during adolescence: The importance of parents, siblings, and family economics. Child Development, 78, 1526–1542.

Wilson, R . D. (2000). Amniocentesis and chorionic villus sampling. Current Opinion in Obstetrics & Gynecology, 12, 81–86.

Wilt, J., Cox, K. S.,  & McAdams, D. P. (2010). The Eriksonian life story: Developmental scripts and psychosocial adaptation. Journal of Adult Development, 17, 156–161. doi:10.1007/ s10804-010-9093-8

Winecoff, A., LaBar, K. S. Madden, D. J., Cabeza, R., & Huettel, S. A. (2011). Cognitive and neural contributions to emotion regulation in aging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 165–176. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq030

Winneke, G. (2011). Developmental aspects of environmental neurotoxicology: Lessons from lead and polychlorinated biphenyls. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 308, 9–15.

Winner, E. (2000). Giftedness: Current theory and research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 153–156.

Winslow, M., & Meldrum, M. (2013). A history of hospice and palliative care. In S. Lutz, E. Chow, & P. Hoskin (Eds.), Radiation oncology in palliative cancer care (pp. 63–71). New York: Wiley.

Wise, D. (2017). Social Security programs and retirement around the world. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Witten, T. M. (2016). The intersectional challenges of aging and of being a gender non- conforming adult. Generations, 40, 63–70.

Wojtunik-Kulesza , K . A ., Oniszczuk , A ., Oniszczuk, T.,  & Waksmundzka-Hajnos, M. (2016). The inf luence of common free radicals and antioxidants on development of Alzheimer ’s disease. Biomedicine  & Pharmacotherapy, 78, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/j. biopha.2015.12.024

Wolff, P. H. (1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Wolke, D., Copeland, W. E., Angold, A.,  & Costello, E. J. (2013). Impact of bullying on adult health, wealth, crime, and social outcomes. Psychological Science, 24, 1958–1970.

Women’s Health Research Institute. (2016). How hormone depletion affects you. Retrieved from menopause.northwestern.edu/content/ how-hormone-depletion-affects-you.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

R e f e R e n c e s 651

WomensHealth.gov. (2010). Menopause basics. Retrie ve d from w w w.omenshealth.gov/ menopause/menopause-basics/index.html.

Wong, D. R.,  & Rothenhaus, T. C. (2014). Management of trauma in the elderly. In J. H. Kahn, B. G. Magauran, Jr.,  & J. S. Olshaker (Eds.), Geriatric emergency medicine: Principles and practice (pp. 59–68). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Wong, P. T. P. (2015). The meaning hypothesis of living a good life: Virtue, happiness and meaning. Retrieved from www.drpaulwong .com/the-meaning-hypothesis-of-living-a- good-life-virtue-happiness-and-meaning/.

Woodbridge, S. (2008). Sustaining families in the 21st century: The role of grandparents. International Journal of Environmental, Cultural, Economic and Social Sustainability. Retrieved from www98.griffith.edu.au/dspace/ bitstream/10072/27417/1/50932_1.pdf.

Worden, W. (1991). Grief counseling and grief therapy: A handbook for the mental health practitioner (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

World Health Organization. (2010). Population- based prevention strategies for childhood obesity. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

World Health Organization. (2012). Seniors and disabilities. Retrieved from new.paho. o r g / h q / i n d e x . p h p ? o p t i o n = c o m _ c o n t e n t & v i e w = a r t i c l e & i d = 7 3 1 6 % 3 A s e n i o r s - a - disabilities-april-2012&catid=4684%3Afch- hldisabilities-and-rehabilitation&Itemid=936& lang=en.

World Health Organization. (2013). Responding to intimate partner violence and sexual violence against women. Retrieved from www .who.int/reproductivehealth/publications/ violence/9789241548595/en/.

World Health Organization. (2015). 10 facts on dementia. Retrieved from www.who.int/ features/factfiles/dementia/en/.

Wray-Lake, L., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (2010). Developmental patterns in decision- making autonomy across middle childhood and adolescence: European American parents perspectives. Child Development, 81, 636–651.

Wrenn, R. L. (1999). The grieving college student. In J. D. Davidson  & K. J. Doka (Eds.), Living with grief : At work, at school, at worship (pp. 131–141). Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel.

Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Murphy, K. C., St. Peters, M., Piñon, M., Scantlin, R., et al. (2001). The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: The Early Window Project. Child Development, 72, 1347–1366.

Wrzus, C.,  & Roberts, B. W. (2017). Processes of personality development in adulthood: The TESSERA framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3): 253-277. doi:10.1177/1088868316652279

Wu, L., & Zhao, L. (2016). ApoE2 and Alzheimer’s disease: Time to take a closer look. Neural Re generation Re se arch , 11, 412–413. doi:10.4103/1673-5374.179044

Wyman, E. (2014). Language and collective fiction: From children’s pretence to social

institutions. In D. Dor, C. Knight,  & J. Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 171– 183). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750.

Xia, Y. R., Do, K. A.,  & Xie, X. (2013). The adjustment of Asian American families to the U.S. context: The ecology of strengths and stress. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (3rd ed., pp. 723–747). New York: Springer.

Xu, X., Zhu, F., O’Campo, P., Koenig, M. A., Mock, V., & Campbell, J. (2005). Prevalence of and risk factors for intimate partner violence in China. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 78–85. doi:10.2105/ AJPH.2003.023978

Yaakobi, E. (2015). Desire to work as a death anxiety buffer mechanism. Experimental Psy- chology, 62, 110–122. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/ a000278

Yang, J., Kanazawa, S., Yamaguchi, M. K., & Kuriki, I. (2016). Cortical response to categorical color perception in infants investigated by near- infrared spectroscopy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 2370–2375.

Yang, T., Yang, X. Y., Cottrell, R. R., Wu, D., Jiang, S., & Anderson, J. G. (2016). Violent injuries and regional correlates among women in China: Results from 21 cities study in China. The European Journal of Public Health, 26, 513–517. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckv193

Yap, M. B. H., Pilkington, P. d., Ryan, S. B.,  & Jorm, A. F. (2014). Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 156, 8–23.

Yárnoz-Yaben, S. (2010). Attachment style and adjustment to divorce. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 210–219. doi:10.1017/ S1138741600003796

Yarwood, A., Huizinga, T. W. J., & Worthington, J. (2016). The genetics of rheumatoid arthritis: Risk and protection in different stages of the evolution of RA. Rheumatology, 55, 1990209. doi:10.1093/rheumatology/keu323

Yngvesson, B. (2010). Belonging in an adopted world: Race, identity, and transnational adoption. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Yoffe, E. (2013). College women: Stop getting drunk. Retrieved from www.slate.com/articles/ double_x/doublex/2013/10/sexual_assault_ and_drinking_teach_women_the_connection .html.

Yoon, S. M. (2005). The characteristics and needs of Asian-American grandparent caregivers: A study of Chinese-American and Korean- American grandparents in New  York City. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 44, 75–94. doi:10.1300/ J083v44n03_06

Young, K. (2013). Adult friendships in the Facebook era. Webology, 10. Retrieved from pirate.24nieuwe.nl/www.webology.org/2013/ v10n1/a103.html.

Young, S. K., Fox, N. A.,  & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1999). The relations between temperament

and empathy in 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1189–1197.

Yousafzai, A. K., Obradović, J., Rasheed, M. A., Rizvi, A., Portilla, X. A., Tirado-Strayer, N., et  al. (2016). Effects of responsive stimula- tion and nutrition interventions on children’s development and growth at age 4 years in a disadvantaged population in Pakistan: A longi- tudinal follow-up of a cluster-randomised fac- torial effectiveness trial. Lancet Global Health, 4, e548–558.

Youssef-Morgan, C. M.,  & Luthans, F. (2013). Psychological capital theory: Toward a positive holistic model. In A. B. Bakker (Ed.), Advances in positive organizational psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 145–166). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing.

Yu, T., Pettit, G. S., Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (2010). The interactive effects of marital conflict and divorce on parent- adult children’s relationships . Jour nal of Marriage and Family, 72, 282–292. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00699.x

Yuan, S., & Fisher, C. (2009). “Really? She blicked the baby?”: Two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychological Science, 20, 619–626.

Yuen, H. K.,  & Vogtle, L. K. (2016). Multi- morbidity, disability and adaptation strategies among community-dwelling adults aged 75 years and older. Disability and Health Journal, 9, 593–599. doi:10.1016/j.dhjo.2016.03.004

Yumoto, C., Jacobson, S. W.,  & Jacobson, J. L. (2008). Fetal substance exposure and cumula- tive environmental risk in an African American cohort. Child Development, 79, 1761–1776.

Zabriskie, R. B., & Kay, T. (2013). Positive leisure science: Leisure in family contexts. In T. Freire (Ed.), Positive leisure science (pp. 81–99). New York: Springer.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E.,  & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136.

Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Wilson, A. C., Trancik, A.,  & Bazinet, A. (2011). Emotion regulation profiles , temperament , and adjustment problems in preadolescents. Child Development, 82, 951–966.

Zarrett, N., Fay, K., Li, Y., Carrano, J., Phelps, E., & Lerner, R. M. (2009). More than child’s play: Variable- and pattern-centered approaches for examining effects of sports participation on youth development. Developmental Psychology, 45, 368–382.

Zaslow, M. J.,  & Hayes, C. D. (1986). Sex differences in children’s responses to psychosocial stress: Toward a cross-context analysis. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown,  & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 285–337). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zelazo, P. R. (1993). The development of walking: New findings and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 99–137.

Zetumer, S., Young, I., Shear, M. K., Skritskaya, N., Lebowitz, B., Simon, N. et  al. (2015). The

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

652 R e f e R e n c e s

impact of losing a child on the clinical presentation of complicated grief. Journal of Affective Disorders, 170, 15–21. doi:10.1016/j. jad.2014.08.021

Zhai, F., Waldfogel, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2013). Estimating the effects of Head Start on parenting and child maltreatment. Child and Youth Services Review, 35, 1119–1129.

Zhan, H. J. (2006). Joy and sorrow: Explaining Chinese caregivers’ reward and stress. Journal of Aging Studies, 20, 27–38. doi:10.1016/j. aging.2005.01.002

Zheng, Z., Shemmassian, S., Wijekoon, C., Kim, W., Bookheimer, S. Y.,  & Pouratian, N. (2014). DTI correlates of distinct cognitive impairments in Parkinson’s disease. Human Brain Mapping. 35, 1325–1333. doi:10.1002 / hbm.22256

Zhou, L., Lu, J., Chen, G.,  & Yao, Y. (2017). Is there a paradox of aging: When the negative aging stereotype meets the positivity effect in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 43, 80–93. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2017. 1258254

Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S. H., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Guthrie, I. K., et  al. (2002). The relations of parental warmth and positive expressiveness to children’s empathy- related responding and social functioning:

A longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 893–915.

Zhou, Q., Wang, Y., Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S., Tein, J-W.,  & Deng, X. (2008). Relations of parenting and temperament to Chinese children’s experience of negative life events, coping efficacy, and externalizing problems. Child Development, 79, 493–513.

Zhu, W., Chen, Q., Tang, C., Cao, G., Hou, Y., & Qiu, J. (2016). Brain structure links every- day creativity to creative achievement. Brain and Cognition, 103, 70–76. doi:10.1016/j. bandc.2015.09.008

Z igler, E .,  & Finn-Stevenson, M. (1992). Applied developmental psychology. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental p s y c h o l o g y : A n a d v a n c e d t e x t b o o k (pp. 677–729). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Helfand, M. (2008). Ten years of longitudinal research on U.S. ado- lescent sexual behavior: Developmental corre- lates of sexual intercourse, and the importance of age, gender and ethnic background. Devel- opmental Review, 28, 153–224.

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 1–17.

Z immerman, B. J. (2001). The ories of self-reg ulate d le ar ning and ac ademic achievement: An overview and analysis. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self- regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 1–37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zinar, S. (2000). The relative contributions of word identification skill and comprehension- monitoring behavior to reading comprehension ability. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 363–377.

Zinn, J. O. (2016). Risk taking. In A. Burgess, A. Alemanno,  & J. O. Zinn (Eds.), Routledge handbook of risk studies (pp. 344–355). New York: Routledge.

Zosh, J. M., Lytle, S. R., Golinkoff, R. M.,  & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2017). Putting the education back in educational apps: How content and context interact to promote learning. In R . Barr  & D. N. Linebarger (Eds.), Media exposure during infancy and early childhood (pp. 259–282). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

Zukow-Goldring, P. (2002). Sibling caregiving. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 253–286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Note: Page numbers in italics indicate figures, tables, or illustrations.

Aasland, O. G., 394 Abakoumkin, G., 567, 575 Abbott, G., 389 Abela, A., 362 Aberson, C. L., 357 Aboud, F. E., 91 Abraham, A., 485 Abrams, D., 394, 517 Abrams, R., 573 Achor, S., 394 Ackerman, B. P., 150 Acredolo, L. P., 142 Adams, J., 63, 358 Adams, M. J., 212 Adams, R. B., Jr., 343 Adams, R. E., 245 Adams, R. G., 356, 520 Adamy, J., 547 Addis, D. R., 483 Addo, F. R., 367 Adelman, L., 327 Adhikari, K., 422 Adler, L. L., 362 Adler, R. A., 425 Adolph, K. E., 97, 98, 99, 100 Adolphus, K., 219 Afilalo, J., 526, 527 Agabio, R., 308 Ahmad, F., 362 Ahmad, I., 296 Ai, A. L., 512, 513 Ainsworth, M. S., 158 Aisenbrey, S., 409 Ajrouch, K. J., 516, 517 Akashi, J., 555 Aksan, N., 86 Aksoy, S., 545 Alatupa, S., 248 Alavi, M. V., 472 Al-Bar, M. A., 544 Albert, D., 246, 275 Alberts, A. E., 81 Albuquerque, S., 573 Aldao, A., 180 Alden, D. L., 555 Aldridge, V., 90 Aldwin, C. M., 325, 422, 423,

502–503, 512 Alegría-Flores, K., 301 Alexander, G. M., 179, 180 Alfi-Shabtay, I., 148 Ali, M., 10, 11, 11, 495 Alias, F., 544, 545 Alibali, M. W., 125, 190, 211, 212 Alink, L. R. A., 241 Alkadry, M. G., 409 Allaire, J. C., 437, 503 Allemand, M., 442 Allen, J., 216 Allen, J. P., 233, 246 Allen, K. R., 450 Allexandre, D., 396

Alley, J. L., 451 Almeida, J., 375 Alspach, J. G., 558 Altimier, L., 66 Altschul, I., 232, 311–312 Amaro, C. M., 272 Amato, P. R., 230, 238, 239, 376, 381 Ambady, N., 256 Amso, D., 107 Andersen, G. J., 472, 473 Anderson, E. R., 376, 381 Anderson, S. W., 94 Anderson, W. A., 520 Andreassi, J. K., 412 Angel, J. L., 516 Angel, R. J., 516 Angelou, M., 212, 345 Antheunis, M. L., 357 Anticevic, A., 343 Antonucci, T. C., 516 Anzures, G., 107, 108 Apfelbaum, E. P., 256 Apgar, V., 81 Aponte, M., 394 Arbeev, K. G., 467 Archer, J., 241, 562, 564, 565 Ardelt, M., 486, 487, 512 Arizmendi, B., 567 Armstrong, P. I., 391, 392 Arndt, J., 552 Arnett, J. J., 318, 319, 320, 323, 356 Aron, A., 362 Aronson, J., 203 Arrau, C., 197 Arseneault, L., 71 Årseth, A. K., 323 Artazcoz, L., 406 Arterberry, M. E., 104, 107, 166 Arthur, J., 369 Arunachalam, S., 144 Aryee, S., 410, 412 Asarnow, J. R., 310 Asbridge, M., 62 Asendorpf, J. B., 248 Ashcraft, M. H., 213–214 Asher, S. R., 243, 244 Aslin, R., 140 Aslin, R. N., 140 Asoodeh, M. H., 369 Assanangkornchai, S., 564 Astone, N. M., 366 Attia, E., 272 Attig, T., 561, 562, 572 Au, T. K., 143 Auertin-Leheudre, M., 480 Aunola, K., 230 Awa, W. L., 396 Awadi, M. A., 558 Awong, T., 295 Ayers, T. S., 565–566 Aziz, T., 362

Bachman, B., 395 Bachman, H. J., 216 Bachman, J. G., 305, 306

Bade, M. K., 433 Badger, S., 319 Baer, D. M., 14 Bagbey Darian, C. D., 561, 562, 564, 566 Bagnall, A.-M., 396 Bahn, R. S., 394 Bahrick, L. E., 108 Bailey, J. A., 231 Bailey, J. M., 183 Baillargeon, R., 111, 127–128 Baird, D. D., 59 Baker, L., 212 Baker, L. A., 451 Baker, P. N., 58 Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., 48,

86, 267 Baker-Ward, L., 133 Bakolis, I., 60 Baldacchino, A., 328 Baldi, R. A., 412 Baldridge, D. C., 393 Balk, D. E., 543, 571, 572 Ballard, R. H., 382 Baltes, B. B., 19 Baltes, M. M., 19 Baltes, P. B., 19, 19, 19–20, 22, 334–335,

437, 438, 464, 465, 486, 487, 526 Baluch, B, 220 Bambra, C., 406 Banaji, M. R., 183 Band, G. P. H., 481 Bandura, A., 14, 14, 21, 181 Banerjee, K., 128 Banks, J. B., 483 Bannard, C., 148 Barac, R., 145 Barber, B. K., 230 Barber, L. K., 389 Barbey, A. K., 343 Barca, L., 210 Barch, D. M., 343 Barenboim, C., 253 Barker, E., 70 Barkley, R. A., 208 Barlow, M., 503 Barnes, J. N., 435 Barnett, A. E., 448 Baron, A. S., 256 Baron, R., 64 Baron-Cohen, S., 113 Barr, R., 132, 146 Barra, M., 400 Barrera, M., 572 Barrick, M. R., 392 Barros, R., 211 Bartkowiak, G., 516 Bartl, C., 423 Bartl, R., 423 Bartlett, D. B., 434 Barton, C., 495 Barton, M. E., 149 Bar-Tur, L., 574 Barzilai, N., 466 Basso, K. H., 267 Baste, V., 61

Basten, U., 339–340 Bastida, E., 513 Bastos, A., 381 Bates, E., 145 Bates, J. E., 86, 228 Bates, J. S., 444 Batt, M. E., 436 Battista, R. N., 476 Baucom, B. R., 380 Baude, A., 238 Bauer, C. R., 64 Bauer, P. J., 132, 133 Baum, C. L., II, 409 Bauman, U., 445 Baumann, A., 545 Baumeister, R. F., 347 Baumrind, D., 228 Bavelier, D., 251 Bavishi, A., 430 Baxter, A. J., 112 Bayl-Smith, P. H., 394 Beach, S. R. H., 371, 381 Beal, C. R., 150 Beanland, V., 481 Beardsall, L., 235 Beauchamp, G. K., 56, 103 Beausaert, S., 405 Beavers, T., 474 Bechtold, K. T., 183 Beck, A. T., 490 Beck, E., 238 Beck, V., 517 Becker, B. J., 179 Becker, C. B., 272 Becker, J. B., 268 Beckes, L., 357 Bedir, A., 545 Beelmann, A., 256 Behl, C., 469 Behnke, A. O., 295 Behnke, M., 59 Belgrad, S. L., 150 Belser, A., 505 Belsky, G., 427 Belsky, J., 86, 267, 298, 374 Benbow, C. P., 200 Bender, P. K., 170 Benedict, M. D., 60 benenden health, 421 Benenson, J., 243 Bengtson, V. L., 525 Bennett, K. M., 521, 524 Bent, K. N., 575 Bentley, A., 161–162 Benton, S. L., 212 Bereiter, C., 212 Berenbaum, S. A., 177, 183 Berg, C. A., 179, 521, 560 Bergdahl, E., 549 Bergdahl, J., 433 Berger, R. G., 467 Berger, S. E., 97, 98, 100 Bergman, K. N., 233 Bergsma, A., 487 Berk, L. E., 138

Name Index

N a m e I N d e x 653

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

654 N a m e I N d e x

Berk, R. A., 394 Berko, J., 147 Berkowitz, M. W., 283 Bermejo-Toro, L., 395 Bernadotte, A., 466 Bernard, K., 161 Berndt, T. J., 244 Bernier, A., 138 Berninger, G., 149 Berns, N., 563 Berry, J., 482, 484 Bersoff, D. M., 282 Bertenthal, B. H., 100 Bertenthal, B. I., 107 Best, J. R., 220 Beste, C., 102 Beuker, K. T., 143 Bewick, B., 328 Bhatt, R. S., 166 Bhutta, M., 475 Bialystok, E., 145 Bian, F., 378 Bian, L., 111 Biblarz, T. J., 378 Bidwell, M., 394 Bierhoff, H.-W., 361 Biggs, S., 404 Bigler, R. S., 177, 183, 256, 257 Biles, S., 196 Binet, A., 198–199 Bingenheimer, J. B., 312 Bird, E. R., 327 Bird, S., 274 Birnbaum, G. E., 371 Bjerkedal, T., 238 Bjork, J. M., 329 Bjorklund, D. F., 157, 193, 228 Blachford, D., 378 Blachnio, A., 357 Black, J. E., 94 Blackhall, L. J., 556 Blackstone, A., 373 Blair, B. L., 244 Blair, C., 131 Blair, P. S., 83 Blakemore, J. E. O., 177–178 Blanchard, C. M., 415 Blanchard-Fields, F., 412, 437 Blankson, A. M., 86 Blieszner, R., 356, 357, 358, 519, 520 Bligh, M. C., 395 Bloom, L., 144, 148, 150 Bloom, P., 128, 142, 143 Blozis, S. A., 312 Blümel, J. E., 428 Blumenthal, H., 451, 452 Blume-Peytavi, U., 422 Bluntschli, J. R., 553 Boals, A., 483 Bobbitt, K. C., 161–162 Böckerman, P., 394 Boden, M. T., 343 Bodenmann, G., 367 Boehm, S. A., 400 Boerner, K., 564 Bohlmeijer, E., 508 Böhm, R., 256 Boiger, M., 163 Boivin, M., 294 Boker, S. M., 300 Boldt, L. J., 160 Bolton, J. M., 572 Bonanno, G. A., 562, 564–565 Bondmass, M. D., 564

Bons, T. A., 244 Booker, J. A., 167, 487 Boon, H., 574 Booth, J. N., 220 Booth-LaForce, C., 160 Borch, C., 244 Bordere, T. C., 563 Börjesson, M., 436 Bornstein, M. H., 59, 72, 86, 166,

172, 296 Bortfeld, H., 140 Borthick, A. F., 321 Bos, H. M., 378 Boschma, J., 515 Bosco, A., 395 Boseovski, J. J., 254 Bosma, H. A., 293 Bosman, E. A., 439 Boss, P., 566, 567 Bosshard, G., 545 Boswell, G., 512 Boswell, J., 477 Bouchard, T. J., 48, 200 Bowden, D. E., 393 Bowen, C. E., 443 Bowen, R., 292 Bowker, A., 220, 274 Bowker, J. C, 245 Bowlby, J., 157–158 Bows, H., 532 Boyce, W. T., 48 Boyczuk, A. M., 447 Boyd, L. M., 376, 381 Boyle, J. M., Jr., 545 Boyle, P. J., 523–524 Bradbury, T. N., 370, 373, 380 Bradshaw, C. P., 249 Brainerd, C. J., 125 Brancucci, A., 340 Brandtstädter, J., 348, 349 Bratkovich, K. L., 564 Braun, S. K., 106 Braungart, J. M., 50 Braungart-Rieker, J. M., 164 Braver, S. L., 379, 381 Braza, P., 230 Brazelton, T. B., 81 Brechwald, W. A., 246 Brehmer, Y., 482 Bremner, J. G., 107, 128 Brendel, R. W., 560 Brendgen, M., 245, 311 Brenn, T., 524 Brennan, R. T., 312 Brenner, J. S., 220, 221 Brescoll, V., 312 Bretherton, I., 145, 159 Bridge, J. A., 310 Briley, D. A., 49 Brindis, C. D., 299 Brissette, I., 357 Brizzee, K. R., 474 Brockington, I., 69 Brody, G. H., 234, 235, 236 Brody, N., 199 Bromell, L., 520 Bromley, D. B., 253 Bronfenbrenner, U., 17, 17–18, 22, 228 Brooker, R. J., 164 Brooks-Gunn, J., 58, 72, 110, 221, 242,

267, 268, 274, 298 Brophy, J. E., 216 Brophy, M., 112 Brossoie, N., 532

Brown, B. B., 245, 246 Brown, C., 477 Brown, D. A., 134 Brown, D. C., 526–527 Brown, E. G., 172 Brown, J. R., 167, 235 Brown, J. W., 518 Brown, L., 52 Brown, S. D., 304, 390, 391 Brownell, K. D., 273 Brubacher, J. R., 62 Bruck, M., 133, 134 Brummelman, E., 295 Brunsma, D. L., 294 Bryant, P., 211 Brymer, E., 320 Brysiewicz, P., 563 Buchanan, C. M., 268 Buchanan, T., 359 Buchtel, E. E., 442 Buckingham-Howes, S., 64 Budgeon, S., 365 Buehler, C., 246 Buhl, H., 447 Buhle, J. T., 168 Bui, E., 565 Bukowski, W. M., 244, 245 Bulf, H., 107 Bullard, L., 239 Bullock, M., 110 Bumpus, M. F., 234 Burani, C., 210 Burger, A., 405 Buriel, R., 228 Burk, W. J., 244 Burke, B. L., 552, 553 Burke, R., 294 Burnet, K. L., 238 Burnette, D., 451 Burns, S. T., 375 Burraston, B., 245 Burt, S. A., 231 Burton, L. M., 525 Burton-Chellew, M. N., 157 Bush, G. W., 11 Buss, D., 361 Buss, K. A., 86, 167, 168 Bussey, K., 181 Bustos, M. L. C., 543 Butcher, V., 415 Buttelmann, D., 256 Butters, M. A., 435 Byock, S. D., 323 Byrnes, J. P., 278 Bywaters, P., 240

Cabeza de Baca, T., 267 Cable, N., 308 Cacciatore, J., 563 Cacioppo, J. T., 362, 509 Cacioppo, S., 362 Cadell, S., 575 Cagney, K. A., 520 Cahill, E., 449, 450 Cahill, J., 161 Cahill, K. E., 514, 515, 516 Cain, K., 211, 212 Caldera, Y. M., 172 Call, V. R. A., 373 Callaghan, T., 169 Callahan, C. M., 204 Calment, J., 3–4, 4 Calvert, S. L., 250, 252 Calvete, E., 301

Camberis, A.L., 374 Cameron, L., 244, 257 Cameron, R. E., 451 Camp, C. J., 484, 495, 496 Camp, T. G., 451 Campbell, J., 430 Campbell, J. M., 102 Campbell, K., 359 Campbell, R., 145 Campinha-Bacote, J., 394 Campos, J. J., 99, 105 Camras, L. A., 166 Candel, I., 133 Cantlon, J. F., 135 Cao, R., 247 Capaldi, D. M., 380 Capistrant, B., 449 Cappeliez, P., 521 Cappell, K. A., 482 Caraviello, R., 503 Caravolas, M., 210 Cardoso-Leite, P., 251 Carlander, I., 549 Carlo, G., 174, 175, 295, 375 Carlson, J., 201, 203 Carlson, M. D., 298 Carlson, S. M., 138 Carlson Jones, D., 267 Carmalt, J. H., 371 Carnelley, K., 416 Carnevale, A. P., 322–323 Caro, I., 272 Caron, C. D., 531 Carpendale, J. I. M., 255 Carpenter, M., 149, 173 Carpenter, P. A., 212 Carpenter, R., 84 Carr, A., 490, 491 Carr, D., 357, 520, 524 Carré, J. M., 311 Carrère, S., 381 Carroll, J. B., 195–196 Carroll, J. L., 84 Carstensen, L. L., 19, 509, 519, 521 Carter, C. S., 174 Cartin, A., 69 Carver, C. S., 357 Carver, K., 297 Casals, P., 20 Case, T. I., 164 Caselli, M. C., 142 Casey, B. J., 93, 94, 265, 266, 275 Cash, S. J., 310 Casper, W. J., 365 Caspi, A., 442 Cassel, C. K., 550 Cassidy, B. S., 343 Cassidy, J., 160, 296 Casterline, J. B., 319 Castro, A. S., 319 Cattell, R. B., 195 Caudle, K., 94 Cauffman, E., 305 Cauvet, E., 144 Cavanaugh, J. C., 330, 438, 449, 523 Ceci, S. J., 133 Cerankosky, B. C., 252 Champagne, F. A., 58 Champion, T. B., 202 Chamsi-Pasha, H., 544 Chan, H., 62 Chandler, E. M., 556 Chandler, M. J., 255 Chang, B., 516, 517

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 655

Chang, J. E., 563 Chang, S.-S., 407 Chang, T.-S., 357, 520 Chao, R. K., 230 Chaplin, T. M., 180 Chapman, B., 369 Chapman, P. D., 199 Charchar, F. J., 466 Charles, S. T., 509, 519, 521 Charlton, B., 551 Charness, N., 439 Chase-Lansdale, P. L., 58 Chasteen, A. L., 348 Chaudhari, A., 466 Cheadle, J., 381 Chein, J., 246 Chen, E., 233 Chen, J., 197 Chen, X., 247 Chen, Y., 113, 343 Chen, Z. Y., 238 Cheng, E., 415 Cheng, S., 375 Cheng, T., 234 Cheng, T. C., 509 Cheng, Z-J., 215 Chen-Gaddini, M., 296 Cherbuin, N., 513 Cherko, M., 475 Cherry, K. E., 481 Chess, S., 85 Chesson, B., 572 Cheuk, G., 474 Cheung, C. K., 403 Cheung, L. W. Y., 72 Cheung, W. W., 271 Chew-Graham, C. A., 489, 491 Chi, M. T. H., 439 Chiao, C.-Y., 523 Chirgwin, S., 393 Chiu, M. M., 215 Choe, D. E., 234–235 Choi, E., 517 Choi, H. S., 416 Choi, M., 452 Choi, Y., 294 Chomitz, V. R., 72 Chow, B. W., 210 Chow, S., 234–235 Christ, C. C., 174 Christ-Crain, M., 430 Christensen, A., 380 Chronaki, G., 167 Chun, S., 415 Church, A. T., 442 Ciani, E., 515 Cicchetti, D., 240, 241 Cicero, C., 503 Cicirelli, V. G., 550, 553 Cillessen, A. H. N., 244 Ciscato, E., 367 Cisneros, S., 212 Clark, R., 71 Clarke, P. J., 206 Clarke, V., 575 Clay, C. M., Jr., 11 Claypool, H. M., 177 Cleary, J. F., 556 Cleary, M. N., 322 Clements, D. H., 213 Cleveland, K. C., 134 Clevinger, A. M., 483 Clifton, R. K., 100 Clinton, B., 11

Clinton, H., 406 Coan, J. A., 357 Coates, B. A., 379 Coelho, J. S., 271 Coffman, J. L., 193 Cohen, A. O., 320 Cohen, F., 406 Cohen, H. J., 513 Cohen, M. D., 512 Cohen, R. W., 73 Cohen, S., 58, 431 Cohen Kadosh, K., 94 Cohen-Mansfield, J., 515 Coie, J. D., 248 Colby, A., 282 Cole, D. A., 309 Cole, M., 138 Cole, P. M., 166 Coleman, J., 517 Coleman, M., 377 Coleman-Jensen, A., 90 Coles, R. L., 375 Collardeau, F., 383 Collins, R. L., 298 Collins, W. A., 296, 297, 298, 358 Collins-Colosi, K., 572 Colombo, B., 59 Coltrane, S., 410 Commons, M. L., 340 Compton, W. C., 450 Conger, K. J., 312 Connidis, I. A., 521 Conradt, E., 48 Conry-Murray, C., 178 Constantin, L. P., 412 Conti, R., 416 Conway, A. M., 164 Cook, D. A., 394 Cook, E. C., 246 Cooke, L. J., 90 Cooklin, A. R., 234 Coombs, M. A., 562 Coon, D. W., 367, 522 Cooney, T. M., 294, 382 Copen, C. E., 366 Coplan, R. J., 170 Copley, S. J., 472 Coppus, A. M. W., 44 Corbett, B. A., 413 Cordes, C. L., 395 Cornelius, M., 59 Cornwall, A., 398 Corr, C. A., 551, 555 Corr, D. M., 551, 555 Corriveau, K. H., 143 Cortiella, C., 205 Cortina, L. M., 402 Cosby, B., 402 Cosmides, L., 163 Costa, P. T., Jr., 441, 442 Costigan, K. A., 55 Costin, S. E., 173 Cotton, S. R., 520 Coughlin, C. A., 193 Coulton, C. J., 240 Counts, D., 543 Cousminer, D. L., 267 Cowart, B. J., 103 Cox, A., 146 Cox, C. B., 452 Cox, K. S., 444 Coyne, C. A., 58 Cram, B., 409 Crano, W. D., 307

Crawford-Williams, F. M., 327 Cremeens, P. R., 172 Crick, N. R., 244, 247, 248, 311 Cristia, A., 139 Crnic, K. A., 71 Crocetti, E., 291, 292, 293 Crocker, J., 295 Crockett, L. J., 230 Croghan, C. F., 523 Crombleholme, T. M., 66 Crosby, L., 380 Cross, S., 347 Crouter, A. C., 181, 234 Crown, J. S., 371 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 268 Csizmadia, A., 294 Cukier, H. N., 495 Cummings, E. M., 160, 228, 233,

234–235, 247–248, 312 Cummings, K., 561, 562, 564 Cumsille, P., 234 Cundiff, N. L., 400 Cunningham, A. E., 211 Cunningham, W. R., 335 Curran, T., 221 Currie, J., 61 Curtis, J. R., 555 Curtis, R., 377 Curwin, A., 146 Cvencek, D., 177 Cyr, C., 241 Cyrus, M., 263 Czaja, S. J., 240, 484

da Costa, J. F., 404 Dadgari, A., 529 Dai, B., 415 Dalege, J., 181 Dalton, C., 505 Daly, B. J., 556 Damian, R. I., 485 Danaher, D. L., 179 Daneman, M., 212 D’Angelo, D., 550 Daniels, H., 137 Dannar, P. R., 393 Dannefer, D., 20 Darejeh, A., 440 Darling, N., 234 Daum, M. M., 149 Davidov, M., 173 Davidson, F. H., 257 Davidson, M. M., 257 Davies, L., 239, 366 Davies, P. T., 233, 235, 473 Davis, B., 375 Davis, E. L., 167 Davis, E. M., 320 Davis, E. P., 58 Davis, K. M., 60 Davis, N. S., 84 Davis, P. E., 170 DeAndrea, D. C., 357 Deane, G., 447 Deary, I., 29, 199, 200 Deary, I. J., 200, 442 Deater-Deckard, K., 230 Debast, I., 442 De Beni, R., 211 de Bloom, J., 416 Declercq, E., 69 DeFrain, J., 563 Degner, J., 181 de Haan, M., 71, 107

Dehaene-Lambertz, G., 148 de Jong, P., 531–532 De Jong Gierveld, J., 357–358 Dekas, K. H., 388 DeKeyser, R., 148 Dekhtyar, S., 200 DeKosky, S. T., 469, 492 Delaney, C., 82 DeLiema, M., 532 De Lisi, R., 251 DeLoache, J. S., 146, 166 DeLucia-Waack, J. L., 382 DelVecchio, W. F., 442 de Magalhães, J. P., 467 Dement, W. C., 84 Demir, A., 95 Demir, M., 356, 357 Denckla, C. A., 356 De Neys, W., 191 Denford, S., 300 Deng, C.-P., 391, 392 Denham, J., 466 Denissen, J. A., 292 Denissen, J. J. A., 248 Denney, N. W., 436 DePaulo, B. M., 365 de Pommereau, I., 517 Deprest, J., 66 De Rango, F., 462 de Rezende, L. F. M., 271 Deschacht, N., 403 DeSilver, D., 516, 517 Desmond, N., 375 De Strooper, B., 468, 469, 493 Devine, T. A. J., 112 de Vries, B., 357 De Wolff, M. S., 160 Dey, S., & Ghosh, J., 369 Diamond, A., 43, 131 Diamond, L. M., 300 Diaz, A., 375 Diaz, D., 547 DiBello, K. K., 564 Dickens, B. M., 545 Dickinson, G. E., 570 Dick-Read, G., 68 Di Domenico, S., 340 Diego, M., 72 Diehl, M., 437 Diemer, M. A., 302 Diersch, N., 439 Di Fabio, A., 389 DiGiacomo, M., 574–575 Dijkstra, J. K., 244 Dik, B., 388 Dillaway, H., 428 Dilworth-Anderson, P., 512 di Masi, M. N., 395 Di Masso, A., 416 Dionne, R., 375 DiPietro, J. A., 55, 58 Dirks, M. A., 235 Dishion, T. J., 170, 245 Dishman, R. K., 503 Divan, H. A., 61 Diversi, M., 295 Dixon, J., 416 Dixon, P., 375 DiYanni, C., 128 Do, K. A., 375 Dobbins, M., 308 Dobkin, P. L., 513 Dobson, A. J., 528 Dodge, K. A., 228, 311, 312

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

656 N a m e I N d e x

Doerwald, F., 343, 521 Doka, K. J., 524, 525 Dolenszky, E., 243 Domina, T., 305 Dommaraju, P., 362 Donaldson, C. D., 307 Donaldson, W. V., 520 Donne, J., 568–569 Donnellan, M. B., 393 D’Onofrio, B. M., 58, 59, 64 Doohan, E.-A. M., 381 Doris, J. M., 349 Dorn, L. D., 266 Dörner, J., 443 Doss, B. D., 377 Doubeni, C. A., 462 Doumen, S., 293 Dove, H., 99 Dovey, T. M., 90 Dowd, J. J., 348 Doyle, J. M., 294 Dozier, M., 161 Drapeau, S., 238 Drew, L. M., 524 Driscoll, A. K., 230 Drury, L., 394, 517 Ducharme, F., 531 Duckworth, A. L., 199 Duff, L. J., 220 Duggan, P. M., 275 Dulac, M. C., 480 DuMont, K. A., 240 Dunbar, K. N., 278 Duncan, G., 201 Duncan, L. E., 446 Duncanson, S., 133 Dunham, F., 146 Dunham, P. J., 146 Dunkley, L., 367, 522 Dunlop, W. L., 347 Dunn, J., 112, 167, 235, 236, 239 Dunphy-Lelii, S., 111 Dunsmore, J. C., 167, 487 Dunson, D. B., 59 Dupuis, S., 377 Durgadevi, P., 52 Durik, A. M., 71 Durmysheva, Y., 485 Durst, D., 378 Dutta, D., 466 Dutton, W. H., 359 Duval-Tsioles, D., 383 Dye, L., 219 Dykas, M. J., 160, 296

Eagleton, S. G., 234 Eagly, A. H., 179, 184 Earle, S., 573 Earls, F. J., 312 Easterbrook, M. A., 107 Eaton, D. K., 298 Ebberwein, C. A., 407 Ebersole, P., 424 Ebstein, R. P., 48 Eccles, J. S., 220, 268 Eddleston, K. A., 393 Edelson, L. R., 90 Edelstein, B., 483 Edmondson, D., 512 Edwards, C. A., 246 Edwards, L. A., 94 Edwards, M. R., 69, 412 Edwards, P. E., 179 Egeland, B., 233, 312

Ehlert, U., 430 Ehrenberg, M., 383 Ehrlich, K. B., 296 Eiden, R. D., 64 Eime, R. M., 220, 274 Eisenberg, M. E., 301 Eisenberg, N., 173, 174, 175, 296 Eisma, M. C., 565 Eizenman, D. R., 107 Ekerdt, D. J., 464, 531 Elbert, T., 95 Eley, T. C., 50 Elkind, D., 291, 292 Ellis, A. W., 210 Ellis, B. J., 48, 267 Ellis, J. W., 452 El Nokali, N. E., 216 Else-Quest, N. M., 179, 180 Emerson, R. W., 135 Emery, R. E., 269 Endenburg, N., 170 Engel, S. M., 58 Engelberg, A., 365 English, D., 309–310 English, T., 519, 521 Englund, M. M., 200, 233, 312 Enslin, C., 394 Ensminger, M. E., 308 Ensor, R., 167 Erickson, K. I., 435 Ericsson, K. A., 220, 439, 486 Erikson, E. H., 12–13, 13, 21, 156–157,

290, 321, 323, 508, 509 Erikson, J. M., 508 Esch, T., 434, 513 Eskritt, M., 192 Eslinger, J. G., 452 Espy, K. A., 59 Etezadi, S., 487 Evans, A. D., 133 Evans, D. G. R., 477 Evans, G. D., 374 Evans, J. St. B. T., 277 Even-Zohar, A., 524, 525 Everaerts, D., 191 Evertsson, M., 409 Extremera, N., 197 Eyler, F. D., 59

Faber, A. J., 383 Fabes, R. A., 168 Fabius, C. D., 451 Fagard, J., 101 Fajgenbaum, D., 572 Falbo, T., 238 Fang, F., 111 Fänge, A. M., 505 Farr, R. H., 300 Farrant, B. M., 112, 175 Farrington, D. P., 312 Farris, D. N., 447, 448 Farver, J. M., 169 Fasig, L. G., 110 Faubion, S. S., 429 Faucher, E. H., 552 Faux, A. L., 108 Favre, B., 325 Fazel, S., 71 Feigenson, L., 135 Fein, E., 359 Feinberg, M. E., 236 Fekkes, D., 362 Feldman, H. M., 179 Feldman, K., 447

Feldman, R., 82 Felix, E., 528 Felkey, A. J., 380–381 Felling, A. J. A., 523 Feltz, A., 555 Feng, Z., 523–524 Fenson, L., 142, 144 Ferguson, R., 306 Fergusson, D. M., 58 Fernández-Alcántara, M., 567 Fernández-Berrocal, P., 197 Fernández-Carro, C., 532 Fernyhough, C., 138, 170 Ferrari, M., 487 Ferrarini, T., 410 Ferreol-Barbey, M., 211 Ferrero, J., 269 Festini, S. B., 469 Fidler, E., 42 Fiebach, C. J., 339–340 Field, M. J., 550 Field, T., 69, 72 Field, T. M., 149 Fierros, E., 197 Fifer, W. P., 84 Figueiredo, B., 381 Filisetti, L., 174 Fincham, F. D., 371, 381 Fine, M., 295 Fingerman, K. L., 320, 447, 519 Finkbeiner, N. W. B., 496 Finkenauer, C., 567 Finnie, V., 172 Finn-Stevenson, M., 202 Fiori, K. L., 356 Fischer-Shofty, M., 431 Fisher, C., 144 Fisher, H., 362 Fisher, J. E., 505 Fisher, L. L., 428, 430 Fiske, S. T., 343 Fitzgerald, J., 213 Fitzgerald, M., 343 Fitzsimons, A. M., 573 Fivush, R., 133 Flap, H., 359 Flavell, J. H., 125, 190, 193 Fleming, P. J., 83 Fletcher, J., 112 Fletcher, P. C., 447 Flores, R. A., 486 Flynn, J. R., 200 Fogarty, K., 374 Fogel, A., 164 Folkman, S., 431, 432 Forbes, D., 179 Foreman, G., 11 Formosa, M., 439 Fortin, M., 375 Foshee, V. A., 301 Foster, A., 520 Foster, B. P., 404 Foster, E. M., 250 Foster, S. E., 308 Foster, S. H., 150 Fouad, N., 389, 392 Fowler, F., 230 Fowler, K. L., 554 Fox, M. D., 545 Fox, M. J., 495 Fox, N. A., 174 Fox, S. E., 94 Fozard, J. L., 325 Fraley, R. C., 346

Franceschi, K. A., 573 Frank, M. G., 433, 437 Frank, N., 378 Franken, A., 244 Franken, N., 570 Franklin, A., 150 Franses, P. H., 485 Frase, P., 412 Frazier, J., 11 Frazier, L. D., 347 Fredricks, J. A., 220 Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I., 367, 522 Freitas, A. A., 467 French, S. E., 293 Freud, S., 12 Fried, A., 308 Friedemann-Sánchez, G., 449 Friedrich-Mai, P., 414 Friesen, M. D., 381 Frisch, B., 423 Frith, C. D., 343 Frith, U., 343 Froehlich, D. E., 405 Frohlinger, C., 400 Fruhauf, C. A., 450, 451 Fu, V. R., 230 Führer, A., 361 Fujimoto, K., 308 Fuligni, A. J., 291, 293 Fuller-Thompson, E., 451 Fullerton, C. S., 572 Fung, H. H., 348 Furman, W., 244, 297, 298 Furrow, J. L., 284 Furukawa, E., 166

Gaddis, A., 268 Gagliardi, A., 70 Gagne, J. R., 180 Gagnon, C., 294 Galán, C. A., 311 Galichon, A., 367 Galle, M. E., 139 Galloway, J. C., 99 Galperin, A., 358 Galván, A., 94 Gamaldo, A. A., 437 Gamba, F., 561 Gamble, W. C., 375 Gandhi, M., 175 Gandy, S., 469, 492 Ganong, L., 377 Gans, D., 448 Ganzini, L., 545 Garber, J., 309 Garby, A., 524, 525 Garcia, J. R., 360 Garcia Coll, C., 294 Garciaguirre, J. S., 97 Gardiner, P. A., 528 Gardner, H., 196–197, 305 Gardner, M., 221, 274 Garnier, H., 184 Garrard, E., 545 Garrido, M. M., 547 Garthe, R. C., 301 Gartstein, M. A., 85, 86 Garvey, C., 149 Gary, H. E., 128 Gass, K., 235 Gassaway, L., 367, 522 Gau, J., 273 Gaudron, J.-P., 414 Gavin, L. A., 244

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 657

Gdula, J. A., 233 Ge, X., 269 Geary, D. C., 157, 183, 199 Geerdts, M. S., 128 Gefen, D., 357 Gelles, R. J., 364 Gelman, G., 129 Gelman, R., 135 Gelman, S. A., 126, 128, 144, 177, 178,

181, 254 Gentile, D., 252 George, L. K., 513 Germanotta, S. J. A., 348 Gerry, D. W., 108 Gershoff, E. T., 232, 311–312 Gervais, S. J., 397 Geurts, S. A. E., 416 Ghanim, D., 364 Ghetti, S., 133–134, 193 Ghosh, S., 449 Gibbs, J. C., 282 Gibran, K., 359, 388 Gibson, E., 105 Gibson, K. R., 91 Gilbert, K., 389 Gilbreth, J. G., 239 Gildengers, A. G., 435 Gilding, M., 357, 520 Giles, J. W., 177 Gilleard, C., 21, 464 Gilligan, M., 520 Gilliver, M., 473 Gillmore, J., 255 Gilman, J. M., 329 Gilmer, D. F., 325, 422, 423, 502–503 Gilsanz, V., 423 Gilson, L. L., 393 Giménez-Dasi, M., 170 Gino, F., 543, 561 Giordano, G., 198 Giordano, P. C., 301, 358, 363 Giovanello, K. S., 483 Gire, J., 543, 549, 563 Givertz, M., 370 Gizer, I. R., 208 Gladwell, M., 486 Glaser, R., 433 Glassner, A., 278 Glenmullen, J., 555 Glenn, J., 507 Glick, G. C., 244 Glück, J., 487 Glusman, M., 143 Gmeindl, L., 482 Godfrey, D., 367, 522 Godleski, S. A., 179 Goeke-Morey, M. C., 247–248 Golant, S. M., 505 Gold, J. M., 383 Goldbach, J. T., 307 Goldberg, A. E., 378 Goldenberg, J. L., 552 Goldfield, B. A., 144 Goldin-Meadow, S., 95, 142, 150 Goldsmith, H. H., 86, 167, 180 Goldstein, M. H., 142 Goleman, D., 343 Golinkoff, R. M., 141, 143, 144, 149 Golkar, A., 395 Golombok, S., 52 Gondoli, D. M., 296 Gonyea, J. G., 450, 451 Gonza, G., 405 Good, T. L., 216

Goodman, M., 175 Goodman, S. H., 69 Goodnow, J. J., 230 Goodsell, T. L., 365 Goodwin-Smith, I., 495 Goodwyn, S. W., 142 Gordon, B. N., 133 Gordon, L. U., 431 Gordon, R., 404 Gordon, R. A., 58 Gordon, T. A., 563 Gordon-Salant, S., 325 Gore, P. A., 304 Gorman, J. M., 434 Gornick, J. C., 412 Gosso, Y., 169 Gostin, L. O., 546 Gottman, J. M., 231, 379 Gough, P. B., 211 Gousse, M., 367 Graber, J. A., 269 Grady, C., 482 Graef, R., 268 Graham, E. K., 443 Graham, S., 212, 213, 248, 249 Graham, S. A., 150 Graham-Bermann, S. A., 312 Grammer, J., 193 Grammer, J. K., 193 Granbom, M., 505, 529 Grande, A., 196 Granrud, C. E., 106 Grant, L. M., 445 Graves, J. M., 305 Graves, R., 141 Grawitch, M. J., 389 Gray-Little, B., 295 Greenberg, J., 552 Greenberg, M. T., 71, 312 Greenberger, E., 230 Greene, D., 574 Greene, S. M., 376, 381 Greenfield, E. A., 357–358, 517, 520,

523, 524 Greenough, W. T., 94 Greenwald, A. G., 177 Greer, D. M., 544 Gregory, A. M., 309 Greitemeyer, T., 357 Gretebeck, R. J., 527 Greving, K. A., 371 Griesemer, B. A., 274, 275 Griffin, B., 394 Griffin, M. L., 395 Griffith, J., 531 Grigorenko, E. L., 198, 203, 334 Gripshover, S. J., 90 Grizenko, N., 58 Groh, A., 505 Groh, A. M., 160, 172 Gross, J. J., 168 Grosse, G., 150 Grossman, I., 487 Grossmann, T., 94, 166 Grotevant, H. D., 236–237 Gruenewald, T. L., 445 Grühn, D., 342, 443, 446 Grunow, D., 409 Grusec, J. E., 172, 228, 295 Grysman, A., 179 Gu, X., 343 Gubernskaya, Z., 373 Gudmundsdottir, M., 428 Gudmundson, J. A., 86

Guerrero, A. P. S., 363 Guitton, M. J., 359 Gulish, A., 322–323 Gunderson, E. A., 135 Gunes, S., 430 Güngör, D., 296 Gunnell, D., 407 Güntürkün, O., 102 Guo, J., 347, 444, 445 Gupta, S., 304 Gurucharri, C., 255 Gustafson, K., 382 Gutchess, A. H., 343 Gutiérrez, I. T., 570

Haase, C. M., 349 Hafdahl, A. R., 295 Hagestad, G. O., 20, 346 Haier, R. J., 339, 340 Haight, B. K., 508–509 Haight, B. S., 508–509 Hains, C.-A. M., 545, 555 Haist, F., 94 Håkansson, P., 357 Hakeem, G. F., 84 Halberda, J., 135 Hale, L., 271 Hales, S., 550, 562 Hale-Smith, A., 512 Haley, W. E., 448, 449, 523, 562 Halford, J. C. G., 90 Halgunseth, L. C., 230 Halim, M. D., 183 Halim, M. L., 170 Hall, E. V., 393 Hall, J. A., 356, 358 Hall, S. S., 319, 369 Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., 181 Hallford, D., 508 Halliwell, D., 570 Halpern, D. F., 179 Halpern, L. F., 84 Halpern-Meekin, S., 367 Halverson, C. F., 182 Hambrick, D. Z., 439, 486 Hamilton, B. E., 59 Hamilton, R. K. B., 343 Hamm, J. V., 244 Hammar, M., 330, 477 Hammer, L. B., 305, 409 Hance, V. M., 389 Handren, L. M., 307 Hannon, E. E., 104 Hansal, A., 370 Hansen, J.-I. C., 414 Hansen, S. R., 359 Hansson, R. O., 574 Harden, K. P., 49, 298 Hareven, T. K., 20, 447 Haria, P., 212 Hariri, A. R., 311 Harley, D. A., 367, 522, 532–533 Harrington, K. V., 393 Harris, D. L., 562, 563, 568 Harris, K. R., 213, 294 Harris, P. L., 143 Harrison, K., 272 Harrist, A., 271 Hart, W., 358 Harter, S., 110, 294, 295 Hartman, H., 399 Hartung, P. J., 389, 391 Hartup, W. W., 171, 172, 244 Harvey, I. S., 512

Haselton, M. G., 358 Hashimoto, K., 489 Haslam, S. A., 400 Hassan, Z., 365, 366 Hassed, C. S., 513 Hatania, R., 131 Hatton, D., 208–209 Hawley, P. H., 246 Haworth, J., 404 Hay, D. F., 179, 248 Hayden, E. C., 66 Hayes, C. D., 179 Hayflick, L., 461, 466 Hayne, H., 132 Hayslip, B., Jr., 451, 452, 483 Hayward, M. D., 521 Hayward, R. D., 513 Hazell, P. L., 208 Health.gov, 330, 477 Heath, C., 512 Heathcote, D., 482 Hechtman, L., 208 Heck, A., 166 Heckhausen, J., 349, 503 Hedgepath, A., 402 Hegewisch, A., 399 Heilman, K. M., 485 Heine, S. J., 442 Heinmiller, B. M., 104 Heinzel, S., 482 Heller, S. B., 306 Helson, R., 443 Hembacher, E., 193 Hemer, S. R., 543 Hemphill, S. A., 307 Henderson, H. A., 86 Henderson, N. D., 200 Henderson, S., 357, 520 Hendriks, A. A. J., 442 Hendriks, G.-J., 491 Hendry, M., 546 Henkens, K., 516 Hennig, K. H., 283 Henrie, J. A., 553 Hensch, T. K., 139 Henson, R., 246 Heo, J., 416 Hepper, P., 56 Heppner, R. S., 400 Herlitz, A., 179, 200 Herman, M., 295 Hernández, B., 416 Hernández, V., 395 Hernandez-Reif, M., 72 Heron, J., 241 Heron, M., 275, 325 Herrenbrueck, L., 359 Herrnstein, R. J., 202 Hershey, D. A., 516 Hertzog, C., 413, 414, 484 Hespos, S. J., 128 Hess, P., 424 Hess, T. M., 344–345 Heydens-Gahir, H. A., 19 Heyman, G. D., 177, 254 Hiatt, K., 557 Hiatt Racer, K., 343 Hickson, L., 475 Higashibara, F., 166 Higgs, P., 21, 464 Hilden, K., 194 Hilger, K., 339–340 Hill, B., 416 Hill, J. L., 72

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

658 N a m e I N d e x

Hill, N. E., 181 Hill, P. L., 275 Hill, R. T., 410 Hills, L., 445 Hills, W. E., 451 Hillsdon, M., 414 Hill-Soderlund, A. L., 164 Hilty, D. M., 413 Hindman, A. H., 146 Hines, M., 181, 183 Hinton, A., 393 Hipp, L., 410 Hirsh-Pasek, K., 143, 144 Hitler, A., 129 Hodes, G. E., 489 Hodnett, E. D., 69 Hoeppner, B. B., 327 Hofer, J., 445 Hofer, M. A., 158 Hofer, S. M., 339, 437 Hoff, E., 144, 145 Hoff, E. L., 141, 145, 148, 149 Hoff-Ginsberg, E., 142 Hoffman, E., 450 Hoffman, M. L., 284 Hofhuis, J., 400 Hogan, A. M., 71 Hogge, W. A., 62 Holden, G. W., 228 Holland, J. M., 303–304, 390, 564, 575 Holland, K. J., 402 Hollich, G. J., 143 Holowka, S., 141 Holton, E. F., III, 439 Holtzer, R., 481 Hom, P. W., 394 Homma, A., 375 Homma, M., 412 Hommel, B., 481 Honda-Howard, M., 412 Hong, J. S., 252 Hood, L., 150 Hooker, K., 347 Hooven, C., 231 Hopkins, B., 99 Horn, J., 439 Horn, J. L., 336, 337, 338–339, 437 Horowitz, S. H., 205 Horwitz, B. N., 359 Horwood, S., 481 Hostetler, A. J., 503 Hotz, V. J., 237 Hough, L., 199 Houston, D. M., 140 Howard, D. V., 482 Howard, J. H., Jr., 482 Howard, J. T., 393 Howard, L. W., 395 Howe, M. L., 133 Howe, N., 235 Howes, C., 169 Hoyt, A. T., 60 Hoza, B., 244 Hsiao, C.-Y., 523 Hsiao, W.-H., 357, 520 Hsu, H.-C., 509 Huang, G. C., 245 Hubbard, R. R., 375 Hudson, J. A., 179 Huffman, K. M., 434 Hughes, C., 167, 328 Hughes, J. M., 257 Hughes-Kirchubel, L., 371, 377 Hugo, V., 280

Huisman, M., 332 Huizenga, H. M., 165 Huizinga, T. W. J., 426 Hulbert-Williams, N. J., 545, 555 Hulette, A. C., 170 Hull, T. H., 366 Hulme, C., 205, 206–207, 210 Humphrey, H. E. B., 62 Humphris, G., 328 Hunt, E., 201, 203 Hunt, J. B., 343 Hunt, J. M., 393 Hunter, D., 486 Hunter, J. E., 31, 200 Huston, A. C., 161–162, 250 Huth-Bocks, A. C., 160–161 Huttenlocher, J., 145 Huynh, V. W., 291 Hwang, M. J., 400 Hwang, N., 305 Hyde, J. S., 71, 179, 180, 181 Hyer, L., 448, 449, 452

Ialongo, N., 309 Iarocci, G., 205 Ibrahim, R., 365, 366 Iervolino, A. C., 183 Igarashi, H., 503, 573 Imuta, K., 173 Ingersoll-Dayton, B., 523 Inhelder, B., 191 Inoue, M., 560 Insabella, G. M., 382 Irish, L. S., 319 Irwin, N., 387 Isaacowitz, D. M., 509 Ismail, Z., 492 Ispa, J. M., 230, 294 Israel, A. C., 240, 242 Issanchou, S., 103 Issel, I. M., 519 Iverson, J. M., 142 Ivory, B. T., 320 Iwarsson, S., 505, 528, 529 Izard, C. E., 164 Izumi, S., 558

Jacklin, C. N., 179, 181 Jackson, K., 448 Jackson, M. A., 516 Jacobson, J. L., 61, 62, 64 Jacobson, S. W., 61, 62, 64 Jagnow, C. P., 56 Jágr, J., 325 Jainer, A. K., 490 James, J., 267 James, J. B., 514 James, L., 14 James, W., 508 Jansen, B. R. J., 165 Janson, H., 86 Janssen, I. R. R., 330, 477 Jarvin, L., 198, 334 Jaswal, V. K., 143 Jayasinha, R., 518 Jayasundera, T., 322–323 Jencks, C., 376 Jenkins, C. L., 574 Jenkins, E., 213 Jenkins, J., 235 Jenkins, J. A., 531 Jenkins, K. R., 521 Jernigan, T. L., 265 Jeste, D. V., 490

Jette, A. M., 527 Jiang, W., 473 Jiang, Z., 404 Jiao, Z., 244 Jipson, J. L., 128 Joh, A., 101 John, O. P., 168 Johnson, B., 416 Johnson, E. L., 140 Johnson, H. A., 416 Johnson, K. C. G., 507 Johnson, K. E., 147 Johnson, K. J., 502 Johnson, L., 512 Johnson, M. D., 360, 367 Johnson, M. H., 94, 107 Johnson, R. M., 275, 301 Johnson, S. C., 161 Johnson, S. P., 104, 106, 107, 128 Johnson, W., 442 Johnston, K. E., 377 Johnston, L. D., 307, 308 Joiner, T., 546 Jolie, A., 476, 477 Jonas, E., 552–553 Jones, B. F., 366, 485 Jones, B. K., 445 Jones, D. C., 173 Jones, D. H., 371 Jones, E., 529 Jones, L., 241 Jones, L. C., 256 Jones-Gotman, M., 362 Jonsson, T., 493 Jopp, D., 414 Jopp, D. S., 413 Jordan, A. C., 157 Jordan, A. E., 309 Jordan, N. C., 213 Joseph, D. L., 197 Josselson, R., 393 Joy, M. E., 86 Joyner, K., 297 Joyner, M. J., 435 Juffer, F., 378 Jung, C., 446, 508 Jung, E., 51 Jung, R. E., 339, 486 Juon, H-S., 308 Jusczyk, P. W., 104, 140 Justice, L., 389 Justice, L. M., 210 Juvonen, J., 248, 249

Kahana, B., 505–506 Kahana, E., 505–506 Kahn, P. H., 128 Kaijura, H., 103 Kail, B., 50, 80, 82 Kail, L., 90 Kail, M., 50 Kail, R. V., 180, 191, 276, 277 Kalanithi, L., 524 Kalish, E., 366 Kalish, R. A., 548, 553 Kalmijn, M., 359, 382 Kam, P. K., 403 Kan, D., 412 Kaneshiro, S., 450 Kanwisher, N., 112 Kao, G., 294 Kaplan, H., 99 Kaplan, R. L., 167 Kaplan, S., 544

Kapoor, U., 397 Karam, E., 327 Karasik, L B., 98, 99 Karau, S. J., 179 Karna, A., 249 Karney, B. R., 370, 371, 380 Karniol, R., 100, 169 Karp, N., 557 Karraker, K. H., 177 Karrass, J., 164 Kassim, P. N. J., 544, 545 Kast, N. R., 301 Kastenbaum, R., 550, 551, 574 Kaszniak, A. W., 567 Katz, L. F., 231, 233 Katz-Wise, S. L., 181 Kaufman, J., 359 Kaufman, J. C., 195, 197, 198, 485 Kaufman, S. B., 197 Kavšek, M., 72, 106 Kawabata, Y., 244, 247, 248 Kay, T., 416 Kayser, K., 379 Kearney, C. A., 166 Keel, P. K., 272 Keitel, A., 149 Keith, J., 319 Kelemen, D., 128 Keller, H., 137 Kelley-Moore, J., 506 Kellman, P. J., 104, 107 Kelly, K., 403 Kelly, M., 340, 496 Kelly, P., 241 Kendall, J., 208–209 Kendrick, C., 236 Kennedy, H. P., 428 Kennedy, J. F., 518 Kenny, L. C., 58 Kersting, A., 563 Kessler, B., 210 Kessler, R. C., 431 Ketelaar, T., 358 Kettaneh, A., 532–533 Khalil, A., 59 Khashan, A. S., 58 Khurana, A., 298 Kiang, L., 293 Kiaye, R. E., 400 Kidd, E., 148, 170 Kidd, K. K., 203 Kiel, E. J., 168 Kievit, R. A., 340 Killen, M., 283 Kilson, M., 375 Kim, G., 375 Kim, H.-J., 367, 522 Kim, H. K., 380 Kim, J.-Y., 235, 236 Kim, K., 320 Kim, P., 431 Kim, S., 181 Kimmel, D., 523 King, A., 367, 522 King, M. L., Jr., 175 King, M. L., Sr., 175 King, P. E., 284 King, P. M., 320, 321, 341–342 King, S., 58, 267 King, S. V., 451 King, V., 239 Kinicki, A. J., 394, 407 Kinney, J. M., 433, 523 Kinzler, K. D., 126, 143

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 659

Kipnis, O., 298 Kippen, R., 369 Kirkpatrick, L. A., 358 Kirpensteijn, J., 170 Kisilevsky, B. S., 56 Kisker, E. E., 6 Kitchener, K. S., 320, 321, 341–342 Kivett, V. R., 451 Kivnick, H. Q., 508 Klackl, J., 552–553 Klaczynski, P. A., 278 Klahr, A. M., 231 Kleiber, D. A., 415, 416, 517 Klimczuk, A., 531 Klimstra, T. A., 293, 296 Kline, S. L., 367 Knafo-Noam, A., 173, 174 Knee, D. O., 556, 557, 558 Knight, G. P., 175 Knijn, T., 373 Knipscheer, K., 520 Knowles, M. S., 439, 440 Knyazev, G. G., 85 Ko, H.-J., 347 Kobor, M. S., 48 Kochanska, G., 86, 165 Koegl, C. J., 312 Koenig, B. L., 358 Koepke, S., 292 Koestenbaum, P., 553 Kohlberg, L., 182, 280–283 Kohn, J., 57 Kojima, G., 31 Kojola, E., 515 Kolb, B., 95 Kolb, D. M., 400 Kolberg, K. J. S., 59 Kolko, J., 406 Komaromy, C., 573 Kompier, M. A. J., 416 Kondrad, R. L., 143 Koopman-Boyden, P., 414 Kornadt, A. E., 343 Kornhaber, M., 197 Koropeckyj-Cox, T., 373 Koss, C., 464, 531 Kost, K., 299 Kotre, J. N., 444 Kotter-Grühn, D., 343 Kouros, C. D., 233 Koutamanis, M., 252 Kowal, A., 236 Kowalski, R. M., 248, 252 Kowalski, S. D., 564 Kozbelt, A., 485 Kram, K. E., 394 Kramer, L., 235, 236 Krassner, A. M., 86 Kratzer, J. M. W., 360 Krause, N., 487, 512, 513 Krebs, D., 255 Kremer-Sadlik, T., 409, 412, 413 Kreppner, J. M., 237 Kretch, K. S., 98 Krettenauer, T., 283 Kriewitz, K., 416 Krispin, O., 247 Kronbichler, M., 552–553 Krull, A. C., 531 Kübler-Ross, E., 551 Kuchirko, Y., 145 Kuhl, J., 137 Kuhn, L. J., 142 Kukutai, T. H., 376

Kulich, C., 400 Kulik, L., 370, 371, 522, 523 Kumar, V., 40 Kunnen, E. S., 293 Kunz, J. A., 486–487 Kunzmann, U., 486 Kurdek, L. A., 367 Kurtz-Costes, B., 177 Kurup, P. A., 362 Kurup, R. K., 362 Kurzban, R., 157 Kwolek, S., 486 Kypri, K., 327

LaBotz, M., 274, 275 Labouvie-Vief, G., 342, 446 Lachman, M. E., 443, 446 Lacourse, E., 311 Ladd, F., 375 Ladd, G. W., 172 LaFreniere, P., 47 Lagattuta, K. H., 167 La Greca, A. M., 166 Lahey, J. N., 403 Lahti, J., 414, 415 Lai, D. W. L., 448 Laible, D. J., 167, 295 Lake, M. A., 177 Lam, J., 90 Lamanna, M. A., 521 Lamaze, F., 68 Lamb, M. E., 134, 159–160, 247,

379, 381 Lamb, S., 464 Lambert, B. L., 64 Lambert, H. K., 95 Lambert, S., 309 Lamela, D., 381 Lampkin-Hunter, T., 377 Lamson, A., 371 Landis, M., 380, 521 Landis, T., 141 Lang, F. R., 349 Lang, J. C., 389 Langer, N., 340 Langeslag, S. J. E., 362 Langlais, M. R., 376, 377 Lansford, J. E., 230, 238 Lanzl, R. G., 572 Lapsley, D. K., 275 Larson, R., 268 Laurent, H. K., 430 Laursen, B., 244, 296 Lavallee, K. L., 278 Lavelli, M., 164 Lavner, J. A., 371, 373, 380 Lawton, C. L., 219 Lawton, L. E., 410 Lawton, M. P., 18, 22, 503, 504, 505 Layne, L., 573 Lazarus, R. S., 431, 432 Leaper, C., 171, 177, 179, 181 Lebo, P. B., 477 Ledebt, A., 99 LeDoux, J. A., 481 LeDoux, J. E., 434 Lee, C. H., 389 Lee, G. R., 574 Lee, H. J., 362 Lee, J., 193 Lee, K., 131 Lee, K. S., 448, 543 Lee, M.-D., 449 Lee, P. C. B., 404

Lee, S., 214, 215 Lee, S. A., 451 Lee, S. J., 232, 311–312 Leekam, S., 113 Leerkes, E. M., 86 Lefkowitz, E. S., 348 Legare, C. H., 128, 177 Lehning, A. J., 505, 529 Leichtentritt, R. D., 573 Lemery, K. S., 86 Lemieux, K. S., 341 Lenhart, A., 252 Lent, R. W., 304, 390, 391, 392 Leon, G. R., 442 Leos-Urbel, J., 306 LeRoux, H., 505 Lervåg, A., 206–207 Leshikar, E. D., 343 Leslie, S.-J., 129 Lesnoff-Caravaglia, G., 503 Leung, R. K., 307 Leuty, M. E., 414 Levenson, R. W., 379 Levete, S., 319 Levine, A. D., 367 Levine, I. S., 358 Levine, L. E., 110 Levine, L. J., 167, 168 Levine, R. S., 104 Levine, S. C., 95, 135, 179, 180 Levinger, G., 356 Levitt, A. G., 141 Levitt, P., 94 Levkovitz, Y., 431 Levy, B. A., 210 Levy, B. R., 430 Levy, G. D., 181 Levy, S. R., 257 LeWine, H., 327 Lewinsohn, P. M., 490 Lewis, C., 160 Lewis, D. C., 451 Lewis, J. L., 505 Lewis, J. M., 382 Lewis, J. R., 543 Lewis, M., 110, 158, 164, 165, 166, 371 Lewis, M. M., 545 Lewis, S., 404 Lewkowicz, D. J., 108 Lewontin, R., 201 Leys, D., 471 Li, C. M., 475 Li, J., 415, 567 Li, K. Z. H., 481 Li, N., 392 Li, Y., 246, 319 Liang, H., 50 Liao, D. H., 445 Liben, L. S., 177, 183, 184, 256 Libertus, K., 101 Lichtenberg, P., 532 Lichtenthal, W. G., 575 Lichter, D. T., 371 Lickliter, R., 108 Liddle, H. A., 235 Liebal, K., 143 Lieber, J., 572 Lieberman, E., 72 Light, L. L., 482 Ligtenberg, L., 360 Li-Korotky, H.-S., 475 Lilgendahl, J. P., 347 Lillard, A. S., 170 Lim, V., 234

Limb, G.E., 375, 451 Limber, S., 164 Lin, C. C., 230 Lin, W., 238 Lindberg, S. M., 179 Lindbergh, C. A., 503 Linden, D. E. J., 10, 469, 480 Linden, J. A., 532 Lindenberger, U., 19 Lindgren, L., 433 Lindsay, D. S., 133 Lindsey, E. W., 172 Linebarger, D. L., 146 Linebarger, D. N., 146 Linn, M. C., 179 Linver, M. R., 221, 274 Lipp, I., 340 Lippke, S., 435 Lips, H. M., 401 Lipsitt, L. P., 84, 131 Lips-Wiersma, M., 388 Lipton, A., 425 Liston, S., 11 Liszkowski, U., 149 Litovsky, R., 104 Littlecott, H. J., 219 Litz, B., 563 Liu, C.-C., 495 Liu, D., 254 Liu, G., 506 Liu, R. X., 238 Livesley, W. J., 253 Livingston, G., 382 Lo, M., 362 Lobliner, D. B., 256 Lobo, M. A., 99 Lobo, S. A., 98 LoBue, V., 128, 166 Lochman, J. E., 248 Loheswaran, G., 329 Loken, E., 348 Lomanowska, A. M., 359 Long, M. V., 442 Lonial, S., 404 Looney, L., 174 Lopata, H. Z., 575 Lopez, A. B., 291 López Turley, R. N., 375 Lord, K., 531 Lorde, 324 Lorenz, F. O., 516 Loretto, W., 514, 515 Loseke, D. R., 364 Loucky, J., 184 Loughlin, G. M., 84 Lovén, J., 179 Lovinger, D. M., 329 Lowrance, W., 560 Lozoff, B., 84 Lu, F. J. H., 221 Lubans, D., 220 Lubinski, D., 200, 204 Lucero-Liu, A. A., 375 Lucette, A., 513 Luders, E., 513 Ludke, R. L., 556 Luhmann, M., 564 Luke, B., 52 Lukowski, A. F., 132 Lundström, J. N., 362 Lung, F-W., 99 Luo, B., 533–534 Luo, R., 137 Luo, S., 359

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

660 N a m e I N d e x

Luong, G., 519 Lushington, K., 83 Luszcz, M. A., 495 Luszczynska, A., 435 Luthans, F., 394 Lütkenhaus, P., 110 Luxenberg, S., 366 Lyall, K., 112 Lyng, S., 320 Lyon, T. D., 134 Lyster, S-A. H., 206–207 Lyster, S. H., 210 Lytton, H., 50, 181

Ma, Y-Y., 197 Maatz, L., 402 MacArthur, C. A., 212, 213 Maccoby, E. E., 170–171, 179, 181 MacDonald, K., 47 Mace, J. H., 483 Maciejewski, P. K., 564 Mackenzie, P., 467 Mackenzie, S. H., 320 Mackie, D. M., 177 Macnamara, B. N., 486 MacWhinney, B., 148 Maddow-Zimet, I., 299 Magai, C., 521 Magee, L., 271 Magilvy, J. K., 575 Magnuson, K., 201 Maguire, E. A., 95 Mahady, G. B., 428 Mahmood, T., 52 Mahon, N. E., 309 Maitre, N. L., 94 Makel, M. C., 204 Makhijani, M. G., 179 Malberg, N. T., 572 Malkinson, R., 573, 574 Malone, M. L., 484, 495 Malone, P. S., 312 Mancini, A. D., 562 Mandara, J., 293 Mandel, D. R., 104 Mandela, N., 543 Mandemakers, J. J., 407 Mange, A. P., 43 Mange, E. J., 43 Mangelsdorf, S. C., 165 Mankus, A. M., 343 Mannering, A. M., 170 Mantler, J., 405 Manuti, A., 395 Mao, P., 271 Maple, M., 573 Maratsos, M., 148 Marcia, J., 393 Marcus, G. F., 147 Marcus, R., 327 Mares, M-L., 250 Margett, T. E., 128 Margrett, J. A., 437 Markman, E. M., 90 Markovits, H., 243 Marks, A. K., 294 Marks, N. F., 517 Markus, H., 347 Marlier, L., 103 Marschik, P. B., 102 Marsh, H. W., 294 Marsh, K., 394 Marshall, S., 575 Marsiglio, W., 374

Martens, A., 552 Martin, A. J., 294 Martin, C. L., 171, 177, 182, 183 Martin, J., 181 Martin, J. A., 59, 69 Martin, M., 442 Martin, N., 90 Martin, S. P., 366 Martinez, M. E., 73 Martínez, M. L., 234 Martin-Storey, A., 300 Martin-Uzzi, M., 383 Mash, H. B. H., 572 Mask, L., 415 Masunaga, H., 439 Masur, E. F., 145 Materne, C. J., 495 Materstvedt, L. J., 545 Mather, M., 469, 509, 510 Matheson, C. C., 169 Mathiesen, K. S., 86 Mathur, M. B., 431 Matthews, D., 148 Mattis, J., 476 Mattys, S. L., 140 Matz-Costa, C., 514 Maume, D. J., 412 Mauno, S., 234 Maurer, D., 108 Mavellia, H., 516 Maxfield, M., 550 May, P. A., 60 Maybery, M. T., 112 Mayer, H. U., 20 Mayer, J. D., 343 Mayer, R. E., 437 Maynard, A. E., 235 Maynard, B., 546, 547 Mayo Clinic, 331, 435 Mazur, E., 319 McAdams, D. P., 346, 347, 444–445 McAdams, T. A., 231 McAlaney, J., 328 McCabe, J. E., 69, 70 McCabe, M., 295 McCall, R. B., 87 McCartney, K., 49 McCarty, W. P., 395 McClinton, B. E., 514 McClung, M. R., 425 McCollum, L., 332–333 McCormick, C. B., 193 McCormick, C. M., 311 McCoy, J. K., 236 McCoyd, J. L. M., 524, 563, 572, 573 McCrae, R. R., 441, 442 McCubbin, H., 370 McCullough, B. M., 520 McCutchen, D., 213 McDaniel, M. A., 301 McDermott, J. M., 236–237 McDonald, K. L., 245 McElwain, N. L., 160 McGarry, K., 524 McGinley, M., 175 McGinty, T., 547 McGraw, M. B., 96 McGuire, S., 236 McHale, J. P., 233 McHale, S. M., 171, 234, 235, 236, 296 McKay, B., 433 McKee, A. C., 497 McKee, K. J., 330, 477 McKee-Ryan, F., 405, 406, 407

McLanahan, S., 376 McLaughlin, K. A., 95 McLean, K. C., 347 McLeod, K., 192 McLymont, R., 398 McMahon, R. J., 228 McMurray, B, 143 McMurray, B., 139, 144 McNally, R. J., 565 McNulty, M., 490, 491 McNutt, B., 575 McPherson, M. C., 532 McQuade, J. D., 311 Meck, E., 135 Medart, P., 474 Medicare.gov, 534 MedlinePlus, 428 Medwell, J., 213 Meeker, J. D., 60 Meeus, W., 291 Mehta, C. M., 244 Meijer, A. M., 371, 373 Meilaender, G., 545 Meins, E., 170 Mejia, S., 347 Melby, J. N., 231 Melby-Lervåg, M., 206, 207, 210 Meldrum, M., 556 Mellor, D., 508 Melson, G. F., 170 Meltzoff, A. N., 132, 167, 177 Menaker, R., 394 Ménard, C., 489 Mendez-Luck, C. A., 449 Mendle, J., 269, 298 Mennella, J. A., 56, 103 Mensch, B. S., 319 Mercer, T., 484 Merianos, A. L., 230 Merten, M. J., 234 Merton, T., 512 Mertz, M. E., 524, 525 Mervis, C. B., 147 Merz, E.-M., 357–358 Merz, M. Y., 555 Meschede, T., 516 Mesquita, B., 163 Messer, D., 138 Meunier, D., 483 Meyer, M., 129 Micalizzi, L., 85 Michel, A., 395 Michel, G. F., 102 Mickler, C., 443 Midgette, E., 212 Midlarsky, E., 505 Miethi, A., 356 Mietkiewicz, M.-C., 525 Miga, E. M., 233 Mikhelson, V. M., 466 Miklowski, C., 20 Miles, J., 322, 323 Mileva-Seitz, V. R., 83 Miller, A. B., 476 Miller, A. C., 544, 545 Miller, D. I., 179 Miller, D. T., 328 Miller, G. E., 233 Miller, J. G., 282 Miller, K. I., 449 Miller, L. C., 99 Miller, L. S., 503 Miller, M. M., 180 Miller, P. C., 228

Miller, P. H., 125 Miller, P. J., 570, 571 Miller, P. M., 179 Miller, R. L., 208 Miller, S. A., 256 Miller, T. W., 208 Miller, V. M., 429 Miller-Martinez, D., 519 Milligan, C., 549 Mills, T. L., 525 Minnotte, K. L., 368 Miranda, J., 310 Miranda, L-M., 486 Mischel, W., 181 Mishra, G. D., 528 Mitchell, B. A., 447, 448 Mitchell, T. B., 272 Mitteldorf, J., 467 Mize, J., 172, 231 Modecki, K. L., 238 Moeller, J. R., 545, 554 Moen, P., 393, 515, 516 Moerk, E. L., 147 Moffitt, T. E., 311 Moghaddam, B., 265 Mokdad, C., 90 Molin, P., 493 Molina, B. S. G., 208 Molina, L. C., 519 Mollborn, S., 59 Mollnow, E., 62 Molnar, D. S., 64 Monahan, A., 514, 515 Monahan, K. C., 246, 305, 313 Monden, C. W. S., 407 Mondloch, C. J., 107 Monk, C., 58 Monk, C. S., 93 Monk, T. H., 575 Montague, D. P., 166 Mont’Alvao, A., 306 Montesanto, A., 462 Moon, P. J., 558 Moone, R. P., 523 Moore, K. L., 44, 63 Moore, M. K., 132 Moorman, S. M., 357–358, 520, 523,

524, 560 Morais, M. L. S., 169 Morfei, M. Z., 347 Morgan, B., 91 Morgan, J. L., 140 Morgan, J. P., 572 Morgan, P. L., 208 Morris, P., 228 Morris, W. L., 365 Morrissey, T. W., 410 Morrow-Howell, N., 517 Mortimer, J. T., 306 Morton, J., 107 Morycz, R., 520 Moses, L. J., 167 Mosten, F. S., 382 Mõttus, R., 442 Mount, M. K., 392 Mountain, G., 161 Mounts, N. S., 172 Mouras, H., 342, 446 Moye, J., 560 Moyer, A. M., 429 Mozaffarian, D., 470 Mrayyan, M. T., 558 Mühlig-Versen, A., 443 Muise, A., 371

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 661

Mullen, C. M., 448 Muller, E. D., 563 Mulvey, K. L., 283 Munoz, R. T., 545 Murao, K., 471 Murphy, L. M., 244 Murphy, N., 138 Murray, C., 202 Mussolin, C., 134 Musson, G., 394 Mustanski, B. S., 267 Mutchler, J. E., 451, 502 Muter, V., 211 Muzio, J. N., 84 Mwanyangala, M. A., 509 Myers, M. M., 84 Mylander, C., 150

Nabe, C. M., 551 Nadig, A. S., 150 Naef, R., 564, 574 Nagaratnam, K., 474 Nagaratnam, N., 474 Nagel, S. K., 230 Nagl, M., 563 Nahemow, L., 18, 22, 503, 504 Naigles, L. G., 144 Nánez, J., Sr., 105 Narayan, A. J., 233, 312 Narciso, I., 573 Nation, K., 206, 211 Nawijn, J., 416 Nayernouri, T., 545 Neal, M. B., 409 Neblett, E. W., 293 Needham, A, 101 Neese, R. M., 565 Neft, N., 367 Nehamas, A., 356 Neimeyer, R. A., 564, 575 Neiss, M., B., 295 Nelson, C. A., 93, 94, 107 Nelson, E. L., 102 Nelson, K., 110, 142, 145 Nelson, L. J., 319 Nelson, M. A., 274 Nelson, P. T., 493 Neltner, J. H., 493 Nerenberg, L., 532 Nesdale, D., 256 Neugarten, B. L., 346, 450 Neuman, Y., 278 Neville, H. J., 131 Newberg, A. B., 513 Newbury, J., 145 Newcombe, N., 125, 126 Newcombe, N. S., 132 Newman, A., 556 Newman, B. M., 12 Newman, D. A., 197 Newman, J. P., 343 Newman, P. R., 12 Newman, R. S., 141, 146 Newport, E. L., 140 Newsom, J. T., 450 Newton, E. K., 175 Newton, N. J., 445, 446 Ng, R., 516 Nga, L., 510 Ngan, R. M. H., 403 Nguyen, S. P., 177 Ni, Y., 215 Nickerson, C. J., 321 Nicklaus, S., 103

Nicolas, G., 493 Nielsen, M. K., 562 Nies, M. A., 516 Nigg, J. T., 208 Nigro, N., 430 Nikolaidis, A., 339 Nilsen, E. S., 150 Nimrod, G., 414 Nisbett, R. E., 200, 201 Nocita, G., 170 Norris, D. R., 405, 406 Norström, T., 410 Norton, M. I., 543, 561 Nosek, M., 428 Nugent, J. K., 81 Nunes, T., 211 Nurius, P., 347 Nurmi, J.-E., 230 Nurmsoo, E., 143 Nuzum, D., 573 Nyad, D., 325 Nyaradi, A., 90 Nyberg, L., 433 Nye, C. D., 304 Nylenna, M., 394

Oakhill, J. V., 211, 212 Oaten, M., 164 Obama, B., 406 Obergefell, J., 369 O’Brien, B. J., 466 O’Brien, M., 86 Oburu, P. O., 452 Ochs, E., 409, 412, 413 Ochsner, K. N., 168 Ocklenburg, S., 102 O’Connor, M.-F., 567 O’Donoghue, M., 376 Oedekoven, C. S. H., 483 Ogden, C. L., 271 O’Hara, M. W., 69, 70 Ojanen, T., 295 Okonski, B., 573 O’Leary, K. D., 363, 364 Olinghouse, N. G., 212, 213 Oliveira, B. S., 466 Oliveira, T. C., 404 Olsen, B., 167 Olsen, J. A., 230 Olshaker, J. S., 532 Olson, A. M., 523 Olson, D. H., 370 Olson, I. R., 132 Olson, K. R., 300 Olson, S. L., 168, 234–235 Olszewski-Kubilius, P., 204 Oltjenbruns, K. A., 571, 572 Omori, M., 410 O’Neal, C. W., 516 O’Neil, R., 172, 230 O’Neill, D. K., 150 O’Neill, K., 564–565 Ontai, L. L., 167 Ooi, L. L., 170 Oostenbroek, J., 132, 343 Opfer, J. E., 128, 130, 134 Ordy, J. M., 474 Orel, N. A., 367, 450, 522 O’Riley, A., 483 Oriña, M. M., 298 Ornstein, P. A., 133, 193 O’Rourke, L., 328 O’Rourke, N., 521 Ortiz-Walters, R., 393

Östgren, C. J., 330, 477 Ostrov, J. M., 179 Oswald, A. J., 509 Oswald, F. L., 199, 486 Otta, E., 169 Ottaway, A. J., 382 Oudekerk, B. A., 358 Over, H., 173 Owen, C. J., 447 Ownby, R. L., 484

Pace, A., 230 Packer, D. J., 348 Paech, J., 435 Paggi, M. E., 414, 415 Paikoff, R., 298 Pajares, F., 193 Palkovitz, R., 374 Palladino, P., 211 Palm, G., 374 Palmer, A. M., 436 Palmer, B. W., 490 Palmérus, K., 452 Palmore, E., 513 Pals, H., 373 Pan, Z., 250 Panay, N., 429 Pantano, J., 237 Papa, A., 563, 564–565 Papp, L. M., 247–248 Parault, S. J., 193 Pargament, K. I., 512 Park, C. L., 512 Park, D. C., 469 Park, G., 200 Park, J., 449, 450 Park, Y. C., 552, 553 Parke, R. D., 172, 228 Parker, J. G., 244 Parker, L. D., 523 Parkes, C. M., 551 Parkman, A. M., 370 Parrella, N., 434, 502 Parritz, R. H., 168 Parrott, T. M., 525 Parten, M., 169 Pascalis, O., 107, 140 Pascoe, A., 565 Pascual, B., 112 Passarino, G., 462 Pasupathi, M., 347 Paterson, S. E., 503 Paterson, T. S. E., 503 Patrick, S., 371 Patry, D. A., 415 Patterson, C. J., 378 Patterson, G. R., 232 Patterson, M. M., 256, 257 Pattie, A., 29 Patton, F., 294 Patton, L. D., 321, 322 Pauker, K., 256 Paulson, D., 394 Paus, T., 92 Pausch, R., 549 Pearce, K. A., 436 Pearlin, L. I., 449 Pears, K. C., 241 Pearson, F., 377 Pearson, J., 238 Pegg, S., 415 Peiró, J. M., 516 Pelham, W. E., 208 Pellegrini, A. D., 228

Pellegrino, J. W., 180 Pellicano, E., 113 Pence, K., 210 Penson, R. T., 543 Pereira, M., 573 Perin, D., 213 Perrone, K. M., 392 Perry, D. G., 295 Perry, T. B., 244 Perry, W. I., 321 Persaud, T. V. N., 44, 63 Peter, J., 252 Peters, A. M., 147 Peters, H. E., 366 Peterson, C. C., 111 Peterson, L., 174 PetersonLund, R. R., 320 Petitto, L. A., 141 Petrill, S. A., 47 Pettit, G. S., 172, 228, 231 Pfaff, D. W., 362 Pfoff, M. K., 575 Phillips, K. W., 393 Phillips, L. R., 532 Phinney, J. S., 293, 294 Phipps, M. G., 299 Piaget, J., 15, 15–16, 21, 117, 118–129,

190, 191, 199 Pickering, C. E. Z., 532 Piehler, T. F., 245 Pienta, A. M., 521 Pillay, H., 403 Pincus, T., 332–333 Pineda-Pardo, J. A., 339, 340 Pinette, M. G., 69 Pinto, K. M., 410 Piolat, A., 211 Piquero, A. R., 311, 313 Piquet, B. J., 358 Pisoni, D. B., 104 Place, S., 145 Plaumann, M., 396 Plewis, I., 82, 83 Plomin, R., 46, 47, 49, 87, 200 Plucker, J. A., 197 Plunkett, K., 130 Poehlmann, J., 72 Poldrack, R. A., 470 Polit, E. F., 238 Polivka, L., 533–534 Pomerantz, E. M., 215 Pons, F., 170 Poole, D. A., 133, 134 Poon, L. W., 442 Pope, T. M., 551 Popenoe, D., 366 Popper-Giveon, A., 393 Porter, K. E., 367, 522 Porter, R. H., 103 Portnoy, A., 558 Posner, M. I., 513 Potočnik, K., 516 Poulin, F., 245 Poulson, C. L., 142 Powdthaveea, N., 509 Powell, B., 375 Power, T. L., 553 Pratt, H. D., 393 Prentice, D. A., 328 Pressley, M., 193, 194, 212 Preston, C., 216 Priaulx, N., 545 Price, T. S., 60 Price, V., 397

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

662 N a m e I N d e x

Priess, H. A., 181 Prieto-Ursúa, M., 395 Prigerson, H. G, 567 Prince-Paul, M., 556 Prinstein, M. J., 246 Prot, S., 250 Proulx, C. M., 370, 521, 523 Provins, K. A., 102 Pruett, M. K., 382 Pryor, L., 271 Puhl, R. M., 273 Pullen, P. C., 210 Pullen, S. M., 344–345 Pushkar, D., 487 Pynoos, J., 503, 529, 531 Pyszczynski, T., 552, 553

Quakenbush, C., 535 Qualls, S. H., 488 Quas, J. A., 134 Queen, T. L., 560 Quinn, J. F., 514, 515, 516 Quinn, P. C., 107 Quirin, M., 552

Raab, G. M., 523–524 Raabe, T., 256 Racz, S. J., 228 Raikes, H., 210 Rakic, P., 92 Ralph, L. J., 299 Ramakumar, S., 375 Ramalingam, M., 52 Randall, A. K., 367 Rangul, V., 220 Rankin, C. T., 367 Rao, U., 309 Raschick, M., 523 Ratai, E., 112 Ratib, O., 423 Ratner, N. B., 141 Raudenbush, S. W., 142 Ravid, D., 148 Rawlins, W. K., 519 Ray, M., 489, 491 Reardon, S., 66 Redzanowski, U., 487 Reed, A. E., 521 Reese, E., 146 Regev, I., 515 Rehan, W., 241 Reich, P. A., 142 Reid, R., 213 Reimer, M. S., 165 Reiser, L. W., 268 Remedios, J. D., 348 Ren, Q., 460 Renshaw, K. D., 371 Repacholi, B. M., 167 Repetti, R. L., 412 Repovs, G., 343 Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., 482 Revenson, T. A., 523 Reyes, H. L., 301 Reynolds, D., 548 Reznick, J. S., 144 Rhea, S.-A, 200 Rhoades, G. K., 377 Rhoades, K. A., 233 Rhodes, M., 178, 256 Rideout, V., 250, 251, 252 Ridings, C., 357 Riedmann, A., 521 Riggs, D. S., 371, 377

Riggs, M. L., 381 Riina, E. M., 296 Rijken, A, J., 373 Riley, M. W., 19 Rindermann, H., 201 Ripp, J., 529 Ritchie, K. L., 228 Rittle-Johnson, B., 214 Rivas-Drake, D., 293 Rivera, D., 486 Rizzuto, T. E., 481 Robbins, A., 323 Roberto, K. A., 356, 357, 358, 450, 519,

520, 525, 532 Roberto, M., 329 Roberts, A. E., 546 Roberts, B. W., 346, 442, 443, 444 Roberts, J. E., 572 Roberts, K. P., 133 Roberts, R. M., 327 Roberts, S., 129 Robertson, C. E., 112 Robertson, I., 415 Robinaugh, D. J., 565 Robins, S., 210 Robinson, J., 257 Robinson, M., 135 Robinson, O. C., 323 Robinson, S. R., 99 Robinson-Wood, T., 367 Robson, S. M., 394 Roby, A. C., 170 Rocissano, L., 150 Rockwood, K., 493, 526, 527 Rocque, G. B., 556 Rodeck, C. H., 66 Rodrigues, C., 371 Rodriguez-Galán, M. B., 375 Roehling, P., 393 Roffwarg, H. P., 84 Rogers, L. A., 213 Rogoff, B., 137 Rohde, P., 273 Rohmann, E., 361 Rohr, R., 512 Rollins, N. C., 89 Romney, D. M., 181 Roosevelt, F. D., 533 Rosales, N., 210 Rose, A. J., 171, 243, 244, 245, 298 Rose, D. M., 404 Rosenbaum, J. L., 573 Rosenblatt, P. C., 564 Rosengren, K. S., 570, 571 Rosinski, P., 389 Ross, H., 181 Ross, H. S., 235 Ross, J., 110 Ross, N. A., 528 Rosso, B. D., 388 Rosta, J., 394 Rostila, M., 564 Roszko, E., 543 Roth, J. L., 221, 274 Rothbart, M. K., 85, 167 Rothenhaus, T. C., 532 Rounds, J., 391, 392 Roussey, J., 211 Rovee-Collier, C., 131, 132 Rowe, M. L., 141, 142, 145 Rowe, M. M., 395 Rowles, G. D., 528 Rowling, J. K., 212 Roxburgh, S., 409

Roy, K., 374 Rubin, K. H., 169, 245, 247, 248 Rubin, S. S., 573 Ruble, D. N., 177, 182, 183, 267 Rudd, M. D., 310 Rudolph, K. D., 171 Rudy, D., 230 Rueda, M. R., 167 Rueger, S. Y., 296 Ruhm, C. J., 409 Ruiz, M. E., 531 Ruppanner, L., 412 Ruscher, J. B., 416 Rushton, J. P., 244 Russell, A., 172 Russell, J. A., 165 Russell, L. T., 377 Russell, S. T., 230 Russo, S. J., 489 Rutland, A., 283 Ryan, A. M., 244 Ryan, E. G., 327 Ryan, M. K., 400 Rye, M. S., 381

Saarento, S., 249 Sabatino, C. P., 560 Sacker, A., 308 Saewyc, E. M., 300 Saffran, J. R., 140 Sagi, A., 161 Sahni, R., 84 Saint-Georges, C., 141 St. George, I. M., 267 St. James-Roberts, I., 82, 83 Sais, E., 145 Sakaki, M., 510 Sakraida, T. J., 381 Sakyi, K. S., 245 Sala Frigerio, C., 468, 469, 493 Salamoun, M., 327 Salmivalli, C., 249 Salomaa, R., 389 Salovey, P., 343 Salthouse, T. A., 480 Samanta, T., 503 Sameroff, A. J., 234–235 Samuelson, L. K., 143, 144 Sánchez-Álvarez, N., 197 Sandberg, S., 401 Sanders, M., 233 Sandler, I. N., 233, 239, 565–566 Sandman, C. A., 58 Sandoval, K., 451 Sanford, L., 472 Sangrigoli, S., 108 Sann, C., 108 Sanson, A., 179 Santilli, S., 389, 391 Santo, J. B., 245 Sargent, C. S., 321 Sargent, L. D., 514, 515 Saroglou, V., 283 Saudino, K. J., 85, 179 Saunders, C., 556 Savci, E., 378 Savela, S., 466 Savelsbergh, G. J. P., 99, 100 Savickas, M. L., 389, 390 Saxe, G. B., 198 Sbarra, D. A., 357, 381 Scantlebury, N., 277 Scardamalia, M., 212 Scarr, S., 49

Schaal, B., 103 Schacter, D. L., 483 Schaie, K. W., 336–337 Scharlach, A. E., 505, 529 Scheele, D., 362 Scheff, S. W., 493 Scheibe, S., 486 Scheidt, R. J., 528, 529 Scheier, M. F., 357 Schein, S. S., 570 Schermerhorn, A. C., 228, 234–235 Schindler, O., 282 Schlabach, S., 376 Schlomer, G. L., 374 Schmidt, F. L., 31, 200 Schmitt, D., 360, 361 Schneider, J., 573 Schneider, M., 214 Schneider, S., 359 Schneider, W., 193, 277 Schneiders, J., 309 Schoeni, R. F., 524 Schofield, T., 238 Schoklitsch, A., 445 Schoon, I., 391 Schott, W., 409 Schoulte, J., 563 Schuetze, P., 64 Schultz, D. P., 442 Schultz, S. E., 442 Schulz, D. J., 394 Schulz, R., 503, 520 Schulz-Helk, R. J., 496 Schut, H., 550, 564 Schutz, R. W., 220, 265 Schüz, B., 330, 477 Schwab, A. K., 357 Schwade, J. A., 142 Schwanenflugel, P. J., 193 Schwartz, A. E., 306 Schwartz, B., 388 Schwartz, C., 103 Schwartz, C. E., 86 Schwartz, D., 245 Schwartz, R. B., 306 Schwartz-Mette, R. A., 245 Schwarz, B., 528, 529 Schwarzer, R., 435 Schwerdtfeger, A., 414 Scott, R., 295 Scott, R. M., 111 Scott, W. A., 295 Scott-Phillips, T. C., 150 Scrimgeour, M. B., 233 Seal, J., 244 Seaman, P. M., 518 Sedikides, C., 295 Sedivy, J. C., 150 Seeman, T. E., 445 Segal, D. L., 488, 489–490, 491 Segers, M., 405 Segrin, C., 370 Sehgal, A., 476 Seidl, A., 140 Seidner, L. B., 169 Seligman, M. E., 199 Selkoe, D. J., 495 Selman, R. L., 254–255 Sels, L., 403 Seltzer, J. A., 448 Sénéchal, M., 210 Seponski, D. M., 451 Serrat, R., 508 Seward, R. R., 374

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 663

Seychell, A., 370, 371 Seyda, B. A., 573 Shafer, K., 375, 451 Shahaeian, A., 111 Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., 431 Shanafelt, T. D., 395 Shanahan, L., 236, 296 Shanahan, M. J., 306 Shannon, C. S., 415 Shao, J., 488 Share, D. L., 211 Shariff, A. F., 163 Sharma, A., 397 Sharma, M., 434 Sharma, V., 397 Shastri, T., 404 Shaw, D. S., 312 Shaw, H., 273 Shaw, S. S., 573 Shayshon, B., 393 Shell, R., 174 Shelleby, E. C., 312 Shen, H., 402 Shen, S., 128 Sheridan, M. A., 95 Sherlock, H., 395 Sherman, D. K., 203 Sherwin, S., 545 Sheu, H., 390 Shi, R., 140 Shim, H. S., 471 Shin, H., 244 Shin, Y. L, 169 Shiner, R., 442 Shipp, A. J., 403 Shoemaker, C., 357 Shoji, K., 395 Shonkoff, J. P., 433 Shore, J. C., 562 Shouldice, A., 164 Shrager, J., 213 Shrestha, S., 166 Shrout, P. E., 97 Shu, B-C., 99 Shulman, S., 298 Shuster, M., 166 Shutts, K., 177, 183 Sidebotham, P., 241 Siegler, R. S., 125, 126, 130, 134, 135,

190, 211, 212, 213 Sieving, R. E., 301 Silbereisen, R. K., 269 Silk, J., 296 Silk, J. S., 230 Silva, L. C. R., 466 Silva, P. A., 267 Silver, M. P., 514, 515 Silver, N., 379 Silverman, W. K., 166 Silvers, J. A., 168 Silverstein, M., 524 Simon, T., 198–199 Simons, B. S., 231, 395 Simonton, D. K., 204, 485 Simpson, J. M., 84 Sinclair, R. R., 405 Sinclair, S., 365 Singh, A. M., 400 Singh, D., 440 Singh, J. A., 426, 469 Singh, S., 319 Sinnott, J. D., 341 Sippola, L. K., 244 Siu, T. M. Y., 348

Skinner, B. F., 13–14, 14, 21, 147 Skinner, E. A., 168, 230 Skogan, W. G., 395 Skoglund, R. R., 525 Skolnick, A. A., 550 Skoog, T., 269 Skouteris, H., 410 Skrajner, M., 496 Slater, A., 107 Slater, A. M., 128 Slaug, B., 505 Slaughter, V., 112, 256 Sleddens, E. F. C., 271 Sleeter, C. E., 6 Slobin, D. I., 147 Slobodskaya, H. R., 85 Slomkowski, C., 235 Smetana, J. G., 296 Smith, D. T., 410 Smith, E. R., 177 Smith, J., 464, 465, 526 Smith, J. A., 323 Smith, J. R., 573 Smith, L. B., 96, 131, 144 Smith, R. S., 72 Smith, S. M., 553 Smith, T. E., 179 Smith, W. J., 393 Smith-Bynum, M. A., 375 Smith-Howell, E. R., 556 Smits, S. J., 393 Smoll, F. L., 220, 265 Smucker, D. R., 556 Smyer, M. A., 488, 514 Smyke, A. T., 378 Snell, E. K., 146 Snow, C. W., 82 Snow, D., 141 Snow, M. E., 181 Snowling, M. J., 205, 206, 207 Snyder, L., 145 Sobal, J., 271 Soley, G., 104 Solomon, G. E. A., 129 Sommer, S., 377 Son, C. V., 558 Song, A. V., 204 Song, L., 145 Song, S., 210 Song, Z., 407 Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., 208 Sorenson, A., 295 Sorg, S. F., 496 Soussignan, R., 103 South, K., 273 Spacek, S., 267 Speaks, S. Z., 414 Spear, M., 166 Spector, F., 108 Spector, P. E., 412 Spelke, E. S., 126, 148, 183 Spicer, J., 58 Spielberg, S., 281 Spiers, H. J., 95 Spinath, F., 49 Spinrad, T. L., 173 Spivak, D. L., 466 Spivak, I. M., 466 Spokus, D. M., 403 Sprang, G., 452 Sritharan, R., 360 Srivastava, S., 443 Staff, J., 306 Stafford, L., 367

Stalp, M. C., 416 Stamatakis, E. A., 483 Staneva, A., 58 Stanik, C. E., 296 Stanley-Stevens, L., 374 Stanovich, K. E., 277 Starr, J., 29 Stattin, H., 230, 269 Staudinger, U. M., 19, 443, 486, 487 Steck, A., 562, 564 Steck, B., 562, 564 Steeger, C. M., 296 Steele, C. M., 203 Steele, R., 272 Stein, D. J., 362 Stein, J. H., 268 Steinberg, L. D., 246, 268, 275, 296, 298,

305, 313 Stenberg, G., 167 Sternberg, K., 482 Sternberg, K. J., 247 Sternberg, R. J., 180, 195, 197–198, 203,

334, 335, 358, 482 Sterns, H. L., 403, 516, 517 Steuerle, C. E., 535 Stevens, H., 305 Stevens, N., 244 Stevens Aubrey, J., 360 Stevenson, H. W., 214, 215 Stevenson, J., 295 Stevenson, R. J., 164, 165 Stevenson-Hinde, J., 164 Stewart, A. J., 446 Stewart, J. S., 392, 393 Stewart, M. D., 373 Stewart, S. D., 521 Stice, E., 273 Stigler, J. W., 215 Stiles, J., 92, 265 Stockdale, M. S., 400 Stokes, J., 573 Stoltenberg, S. F., 174 Stoltz, K. B., 405 Stoneman, A., 236 Stoner, S., 483 Strathearn, L., 431 Strawbridge, W. J., 527 Strenze, T., 200 Streri, A., 108 Strinic, V., 545 Stroebe, M. S., 550, 562, 564, 565, 567,

570, 573, 574, 575 Stroebe, W., 550, 567, 575 Strohmeier, D., 249 Strong, C., 308 Strough, J., 179, 244 Sturge-Apple, M. L., 235 Sturman, D. A., 265 Su, J., 308 Subotnik, R. F., 204 Sugimoto-Matsuda, J. J., 363 Sugiura, M., 10, 437, 469, 470, 480 Suja, C., 467 Sullivan, L., 516 Sullivan, M. W., 164 Sullivan, T. N., 301 Sulloway, F. J., 238 Sumter, S. R., 360 Sung, S., 48 Suo, C., 435 Super, C., 390 Super, C. M., 99 Super, D., 302–303, 390–391 Supple, A. J., 308

Suri, R., 82 Suzuki, L., 203 Swain, J. E., 431 Swanson, R. A., 439 Sweeney, C., 575 Sweeney, M. M., 239 Sweeper, S., 238 Swift, H. J., 394, 517 Swift, T., 246 Swindell, R., 439 Swingle, A. L.-V., 322 Symonds, W. C., 306

Tabor, J., 238 Tach, L., 367 Tager-Flusberg, H., 146 Tahiroglu, D., 170 Talbot , L. A., 415 Tam, K.-P., 552 Tamang, B. L., 166 Tamayo, G. J., 449 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., 98, 99, 137,

145, 172 Tan, K. L., 83 Tang, F., 517 Tang, P.-L., 553 Tang, Y.-Y., 513 Tangney, J., 166 Tanji, J., 436 Tanner, J. M., 264–265 Tardif, T., 142 Taylor, E., 208 Taylor, E, M., 356, 520 Taylor, J. H., 284 Taylor, J. L., 439 Taylor, M., 170, 178 Taylor, M. G., 177, 178, 181 Taylor, S. E., 343, 431 Teachman, J., 239, 375 Teaster, P. B., 532–533 Tedrow, L., 375 Tegethoff, M., 58 Tenenbaum, H. R., 181 Terkel, S., 388, 395 Terman, L., 199 Terracciano, A., 442 Teskey, G. C., 95 Thai, J., 477 Thayer, S. M., 236 Thelen, E., 96, 98 Thelen, M. H., 267 Therborn, G., 366 Thiessen, E. D., 140 Thomas, A., 85 Thomas, F., 383 Thomas, J. R., 295 Thomas, M. L., 487 Thomas, R., 242 Thompson, A. E., 180 Thompson, C. L., 563 Thompson, G. E., 451 Thompson, J., 201, 556 Thompson, R. A., 158, 160–161, 164,

167, 175 Thompson, R. J., 343 Thoreau, H. D., 175 Thornton, W. L., 503 Thorpe, H., 161 Thorsen, M. L., 239 Thuell, S., 551 Thurkettle, M. A., 321 Tiffany, D. W., 349 Tiffany, P. G., 349 Tincoff, R., 140

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

664 N a m e I N d e x

Tinker, E., 144, 148 Tobin, D. J., 422 Tolkko, T., 439 Tomasello, M., 149, 150, 173 Tomolillo, C., 357 Tones, M., 403 Tong, C., 394 Tooby, J., 163 Tordera, N., 516 Torges, C. M., 446 Toril, P., 481 Torres, D., 325 Torres, L., 558 Toth, S. L., 240 Toumbourou, J. W., 307 Tower, L. E., 409 Towne, T. J., 439 Tracey, T. J. G., 304 Tracy, B., 213 Tracy, J. L., 163 Trainor, L. J., 104, 108 Treiman, D. J., 460 Treiman, R., 210, 212 Tremblay, R. E., 311 Trim, R. S., 308 Troutman, M., 516 Trump, D., 406 Trzaskowski, M., 47 Trzesniewski, K., H., 393 Tsuno, N., 375 Tu, M. C.-H., 394 Tucker, M. S., 215 Tucker-Drob, E. M., 49 Tulkin, D. O., 410 Tullett, A., 358 Tulviste, P., 137 Tunmer, W. E., 211 Turiel, E., 178, 282 Turkheimer, E., 269 Turner, R. N., 244, 257 Twenge, J. M., 295 Tyler, L. K., 483 Tynes, B. M., 257, 293

Uchino, B. N., 520 Udry, J. R., 297 Uhlenberg, P., 382 Ukraintseva, S. V., 467 Ullian, D. Z., 182 Ulrich, B. D., 98 Umaña-Taylor, A. J., 293, 295 Unger, J. B., 308 Unger, O., 169 Updegraff, K. A., 235, 236 Ursano, R. J., 572 Usher, E. L., 193 Utman, J. A., 141 Utz, R.L., 565

Vaala, S. E., 146 Vacha-Haase, T., 520 Vaillancourt, T., 312 Vaish, A., 166, 173 Valdes, A. M., 426 Valente, T. W., 308 Valenza, E., 107 Valjean, H., 280 Valjean, J., 280 Valkenburg, P. M., 250, 252 Vallerand, R. J., 395–396 van Aken, M. A. G., 248 Vance, J. D., 406 Vandenbosch, L., 360 van den Brink, H. M., 412

van den Wijngaart, M. A. G., 523 van den Wittenboer, G. L. H.,

371, 373 van der Geest, S., 543 van der Kamp, J., 100 van der Pas, S., 414 van der Rijt, P. G. A., 400 van der Veen, F. M., 362 Vander Wal, J. S., 267 Van de Walle, G. A., 128 Van Duijvenvoorde, A. C., 165 van Dulmen, M., 358 van Goozen, S. H. M., 311 van Hof, P., 100 van IJzendoorn, M. H., 48, 86,

160, 267 van Klaveren, C., 412 van Lith, H. A., 170 van Marle, K., 128 van Muijden, J., 481 van Praag, B., 412 van Rijsweijk, L., 243 Vansteenkiste, S., 403 van Tilburg, T., 520 van Wanrooy, B., 413 van Wieringen, P. C. W., 99 Varkal, M. D., 491 Vasta, R., 147 Vautier, S., 414 Vazire, S., 349 Veenema, S., 197 Veiga, J. F., 393 Vélez, C. E., 239 Venditti, L., 525 Venkatesh, B., 496 Ventura, S. J., 59 Verbrugge, L. M., 526–527 Verghese, A., 551 Vernooij-Dassen, M. J. F. J., 523 Vickerstaff, S., 514, 515 Victora, C. G., 89 Vieno, A., 312 Villar, F., 508 Vitaro, F., 294 Vlug, M., 400 Voegtline, K. M., 55 Vogel, D. A., 177 Vogtle, L. K., 529 Volling, B. L., 235 Volpe, U., 393, 394 von Ranson, K. M., 272 Vorhees, C. V., 62 Vormittag, K., 434, 502 Vosper, J., 238 Voss, M. W., 468 Votruba-Drzal, E., 216 Vouloumanos, A., 139 Voyer, D., 179, 180 Vraneković, J., 44 Vukman, S. N., 348 Vygotsky, L., 16, 16–17, 21, 117,

136–138 Vythilingum, B., 362

Wachs, T. D., 86 Wadlington, W., 381 Wadsworth, S. M., 371, 377 Wahl, H.-W., 528, 529 Waites, C., 451 Walberg, H. J., 216 Waldfogel, J., 72, 242 Waldman, I. D., 208 Walk, R., 105 Walker, A. C., 543

Walker, J., 362 Walker, L. E. A., 363 Walker, L. J., 283, 284 Walker, P., 108 Walker, R., 61 Walker-Andrews, A. S., 166 Wallace, L. M., 272 Wallace, S. P., 519 Wallerstein, J. S., 382 Walter, C. A., 524, 563, 572, 573 Walter, U., 396 Walther, A. N., 381 Wanberg, C. R., 407 Wang, L., 247 Wang, Q., 133 Wang, S.-H., 128 Wang, S-w., 412 Wang, W., 379 Wang, W.-C., 483 Wansink, B., 271 Wanzer, S. H., 555 Ward, B. W., 73 Waren, W., 373 Warmuth, K. A., 233 Warner, B., 66 Warner, M. E., 410 Warner, S. C., 426 Warnock, F. F., 103 Warr, P., 415 Warren-Findlow, J., 519 Wasik, B. A., 146 Wasserstein, S., 166 Waterhouse, L., 197 Waters, E., 160 Waters, H. S., 212 Waters, P., 294, 295 Watkins, J. F., 528 Watkins, S., 250 Watson, J., 13, 21 Watson, R. W., 430 Watts, D., 327 Wax, J. R., 69 Waxman, S., 142 Wayland, S., 567 Weaver, C. C., 328 Weaver, D. A., 524 Weaver, J., 238 Webb, N. B., 570, 571 Webb, S. J., 93 Weber, A., 367 Weber, D., 200 Webster, J. D., 508 Webster, N. J., 516 Webster-Marketon, J., 433 Wechsler, H., 327, 328 Wegman, L. A., 398 Wegman, M. E., 84 Wei, W., 179 Weichold, K., 269 Weinstein, K. K., 450 Weinstock, M. P., 278 Weintraub, J. R., 393 Weis, R., 252 Weisgram, E. S., 177 Weisner, T. S., 184 Weiss, D., 343 Weiss, L. G., 200 Wellman, H. M., 111–112, 126, 128,

167, 254 Welsh, D. P., 297 Weltzin, T. E., 272 Wentkowski, G., 524 Wentzel, K. R., 174 Werker, J. F., 139, 140

Werner, E. E, 72 Werner, H., 98 Werner, N. E., 172 Werner, T. L., 415 Werth, J. L., Jr., 545 Wertsch, J. V., 137 West, M. D., 468 West, S. A., 157 Westerhof, G. J., 508 Westphal, M., 562 Westra, T., 99 Westrate, N. M., 487 Wettstein, R. M., 560 Whalley, L. J., 468 Whipple, N., 138 White, H., 496 White, K. D., 381 White, L., 239 White, M., 371 White, N., 167 Whitehurst, G. J., 147 Whiteman, S., 235, 236 Whitesell, N. R., 294, 295 Whiting, B. B., 179 Whittle, M. J., 66 Whitty, M. T., 359 Wickrama, K. A. S., 516, 522 Wicks-Nelson, R., 240, 242 Widen, S. C., 165 Widman, L., 300 Widmayer, S. M., 149 Widom, C. S., 240 Wijdicks, E. F. M., 544 Wilber, K. H., 532 Wilcox, T., 179, 180 Wiles, J. L., 518 Wilhelm-Leen, E. R., 526 Wilkinson, S., 545 Williams, A. L., 234 Williams, J., 400 Williams, J. C., 393 Williams, S., 267 Williams, S. T., 312 Williamson, G. M., 58 Willis, S. L., 503 Willoughby, B. J., 319 Wilner, A., 323 Wilson, R. D., 66 Wilson-Mitchell, J. E., 184 Wilt, J., 444, 445 Winburg, J., 103 Winecoff, A., 510–511 Wink, P., 512 Winneke, G., 61 Winner, E., 204 Winslow, M., 556 Wise, D., 514 Wiswall, M., 306 Witherington, D. C., 128 Witmer, H., 357 Witten, T. M., 367, 522 Wogrin, C., 575 Wojtunik-Kulesza, K. A., 467 Wolchik, S. A., 239, 565–566 Wolf, A. W., 84 Wolf, M. M., 14 Wolff, P. H., 82 Wolford, J., 251 Wolke, D., 248 Wong, A. T., 483 Wong, D. R., 532 Wong, P. T. P., 564 Wood, E., 557 Wood, W., 184

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

N a m e I N d e x 665

Woodbridge, S., 451 Woodin, E. M., 233 Woodward, A., 166 Woodward, L. J., 58 Woollett, K., 95 Worden, W., 561 Worrell, F. C., 204 Worthington, J., 426 Wortman, C. B., 565 Wray, D., 213 Wray-Lake, L., 234 Wrenn, R. L., 572 Wright, G. R. T., 323 Wright, J. C., 146, 250 Wright, S., 388 Wrosch, C., 349, 503 Wrzesniewski, A., 388 Wrzus, C., 443, 444 Wu, B., 319 Wu, H.-S., 523 Wu, L., 495 Wu, S., 509 Wu, X., 160

Wyman, E., 169 Wynn, K., 134

Xia, Y. R., 375 Xie, X., 375 Xu , X., 364

Yaakobi, E., 550 Yakushko, O., 575 Yang, J., 104, 364 Yap, M. B. H., 309 Yárnoz-Yaben, S., 381 Yarwood, A., 426 Yashin, A. I., 467 Yelich-O’Connor, E. M. L., 324 Yeung, S. E., 503 Yngvesson, B., 377 Yoffe, E., 327 Yon, Y., 532 Yonas, A., 105, 106 Yoon, S. M., 452 Young, K., 357 Young, S. K., 174

Yousafzai, A. K., 91 Youssef-Morgan, C. M., 394 Ytterstad, E., 524 Yu, P., 369 Yu, T., 382 Yu, X., 412 Yuan, S., 144 Yuen, H. K., 529 Yumoto, C., 64 Yunger, J. L., 228

Zabriskie, R. B., 416 Zahn-Waxler, C., 173, 174 Zaitchik, D., 129 Zajacova, A., 526–527 Zalewski, M., 168 Zanjani, F., 336–337 Zarotney, J. R., 575 Zarrett, N., 221, 274 Zaslow, M. J., 179 Zeanah, C. H., 161 Zelazo, P. R., 80 Zetumer, S., 573

Zhai, F., 242 Zhan, H. J., 449 Zhang, B., 415 Zhang, G., 359 Zhang, J., 505–506 Zhang, M., 529 Zhang, Y., 128 Zhao, L., 495 Zheng, Z., 496 Zhou, L., 509 Zhou, Q., 228, 230 Zhu, W., 485 Ziegler, C., 469 Zigler, E., 202 Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., 168, 242 Zimmerman, B. J., 193 Zimprich, D., 442 Zinar, S., 211 Zinn, J. O., 320 Zollfrank, B. C. C., 573 Zosh, J. M., 146 Zukow-Goldring, P., 235 Zweigenhaft, R. L., 238

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

666 N a m e I N d e x

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

Note: Page numbers in italics indicate figures, tables, or illustrations.

A Better Balance, 374 AAIDD Ad Hoc Committee

on Terminology and Classification, 205

AARP, 429–430, 452 abandonment, in elder abuse, 532 ABCA7, 495 ABCDE model of friendship, 356 abuse

child, 239–242 elder, 532–533

abusive relationships continuum of progressive behaviors

in, 364 dating violence, 300–301 defined, 363 in young and middle adulthood,

363–364 academic skills, 209–217

math, 213–215 reading, 209–212 school-based influences on, 216 teacher-based influences on, 216–217 writing, 212–213

acamprosate (Campral), 330 accommodation, defined, 119 achievement, 291, 291, 292 acquired immunodeficiency syndrome

(AIDS), 298, 299, 299 active euthanasia, defined, 545 active life expectancy, defined, 462 activities of daily living (ADLs),

defined, 526 acuity, 325, 473 adaptation, 472 adaptation level, defined, 504 adolescence, 263–284

biracial children, 294, 375–376 brain growth in, 265, 266 cognitive development in, 276–284 content knowledge of, 277, 279 risky behavior and, 275, 320, 328 See also middle childhood

adolescent egocentrism, defined, 291–292, 292

adolescent-limited antisocial behavior, defined, 311

adopted children, 236–237 heredity and, 46–47

adoptive parenting, 377–378 aerobic exercise

defined, 435 guidelines for, 436

affect, grief and, 563 African Americans

ADHD and, 208–209 altruism and, 175 Alzheimer’s disease and, 495 cardiovascular diseases and, 470 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 367 depression and, 489 division of household labor and, 410

divorce and, 379 educational attainment and, 321 ethnic identity and, 293, 294 face-recognition skills and, 107–108 glass ceiling and, 400 grandparenthood and, 451 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 hospice and, 556 intelligence tests and, 201 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 longevity and, 463 marriage and, in late life, 521 obesity rates and, 332 occupational choice and, 397 occupational development and, 399 parenting and, 375, 376 prejudice and, 257 racial discrimination leading to

depression in, 309–310, 310 remarriage and, 383 reproductive changes and, age-

related, 428 retirement and, 515, 516 self-esteem and, 295 sibling relations and, 236 singlehood and, 366 as single parents, 376 social support and, in late life, 519 spirituality and, 512 stereotype threat and, 203 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 teenage pregnancy and, 299 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 violence in relationships and, 363 widowhood and, 523

afterbirth, 68 age discrimination

defined, 403 in workforce, 403

Age Discrimination in Employment Act, 403

age of viability, defined, 55, 56, 56 Agency for Healthcare Research and

Quality, 332, 333 age-related macular degeneration,

defined, 473 AgingStats.gov, 529, 531, 533

aggression, 248–249 delinquency and, 312 gender-related differences in, 179 heredity and, 311 hostile, 248 instrumental, 248 PTSD and, 371 spouse-directed, 371 testosterone and, 311 violence on television and, 250 See also bullying

aging brain efficiency and, 437 cellular theories of, 466–467 demographics of, 458–461, 460

of emotional brain, 510–511, 511 goal of healthy, 502–503 metabolic theories of, 466 physiological changes of, 468–475 programming theories of, 467

aging parents, 448–450 filial obligation to, 448–449 stresses and rewards of caring for,

449–450, 450 agreeableness, defined, 442 AIDS, prenatal development and, 61, 61 air pollution, prenatal development and,

61, 62 Alcoholics Anonymous (AA), 329 alcohol use

binge drinking, 327–328, 328 brain and, effect on, 329 date rape related to, 327 fetal alcohol syndrome and, 60, 60 prenatal development and, 58, 59, 60,

60, 62, 64, 72 small-for-date babies and, 72 teenage drinking, 307–308 in young and middle adulthood,

326–330 See also alcohol use disorder

alcohol use disorder, 328–330 defined, 328 symptoms of, 328, 329 treatment for, 329–330

alert inactivity, defined, 82 alienation, defined, 395 alleles

behavioral genetics and, 45–46 defined, 41 dominant, 42–46, 46, 66 extroversion and, 45–46, 46 Huntington’s disease and, 44 phenylketonuria and, 43, 47–48 recessive, 42–43, 45–46, 66 sickle-cell trait and, 42, 43, 66

ALSPAC Study Team, 241 alternative caregiving, attachment and,

161–162 altruism

competence and, 174 costs of, 174 defined, 173 distinguished from prosocial

behavior, 173 mood and, 174 responsibility and, 173 situational influences on,

173–174, 176 skills underlying, 173 socialization of, 175

Alzheimer’s Association, 491–494 Alzheimer’s disease, 492–495

age-adjusted death rates for, 542 cause of, 493, 495 defined, 492 diagnosing, 492, 493, 494 progression of, 493 symptoms of, 492 treatment for, 495

ambiguous loss, defined, 566–567

American Academy of Pain Medicine, 558

American Academy of Pediatrics, 84, 162 American Cancer Society, 326, 430, 476 American College of Obstetricians and

Gynecologists, 66, 68, 71 American Diabetes Association,

475–476 American Heart Association, 330 American Indians. See Native

Americans American Lung Association, 326, 472 American Psychiatric Association,

208, 483 American Psychological Association, 32 American Society for Reproductive

Medicine, 52 American Stroke Association, 471 Americans with Disabilities Act, 33 amniocentesis, defined, 65, 65 amnion, defined, 54 amniotic fluid, defined, 54 amygdala, 395, 509–510, 511, 543, 552 anabolic steroids, 274, 275 analytic ability, defined, 197 androgen, 183, 266 Angelina Jolie effect, 477 anger

development of, 164 expression of, cultural differences

in, 166 gender-related differences in, 180 recognizing, in others, 166, 167 regulating, 167, 168

animism, defined, 123 anniversary reaction, defined, 564 anorexia nervosa, defined, 272–273 anticipatory grief, defined, 562 antioxidants, 467 anxiety disorders, defined, 490–491 APOE-ε4, 493, 495 apolipoprotein ε3 (APOE-ε3), 493, 495 apolipoprotein ε4 (APOE-ε4), 493, 495 appearance

age-related changes in, 422–423 as reality, 123, 124, 125

approximate number system (ANS), 134–135

aptitudes for school, 194–203 See also intelligence

arthritis genetics and, 426 medications for, 426 osteoarthritis, 426 osteoporosis compared to, 427 surgical interventions for, 427

artistic personality type, 304 Asian Americans

autobiographical memory and, 133 career preferences of, 304 depression and, 489 divorce and, 379 educational attainment and, 321 emotions expressed by, 166 ethnic identity and, 293, 294, 296 face-recognition skills and, 107–108

Subject Index

S u b j e c t I n d e x 667

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

668 S u b j e c t I n d e x

Asian Americans (continued) filial obligation and, 448 generativity and, 445 glass ceiling and, 400 grandparenthood and, 452 intelligence tests and, 201 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 make-believe and, 169 remarriage and, 383 romantic relationships and, 297 speech and, 142 stress and parenthood and, 375 terms for describing, 9

aspirin for arthritis, 426 prenatal development and, 59, 60, 64

assimilation, defined, 119 assisted living facilities, defined,

529–530 assisted reproductive technology, 52 assortative mating, defined, 359 atherosclerosis, defined, 470 attachment, 157–162

alternative caregiving and, 161–162 avoidant, 159 consequences of, 160 defined, 157 disorganized (disoriented), 159 forms of, 158–159 growth of, 157 in the making, 158 preattachment, 158 quality of, 159–161 reciprocal relationships and, 158 resistant, 159 secure, 158–159 steps towards, 157–158 Strange Situation and, 158, 159 true, 158

attention, defined, 131 attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD), 208–209 auditory organization, 338 authoritarian parenting, defined, 229 authoritative parenting, defined, 229 autism spectrum disorders (ASD),

112–113 autobiographical memory

defined, 133, 483 dementia and, 491 life stories and, 347

autonomic system, assessing, 81 autonomy vs. shame and doubt,

156, 157 autosomes

defined, 40 Down syndrome and, 44

average children, defined, 247 average longevity (life expectancy)

defined, 461, 462 environmental factors in, 462–463 ethnic differences in, 463 gender differences in, 463 genetic factors in, 462 international differences in, 463, 464 in US, 1900-2014, 462

avoidant attachment, defined, 159 Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and

Children, 70 axon, defined, 91

babbling, defined, 141 Babinski reflex, 80, 81 baby blues, 69

“Back to Sleep” campaign, 84–85 backup care, defined, 409 balance, 97 Bandura’s social cognitive theory, 14 basic cry, defined, 82 basic emotions, defined, 164–165

See also emotion battered woman syndrome, defined, 363 behavior

brain–behavior relationships and, 10, 25

delinquency and, 311–312 molecular genetics and, 47 paths linking genes to, 47–50 psychological forces and, 7, 8 See also aggression

behavioral genetics defined, 45 mechanisms and methods of, 45–47 paths linking genes to behavior, 47–50

behavioral phenotypes, 46, 46, 47 behavioral treatments

for ADHD, 208 for alcohol use disorder, 329

behaviorism, 13–14 operant conditioning and, 13 punishment and, 14 reinforcement and, 13, 14 Skinner’s research on, 13, 14 Watson’s research on, 13

behavior therapy, defined, 490 belief system

depression and, 309, 489–490 internal, 489–490

benzodiazepine, 491 bereavement, defined, 561

See also grief beta-amyloid, defined, 468–469, 469 beta-amyloid cascade hypothesis,

defined, 495 beta-blockers, 491 Better Balance, A, 374 bias in workplace, 400–402 Big Five personality traits, 441–442

agreeableness, 442 conscientiousness, 442 extraversion, 441 neuroticism, 441 openness to experience, 442 See also five-factor trait model

bilingualism, 145 binge drinking, defined, 327 binocular disparity, defined, 106 bioethics, defined, 545 biological forces

in biopsychosocial framework, 7, 8 death and, 575 defined, 7 human development created by,

summary of, 21 low birth weight and, 72 mechanics of intelligence and, 437 neuroscience and, 11

biological theories of aging, 466–467 cellular, 466–467 genetic programming, 467 metabolic, 466

biology, naïve, 128–129 biopsychosocial framework, 7–10, 8

biological forces in, 7, 8 defined, 7 interaction of forces in, 10 life-cycle forces in, 7, 9–10 psychological forces in, 7, 8 sociocultural forces in, 7, 8–9

biracial adolescents, 294, 375–376 birth order, 237–238 bisexual youth, 300 blended family, defined, 239 blink reflex, 80, 81 bodily-kinesthetic intelligence, 196, 196 body image, 267 body mass index (BMI), defined, 271,

332, 332 bones, age-related changes in, 423–425 boomerang employee, defined, 403 boomerang kids, 448 bottle-feeding, 90 brain activity

amygdala and, 395, 509–510, 511, 543, 552

death anxiety and, 551–554 imaging techniques and, 10, 25 injuries and, 11 left rostral anterior cingulate cortex

and, 552 meditation and, 513 physiological measures of, 24–25 right caudate nucleus and, 552 spiritual practices and, 513 study of (See neuroscience) temperament, 85–86

brain–behavior relationships, 10, 25, 469–470

brain cancer, 545, 546, 547 brain circuitry

experience-dependent growth and, 95

face-recognition skills and, 107–108 love and, 362

brain damage, 33 brain development

in adolescence, 265, 266 death and, experience with, 571–572 grief expressed by, 572 health in, 270–275 identity in, 290–296 information processing in, 276–279 malnutrition and, 90–91, 91 memory of, 276–277 metacognition in, 277 moral reasoning and, 279–285 nutrition in, 270–273 in period of the fetus, 55 personality and, 303–304, 304 physical development in, 263–275 physical fitness in, 274–275, 275 in prenatal development, 92–93 problem-solving in, 277–278 processing speed of, 276, 276–277 pubertal changes in, 264–269 reasoning in, 277–284 risky behavior and, 275, 320, 328 self-absorption in, 291–292 socioemotional development in,

289–313 of specialized brain, 93–95 sports participation in, 274–275 thinking in, characteristics of,

291–292, 292 well-being in, 275

brain functions autism and, 112 close friendships and, 357 exercise and, 435 mapping, cognitive impairment

and, 511 brain imaging research, 339,

339–340, 437 brain injuries, 11

brain maturation in adolescence, 94, 265, 266 in adulthood, 94 in infants, 8, 91–95 innovative thinking during adulthood

and, 486 prenatal, 8, 91–93 See also brain specialization

brain specialization experience-dependent growth

and, 95 experience-expectant growth and,

94–95 general principles of, 93–94 growth of, in infants, 93–95 in period of embryo, 53–54 in period of fetus, 55

BRCA1, 476, 477 BRCA2, 476 breast cancer

DES and, 64 genetics and, 476 MHT and, 429 screening for, 476

breastfeeding, 70, 83, 89, 90 hormones and, 70 nutrition and, 83, 89, 90

Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory, 17, 17–18, 22

Buddhism/Buddhists, 4, 166, 512 bulimia nervosa, defined, 272–273 bullying, 248–249

aggressors in, 248 cyberbullying, 248, 252 delinquency and, 312 programs to combat, 249, 312 victims of, 248–249

Bureau of Labor Statistics, 407, 408 burnout, defined, 395–396, 396 buspirone, 491

caffeine osteoporosis and, 423 prenatal development and, 59, 60 sleep problems and, 477

cancer, 476 age-adjusted death rates for, 542 age-related risks for, 476 aggressive treatment for, ethics and,

545, 546, 547 alcohol consumption and, 327 Angelina Jolie effect and, 477 anticipatory grief and, 562 antioxidants and, 467 brain, 545, 546, 547 cervical, HPV and, 299 as chronic disease, 476 colorectal, MHT and, 429 death anxiety and, 553 DES and, 64 diet and, 10, 332, 477 disability and, 527 euthanasia and, 545, 546, 547 exercise to lower risk of, 274, 527 genetics and, 66, 476, 477 healthcare disparities and, 333 HIV and, 299 hospice and, 556–557 leisure activities and, 415 lung, 550, 550 MHT and, 429 in middle childhood, 275 in oldest-old, 464 osteoporosis and, 423 ovarian, 476, 477

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 669

pancreatic, 543, 549 period of decline, 550 price of life-sustaining care, 547 prostate, 430, 476 radiofrequency radiation and, 61 screening for, 430, 476, 548 skin, 424 smoking and, 326, 326 steroid use and, 274 stress and, 431 testicular, 64

cancer cervical, 299 cardinality principle, defined, 135 cardiovascular disease

African Americans and, 495 age-adjusted death rates for, 542 age-related risks for, 470–472 alcohol use and, 326, 327 antioxidants and, 467 atherosclerosis, 470 congestive heart failure, 550, 550 CVAs, 470–472 diagnostic structural imaging, 471,

471–472 diet and, 10, 332, 477 disability and, 527 estrogen and, 428 exercise to lower risk of, 274, 435 genetics and, 495 hospice and, 557 LDLs and, 331, 433 longevity and, 462 menopause and, 428, 429 MHT and, 428, 429 steroid use and, 274 stress and, 431 TIAs, 471 vascular dementia, 471–472

cardiovascular system, age-related changes in, 470–472

career construction theory, defined, 389 career plateauing, defined, 404 caregivers/caregiving

of aging parents, 448–450 alternative, 161–162 employed, 408–409 grandparents caring for

grandchildren, 452 for partner, in late adulthood, 523

cataracts, defined, 473 Catholics, 4, 319, 544 cell body, defined, 91 cellular theories of aging, 466–467 Census Bureau, 320, 366, 368, 458, 460 Centers for Disease Control, 300 Centers for Disease Control and

Prevention, 52, 275, 299, 310, 326, 327, 328, 332, 333, 503, 558

Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 530, 531, 534

Central Intelligence Agency, 73 centration, defined, 123, 125 cephalocaudal principle, defined, 54 cephalopelvic disproportion, 71 cerebral cortex, defined, 55, 91 cerebral vascular accidents (CVAs),

defined, 470 cesarean section (C-section),

defined, 71 change, grief and, 563 chicken pox, prenatal development

and, 61 childbirth

adjusting to parenthood, 69–70 approaches to, 68–69

complications, 71, 71–72 infant mortality and, 72–73, 73 stages of labor in, 67–68, 68 See also prenatal development

child-free couples, 371, 373 child maltreatment, 239–242

abusing parents in, 240–241 effects of, on children, 241 preventing, 241–242 resilience and, 241

Child Trends, 305 children

adopted, 46–47, 236–237 aggressive, 248–249 average, 247 biracial, 294, 375–376 child maltreatment, 239–242 controversial, 247 custody of, after divorce, 381–382 death and, understanding of,

570–571 deciding whether to have, 373–375 with disabilities, 204–207 divorce and, 238–239, 381–382 empty nest, 447–448 gifted, 204 grief expressed by, 570–571, 571 IQ to predict outcomes in, 199–200 multiethnic, raising, 375 naïve theories of, 126–130 neglected, 247 popular, 246–247 rejected, 246–248 with special needs, 204–209 See also adolescence; infants; middle

childhood ChildStats.gov, 375 Chinese Americans. See Asian

Americans chlamydia, 61, 299 cholesterol

HDLs, 330, 331–332 LDLs, 330, 331–332 lowering levels of, 331–332 screening, 330, 331

chorionic villus sampling (CVS), defined, 65, 65–66

chromosomes abnormal, 44, 45 defined, 40–41 Down syndrome and, 44, 44, 59, 66 X chromosomes, 40, 44, 45 Y chromosomes, 40, 41, 44, 45

chronic disease, 475–476 cancer, 476 diabetes mellitus, 475–476

chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), defined, 472

chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE), defined, 496–497

cigarette smoking prenatal development and, 59–60,

60, 64 quitting, 326 in young and middle adulthood, 308,

326, 326 circadian rhythm, defined, 476 classical conditioning, defined, 131 climacteric, defined, 428 clinical death, defined, 544 clique, defined, 245 coaches, parents as, 172 cocaine, prenatal development and,

60, 64 cognitive decline, 469, 496

cognitive development academic skills and, 209–217 accommodation and, 119 in adolescence, 276–284 aptitudes for school and, 194–203

(See also intelligence) assimilation and, 119 concrete operational period and, 120 content knowledge and, 277 equilibration and, 119, 120 formal operational period and, 120 guided participation and, 136–137 in infancy and early childhood,

118–130 intelligence and, 334–340, 436–439 intersubjectivity and, 136 lifelong learning and, 439–440 memory strategies and, 277 mental abilities and, 335–337, 337, 338 metacognitive skill and, 277 in middle adulthood, 334–345,

436–440 in middle childhood, 190–217 moral issues and, 279–284 Piaget’s theory of, 118–130, 190–192 preoperational period and, 120,

122–125 principles of, 118–120 problem-solving and, 277–284 processing speed and, 276–277 reasoning and, 277–284 sensorimotor period and, 120, 120–122 special needs of children and,

204–209 Vygotsky’s theory of social

development and, 136–138 working memory and, 276–277 in young and middle adulthood,

334–345 See also cognitive-developmental

theory; information processing; memory

cognitive-developmental theory, 14–17 information-processing theory, 15, 16 Piaget’s theory of, 15, 15–16 (See

also Piaget’s theory of cognitive development)

summarized, 21 Vygotsky’s theory of, 16–17 (See

also Vygotsky’s theory of social development)

cognitive processes, in late adulthood, 479–487

creativity, 485, 485–486 information processing, 479–481 memory, 481–484 wisdom, 486, 487

cognitive reappraisal task, 510, 510 cognitive self-regulation, defined, 193 cognitive therapy, defined, 490 cohabitation, defined, 366–367 cohort effects, defined, 30 collaborative divorce, defined, 382 college

career attainment and, 322, 322–323 changing nature of, 322 enrollment, by income, 321, 321 returning adult students, 322 social interaction in, 321

color, perception of, 104, 105 colorectal cancer, 429 combinatorial reasoning, 191 compensation, 20

See also selective optimization with compensation (SOC) model

competence, 503 altruism and, 174 in competence-environmental press

theory, 18 defined, 18

competence-environmental press theory, 18, 22, 503–505

complex emotions, 165 See also emotion

complicated or prolonged grief disorder, defined, 567

comprehension defined, 210 reading skills and, 205–207, 207, 210,

211–212 conception

to birth, 51–66 (See also prenatal development)

in 21st century, 52 concrete-operational period,

15, 120 Conduct Problems Prevention Research

Group, 312 cones, defined, 104 congenital adrenal hyperplasia

(CAH), 183 congestive heart failure, 550, 550 Congressional Budget Office, 533 conscientiousness, defined, 442 conservation problems, 123,

123, 124 constricting actions, defined, 171 content knowledge, of adolescence,

277, 279 continuity, 6 continuity–discontinuity issue,

defined, 6 contraception, 300 controversial children, defined, 247 conventional level, defined, 281 conventional personality type, 304 cooing, defined, 141 cooperative play, defined, 169 coordinating skills, 98–99 coping

defined, 432 emotion-focused, 433 grief and, 563–567 problem-focused, 432–433 stress and coping paradigm, 431

core knowledge hypothesis, defined, 126

Corporation for National and Community Service, 518

corpus callosum, defined, 92 corrective adaptations, defined, 505 correlational study

defined, 26 in research design, 26–27 summarized, 31

correlation coefficient defined, 26 interpretations of, 27, 27

co-rumination, defined, 245 Council for Adult and Experiential

Learning, 439 Council on Contemporary

Families, 383 counterimitation, defined, 231 counting skills, learning, 135 covenant marriage, defined, 380 creative ability, defined, 197 creativity, in late adulthood, 485,

485–486 CRISPR, 66

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

670 S u b j e c t I n d e x

cross-cultural comparisons adults caring for aging parents,

449–450 appearance changes, age-related, 423 attachment to place, 528 cohabitation, 366–367 coordinating skills, 98–99 couple-forming behaviors,

360–361, 361 death and dying, 543–544 death anxiety, 553 dependent care and work, 410 elder abuse, 532–533 euthanasia, 545 experience-dependent growth, 95 five-factor trait model, 442 frail older adults, 31, 527, 32526–527 friendships in adulthood, 358 gender differences in occupational

selection, 397, 398 generativity, 454–456 grandparents raising

grandchildren, 452 grief reactions, 563 job satisfaction, 394–395 labor force participation rate,

397, 398 leisure activities, 416 marital success, 368, 368–369 of marital success, 368–369 math skills, 214, 215, 215 meta-analysis in, 31, 32 motor skills development, 99 parental styles, 230–231 personal control beliefs, 349 primary mental abilities, 336,

336–337, 337 reflective judgment, 341 role transitions, 319–320 sequential design in, 30 on stability of intelligence from 11 to

age 90 years, 29, 29 of violence in relationships, 364 well-being, 509

cross-sectional study, defined, 30, 31 crowd, defined, 245 crowning, defined, 67, 68 crying, defined, 82 crystallization, defined, 302 crystallized intelligence

defined, 337 description of, 338 developmental patterns for, 339, 339 in hierarchical theories of

intelligence, 195, 196 pragmatics of intelligence and, 437 in school-aged children, 195, 196 in young and middle adulthood, 337,

338, 339, 339 cultural context

bioethics and, 545 child abuse and, 240 intelligence and, 7, 198, 203 life story and, 347 in TESSERA framework of adult

personality development, 444 thinking and, 16

culture of aging in America, 531 anger and, expression of, 166 attachment to place and, 528 bioethics and, 545 burnout and, 412 chastity and, 360 child maltreatment and, 240, 241

cognitive development and, 117, 126 co-sleeping and, 83 couple-forming behaviors and, 360 crying and, 82 death and, children’s understanding

of, 570 death and, definition of, 543 death and, feelings about, 549 death anxiety and, 553 dependent care and, 410 desirable mates and, characteristics

of, 360 division of household labor and, 410 of drinking in college, 328 dying process and, 555 eating disorders and, 272 elder abuse and, 532–533 electronic media and, 249 emotional expression and, 166 ethnic identity and, 293–294 euthanasia and, 545 experience-dependent growth

and, 95 five-factor trait model and, 442 generativity and, 445 grief and, 543, 561, 563, 564, 567 hook-ups and, 360 housing arrangements and, 531 IADLs and, 526 identity and, 347 integrity versus despair and, 509 intelligence and, 337, 337, 338 job satisfaction and, 394 labels and, 9 language and, 141, 142, 145 left-handedness and, 102 leisure and, 416 low birth weight and, 72 make-believe and, 169 marriage and, 319, 369 menarche and, 267–268, 268 mentoring and coaching model

and, 394 moral reasoning and, 282, 283 motor development and, 98–99 mourning rituals and, 543 music and, early sensitivity to, 104 occupational development and, 392 occupational selection and, 391, 397 parenting styles and, 230–231 personal control beliefs and,

348, 349 physician-assisted suicide and, 546 Piaget’s theory and, 126 popular children and, 247 remarriage and, 383 rites of passage and, 324 role transitions and, 319–320 romantic relationships and, 297,

360–362, 361 same-sex friends and, 358 self-concept and, 110 sibling relations and, 235 socialization and, 227, 228 sociocultural forces and, 7, 8–9 stress of parenthood and, 375 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 in systems view of family, 229 temperament and, 86 theory of mind and, 111–112 third-person perspective and, 255 violence in relationships and, 364 Vygotsky’s theory and, 117, 136–138 well-being in late adulthood and, 509

culture-fair intelligence tests, defined, 202, 202

cyberbullying, defined, 252 cytomegalovirus, prenatal development

and, 61, 61

Data and Safety Monitoring Board (DSMB), 429

dating hook-ups, 358, 360 online, 359–360 speed dating, 359 violence, 300–301

death and dying adolescents’ experience with, 571–572 of child in young adulthood, 573 of child or grandchild in late

adulthood, 574 children’s understanding of, 570–571 contextual theory of, 551 dealing with one’s own, 550–551 death anxiety and, 551–554 ethical issues in, 544–548 euthanasia and, 545–546 Kübler-Ross’s stages of, 551 legal and medical definitions of, 544 life-course approach to, 549–550 life-sustaining care and, price of,

547–548 older adult’s feelings toward, 574–575 of parent, 573 of partner in late adulthood, 574–575 of partner in young adulthood,

572–573 physician-assisted suicide and,

546–547 sociocultural definitions of, 543–544 thinking about, 548–554 trajectories of, 550, 550–551 young adult’s feelings toward,

572–573 See also end-of-life issues; grief

death anxiety, 551–554 dealing with, 553–554 defined, 551 terror management theory and, 552,

552–553 deductive reasoning, defined, 191 Defense and Veterans Brain Injury

Center, 496 delinquency, 310–313

biological contributions to, 311 cognitive contributions to, 311 family contributions to, 311–312 juveniles committing serious crimes

and, 313 programs to combat, 312 socioeconomic contributions to, 312

delivery. See childbirth dementia, 491–497

Alzheimer’s disease and, 492–495 CTE and, 496–497 defined, 492 Mini-Mental State Examination

for, 29 Parkinson’s disease and, 495, 496 training persons with, to be group

activity leaders, 496 demographers, defined, 458 dendrite, defined, 91 deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA),

defined, 41 dependent care

employer responses and, 409–410 workers and, effect on, 409

dependent life expectancy, defined, 462 dependent variable, defined, 27 depression, 308–310, 488–490

cause of, 489–490 defined, 308 diagnosing, 488, 489 in older adults, 488–490 postpartum, 69–70 prevalence of, 488, 489 racial discrimination and,

309–310, 310 teen suicides and, 310 treating, 310, 490

depth, perception of, 105–106 developmental coach, defined, 393–394 developmental designs

cross-sectional study, 30 longitudinal study, 28–30 sequential design, 30–31 summarized, 31

developmental dyscalculia, 205, 207 developmental dyslexia, 205–206, 207 developmental research, 22–34

on aging emotional brain, 510–511 design, 26–32 (See also research

design) on eating disorders, program for, 273 ethics in, 32 findings from different studies,

integrating, 31, 32 on gender-related differences,

179–180 on grasping, 101 on holding in grief, costs of, 568 on leisure, long-term effects of, 415 measurement in, 23–26 (See also

measurement in developmental research)

on mental abilities in young and middle adulthood, 336–337, 337

meta-analysis in, 31, 32 on occupational transitions, 407 preschoolers’ essentialist

thinking, 129 on racial discrimination leading to

depression, 309–310, 310 results, 33 romantic attachment, 361, 361 social policy and, 33 on software for nonexperts, 440 on training persons with dementia to

be group activity leaders, 496 on visual-spatial skills, video games

and, 251 on word structure and reading

comprehension, 206–207, 207 developmental theories, 12–22

cognitive-developmental theory, 14–17

definition of theory, 12 ecological and systems approach,

17–18 learning theory, 13–14 life-course perspective, 20–21 life-span perspective, 19–20 psychodynamic theory, 12–13 SOC model, 19–20 summarized, 21–22

diabetes mellitus, 475–476 age-adjusted death rates for, 542 breastfed babies and, 89 as chronic disease, 475–476 defined, 475 diabetic retinopathy and, 473 diet and, 332, 476, 477

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 671

disability and, 527 exercise to lower risk of, 274, 435 glucose levels and, 475 healthcare disparities and, 333 male reproductive changes and, 430 managing, 476 obesity and, 271, 475 Type 1, 475 Type 2, 475

diabetic retinopathy, defined, 473 Dietary Guidelines for Americans

2015–2020 (USDA), 477 diethylstilbestrol (DES), 64 differentiation, defined, 98 diffusion, 291, 291 digestive system, in fetal

development, 55 direct instruction, defined, 231 disabilities

children with, 204–207 chronic disease and, 527 global differences in, 528 intellectual, 205 learning, 205–206 in middle childhood, 204–207 over time, in frail older adults,

526–527, 527 disciplinary practices, altruism and, 175 discontinuity, 6 discrimination

age, 403 gender, 400–401 racial, leading to depression,

309–310, 310 in workplace, 400–402

disease chronic, 475–476 teratogenic, 60–61, 61

disease-modifying anti-rheumatic drugs (DMARDs), 426

disenfranchised grief, defined, 568–569 disgust, as adaptive emotion, 164 disorganized (disoriented) attachment,

defined, 159 disulfiram (Antabuse), 329 divergent thinking, defined, 204 diversity of older adults, 460–461 divided attention, defined, 481 divorce, 238–239

blended families and, 239 children and, effects on, 238–239,

381–382 couples and, effects on, 381 divorce hangover, 381 rates, 379, 380 reasons for, 379–381 relationships with adult children and,

effects on, 382 remarriage and, 239 stress related to, 238, 382 in young and middle adulthood,

379–382 dizygotic twins, defined, 46 docility, defined, 505 dominance hierarchy, defined, 245 dominant, defined, 42

See also alleles do not resuscitate (DNR) order,

defined, 558 dopamine, 329, 362, 469, 495 Down syndrome, 44, 44, 59, 66 drug use, 307–308

adolescent-limited antisocial behavior and, 311

AIDS and, 299

dating violence and, 301 diverse family forms and, 378 employment and, 306 peer pressure and, 244, 246 performance-enhancing, 274, 275 socioeconomic status and, 231 support groups and, 329 teenage drinking and, 307–308 teenage smoking and, 308 television and, 250

dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) test, defined, 424

dual process model (DPM), defined, 565, 565–566

Dutch Asian American, 293, 296 dynamic systems theory, defined, 96 dysphoria, defined, 488

eating disorders, 272–273 anorexia nervosa, 272–273 bulimia nervosa, 272–273 programs for, 272–273

ecological and systems approach, 17–18 Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17–18 competence–environmental press

theory, 18 summarized, 22

ecological theory Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17–18 defined, 17

ectoderm, defined, 53 Eden Alternative, 531 edgework, defined, 320 education

academic skills and, 209–217 aptitudes for school and, 194–203

(See also intelligence) special needs of children and, 204–209

education attainment in emerging adulthood, 321–323, 322 health issues and, 332–333

effortful control, gender-related differences in, 180

egocentrism adolescent, 291–292, 292 defined, 122 in preoperational thinking,

122–123, 254 summarized, 122

ego resilience, defined, 241 E-I-E-I-O framework, 484, 484, 495 elaboration, defined, 192 elder abuse and neglect, 532–533 elective selection, 19 electroencephalography, defined, 93 electronic media, 249–252

cyberbullying and, 248, 252 social media and, 251 television programs, 250 video games, 251–252

embarrassment emergence of, 165 expression of, 165, 166

embryo defined, 53 period of, 53–54, 54

emerging adulthood, 318–324 behavior and, 320–321 defined, 316 developmental periods in, 323–324 education attainment and,

321–323, 322 emotion and logic in, integrating,

342–345 intimacy in, establishing, 323

neuroscience and, 320–321 role transitions in, 319–320 workforce attainment and,

321–323, 322 See also young adulthood

emotion, 163–168 adaptive, 164 aging emotional brain and,

510–511, 511 basic, 164–165 complex or self-conscious, 165, 165 depression and, 309 emerging, 163–168 experiencing and expressing,

164–166, 165 functions of, 163–164 gender-related differences in, 180 in late adulthood, 509–511 pattern of happiness through life, 509 photographs for measuring, 24, 34 recognizing and using others’,

166–167, 167 regulating, 167–168 in young and middle adulthood,

342–345 See also individual emotions

emotional abuse of children, 240 in elder abuse, 532

emotional intelligence (EI), defined, 197, 343

emotion-focused coping, defined, 433 emotion-focused research, 509–510 empathy

altruism and, 175 defined, 173 oxytocin and, 174 perspective taking and, 173, 176

employment career development and, 302–305 part-time, 305–306

empty nest, 447–448 enabling actions, defined, 171 endoderm, defined, 53 end-of-life issues, 554–560

competency evaluation and, 560 defined, 554 do not resuscitate (DNR) order

and, 558 final scenario and, 554–555 healthcare power of attorney and,

558, 559 hospice and, 556–558 living will and, 558 making your end-of-life intentions

known, 558–560 Patient Self-Determination Act and,

559–560 entacapone, 495 enterprising personality type, 304 environment

adapting to and exploring, 120–121 genes and, 47–50 heredity and, 47–50 intelligence and, 200 locomotion and, 98 pubertal events and, timing of,

264–269 temperament and, 86 teratogens and, 61–62

environmental press in competence-environmental press

theory, 18 defined, 18, 503–504 summarized, 22

epidural analgesia, 68 epigenetic principle

defined, 13 heritability coefficients and, 48

episodic memory, defined, 482 Equal Employment Opportunity

Commission, 402, 403 Equality and Human Rights

Commission, 403 equilibration, defined, 119, 120 Erikson’s psychosocial theory, 12–13,

156, 156–157 autonomy vs. shame and doubt, 157 initiative vs. shame guilt, 157 stages of, 12–13, 13 summarized, 21 trust vs. mistrust, 156

essentialism, defined, 128 estrogen

after birth, 69 DES, 64 health conditions related to loss

of, 428 menopause and, 428 osteoporosis and, 423, 428 perimenopause and, 428 puberty and, 266 replacement, 425, 428, 429 SERMs, 425

ethics codes of ethical conduct, 32 in developmental research, 32 universal ethical principles, 281

Ethics Committee of the American Society for Reproductive Medicine, 52

ethnic identity, 293–294 consequences of, 294 defined, 293 phases in achieving, 293

ethnicity depression and, 489 diabetes mellitus and, 475 educational attainment and, 321 equal pay for equal work and,

401, 401 face-recognition skills and, 108 generativity and, 445 grandparenthood and, 451–452 grief and, 563 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 health issues and, 332–333 health-related quality of life and, 528 intelligence and, 201–203 longevity and, 463 multiethnic children and, raising, 375 nursing home placement and, 531 nutrition and, 330 occupational selection and

development and, 399–400 parenting and, 375–376 retirement and, 514, 515–516 sexual minority youth and, 300 sociocultural forces and, 7, 8–9 socioeconomic status and, 528

eugenics, defined, 52 European Americans

ADHD and, 208–209 age-related macular degeneration

and, 473 altruism and, 175 autobiographical memory and, 133 binge drinking and, 328 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 367

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

672 S u b j e c t I n d e x

European Americans (continued) in developmental research, 49 diversity of older adults and, 460 division of household labor and, 410 educational attainment and, 321 emotions expressed by, 166 face-recognition skills and, 107 filial obligation and, 448 gender-related properties and,

reasoning about, 178 generativity and, 445 grandparenthood and, 451 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 intelligence tests and, 201 labor force participation rate and, 398 leading cause of death among, 325 longevity and, 463 make-believe and, 169 marriage and, in late life, 521 parenting and, 375, 376 part-time employment and, 305 prejudice and, 257 prosocial behavior of, 175 remarriage and, 382–383 reproductive changes and, age-

related, 428 self-esteem and, 295 singlehood and, 366 as single parents, 376 spousal caregivers and, 523 stress of parenthood and, 375 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 widowhood and, 523, 575

European Association of Palliative Care, 545

euthanasia, defined, 545 evolutionary psychology, defined, 157 exchange theory, defined, 369 exercise

aerobic, 435, 436 benefits of, 435, 435 in middle adulthood, 434–436 in middle childhood, 220

exosystem in Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17, 18 defined, 18

experience-dependent growth, defined, 95

experience-expectant growth, defined, 94

experiment, defined, 27 experimental studies

in research design, 27–28 summarized, 31

expertise acquiring, 438–439 software for nonexperts, 440

explicit memory, defined, 482 exploitation, in elder abuse, 532–533 expressive style, defined, 145 extended family, defined, 372 external memory aids, defined, 484 extraversion, defined, 441 extremely low birth weight, defined, 72

face-recognition skills, 107–108 factor, defined, 335 factor analysis, 335 false beliefs, growing understanding

of, 112 families

aging parents and, 448–450 blended, 239 child maltreatment and, 239–242

dependent care and, 409–410 diverse forms of, 376–378 divorce and, 238–239, 379–382 employed caregivers and, 408–409 empty nest and, 447–448 grandparenthood and, 450–452 great-grandparenthood and,

524–525 in late adulthood, 520–525 in middle adulthood, 372–383,

447–452 in middle childhood, 228–242 multiethnic, 375, 375–376 multiple roles in, 410–413, 411 parental roles in, 372–376 parenting styles and, 229–235 remarriage and, 382–383, 383 siblings and, 235–238, 520 step-, foster-, adoptive, and same-sex

couple parenting and, 377–378 systems view of, 228–229 work and, 408–413 in young and middle adulthood,

372–383 “Families for Safe Dates,” 301 familism, defined, 375 Family and Medical Leave Act of 1993

(FMLA), 374, 409 fast mapping, 143–144

cognitive factors and, 144 constraints on word names and, 143 defined, 143 developmental change in word

learning and, 144 joint attention and, 143 naming errors and, 144 sentence cues and, 144

fear as adaptive emotion, 163 as basic emotion, 164 development of, 164, 166 empathy and, 173 expression of, 165 function of, 163 recognizing, in others, 166, 167 regulating, 167, 168 stranger wariness and, 164

Federal Bureau of Investigation, 310 fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD),

defined, 60 fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), 60, 60 fetal medicine, 66 fetal surgery, 66 fetal therapy, defined, 66 filial obligation, defined, 448 final scenario, defined, 554–555 financial exploitation, in elder abuse,

532–533 fine motor skills, 100–102

defined, 96 grasping, 100–101 handedness, 101, 102 reaching, 100–101

five-factor trait model, 441–443 across adulthood, 442–443 culture and, 442 dimensions of, 441–442 See also Big Five personality traits

fluid intelligence defined, 337 description of, 338 developmental patterns for, 339, 339 in hierarchical theories of

intelligence, 195, 196 mechanics of intelligence and, 437

in school-aged children, 195, 196 in young and middle adulthood, 337,

338, 339, 339 foreclosure, 291, 291, 292 formal-operational period, 15, 120,

190–191, 192 foster-parenting, 377–378 four-component model, defined,

564–565 Fourth Age, 464–465 frail older adults, 526–528

defined, 526 disability over time and,

526–527, 527 functional limitations of, 527 global differences in disability of, 528 socioeconomic factors and, 527–528

free radicals, defined, 466, 467 friendships

ABCDE model of, 356 benefits of, 244–245, 357 consequences of, 245 defined, 243 friends in, 244 in late adulthood, 519–520 men’s, women’s, and cross-sex, 358 in middle childhood, 243–245 online, 357 sibling, 357–358 themes characterizing, 357 in young and middle adulthood,

356–358 frontal cortex, defined, 92 functional health, defined, 528 functional magnetic resonance imaging

(fMRI) of aging emotional brain,

510–511, 511 defined, 93

functional neuroimaging, defined, 470

gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), 329 Gardner’s theory of multiple

intelligences, 196, 196–197 gay and lesbian partnerships. See same-

sex partnerships gay youth, 300 gender, screening to determine, 45 gender discrimination, defined, 400 gender identity

biological influences on, 183 defined, 182 of sexual minority youth, 300

gender-related differences, 179–180 in aggression, 179 in anger, 180 eating disorders and, 272 in effortful control, 180 in emotions, 180 gender stereotypes and, 179–180 grief and, expression of, 563 grief and, risk factors in, 562 in guilt, 180 in happiness, 180 in labor force participation rate,

397, 398 in longevity, 463 in mathematics, 179 in memory, 179 in motor skills of middle

childhood, 220 in occupational selection and

development, 399–400 research on, 179–180 in retirement, 515–516

in social influences, 179 in spatial ability, 179, 180 in verbal ability, 179 in workplace, 400–402

gender roles, 176, 184 biological influences on, 183 evolving, 184 social, 176

gender-schema theory, defined, 182, 182–183

gender stereotypes, 177–180 defined, 177 gender-related differences and,

179–180 learning, 177–178 research on, 178, 178

gender typing, 180–183 gene, defined, 41 general intelligence, 195, 195–196 generativity

defined, 444 McAdams’s model of, 444–445, 445

genetic counseling, 64–65 genetic disorders, 43–44, 45

abnormal chromosomes, 44, 45 inherited, 43–44

genetics behavioral, 45–47 biological forces and, 7, 8 disorders of, 43–44, 45 intelligence and, 201 longevity and, 462 paths linking genes to behavior,

47–50 programming theories of aging

and, 467 prosocial behavior and, 174 pubertal events and, timing of,

264–269 research in molecular genetics, 47, 48

genital herpes features of, 299 prenatal development and, 61, 61

genital human papilloma virus (HPV), 299

genotype, defined, 41 genotype–phenotype link, 47–48 German measles, prenatal development

and, 61, 61 germ disc, defined, 53 gifted children, 204 gig economy, 387 glass ceiling, defined, 400 glass cliff, defined, 400 glaucoma, defined, 473 Global Initiative to End All Corporal

Punishment of Children, 232 gonorrhea, 299 grammar, acquisition of, 147–148

behaviorist answer, 147 cognitive answer, 148 linguistic answer, 147–148 social-interaction answer, 148

grammatical morphemes, defined, 147 grandchildren

death of, 574 grandparents caring for, 452 interacting with, 450, 451

grandparenthood, 450–452 caring for grandchildren, 452 ethnic differences in, 451–452 interacting with grandchildren,

450, 451 meaning of, 451

great-grandparenthood, 524–525

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 673

grief adolescent’s expression of, 572 ambiguous loss and, 566–567 anticipatory, 563 children’s expression of, 570–571, 571 complicated or prolonged, 567 coping with, 564–567 culture and, 543 defined, 561 disenfranchised, 568–569 dual process model and, 565,

565–566 four-component model and, 564–565 holding in, costs of, 568 model of adaptive grieving dynamics

and, 566, 566 older adults’ expression of, 574–575 process, 561–563 reactions, 563–564 risk factors in, 562–563 themes, 563 young adults’ expression of, 572–573

grief work, defined, 563 grief work as rumination hypothesis,

defined, 564 groups, 245–246

peer influence of, 246 structure of, 245–246

growth. See physical growth guided participation, defined, 136–137 Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament

Survey (GZTS), 442 guilt

emergence of, 165 expression of, 165 gender-related differences in, 180 initiative vs., 156, 157

Guttmacher Institute, 299

habituation, defined, 131 handedness, 101, 102 happiness

as adaptive emotion, 164 development of, 164 expression of, 165 gender-related differences in, 180

Harvard School of Public Health, 327 Hayflick limit, 466 health

in adolescence, 270–275 biological forces and, 7, 8 chronic disease and, 475–476 education issues in, 332–333 ethnic issues in, 332–333 exercise and, 434–436, 435 healthcare disparities in US, 333, 333 in late adulthood, 475–478 lifestyle factors in, 325–333, 476, 477 in middle adulthood, 325–333,

430–436 physical fitness and, 274–275, 275 socioeconomic issues in, 332–333 stress and, 430–434 in young and middle adulthood,

325–333 See also nutrition; physical fitness;

well-being healthcare power of attorney

defined, 558 example of, 559

healthy aging, defined, 503 hearing

age-related changes in, 473–475, 474 sense of, 103–104

Helpguide.org, 409

hemispheres defined, 92, 92 left, 92, 92, 94, 95, 139, 141, 148, 362 right, 92, 92, 94, 196

Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 559 hepatitis B, 299 heredity

adopted children and, 46–47 aggression and, 311 behavioral genetics and, 45–47 depression and, 309 environment and, 47–50 intelligence and, 45, 48, 200 mechanisms of, 40–43 obesity, 271 prosocial behavior and, 174 self-esteem and, 295 temperament and, 85 twins and, 46, 47

heritability coefficient, defined, 48 heroin, prenatal development and, 60 heterocyclic antidepressants

(HCAs), 490 heterozygous, defined, 41 hierarchical intelligence theories, 195,

195–196 high-density lipoproteins (HDLs)

cholesterol screening and, 330, 331 defined, 328 lowering levels of, 331–332

hippocampus, 95, 132, 433, 483, 486 Hispanic Americans. See Latino

Americans historical context, in life-span

perspective, 19 Holland’s personality-type theory,

303–304, 304 career development and, 303–304 personality types in, 303, 304

homogamy, defined, 369 homozygous, defined, 41 hook-ups, 358, 360 hope, defined, 156 hormones

after pregnancy, 69 breast-feeding and, 70 diethylstilbestrol and, 64 in fetal medicine, 66 implantation and, 53 in period of the fetus, 55 postpartum depression and, 69, 70 during pregnancy, 48, 58 stress and, 48, 58

hospice deciding on, 557–558 defined, 556 hospital care compared to, 556 pain management, 558 palliative care provided by, 556 principles underlying, 556 terminal diseases and, 556–557 types of, 556

Hospice Foundation of America, 557 hostile aggression, defined, 248 household chores, 410, 411 housing options, in late adulthood,

528–532 aging in place, 528–529 assisted living, 529–530 emerging housing arrangements,

531–532 Medicare beneficiaries, 530,

530–531 nursing homes, 530–531

HPC1, 476

human development accommodation and, 119 applying (See human development,

applying) basic forces in, 7–10 (See also

biopsychosocial framework) continuity–discontinuity issue in, 6 defined, 5 nature–nurture issue in, 6 neuroscience and, 10–11 recurring issues in, 5–6 research, 22–34 (See also

developmental research) theories, 12–22 (See also

developmental theories) thinking about, 5–7 universal and context-specific-

development issue in, 6–7 human development, applying

accommodation and assimilation, 119 aging parents, 448 alternative caregiving, 161–162 becoming rich and famous in

adulthood, 324 career development, 302–305 combinatorial reasoning, 191 competence–environmental press

theory, 503–505 describing others, 253–254 developmental forces, 11 false beliefs, growing understanding

of, 112 niche-picking, 50 Schindler’s list, 282

Human Genome Project, 41 human immunodeficiency virus (HIV),

298, 299 human papilloma virus (HPV), 299 Hunter Imagination Questionnaire

(HIQ), 486 Huntington’s disease, defined, 44 hyperactivity, in ADHD, 208 hypoxia, defined, 71

identity in adolescence, 290–296 ethnic, 293–294 gender, 182, 183 peers and, 293 phases or statuses of, 291, 291 self-esteem and, 294–295 sexual minority youth, 300 thinking and, characteristic of,

291–292, 292 illusion of invulnerability, defined,

292, 292 imaginary audience, defined, 292, 292 imitation, 132 imitation or observational learning,

defined, 14 impaired reading comprehension, 205,

206, 207 implantation, defined, 53 implementation, defined, 303 implicit memory, defined, 482 impression formation, 343–345

defined, 341 studying, 343–345, 344

impulsivity, in ADHD, 208 inattention, in ADHD, 208 incomplete dominance, defined, 42 incontinence, defined, 492 independent variable, defined, 27 infant-directed speech, defined, 141 infant mortality, defined, 72–73, 73

infants brain structures of, 92, 92–93 growth of, 87–91, 88, 89 mortality rate, 72–73, 73 motor skills of, 96–102 nervous system of, 91–95 nutrition in, 89–90 specialized brain growth in, 93–95 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 infatuation, 358–359 information processing

in adolescence, 276–279 attention and, 131 content knowledge and, 277 dividing attention among multiple

tasks and, 481 general principles of, 130–131 in infancy and early childhood,

130–136 in late adulthood, 479–481 learning and, 131–132 memory strategies and, 132–134,

192, 277 metacognition and, 193–194,

194, 277 number skills and, learning,

134–136, 135 problem-solving and, 277–278 reasoning and, 277–278 in school-aged children, 192–194 speed of, 276, 276–277, 480 summarized, 279 working memory and, 276–277

information-processing theory cognitive-developmental theory and,

15, 16 defined, 16 summarized, 21

inhibition (shyness), 174 initiative vs. shame guilt, 156, 157 instrumental activities of daily living

(IADLs), defined, 526 instrumental aggression, defined, 248 instrumental orientation, defined,

280–281 integration, defined, 98 integrity versus despair, defined, 508 intellectual disability

defined, 205 learning disabilities distinguished

from, 205 risk factors for, 205

intelligence auditory organization and, 338 bodily-kinesthetic, 196, 196 categories of intellectual skill, 195,

195–196 crystallized, 195, 196, 337, 338,

339, 339 environmental factors and, 200 ethnicity and, 201–203 expertise and, 438–439 fluid, 195, 196, 337, 338, 339, 339 general, 195, 195–196 genetic factors and, 201 heredity and, 45, 48, 200 interpersonal, 196, 196, 197 intrapersonal, 196, 196 linguistic, 196, 196, 197 logical-mathematical, 196, 196, 197 long-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 mechanics of, 437–438, 438 mental abilities and, 335–337, 337

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

674 S u b j e c t I n d e x

intelligence (continued) in middle adulthood, 334–340,

436–439 in middle childhood, 194–203 musical, 196, 196, 197 naturalistic, 196, 196 neuroscience research and, 339–340 P-FIT and, 339–340 practical, 436–437 pragmatics of, 437–438, 438 short-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 socioeconomic factors and, 201–203 spatial, 196, 196, 197 stability of, from 11 to age 90 years,

29, 29 testing (See intelligence tests) theories of (See intelligence theories) views of, 334–336 visual organization and, 338 in young and middle adulthood,

334–340 intelligence quotient (IQ)

defined, 199 determining, 199 environmental factors, 200 ethnicity and, 203 genetic factors, 201 hereditary factors, 200 intellectual disability and, 205 occupational success predicted by,

199–200 to predict outcomes in children’s

lives, 199–200 socioeconomic factors, 201 stereotype threat and, 203

intelligence tests, 198–200 of adults’ intelligence, 437 bias in, 202 culture-fair intelligence tests,

202, 202 development of, 198–199 ethnicity and, 201–203 intelligence quotient, 199 reliability/validity of, 199–200 scores, interpreting, 203 skills in taking, 202 socioeconomic status and, 201–203 Stanford-Binet test, 199 stereotype threat, 203 WISC-V, 199, 200

intelligence theories, 195–198 Gardner’s theory of multiple

intelligences, 196, 196–197 hierarchical, 195, 195–196 psychometricians and, 195 Sternberg’s theory of successful

intelligence, 197–198 interacting with others, 168–176

See also play; prosocial behavior interindividual variability, defined, 334 internal belief system

defined, 490 depression and, 489–490

internal memory aids, defined, 484 internal working model, defined, 160 International Labour Organization, 402 interpersonal intelligence, 196, 196, 197 interpersonal norms, defined, 281 intersensory redundancy, defined, 108 intersex youth, 300 intersubjectivity, defined, 136 intimacy, 321, 323 intimacy versus isolation, defined, 323 intrapersonal intelligence, 196, 196

Intuit, 387 investigative personality type, 304 in vitro fertilization, defined, 52 irregular or rapid-eye-movement (REM)

sleep, defined, 83–84

Jews, 4, 282, 543 job satisfaction, 394–396

alienation and, 395 burnout and, 395–396, 396 defined, 394

joint custody, defined, 238 joints, age-related changes in, 426–427 journals for communicating research

results, 33 joy

as basic emotion, 164 of play, 169–172 regulating, 167

Judaism, 319

Kaiser Family Foundation, 534 kinetic cues, defined, 105 kinkeeper, defined, 447 Klinefelter’s syndrome, 45 knowledge-telling strategy, defined, 212 knowledge-transforming strategy,

defined, 212 Kohlberg’s theory of moral

development, 280–283 evaluating, 282–283 Heinz story, 280 levels of moral reasoning, 280–281

Korean Americans. See Asian Americans

Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS), 429

labels, cultural, 9 labor, stages of, 67–68, 68 labor and delivery. See childbirth Lady Gaga, 348 language

exposure, impact of, 139–140 growth, encouraging, 146

language development, 139–151 communicating with others, 149–150 first words and, 142–146 milestones of, 151 speaking in sentences, 146–148 speech and, 139–142

late adulthood, 458–497 caring for partner in, 523 cognitive processes in, 479–487 creativity in, 485, 485–486 death and, feelings toward, 574–575 death of child or grandchild in, 574 death of partner in, 574–575 demographics of aging in,

458–461, 460 depression in, 488–490 diversity of, 460–461 emotions in, 509–511 family in, 520–525 frail older adults and, 526–528 friendships in, 519–520 great-grandparenthood in, 524–525 grief expressed in, 574–575 health in, 475–478 housing options in, 528–532 information processing in, 479–481 integrity versus despair in, 508 life review in, 508–509 longevity in, 461–464 marriage in, 521–522

memory in, 481–484 mental health and intervention in,

488–497 nutrition in, 477, 478 physical changes in, 465–475 population pyramids and, 458, 459 psychosocial aging and, theories of,

502–507 retirement in, 514–518 same-sex partnerships in, 522 siblings in, 520 sleep in, 476, 477 social issues in, 525–535 socioemotional selectivity in,

520–521 spirituality in, 512–513 Third–Fourth Age distinction and,

464–465 well-being in, 509 widowhood in, 523–524 wisdom in, 486, 487

Latino Americans ADHD and, 208–209 altruism and, 175 career preferences of, 304 cohabitation and, 367 depression and, 489 division of household labor and, 410 educational attainment and, 321 ethnic identity and, 293 grandparenthood and, 451 grief and, 563 healthcare disparities and, 333, 333 intelligence tests and, 201 labor force participation rate and, 398 marriage and, in late life, 521 obesity rates and, 332 parenting and, 375 prejudice and, 257 prosocial behavior of, 175 reproductive changes and, age-

related, 428 retirement and, 516 romantic relationships and, 297 self-esteem and, 295 singlehood and, 366 social support and, in late life, 519 spirituality and, 513 teenage pregnancy and, 299 teen suicide and, 310 terms for describing, 9 unemployment rates and, 407 violence in relationships and, 363

lead, prenatal development and, 62 LeanIn.org, 400, 401, 409, 412 learning, 131–132 learning disability

defined, 205–206 developmental dyslexia, 205–206, 207 impaired reading comprehension,

205, 206, 207 intellectual disability distinguished

from, 205 mathematical learning disability,

205, 207 learning theory, 13–14

behaviorism, 13–14 social learning theory, 14 summarized, 21

left rostral anterior cingulate cortex, 552 leisure, 413–416

activities, types of, 413–414 consequences of, 415–416 defined, 413 developmental changes in, 414–415

long-term effects of, 415 stress and, 415, 416

lesbian and gay couples. See same-sex partnerships

lesbian and gay youth, 300 lesbian youth, 300 levodopa, 495 LGB partnerships, 522 LGBTQ relationships, 367–368 life-course persistent antisocial

behavior, defined, 311 life-course perspective, 20–21

defined, 20 dimensions of, 20 summarized, 22

life-cycle forces in biopsychosocial framework, 7,

9–10 death and, 575 defined, 7 human development created by,

summary of, 21 longevity and, 461 multiple causation and, 19 neuroscience and, 11 in romantic relationships, 362–363

life expectancy. See longevity lifelong learning, 439–440 life review, defined, 508–509 life span, redefining, 4 life-span construct, defined, 346 life-span perspective, 19–20

defined, 19 key features of, 19 SOC model and, 19 summarized, 22

life story defined, 346 McAdams’s life-story model,

346–347 lifestyle, 325–333, 365–372, 476, 477

alcohol consumption and, 326–330 cohabitation and, 366–367 LGBTQ relationships and, 367–368 marriage and, 368–372 nutrition and, 330–332, 477, 478 singlehood and, 365–366 sleep and, 476, 477 smoking and, 326, 326

Lighthouse International, 473 limbic system, 266 linear perspective, defined, 106 linguistic intelligence, 196, 196, 197 living will, defined, 558 locomote, defined, 96 locomotion, 96–99

balance and, 97 beyond walking, 99 coordinating skills and, 98–99 dynamic systems theory and, 96 environmental cues and, 98 overview of, 97 posture and, 97 stepping and, 98 toddling and, 96

logic, in young and middle adulthood, 342–345

logical-mathematical intelligence, 196, 196, 197

longevity, 461–464 active life expectancy and, 462 average, 461–463, 462, 464 dependent life expectancy and, 462 environmental factors in, 462–463 ethnic differences in, 463

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 675

gender differences in, 463 genetic factors in, 462 international differences in, 463, 464 maximum, 461 in US, 1900-2014, 462

longitudinal studies defined, 28 on grief reactions, 564 on primary mental abilities, 336,

336–337, 337 in research design, 28–30 on stability of intelligence from 11 to

age 90 years, 29, 29 summarized, 31

long-term memory, defined, 192 LongTermCare.gov, 531 loss-based selection, 19 Lothian (Scotland) Birth Cohort, 29, 29 low birth weight, defined, 72 low-density lipoproteins (LDLs)

cardiovascular disease and, 331, 433 cholesterol screening and, 330, 331 defined, 328 lowering levels of, 331–332

lung cancer, 550, 550

macrosystem in Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17, 18 defined, 18

macula, 473 mad cry, defined, 82 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 470 magnetoencephalograpy, 470 make-believe, 169–170 malnourished

defined, 90 growth and, 90–91, 91

marijuana, prenatal development and, 60 marital adjustment, defined, 368 marital quality, defined, 368 marital satisfaction, defined, 368 marital success

defined, 368 emotion and, 380 predictors of, 368, 369

marriage, 368–372 early years of, 370–371 first, median age at, 368 happiness in, 371–372 in late adulthood, 521–522 life course–stress process framework,

522, 522 marital adjustment and, 368 marital quality and, 368 marital satisfaction and, 368 marital success and, 368, 369, 380 military deployments and, 371, 371 rites of passage and, 319 same-sex, 367, 369, 378 vulnerability-stress-adaptation model

and, 370, 370 work-related stress and, 234

material exploitation, in elder abuse, 532–533

math skills, 213–215 concepts in, 213–214 gender-related differences in, 179 international comparison,

214–215, 215 learning disability in, 205, 207 techniques for mastering, 214

maturation, 264–269 brain growth in adolescence and,

265, 266 mechanisms of, 266–267

physical growth and, 264–265, 265 rate of, 268–269 sexual, 266 signs of, 264–266 See also puberty

maximum longevity (life expectancy), defined, 461

McAdams’s life-story model, 346–347 meaning-mission fit, defined, 389 measurement in developmental

research, 23–26 photographs in, 24, 34 physiological measures, 24–25 reliability of, 25 representative sampling, 25, 26 sampling behavior with tasks, 24, 24 self-reports, 24 summarized, 25 systematic observation, 23–24 validity of, 25

mechanics of intelligence conceptualization of, 437–438, 438 defined, 437–438, 438

mediators, parents as, 172 Medicare, 534–535

housing options, benefits for, 530, 530–531

medications for ADHD, 208 for alcohol use disorder, 329–330 for Alzheimer’s disease

symptoms, 495 for anxiety disorders, 491 for arthritis, 426 for ASD symptoms, 113 assisted living and, 529 for birth pain, 68, 69 for cholesterol problems, 331–332 for depression, 310, 434, 490 for diabetes, 476 DMARDs, 426 for erectile dysfunction, 430 errors in taking, 479, 484 HCAs, 490 IADLs and, 526 interactions and side effects, 479, 489 MAO inhibitors, 490 Medicare and, 534 medication-induced dementia, 494 memory aids for taking, 479, 484, 484 menopausal hormone therapy,

428–429 metabolism and, age-related changes

in, 490 opioids for pain management, 558 for osteoporosis, 425 for Parkinson’s disease, 495, 496 in physician-assisted suicide, 546, 547 in problem-focused coping, 432–433 for PTSD, 434 regimens, adherence to, 479, 484,

484, 505 SERMs, 425 SSRIs, 490, 491 statins, 331–332 teratogenic drugs, 59–60, 60

meditation, 513 medroxyprogesterone, 429 memory

abnormal changes in, 483 of adolescence, 276–277 aids, 484 autobiographical, 133, 347, 483 in early childhood, 132–134 E-I-E-I-O framework, 484, 484

explicit, 482 gender-related differences in, 179 implicit, 482 in late adulthood, 481–484 long-term acquisition and

retrieval, 338 phonological, 145 preschoolers as eyewitnesses and,

133–134 problems, remediating, 484 short-term acquisition and retrieval

and, 338 working, 481–482 in young and middle adulthood,

338, 347 menarche

defined, 266 response to, 267–268

menopausal hormone therapy (MHT) defined, 428 WHI study on, 429

menopause defined, 428 MHT, 428–429

mental abilities, in young and middle adulthood, 335–337, 337

primary, 335, 335–337 Seattle Longitudinal Study,

336–337, 337 secondary, 335, 335–336, 338, 339

mental age (MA), defined, 198 mental hardware

defined, 130 in information-processing theory, 16

mental health anxiety disorders and, 490–491 dementia and, 491–497 depression and, 488–490 in late adulthood, 488–497 steroid use and, 274

mental operations, defined, 190 mental retardation. See intellectual

disability mental software

defined, 130 in information-processing theory, 16

mentor, defined, 393–394 mercury, prenatal development and, 62 mercy killing, 545 mesoderm, defined, 53 mesosystem

in Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17, 18 defined, 18

meta-analysis defined, 31 in developmental research, 31, 32

metabolic theories of aging, 466 metabolism, defined, 328, 477 metacognition, 193–194, 194

adolescence, 277 school-aged children, 193–194, 194

metacognitive knowledge, defined, 193 metamemory, defined, 193 Mexican Americans. See Latino

Americans microsystem

in Bronfenbrenner’s theory, 17, 17, 18 defined, 18

midbrain, 495 middle adulthood, 356–383, 421–452

abusive relationships in, 363–364 alcohol use in, 326–330 cigarette smoking in, 326, 326 cognitive development in, 334–345,

436–440

crystallized intelligence in, 337, 338, 339, 339

divorce in, 379–382 emotion in, 342–345 exercise and, 434–436 family in, 372–383, 447–452 fluid intelligence in, 337, 338,

339, 339 friendships in, 356–358 health in, 325–333, 430–436 intelligence in, 334–340, 436–439 LGBTQ, 367–368 lifestyle and, 365–372 logic in, 342–345 memory in, 338, 347 mental abilities in, 335–337, 337 nutrition in, 330–332 obesity in, 332 parents/parenting in, 372–376 personality in, 243–247, 441–446 physical changes in, 422–436 physical development in, 325,

422–430 relationships in, 356–383 romantic relationships in, 297–298,

358–363 stress in, 430–434 thinking in, 340–342 violence in relationships, 363–364

middle childhood, 227–257 academic skills of, 209–217 alcohol use in, 307–308 aptitudes of, for school, 194–203 cancer in, 275 cognitive development of, 190–217 depression in, 310 disabilities in, 204–207 electronic media and, 249–252 exercise and, 220 family in, 228–242 friendships in, 243–245 intelligence in, 194–203 motor skills in, 219, 219–221 obesity in, 271–272 occupational development in, 302–305 parent–child relationships in,

228–242 peers in, 243–249 physical development in, 218–221 physical fitness in, 220 physical growth in, 218, 218–219 pregnancy in, 59 relationships in, 228–242 smoking in, 308 socioemotional development in,

227–257 with special needs, 204–209 sports participation in, 220–221 suicide in, 275, 310 teenage mothers in, 59 understanding others in, 253–257 work in, 302–306

midlife correction, defined, 446 military deployments, marriage and,

371, 371 Million Women Study, 429 mindfulness-based stress reduction,

defined, 434 Mini-Mental State Examination, 29 mistrust, trust vs., 156, 156 modeling, altruism and, 175 model of adaptive grieving dynamics

(MAGD), defined, 566 Modern Maturity sexuality study

(AARP), 429

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

676 S u b j e c t I n d e x

molecular genetics, research in, 47, 48 monoamine oxidase (MAO)

inhibitors, 490 monozygotic twins, defined, 46, 47 mood

altruism and, 174 exercise for improving, 435 pubertal changes and, 268

moral reasoning, 279–284 Kohlberg’s theory of moral

development, 280–283 promoting, 283–284 religion and, 283–285

moratorium, 291, 291 Moray House Test, 29 Moro reflex, 81 motion parallax, defined, 105 motor skills

assessing, 81 defined, 96 fine, 100–102 gender differences in, 220 in infants, 96–102 locomotion, 96–99 in middle childhood, 219, 219–221 physical fitness and, 220 sports participation and, 220–221

mourning, defined, 561 See also grief

multichannel encephalography, 470 multidimensional, defined, 334 multidirectionality

defined, 334 in life-span perspective, 19

multiethnic families, 375, 375–376 multiple causation, in life-span

perspective, 19 multiple intelligences, Gardner’s theory

of, 196, 196–197 musical intelligence, 196, 196, 197 Muslims, 4, 8 myelin, defined, 92

naïve theories of children, 126–130 naïve biology, 128–129 naïve physics, 127, 127–128

naltrexone, 330 narrative

defined, 445 grief and, 563

NASA, 507 National Academy of Social

Insurance, 534 National Adult Reading Test, 29 National Alliance for Caregiving &

AARP, 449 National Association for the Education

of Young Children, 409 National Cancer Institute, 64 National Center for Assisted Living,

529–530 National Center for Education Statistics,

321, 322 National Center for Health Statistics,

373, 376, 461, 462, 463, 475, 476, 528, 542

National Center for State Courts, 532 National Eye Institute, 473 National Federation of State High

School Associations, 274 National Heart, Lung, and Blood

Institute, 429 National Institute of Arthritis and

Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, 423, 424, 426, 427

National Institute of Biomedical and Bioengineering, 497

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 84–85

National Institute of Mental Health, 434, 488, 489, 490–491

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), 57, 497

National Institute on Aging, 468, 470, 528

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA), 327, 328

National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, 332

National Institutes of Health (NIH), 32, 84–85, 113

National Osteoporosis Foundation, 423, 424

National Women’s Law Center, 401 Native Americans

Apache menarche celebration, 267, 268

binge drinking and, 328 cardiovascular disease and, 470 career preferences of, 304 ethnic identity and, 293, 294 grandparenthood and, 451 nutrition and, 330 parenting and, 375 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 swaddling and, 82 teen suicide and, 310 terms for describing, 9

Native Hawaiians, 470, 489 naturalistic intelligence, 196, 196 naturalistic observation

defined, 23 summarized, 25

nature–nurture issue, defined, 6 near infrared spectroscopic imaging

(NIRSI), 470 negative reinforcement, 14 negative reinforcement trap,

defined, 232 neglect

of children, 239 in elder abuse, 532

neglected children, defined, 247 Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale

(NBAS), 81–82 nervous system, of infants, 91–95

brain structures, 92, 92–93 specialized brain, growth of, 93–95

neural efficiency hypothesis, defined, 340

neural network connections, 483 neural plate, defined, 92 neuritic plaques, 468, 493, 495 neurochemicals, love and, 362 neurofibrillary tangles, defined, 468 neuroimaging

brain–behavior relations and, 25, 469–470

functional, 470 neuroscience and intelligence and,

339, 339–340 response to threat and, 357 structural, 470

neurons beta-amyloids, 468–469, 469 changes in, age-related, 468–470 defined, 91

neuroimaging, 469–470 neurotransmitters, 469 structure of, 468

neuropsychological tests, 471–472, 483 neuroscience

defined, 10–11 romantic relationships and, 362–363

neuroscience research, 339–340 on binge drinking, 329 emotion-focused, 509–510 neural efficiency hypothesis, 340 P-FIT, 339–340

neurostimulator, 496 neuroticism, defined, 441 neurotransmitters, defined, 91, 469 New York Times, 328 newborns, 80–86

alert inactivity and, 82 assessing, 80, 81–82 crying and, 82–83 infant mortality and, 72–73, 73 preterm or premature, 71–72 reflexes of, 80–82, 81 sleeping and, 82, 83–84 small-for-date babies, 72, 72 states of, 82–85 temperament of, 85–86 waking inactivity and, 82 well-being of, 81

NIAAA, 328, 329 NICHD Early Child Care Research

Network, 161 niche-picking, defined, 49 nicotine. See cigarette smoking NIHSeniorHealth, 488, 489 non-invasive prenatal testing (NIPT),

defined, 66 nonshared environmental influences,

defined, 50 North American Menopause Society,

428, 429 Not Alone campaign, 363 NotAlone.gov, 363 nuclear family, defined, 372 number skills, learning, 134–136, 135

approximate number system, 134–135

counting, 135 nursing home, defined, 530 nutrition

in adolescence, 270–273 breastfeeding and, 83, 89, 90 cancer and, 10, 332, 477 cardiovascular disease and, 10,

332, 477 diabetes mellitus and, 332, 476, 477 Dietary Guidelines for Americans

2015–2020, 477 eating disorders and, 272–273 ethnicity and, 330 infant growth and, 89–90 in late adulthood, 477, 478 obesity and, 271–272, 332, 332 in prenatal development, 57 in young and middle adulthood,

330–332

obedience orientation, defined, 280 obesity, 271–272

body mass index and, 271, 332, 332 breastfed babies and, 89 causes of, 271–272 in middle childhood, 271–272 weight-loss programs for, 272 in young and middle adulthood, 332

object permanence, defined, 121 objects

perception of, 106–107, 107, 108 understanding, in sensorimotor

period, 121 observational learning, imitation/

counterimitation and, 231 occipital cortex, 92 occupational choice, 389–391

ethnicity and, 399–400 gender differences in, 397, 398 theories, 390, 390–391 vocational maturity and, 390

occupational development, 392–394 crystallization and, 302 developmental coaches and, 393–394 ethnicity and, 399–400 expectations in, 392–393 implementation and, 303 mentors and, 393–394 in middle childhood, 302–305 personality-type theory and,

303–304 social cognitive career theory and,

304–305, 305 specification and, 303 women and, 398–399, 399

occupational transitions, 404–407 advice for adults trying to

manage, 407 occupational insecurity and, 405 retraining workers and, 404–405 unemployment and, 405–407

OECD, 73, 410, 411 older adults. See late adulthood one-to-one principle, defined, 135 online dating, 359–360 online friendships, 357 open adoption, defined, 237 openness to experience, defined, 442 operant conditioning

defined, 13, 132 Skinner’s, 13, 14

opioid peptides, 329 opioids for pain management, 558 optimization, 20

See also selective optimization with compensation (SOC) model

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), 214, 463–464

organization, defined, 192 orienting response, defined, 131 osteoarthritis, defined, 426, 426

See also arthritis osteoporosis

defined, 423–425 estrogen and, 428 medications for, 425 osteoarthritis and rheumatoid

arthritis compared to, 427 vitamin D and calcium intake and,

424, 425 others, understanding, 253–257

describing others, 253–254 prejudiced against others, 256–257 Selman’s theory of perspective taking

and, 254–255, 255 thoughts of, 254–256

outcome expectations, 304, 390 ovarian cancer, 476, 477 overextension, defined, 144 overregularization, defined, 147 overweight. See obesity oxytocin, 174

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 677

Pacific Islanders, 325, 489 pain cry, defined, 82 PALB2, 476 palliative care, defined, 556 Palmar reflex, 81 pancreatic cancer, 543, 549 parallel play, defined, 169 parental behavior, 231–233

coaching and, 231 direct instruction and, 231 feedback and, 231 imitation/counterimitation and, 231 marital conflict and, 233 modeling, 231 punishment and, 231–233 reinforcement and, 231–232 work-related stress and, 234

parent-child relationships, 234–235 attachment during infancy and, 159 characteristics of children

contributing to, 234–235 child maltreatment and, 239–242 conflict between parents and, 233 divorce and, 238 ethnic diversity and, 375 grief and, 573 in middle childhood, 228–242 play and, 172 storm and stress, myth of, 296 teenage smoking and, 308

parenting styles, 229–235 authoritarian, 229 authoritative, 229 changes in, as children grow, 234 cultural variations in, 230–231 genetic influences on, 231 permissive, 230 socioeconomic variations in, 230–231 temperament/behavior of child and,

234–235 uninvolved, 230

parents/parenting adjusting to parenthood, 69–70 aging parents, 448–450 behavior of (See parental behavior) child maltreatment and, 239–242 as coaches, 172 cost of raising a child from birth to

18, 373 deciding whether to have children,

373–375 ethnic diversity and, 375–376 FMLA and, 374, 409 marital system’s influences on,

233–234 as mediators, 172 obesity and, 271 play influenced by, 172 as playmates, 172 postpartum depression and, 70 single parents, 376, 376–377 as social directors, 172 step-, foster-, adoptive, and same-sex

couples, 377–378 styles of (See parenting styles) teenage mothers and, 59 in young and middle adulthood,

372–376 See also parent-child relationships

parietal cortex, 92, 94, 339 parieto-frontal integration theory

(P-FIT), defined, 339–340 Parkinson’s disease, defined, 495, 497 Parkinson’s Disease Foundation,

469, 495

part-time employment, 305–306 passion, defined, 395 Passion Model, 396, 396 passive euthanasia, defined, 545 peers, 243–249

aggressive children and their victims, 248–249 (See also bullying)

friendship and, 243–245 groups and, 245–246 popular children and, 246–247 rejected children and, 246–248

perception, 102–109 of color, 104, 105 defined, 103 of depth, 105–106 of faces, 107 hearing, 103–104 integrating sensory information,

108–109 of objects, 106–107, 107, 108 seeing, 104–108 smell, 103 of speech, 139–141 taste, 103, 103 touch, 103, 103

perimenopause, defined, 428 period of the fetus, 55–56, 55–56

age of viability, 56 defined, 55 highlights of, 55–56

permissive parenting, defined, 230 persistent vegetative state, defined, 544 personal control beliefs

defined, 348–349 developmental patterns of, 349, 349

personal fable, defined, 292, 292 personality

adjustment, 443 adolescents and, 303–304, 304 behavioral genetics and, 45 birth order and, 238 describing, in others, 253 development of, 7, 8, 12 five-factor trait model of, 441–443 frontal cortex and, 92 gender stereotypes and, 177, 178 generativity and, 444–446 growth, 443 heredity and, 45 identity and, 293 life-span construct and, 346–347 life transition in midlife and, 446 measuring, psychometricians and, 195 in middle adulthood, 441–446 midlife priorities and, changes in,

443–446, 444 personal control beliefs and, 348–349 personality-type theory and,

303–304, 304 possible selves and, 347–348 psychological forces in, 7, 8 temperament and, 85, 86 TESSERA model of, 443, 444 in young adulthood, 243–247

personality adjustment, defined, 443 personality growth, defined, 443 personality-type theory, 303–304, 304

career development and, 303–304 defined, 303 personality types in, 303, 304

perspective taking altruism and, 175 empathy and, 173, 176 oxytocin and, 174 Selman’s theory of, 254–255, 255

Pew Research Center, 4, 366, 383, 458, 460

phenotypes associated with single pairs of

genes, 43 behavioral, 46, 46, 47 defined, 41 dominant, 43, 43 genotype-phenotype link, 47–48 recessive, 43, 43

phenylketonuria (PKU) defined, 43 genotype–phenotype link in, 47–48

phonological awareness, defined, 210 phonological memory, defined, 145 physical abuse

aggressive behaviors and, 363 of children, 239 in dating, 301 in elder abuse, 532

physical changes, in late adulthood, 465–475

biological theories, 466–467 physiological changes, 468–475

physical development in adolescence, 263–275 appearance, 422–423 bones, 423–425 growth, 87–91, 88, 89, 218, 218–219 in infants, 87–95 joints, 426–427 in middle adulthood, 422–430 in middle childhood, 218–221 motor skills, 96–102, 219, 219–221 nervous system, 91–95 nutrition and, 270–273 physical fitness, 220–221,

274–275, 275 puberty, 264–269 reproductive changes, 427–430 sports participation, 220–221 well-being, 275 in young adulthood, 325

physical fitness in adolescence, 274–275, 275 in middle childhood, 220 sports participation and, 220–221,

274–275, 275 physical growth

of brain in adolescence, 265, 266 of infants, 87–91, 88, 89 malnutrition and, 90–91, 91 in middle childhood, 218, 218–219 nutrition and, 89–90 of specialized brain, 93–95 in young adulthood, 325

physical limitations (PLIMs), defined, 526

physical punishment, 232, 240 physician-assisted suicide, defined, 546 physics, naïve, 127, 127–128 physiological changes of aging,

468–475 in cardiovascular system, 470–472 in neurons, 468–470 in respiratory system, 472 sensory changes, 472–475

physiological measures, 24–25 Piaget’s theory of cognitive

development, 15, 15–16, 118–130, 190–192

criticisms of, 125–126, 192 evaluating, 125–126 guidelines for fostering cognitive

development, 125

naïve theories of children and, 126–130

stages of, 15, 15, 190–191, 192 summarized, 21 See also cognitive development

pictorial cues, defined, 106 Pioneer Network, 531 placenta, defined, 53 plasticity

defined, 335 in life-span perspective, 19

play gender differences in, 170–171 joys of, 169–172 make-believe, 169–170 parental influence on, 172 solitary, 170

playmates, parents as, 172 polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs),

prenatal development and, 61, 62, 63, 64

polygenic inheritance, defined, 45 popular children, defined, 246–247 population pyramid, defined, 458, 459 populations

defined, 25 in representative sampling, 25, 26

positive reinforcement, 14 positron emission tomography

(PET), 470 possible selves, defined, 347–348 postconventional level, defined, 281 postformal thought, defined, 341 postpartum depression, 69–70 post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),

defined, 371, 434 posture, 97 practical ability, defined, 197 practical intelligence, 436–437

applications of, 437 defined, 437 measuring, 436, 437

pragmatics of intelligence conceptualization of, 437–438, 438 defined, 437

preattachment, 158 preconventional level, 280–281 preeclampsia, 71 prefrontal cortex, 132, 320, 339, 343,

395, 483, 509–510, 511 preimplantation genetic screening

(PGS), defined, 52 prejudice, 256–257 prenatal development, 51–66

age of mother and, 58–59 defined, 51 embryo, 53–54, 54 fetal medicine and, 66 fetus, 55–56, 55–56 genetic counseling and, 64–65 nutrition and, 57 prenatal diagnosis and, 65–66 risk factors, 57–64 stress and, 58 teratogens and, 59–64 zygote, 51–52, 53, 53

prenatal diagnosis, 65–66 preoperational period, 120, 122–125

appearance as reality, 123, 124 centration and, 123, 125 egocentrism and, 122–123 overview of, 125

preoperational thought, 15 prereflective reasoning, 341 presbycusis, defined, 473

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

678 S u b j e c t I n d e x

presbyopia, defined, 472 preschoolers as eyewitnesses, 133–134 presenillin-1 (PSEN1), 493 presenillin-2 (PSEN2), 493 President’s Council on Physical Fitness,

Sports and Nutrition, 220, 274 preterm (premature), defined, 71–72 preventive adaptations, defined, 505 Preventive and Corrective Proactivity

(PCP), 505–506, 506 pride

emergence of, 165 expression of, 165, 166

primary appraisal, defined, 432 primary control, defined, 349, 349 primary mental abilities, defined, 335,

335–337 primary sex characteristics, defined, 266 Prince, 541, 541 private speech, defined, 138 proactivity, defined, 505 problem-focused coping, defined,

432–433 problem-solving, in adolescence,

277–278 processing speed, of adolescents, 276,

276–277 progesterone, 428, 429 progestin, 428, 429 prolapsed umbilical cord, 71 prosocial behavior, 172–175

altruism distinguished from, 173 defined, 172–173 factors contributing to,

summarized, 176 heritability of, 174 opportunities for, 175

prostate cancer, 430, 476 Protestants, 4 proximodistal principle, defined, 54 psychodynamic theories, 12–13

defined, 12 Erikson’s psychosocial theory, 12–13 summarized, 21

psychological abuse of children, 240 in elder abuse, 532 psychological capital theory,

defined, 394 psychological forces

in biopsychosocial framework, 7, 8 competence and, 503 death and, 575 defined, 7 human development created by,

summary of, 21 malnutrition and, 91

psychological measures, 25 psychometricians, defined, 195 psychomotor speed, 510 psychosocial aging, theories of, 502–507

competence–environmental press theory, 503–505

goal of healthy aging, 502–503 PCP model, 505–506, 506

psychosocial development, Erikson’s stages of. See Erikson’s psychosocial theory

psychosocial theory, defined, 12 See also Erikson’s psychosocial theory

puberty, 264–269 body image and, 267 changes in, 264–269 defined, 264 menarche and, 267–268

moodiness and, 268 psychological impact of, 267–269 spermarche and, 266, 268 timing of pubertal events, 264–269

punishment benefits of, 232 defined, 14, 231 negative reinforcement trap and, 232 physical, 232, 240 problems with, 232 time-out, 232–233

purpose, defined, 157

qualitative research, defined, 28 quasi-reflective reasoning, 341, 341 queer youth, 300

race sociocultural forces and, 7, 8–9 terms for describing, 9 See also ethnicity

racism, 257 Rape, Abuse, and Incest National

Network, 363 Raven’s Progressive Matrices, 29, 202 reading skills, 209–212

comprehension and, 206–207, 207, 210, 211–212

foundation of, 210 sounding out, 211 word recognition and, 210, 211

realistic personality type, 304 reality shock, defined, 393 reappraisal, defined, 432 reasoning

in adolescence, 277–284 moral, 279–284

recessive, defined, 42 See also alleles

reciprocal relationships, 158 recursive thinking, defined, 256 referential style, defined, 145 reflective judgment

defined, 339 stages of, 341, 341–342

reflective reasoning, 339 reflexes

defined, 80 evaluation of, 81–82 major, overview of, 81 of newborns, 80–82, 81

regular or nonREM sleep, defined, 83–84

reinforcement defined, 13, 14, 231 negative reinforcement trap, 232

rejected children causes and consequences of, 247–248 defined, 247

rejection, 247–248 relational aggression, defined, 179 relationships

family and, 228–242, 372–383 friendships, 356–358 grief and, 563 LGBTQ, 367–368 lifestyle and, 365–372 in middle childhood, 228–242 peers and, 243–249 romantic relationships, 297–298,

358–363 understanding others, 253–257 violence in, 363–364 in young and middle adulthood,

356–383

reliability defined, 25 in human development research, 25

religion adolescents’ moral reasoning and,

283–285 death and, definition of, 543 death education and, 554 describing others and, 253 divorce and, 379 euthanasia and, 545 grief process and, 561 Kohlberg’s theory and, 282 lesbian and gay couples’ right to be

parents and, 378 marital success and, 369, 371 mindful meditation or contemplation

and, 513 reflective judgment and, 341 spirituality in late adulthood and,

512–513 remarriage, 239 representative sampling, 25, 26 reproductive changes, age-related,

427–430 men, 430 in women, 428–430

reproductive technology, assisted, 52 research. See developmental research research design, 26–32

correlational study, 26–27 cross-sectional study, 30 experimental studies, 27–28 longitudinal study, 28–30 sequential design, 30–31 summarized, 31

resistant attachment, defined, 159 respiratory system

age-related changes in, 472 in fetal development, 55

responsibility, altruism and, 173 retirement, 514–518

adjustment to, 516 employment after, 516–517, 517 ethnic differences in, 515–516 gender differences in, 515–516 reasons for retiring, 515–516 volunteering after, 517–518

returning adult students, defined, 322 rheumatoid arthritis, defined, 426, 426

See also arthritis right caudate nucleus, 552 Ritalin (methylphenidate), 208 rites of passage, defined, 319 role confusion, 290 role transitions, 319–320

cross-cultural evidence of, 319–320 defined, 319

Roman Catholics, 544 romantic attachment, 360–361, 361 romantic relationships, 297–298, 358–363

in adulthood, 358–359 couple-forming behaviors in, 360 culture and, 360–362, 361 desirable mates in, characteristics

of, 360 developmental forces in, 362–363 falling in love, 359–362 infatuation in, 358–359

rooting reflex, 80, 81 rubella, prenatal development and, 61, 61

same-sex friendships, 358 same-sex partnerships

in late adulthood, 522

LGB partnerships, 522 LGBTQ relationships, 367–368 marriage, 367, 369, 378 parenting in, 377–378

sample defined, 25 in representative sampling, 25, 26

sampling behavior with tasks, 24, 24 sandwich generation, defined, 447 scaffolding, defined, 137–138 scenario, defined, 346 schemes, defined, 118 Schindler’s list, 282 school-age children. See adolescence;

middle childhood school-based influences on student

achievement, 216 Seattle Longitudinal Study,

336–337, 337 secondary appraisal, defined, 432 secondary control, defined, 349, 349 secondary mental abilities, 335,

335–336, 338, 339 defined, 335 descriptions of, 338 developmental trends in, 337,

339, 339 secondary sex characteristics,

defined, 266 secondhand smoke, prenatal

development and, 60 secure attachment, defined, 158–159 Seeds of Native Health campaign, 330 seeing, 104–108

color, 104, 105 depth, 105–106 perceiving faces, 107 perceiving objects, 106–107,

107, 108 See also vision

selective estrogen receptor modulators (SERMs), defined, 425

selective optimization with compensation (SOC) model, 19–20

defined, 19 life-span perspective and, 19–20 summarized, 22

selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), 490

self-absorption, adolescent, 291–292 self-awareness, 109–113

emergence of, 109–113 self-concept and, 110 theory of mind and, 111, 111–113

self-concept, 290 See also identity

self-conscious emotions, 165 See also emotion

self-efficacy defined, 14 social cognitive career theory

and, 304 self-esteem, 294–295

influences on, 295 self-neglect, elder abuse and, 532 self-reflective stage, 255, 255 self-reports

defined, 24 in human development research, 24 summarized, 25

Selman’s theory of perspective taking, 254–255, 255

semantic memory, defined, 482 sense of place, defined, 528

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

S u b j e c t I n d e x 679

sensorimotor period, 120, 120–122 defined, 120 environment and, adapting to and

exploring, 120–121 objects and, understanding, 121 symbols and, using, 121–122

sensorimotor stage, 15, 15 sensory changes, age-related, 472–475

hearing, 473–475, 474 vision, 472, 472–473

sensory information, integrating, 108–109

sentence cues, 144 sentences, speaking in, 146–148

grammar, acquisition of, 147–148 from two words to complex

sentences, 146–147 separation distress, defined, 567 sequential design

defined, 30 five-factor trait model and, 441 in research design, 30–31 summarized, 31

serotonin, 329, 362, 469, 495 Sex, Romance, and Relationships study

(AARP), 429 sex chromosomes, defined, 40 Sex in America study (AARP), 429 sexual abuse

of children, 239 in elder abuse, 532

sexual behavior, 298–301 contraception and, 300 dating violence and, 300–301 sexual minority youth and, 300 STIs and, 298–299, 299 teen pregnancy and, 299–300

sexually transmitted infections (STIs), 298–299, 299

sexual maturation, 266 See also puberty

sexual minority youth, 300 sexual orientation, 300 shyness. See inhibition (shyness) siblings, 235–238

adopted children and, 236–237 birth order and, 237–238

siblings, in late adulthood, 520 sickle-cell trait

CRISPR and, 66 defined, 42 genetic mechanism responsible for,

41–43, 42 preimplantation genetic screening

and, 52 recessive alleles for, 42, 43, 66 red blood cells of, 40, 40, 41

simple social play, defined, 169 Sinemet®, 495, 496 singlehood, 365–366 single parents, 376, 376–377 single photon emission computerized

tomography (SPECT), 470 skeletal maturity, defined, 423 skin cancer, 424 Skinner’s operant conditioning, 13, 14 sleep, in late adulthood, 476, 477 sleeping, defined, 82

newborns and, 82, 83–84 nonREM sleep, 83–84 REM sleep, 83–84 sudden infant death syndrome and,

84–85 small-for-date babies, 72, 72 smell, sense of, 103

smoking. See cigarette smoking social clock, defined, 346 social cognitive career theory (SCCT)

career development and, 304–305, 305

defined, 390, 390 social contract, defined, 281 social convoy, defined, 519 social directors, parents as, 172 social influences, gender-related

differences in, 179 social-informational stage, 255, 255 social issues, in late adulthood, 525–535

elder abuse and neglect, 532–533 frail older adults, 526–528, 527 housing options, 528–532 Medicare, 534–535 Social Security, 533–534

socialization, defined, 227, 228 social learning theory, 14, 21 social media, 251

cyberbullying and, 248, 252 social personality type, 304 social policy, developmental research

and, 33 social referencing, defined, 167 social role, defined, 177 Social Security, 533–534 Social Security and Medicare Boards of

Trustees, 534 social smiles, defined, 164 social system, assessing, 81 social system morality, defined, 281 societal stage, 255, 255 sociocultural definitions of death,

543–544 sociocultural forces

in alcohol use, 327 in biopsychosocial framework, 7, 8–9 death and, 575–576 defined, 7 grammatical development and, 147 human development created by,

summary of, 21 identity and, 347 life-course perspective and, 20 life span and, 4 low birth weight and, 72 malnutrition and, 91 neuroscience and, 11 in occupational selection, 391 pragmatics of intelligence and, 438 in romantic relationships, 362–363

socioeconomic status birth complications and, 71 delinquency and, 312 health and, 330–331 intelligence and, 201–203 parenting styles and, 230–231 prenatal risk and, 64 puberty and, 267

socioemotional development in adolescence, 289–313 attachment and, 157–162 delinquency and, 310–313 depression and, 308–310 drug use and, 307–308 electronic media and, 249–252 emotions and, 163–168 Erikson’s psychosocial theory and,

156, 156–157 family relationships and, 228–242 gender identity and, 176–184 gender roles and, 176–184 identity and, 290–296

in infancy and early childhood, 154–184

interacting with others and, 168–176 in middle childhood, 227–257 peers and, 243–249 romantic relationships and, 297–298 self-esteem and, 294–295 sexual behavior and, 298–301 storm and stress and, myth of, 296 understanding others and, 253–257 work and, 296–306

socioemotional selectivity defined, 520–521 in late adulthood, 520–521

software for nonexperts, 440 spatial ability, gender-related differences

in, 179, 180 spatial intelligence, 196, 196, 197 special needs, children with, 204–209

ADHD and, 208–209 children with disability, 204–207 gifted children, 204

specification, defined, 303 speech, 139–142

identifying words and, 140–141 language exposure and, impact of,

139–140 perceiving, 139–141 private, 138 speaking in sentences and, 146–148 steps to, 141–142

speed dating, 359 speed of processing, defined, 480 spermarche

defined, 266 response to, 268

spina bifida, defined, 57 spirituality

grief and, 563 hospice and, 557 in late adulthood, 512–513 See also religion

spiritual support, defined, 513 sports participation

in adolescence, 274–275 in middle childhood, 220–221

stable-order principle, defined, 135 stagnation, defined, 444 Stalevo®, 495, 496 Stanford-Binet test, 199, 205 states, of newborn, 82–85

alert inactivity, 82 assessing, 81 crying, 82–83 sleeping, 82, 83–84 waking inactivity, 82

statins, 331–332 stem cell research, 33, 468 step-parenting, 377–378 stepping, 98 stepping reflex, 80, 81 stereotype threat, defined, 203 Sternberg’s theory of successful

intelligence, 197–198 storm and stress, myth of, 296 stranger wariness, defined, 164 Strange Situation, 158, 159 strength, in young adulthood, 325 stress

appraisals of, 423 of caring for aging parents, 449–450 coping and, 423–433 defined, 58 divorce and, 238, 382 first pregnancy and, 373, 374, 375

friendship networks and, 357 hormones released in response

to, 329 leisure and, 415, 416 in LGBTQ relationships, 367 marriage and, 370, 371 in middle adulthood, 430–434 physical effects of, 433 as physiological response, 430–431 prenatal development and, 58 psychological effects of, 434 PTSD, 371, 434 remarriage and, 383 of single parenthood, 377 storm and, myth of, 296 transactional model of, 431 vulnerability–stress–adaptation

model and, 370, 370 of work-family-school conflict, 322 work-related, 234, 364, 430

stress and coping paradigm, defined, 431

stress urinary incontinence, 428 strokes, or cerebral vascular accidents

(CVAs), defined, 470 structural neuroimaging, defined, 470 structured life review, 508–509 structured observations

defined, 23 summarized, 25 in systematic observation, 23–24

structure of intelligence, defined, 335 subjective well-being, defined, 509 successful intelligence, Sternberg’s

theory of, 197–198 sucking reflex, 80, 81 sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS),

defined, 84–85 suicide

depression and, 310 in middle childhood, 275 physician-assisted, 546, 547

Super’s theory of occupational development, 390–391, 391

swaddling, 82 symbols

using, 121–122 words as, 142

synaptic pruning, defined, 93 syphilis

features of, 299 prenatal development and, 61, 61

systematic observation defined, 23 in human development research,

23–24 summarized, 25

systems view of family, 228–229

taste, sense of, 103, 103 tau proteins, 469 teacher-based influences on student

achievement, 216–217 teenagers. See adolescence; middle

childhood telegraphic speech, defined, 147 teleological explanations, defined, 128 television programs, 250 telomerase, defined, 466 telomeres, defined, 466, 467 temperament

attachment and, 157 defined, 85 depression and, 309 environmental contributions to, 86

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

680 S u b j e c t I n d e x

temperament (continued) genetic influence on, 174, 176 hereditary contributions to, 85 of newborns, 85–86 stability of, 86 theory of, 85

teratogens, 59–64 defined, 59 diseases, 60–61, 61 drugs, 59–60, 60 environmental hazards, 61–62 harm associated with, challenges in

determining, 64 influence of, on prenatal

development, 62–64, 63 terminal buttons, defined, 91 terror management theory, defined, 552 TESSERA model, 443, 444 testicular cancer, 64 testosterone

aggression and, 311 decline in, 430 gender typing and, 183 performance-enhancing drugs and, 274 puberty and, 266

texture gradient, defined, 106 thalidomide, 59, 62 thanatology, defined, 542 theory, defined, 12 theory of mind, 111, 111–113

in children with autism, 112–113 cultural differences in, 111–112 defined, 111

thinking adolescent, characteristics of,

291–292, 292 in adulthood, 340–342 onset of, 118–130 (See also cognitive

development) postformal thought, 341 preschooler, essentialist, 129 reflective judgment and, 341,

341–342 See also information processing

Third Age, 464–465 third-person stage, 255, 255 time-out, defined, 232–233 tissue plasminogen activator (tPA), 471 tobacco. See cigarette smoking toddler, defined, 96 toddling, defined, 96 touch, sense of, 103, 103 toxoplasmosis, prenatal development

and, 61 transgender and gender nonconforming

(TGNC), 522 transgender youth, 300 transient ischemic attacks (TIAs),

defined, 471 traumatic brain injury (TBI), 496 traumatic distress, defined, 567 true attachment, 158 trust

vs. mistrust, 156, 156 secure attachment and, 158, 160

Turner’s syndrome, 45 type 1 diabetes, defined, 475 type 2 diabetes, defined, 475

ultrasound, defined, 65, 65 umbilical cord, defined, 54

underextension, defined, 144 undifferentiated stage, 255, 255 unemployment, 405–407

advice for adults trying to manage, 407

effects of, 406, 407, 407 financial support and, 406 politics of, 406

UNICEF, 73, 89 UNICEF-WHO-The World Bank, 90 uninvolved parenting, defined, 230 universal and context-specific-

development issue, defined, 6–7 universal ethical principles, defined, 281 urinary incontinence, 428 U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 380 U.S. Department of Agriculture, 330,

373 U.S. Department of Health and Human

Services, 240, 271 U.S. Department of Labor, 397, 398, 407 U.S. Department of Labor Women’s

Bureau, 397, 400, 401 U.S. Public Health Service, 84 University of Massachusetts

Medical School Center for Mindfulness, 434

validity defined, 25 in human development research, 25

Van Gogh, V., 3 vascular dementia, defined, 471 verbal ability, gender-related differences

in, 179 vernix, defined, 55 very low birth weight, defined, 72 video games, 251–252

visual-spatial skills and, 251 Vietnam War, 11, 438 violence

continuum of progressive behaviors and, 364

dating, 300–301 on television, 250 in young and middle adulthood

relationships, 363–364 vision

age-related changes in, 472, 472–473 in fetal development, 55 See also seeing

visual acuity, defined, 104 visual cliff, defined, 105 visual expansion, defined, 105 visual organization, 338 visual-spatial skills, video games

and, 251 vocational maturity, defined, 390 volunteering, in late life, 517–518 vulnerability-stress-adaptation model,

defined, 370, 370 Vygotsky’s theory of social

development, 16–17, 136–138 private speech and, 138 scaffolding and, 137–138 summarized, 21 zone of proximal development

and, 137

waking activity, defined, 82 Watson’s view of behaviorism, 13, 21

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-V (WISC-V), 199, 200

Wechsler Logical Memory Test, 29 weight-loss programs, 272 well-being

in adolescence, 275 in late adulthood, 509 in newborns, 81 unemployment and, 406, 407

whole-brain death, defined, 544 widowhood, 523–524 will, defined, 157 Winehouse, Amy, 541, 541 Winfrey, Oprah, 196 wisdom, in late adulthood, 486, 487 women

bias and discrimination in workplace and, 400–402

conception and, 52 equal pay for equal work and,

401, 401 first pregnancy and, 59 glass ceiling and, 400 glass cliff and, 400 occupational selection and

development and, 397, 398, 399–400

reproductive changes in, age-related, 428–430

sexual harassment and, 402 Women’s Health Initiative (WHI), 429 Women’s Health Research Institute,

428, 429 WomensHealth.gov, 428 word recognition, defined, 210 words

bilingualism and, 145 developmental change in

learning, 144 fast mapping and, 143–144 first, 142–146 identifying, 140–141 individual differences in learning,

144–145 language growth and,

encouraging, 146 learning styles and, 146–147 phonological memory and, 145 reading comprehension and,

206–207, 207, 210 as symbols, 142 from two words to complex sentences

and, 146–147 work, 302–306, 388–413

age discrimination and, 403 career development and, 302–305 child care and, 161–162 dependent care and, 409–410 employed caregivers and, 408–409 family and, 408–413 gender bias discrimination and,

400–402 in late life, 516–517, 517 meaning of, 388–4389 occupational choice and, 389–391 occupational success predicted by IQ

scores and, 199–200 part-time employment and, 305–306 possible selves and, 347–348 sexual harassment and, 402 stress related to, 234, 430

See also job satisfaction; occupational choice; occupational development; occupational transitions

work–family conflict, defined, 410 working memory, defined, 192, 482 World Health Organization, 90, 271,

363, 364, 491, 528 writing skills, 212–213

contributing to improved writing, 212–213

knowledge-telling strategy and, 212 knowledge-transforming strategy

and, 212 mechanical requirements and, 213 revising, 213 teaching effectively, 213

X chromosomes, 40, 44, 45 X-rays, 44, 62, 470 XXX syndrome, 45 XYY complement, 45

Y chromosomes, 40, 41, 44, 45 young adulthood, 356–383

abusive relationships in, 363–364 alcohol use in, 326–330 becoming rich and famous in, 324 cigarette smoking in, 326, 326 cognitive development in,

334–345 crystallized intelligence in, 337, 338,

339, 339 death of child in, 573 divorce in, 379–382 emotion in, 342–345 family in, 372–383 fluid intelligence in, 337, 338,

339, 339 friendships in, 356–358 grief expressed by, 572–573 growth in, 325 health in, 325–333 intelligence in, 334–340 LGBTQ, 367–368 lifestyle and, 365–372 logic in, 342–345 memory in, 338, 347 mental abilities in, 335–337, 337 nutrition in, 330–332 obesity in, 332 parents/parenting in, 372–376 personality in, 243–247 physical development in, 325 relationships in, 356–383 romantic relationships, 297–298,

358–363 strength in, 325 thinking in, 340–342 violence in relationships, 363–364 See also emerging adulthood

zone of maximum comfort, defined, 504

zone of maximum performance potential, defined, 504

zone of proximal development, defined, 137

zygote defined, 52 period of, 51–52, 53, 53

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

cengage.com/mindtap

Fit your coursework into your hectic life. Make the most of your time by learning your way. Access the resources you need to succeed wherever, whenever.

Study with digital flashcards, listen to audio textbooks, and take quizzes.

Review your current course grade and compare your progress with your peers.

Get the free MindTap Mobile App and learn wherever you are.

Break Limitations. Create your own potential, and be unstoppable with MindTap.

MINDTAP. POWERED BY YOU.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Copyright 2019 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. WCN 02-200-203

  • Cover
  • About the Author
  • Brief Contents
  • Contents
  • Perface
  • To the Student
  • Neuroscience Index
  • Diversity Index
  • Chapter 1: The Study of Human Development������������������������������������������������
    • 1.1 Thinking about Development
    • 1.2 Developmental Theories���������������������������������
    • 1.3 Doing Developmental Research���������������������������������������
    • Summary��������������
    • Key Terms����������������
  • Part 1: Prenatal Development, Infancy, and Early Childhood�����������������������������������������������������������������
    • Chapter 2: Biological Foundations: Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth
      • 2.1 In the Beginning: 23 Pairs of Chromosomes����������������������������������������������������
      • 2.2 From Conception to Birth�����������������������������������
      • 2.3 Influences on Prenatal Development���������������������������������������������
      • 2.4 Labor and Delivery�����������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 3: Tools for Exploring the World: Physical, Perceptual, and Motor Development��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 3.1 The Newborn����������������������
      • 3.2 Physical Development�������������������������������
      • 3.3 Moving and Grasping: Early Motor Skills��������������������������������������������������
      • 3.4 Coming to Know the World: Perception�����������������������������������������������
      • 3.5 Becoming Self-Aware������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 4: The Emergence of Thought and Language: Cognitive Development in Infancy and Early Childhood
      • 4.1 The Onset of Thinking: Piaget's Account
      • 4.2 Information Processing during Infancy and Early Childhood
      • 4.3 Mind and Culture: Vygotsky's Theory
      • 4.4 Language�������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 5: Entering the Social World: Socioemotional Development in Infancy and Early Childhood
      • 5.1 Beginnings: Trust and Attachment�������������������������������������������
      • 5.2 Emerging Emotions����������������������������
      • 5.3 Interacting with Others����������������������������������
      • 5.4 Gender Roles and Gender Identity�������������������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
  • Part 2: School-Age Children and Adolescents��������������������������������������������������
    • Chapter 6: Off to School: Cognitive and Physical Development in Middle Childhood���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 6.1 Cognitive Development��������������������������������
      • 6.2 Aptitudes for School�������������������������������
      • 6.3 Special Children, Special Needs������������������������������������������
      • 6.4 Academic Skills��������������������������
      • 6.5 Physical Development�������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 7: Expanding Social Horizons: Socioemotional Development in Middle Childhood
      • 7.1 Family Relationships�������������������������������
      • 7.2 Peers����������������
      • 7.3 Electronic Media���������������������������
      • 7.4 Understanding Others�������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 8: Rites of Passage: Physical and Cognitive Development in Adolescence
      • 8.1 Pubertal Changes���������������������������
      • 8.2 Health�����������������
      • 8.3 Information Processing during Adolescence
      • 8.4 Reasoning about Moral Issues
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 9: Moving into the Adult Social World: Socioemotional Development in Adolescence�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 9.1 Identity and Self-Esteem�����������������������������������
      • 9.2 Romantic Relationships and Sexuality�����������������������������������������������
      • 9.3 The World of Work����������������������������
      • 9.4 The Dark Side������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
  • Part 3: Young and Middle Adulthood�����������������������������������������
    • Chapter 10: Becoming an Adult: Physical, Cognitive, and Personality Development in Young Adulthood���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 10.1 Emerging Adulthood������������������������������
      • 10.2 Physical Development and Health�������������������������������������������
      • 10.3 Cognitive Development���������������������������������
      • 10.4 Who Do You Want to Be? Personality in Young Adulthood�����������������������������������������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 11: Being with Others: Relationships in Young and Middle Adulthood���������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 11.1 Relationship Types and Issues�����������������������������������������
      • 11.2 Lifestyles and Relationships����������������������������������������
      • 11.3 Family Dynamics and the Life Course�����������������������������������������������
      • 11.4 Divorce and Remarriage����������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 12: Working and Relaxing���������������������������������������
      • 12.1 Occupational Selection and Development��������������������������������������������������
      • 12.2 Gender, Ethnicity, and Discrimination Issues��������������������������������������������������������
      • 12.3 Occupational Transitions������������������������������������
      • 12.4 Work and Family���������������������������
      • 12.5 Taking Time to Relax: Leisure Activities����������������������������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 13: Making It in Midlife: The Biopsychosocial Challenges of Middle Adulthood
      • 13.1 Physical Changes and Health���������������������������������������
      • 13.2 Cognitive Development���������������������������������
      • 13.3 Personality�����������������������
      • 13.4 Family Dynamics and Middle Age������������������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
  • Part 4: Late Adulthood�����������������������������
    • Chapter 14: The Personal Context of Later Life: Physical, Cognitive, and Mental Health Issues����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 14.1 What Are Older Adults Like?���������������������������������������
      • 14.2 Physical Changes and Health���������������������������������������
      • 14.3 Cognitive Processes�������������������������������
      • 14.4 Mental Health and Intervention������������������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 15: Social Aspects of Later Life: Psychosocial, Retirement, Relationship, and Societal Issues������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
      • 15.1 Theories of Psychosocial Aging������������������������������������������
      • 15.2 Personality, Social Cognition, and Spirituality�����������������������������������������������������������
      • 15.3 I Used to Work at . . . : Living in Retirement����������������������������������������������������������
      • 15.4 Friends and Family in Late Life�������������������������������������������
      • 15.5 Social Issues and Aging�����������������������������������
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
    • Chapter 16: Dying and Bereavement����������������������������������������
      • 16.1 Definitions and Ethical Issues������������������������������������������
      • 16.2 Thinking about Death: Personal Aspects
      • 16.3 End-of-Life Issues������������������������������
      • 16.4 Surviving the Loss: The Grieving Process����������������������������������������������������
      • 16.5 Dying and Bereavement Experiences across the Life Span
      • Summary��������������
      • Key Terms����������������
  • Glossary
  • References�����������������
  • Name Index�����������������
  • Subject Index��������������������
    1. 2018-01-22T23:51:15+0000
    2. Preflight Ticket Signature